Genesis of the Alien Cortex

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Genesis of the Alien Cortex Genesis of the Alien Cortex What’s the hidden tale behind the monkeys without tails – the eight “Great Apes” or Hominids? And why did one of them – us – become so numerous, so dominant, and even suit up and lope about on the Moon, while our seven cousins hang on the edge of the endangered species list by their fingernails and presumably hardly ever even gaze up at the stars? What set us on such different evolutionary paths? And how come the track we are on now seems to be headed for the destruction of our whole primate family and our home planet? Thanks mainly to our species, five of our evolutionary cousins are “Critically Endangered” (orangutans and gorillas) and two (common chimpanzees and bonobos) are merely classed as “Endangered”. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species lists us in the conservation category of “Least Concern.” But an increasing number of forward-looking ecologists, anthologists, environmentalists and futurists are not as sanguine. Like bacteria in a fermenting vat before their last and fatal doubling, if you take a snapshot-in-time of our species then we are probably in our heyday. But we are also probably at, or nearing, “peak human”. In January 2020 the infamous Doomsday Clock was set to the closest it’s ever been to midnight – a mere 100 seconds short. Yet optimistic writers, like Steven Pinker, are at pains to point out that in “measure after measure, things have never been better.” For humans, that is. For most wild species outside the orbit of our domestic arrangements, things could hardly be worse. More insightful observers, like mathematician Sidney Smith, who take food, fresh water, waste, sources of energy, lack of biodiversity and human habitat, overpopulation and anthropogenic climate instability into account, come to the opposite conclusion. They think that, thanks to the very successes that Pinker celebrates, we will probably not survive the Holocene mass extinction event – a cataclysm of our own making. By cherry-picking a few positives, millenarian utopians like Pinker find it all too easy to brush off the dire big-picture dynamics of global overshoot, drawdown, collapse and unavoidable species extinction. Aside from such extreme points of view, how did our species get to such an extreme? What accident of evolution took us on such a wild excursion away from our closest primate relatives? The 20th Century saw a veritable cottage industry of theories to explain why humans were unique amongst primates (sotto voce … “superior” in the scala naturae or medieval Christian “Great Chain of Being”). Plausible theories were lined up like a coconut shy and knocked down one after another. One popular theory held that making and using tools set our species apart. Then researchers were shocked to discover that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in southeastern Senegal routinely fashioned spears and used them to hunt bushbabies for meat. Jane Goodall discovered wild chimpanzees making specialized toolkits for foraging army ants, and soon various primates were found to use rocks as tools to crack nuts. Observers have even seen both bonobos and chimpanzees making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that they use for grooming. Darwin had a whole section on primates using tools in his book, Descent of Man (1871), but it seems to have been somewhat overlooked. He even mentioned baboons in Abyssinia having pitched battles, where one species used boulders as weapons and rolled them down on their rivals. So much for our uniqueness! If tools where not the key, others claimed that what set humans apart was “culture”. The theory claimed that humans were uniquely able to transmit behaviors socially, through cultural learning, whereas other primates could not. In the finest millenarian tradition, pedagoguery supposedly allowed humans to bank knowledge from generation to generation and therefore gradually accumulate the know-how to make the internal combustion engine, penicillin and rockets to reach the Moon. However, in the 1940s Japanese primatologists made the rather inconvenient discovery that macaques serendipitously discovered that sweet potatoes could be washed and salted in the sea and they transmitted that cultural invention/habit down the generations to this present day merely by active learning. So culture was definitely not unique to humans. By the 1990s the “cultural primatology” section of the library was overflowing with books about animal culture, and in particular chimpanzee culture. Another theory in the history of the human struggle for Apartheid from other primates held that language was the key differentiator. Famed linguist Noam Chomsky claimed that only humans had language. That idea bit the dust in the 1970s when cognitive scientists at Columbia University taught a chimp, scurrilously named Nim Chimpsky, to string sentences together using sign language. However, Herbert Terrace, the man who led the project, eventually became the most vocal critic of “animal language.” Although Nim learned about 125 signs, he was unable to learn syntax or the importance of word order. So Terrace concluded that no chimp could meaningfully master the art of language. “Animal language” is still controversial and hotly debated by the only species that claims linguistic ability exclusively for itself. It’s hard to untangle whether animals are just clever mimics, merely automatically responding to stimulus (like Clever Hans, a horse that looked like he could do basic arithmetic until it was revealed he was just responding to his human audience) or if they are genuinely communicating complex thoughts and feelings. Parrots are one of six groups of animals that develop some form of verbal language in order to function socially. The other six are various cetaceans, songbirds, bats, hummingbirds and primates. Some other animals also instinctively know how to vocalize and interpret the signals their species rely on to communicate and survive. For example, vervet monkeys use different alarm calls for different predators. The “snake” call makes them stand up and scan the ground, the “eagle” call makes them look up and scatter, and the “leopard” call sends them racing to the tops of the trees. This is all evidence of predator classification and semantic communication, so with such a great start, why haven’t monkeys evolved to hunt leopards to extinction with rifles the way we have? We are both close cousins and it wasn’t that long ago that we shared a common ancestor. So how did we turn the tables on our mutual ancestral enemy and tame it to become the domestic cat, while our other close cousins failed to? At the end of the 20th Century, many researchers hoped that the answer to our supposed uniqueness would be revealed in our genes. In 1990, the Human Genome Project (HGP) was launched with the aim of sequencing the entire human genome and identifying all specifically human genes. Scientists waited with great anticipation, optimistically expecting that a cornucopia of information was about to explode onto the scene. They eagerly anticipated finding the “gene for” everything one could possibly imagine. The “gene for” heart disease, sickle-cell disease, clinical depression, schizophrenia, maybe even the “gene for” suicide or cancer seemed on the cusp of being discovered, with the implication that there would be breakthroughs in the treatment of everything that ails our species. Finally it looked like we would get a complete insight into what really differentiates us from other primates. Excitement built as the book on what makes humans human looked like it was about to be opened (and closed) once and for all. In 2000, biologists literally started making a book to bet on the number of genes in the human genome. At the annual Cold Spring Harbor Genome Meeting, attendees took $1 bets on how many genes the human genome would turn out to have when sequencing was finally complete. The mean prediction was 61,170 genes, with the lowest guess at 27,462 and the highest at 153,478. In 2003, biologists were flabbergasted to discover that the human genome had only somewhere in the range of 20,000 to 25,000 genes! When you consider that the human brain alone has 86 billion highly organized neurons, that seems like a very compact instruction set for our entire blueprint. With so few genes, the whole notion of a “gene for” this or that had to be dropped (in all but the popular press, where it’s still in fashion, much to the chagrin of geneticists). Somewhere in that meager stock of physical functional units of human heredity was the crucial 4-letter coding difference between Noam Chomsky and Nim Chimpsky. By 2005 the chimp genome was finally sequenced and it turned out that our genes are about 98% similar. We are actually about close enough to successfully interbreed. Rumors of the “Humanzee” have circulated since the Tarzan movies came out in the 1920s, and in 2019, human-monkey chimeras were almost certainly created at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies in the US. But don’t expect research like that to ever get published! Bigotry and inter-primate Apartheid are still too firmly entrenched in our culture to allow things like that to get through a wall of massive academic censorship. The genetic difference between individual humans is about 0.1%, on average. The average difference between the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and bonobo (Pan paniscus) genomes and ours is about 1.2%. Gorillas turned out to diverge from us by about 1.6%. The Asian great ape, the orangutan, diverges from us African apes by about 3.1%, and rhesus monkeys by about 7%. The age-old philosophical debate about what constituted the divide between humans and other apes was clarified in 2011 when landmark research pinned down what the essential genetic differences coded for (in as much as the concept of a “gene for” something is valid).
Recommended publications
  • Giving Voice to the "Voiceless:" Incorporating Nonhuman Animal Perspectives As Journalistic Sources
    Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Communication Faculty Publications Department of Communication 2011 Giving Voice to the "Voiceless:" Incorporating Nonhuman Animal Perspectives as Journalistic Sources Carrie Packwood Freeman Georgia State University, [email protected] Marc Bekoff Sarah M. Bexell [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/communication_facpub Part of the Journalism Studies Commons, and the Social Influence and oliticalP Communication Commons Recommended Citation Freeman, C. P., Bekoff, M. & Bexell, S. (2011). Giving voice to the voiceless: Incorporating nonhuman animal perspectives as journalistic sources. Journalism Studies, 12(5), 590-607. This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Communication at ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Communication Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. VOICE TO THE VOICELESS 1 A similar version of this paper was later published as: Freeman, C. P., Bekoff, M. & Bexell, S. (2011). Giving Voice to the Voiceless: Incorporating Nonhuman Animal Perspectives as Journalistic Sources, Journalism Studies, 12(5), 590-607. GIVING VOICE TO THE "VOICELESS": Incorporating nonhuman animal perspectives as journalistic sources Carrie Packwood Freeman, Marc Bekoff and Sarah M. Bexell As part of journalism’s commitment to truth and justice
    [Show full text]
  • Human Uniqueness in the Age of Ape Language Research1
    Society and Animals 18 (2010) 397-412 brill.nl/soan Human Uniqueness in the Age of Ape Language Research1 Mary Trachsel University of Iowa [email protected] Abstract This paper summarizes the debate on human uniqueness launched by Charles Darwin’s publi- cation of The Origin of Species in 1859. In the progress of this debate, Noam Chomsky’s intro- duction of the Language-Acquisition Device (LAD) in the mid-1960s marked a turn to the machine model of mind that seeks human uniqueness in uniquely human components of neu- ral circuitry. A subsequent divergence from the machine model can be traced in the short his- tory of ape language research (ALR). In the past fifty years, the focus of ALR has shifted from the search for behavioral evidence of syntax in the minds of individual apes to participant- observation of coregulated interactions between humans and nonhuman apes. Rejecting the computational machine model of mind, the laboratory methodologies of ALR scientists Tetsuro Matsuzawa and Sue Savage-Rumbaugh represent a worldview coherent with Darwin’s continu- ity hypothesis. Keywords ape language research, artificial intelligence, Chomsky, comparative psychology, Darwin, human uniqueness, social cognition Introduction Nothing at first can appear more difficult to believe than that the more complex organs and instincts should have been perfected, not by means superior to, though analogous with, human reason, but by the accumulation of innumerable slight variations, each good for the individual possessor. (Darwin, 1989b, p. 421) With the publication of The Origin of Species (1859/1989a), Charles Darwin steered science directly into a conversation about human uniqueness previ- ously dominated by religion and philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • Human–Animal Communication*
    AN46CH21-Kulick ARI 26 September 2017 7:48 Annual Review of Anthropology Human–Animal Communication∗ Don Kulick Department of Cultural Anthropology and Ethnology, Uppsala University, 751 26, Uppsala, Sweden; email: [email protected] ANNUAL REVIEWS Further Click here to view this article's online features: t%PXOMPBEmHVSFTBT115TMJEFT t/BWJHBUFMJOLFESFGFSFODFT t%PXOMPBEDJUBUJPOT t&YQMPSFSFMBUFEBSUJDMFT t4FBSDILFZXPSET Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2017. 46:357–78 Keywords First published as a Review in Advance on August animal studies, animal communicators, animal training, ape language, 7, 2017 companion species, ethics, pets The Annual Review of Anthropology is online at by [email protected] on 11/02/17. For personal use only. anthro.annualreviews.org Abstract https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-102116- Since the demise in the 1980s of research by psychologists who attempted 041723 Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2017.46:357-378. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org to teach human language to apes, a range of other perspectives has arisen Copyright c 2017 by Annual Reviews. ⃝ that explore how humans can communicate with animals and what the pos- All rights reserved sibility of such communication means. Sociologists interested in symbolic ∗This article is part of a special theme on interactionism, anthropologists writing about ontology, equestrian and ca- Human–Animal Interaction. For a list of other articles in this theme, see http://www. nine trainers, people with autism who say they understand animals because annualreviews.org/doi/full/10.1146/annurev- they think like animals, and a ragbag of sundry New Age women who claim an-46-themes to be able to converse with animals through telepathy have started discussing human–animal communication in ways that recast the whole point of think- ing about it.
    [Show full text]
  • Defining Language in the Wake of Primate Language Research
    Defining Language in the Wake of Primate Language Research Hanna L. Meyer December 2018 Abstract This text examines language as it is used in the animal language debate through pragmatic and structural linguistic perspectives on primate language research. The surge of primate language studies in America in the 1970's generated a wave of public and academic interest in animal language that continues today in the form of ongoing primate research both in the lab and in the wild. These studies have forced the field to examine the way it conceptualizes language, as well as the current criteria with which we use to define it I argue that the traditional linguistic approach to defining language, which measures language through surface level features, as can be seen in Hockett's design features, cannot fully describe language, and instead must include a more pragmatic perspective in order to more accurately measure primate language. Finally I argue that the term language, as it has historically been used to describe only human language is useless, as its exclusivity ignores the gradient of the complexity of higher mental faculties across the evolutionary tree. 1 Introduction From the domestication of dogs approximately 30,000 years ago (Saey, 2017), of horses 5,500 years ago (Wilfred, 2009), and the creation of the first zoo 5,000 years ago (Boissoneault, 2015), it is clear that the human interest in interacting with different species is deeply rooted in our natural history. Central to our interest in non-human animals is an interest in their communication abilities. It is impossible to understand 1 the cognitive abilities or social hierarchies of a species without an understanding of their system(s) of communication.
    [Show full text]
  • Non-Human Primates and Language: Paper
    Non-human primates and language: paper http://www.angelfire.com/sc2/nhplanguage/ftpaper.html Language competence in NHPs An assessment of the field in the light of a 'universal grammar' "The Berlin wall is down, and so is the wall that separates man from chimpanzee." (Elizabeth Bates) "There is no debate, so I have no opinion." (Noam Chomsky) 0 Introduction The language competence of non-human primates is one of the most controversial issues in present-day linguistics, with disbelief ranging from bored indifference to vitriolic accusations of fraud. The present paper aims to assess the current state of debate from an open-minded, critical and detached perspective. In a first part, a brief outline of earlier research in the language abilities of non-human primates - more precisely of apes (bonobos, urang-utangs, chimpanzees and gorillas) - is sketched. The second part focusses on the landmark studies published by Dr. Emily Sue Savage-Rumbaugh and her colleagues. A third section looks into the views of the Chomskyan field, leading up to the concluding section on the innateness debate. 1 Early research on non-human primates' capability for language 1.1 Attempts to teach NHPs to speak The language capability of non-human primates has been a subject of research since the beginning of this century. In 1909 already did Witmer attempt to teach a chimpanzee to talk. He claims that the chimpanzee was capable of articulating the word ‘mama’. In 1916 Furness taught an orang-utan to say the words ‘papa’ and ‘cup’. After the unexpected death of this orang-utan, Kellogg and Kellogg wanted to follow up this work.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Umwelten in a Changing World
    Tartu Semiotics Library 18 Tartu Tartu Semiotics Library 18 Animal umwelten in a changing world: Zoosemiotic perspectives represents a clear and concise review of zoosemiotics, present- ing theories, models and methods, and providing interesting examples of human–animal interactions. The reader is invited to explore the umwelten of animals in a successful attempt to retrieve the relationship of people with animals: a cornerstone of the past common evolutionary processes. The twelve chapters, which cover recent developments in zoosemiotics and much more, inspire the reader to think about the human condition and about ways to recover our lost contact with the animal world. Written in a clear, concise style, this collection of articles creates a wonderful bridge between Timo Maran, Morten Tønnessen, human and animal worlds. It represents a holistic approach Kristin Armstrong Oma, rich with suggestions for how to educate people to face the dynamic relationships with nature within the conceptual Laura Kiiroja, Riin Magnus, framework of the umwelt, providing stimulus and opportuni- Nelly Mäekivi, Silver Rattasepp, ties to develop new studies in zoosemiotics. Professor Almo Farina, CHANGING WORLD A IN UMWELTEN ANIMAL Paul Thibault, Kadri Tüür University of Urbino “Carlo Bo” This important book offers the first coherent gathering of perspectives on the way animals are communicating with each ANIMAL UMWELTEN other and with us as environmental change requires increasing adaptation. Produced by a young generation of zoosemiotics scholars engaged in international research programs at Tartu, IN A CHANGING this work introduces an exciting research field linking the biological sciences with the humanities. Its key premises are that all animals participate in a dynamic web of meanings WORLD: and signs in their own distinctive styles, and all animal spe- cies have distinctive cultures.
    [Show full text]
  • Bio 314 Animal Behv Bio314new
    NATIONAL OPEN UNIVERSITY OF NIGERIA SCHOOL OF SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY COURSE CODE: BIO 314: COURSE TITLE: ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR xii i BIO 314: ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR Course Writers/Developers Miss Olakolu Fisayo Christie Nigerian Institute for Oceanography and Marine Research, No 3 Wilmot Point Road, Bar-beach Bus-stop, Victoria Island, Lagos, Nigeria. Course Editor: Dr. Adesina Adefunke Ministry of Health, Alausa. Lagos NATIONAL OPEN UNIVERSITY OF NIGERIA xii i BIO 314 COURSE GUIDE Introduction Animal Behaviour (314) is a second semester course. It is a two credit units compustory course which all students offering Bachelor of Science (BSc) in Biology must take. This course deals with the theories and principles of adaptive behaviour and evolution of animals. The course contents are history of ethology. Reflex and complex behaviour. Orientation and taxes. Fixed action patterns, releasers, motivation and driver. Displays, displacement activities and conflict behaviour. Learning communication and social behaviour. The social behaviour of primates. Hierarchical organization. The physiology of behaviour. Habitat selection, homing and navigation. Courtship and parenthood. Biological clocks. What you will learn in this course In this course, you have the course units and a course guide. The course guide will tell you briefly what the course is all about. It is a general overview of the course materials you will be using and how to use those materials. It also helps you to allocate the appropriate time to each unit so that you can successfully complete the course within the stipulated time limit. The course guide also helps you to know how to go about your Tutor-Marked-Assignment which will form part of your overall assessment at the end of the course.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Communication & Language
    9781446295649_C.indd 6 14/06/2017 17:18 00_CARPENDALE ET AL_FM.indd 3 11/14/2017 4:45:07 PM Animal Communication and Human 7 Language LEARNING OUTCOMES By the end of this chapter you should: • Understand how the study of animal communication informs us about the nature and sophistication of human communication. • Be able to discuss the details of the communication patterns of vervet monkeys and honeybees. • Know that attempts to teach apes to speak have been conducted for a hundred years and why those based on behavioural training were inconclusive. • Be able to define what a LAD and a LASS are (and know their theoretical differences). • Be able to discuss the differences between human and animal communication and therefore the complexity of the latter. • Be aware of how more recent training programmes based on social interaction have changed our understanding of how apes may learn to communicate with humans as well as how they have informed our understanding of children’s early language development. Do animals use languages? Can dogs learn words? Rico, a 9-year-old border collie, was able to learn 200 words (Kaminski, Call, & Fischer, 2004). But are these really words in the same sense that humans use them? What Rico had learned was to fetch 200 different 07_CARPENDALE ET AL_CH_07.indd 121 11/14/2017 10:48:26 AM 122 THE DEVELOPMENT OF CHILDREN’S THINKING objects (Bloom, 2004). This is an incredibly impressive feat, but what does it tell us about human languages? When a child learns a word, more is expected than the ability to fetch the object that it identifies.
    [Show full text]
  • How Prejudice Affects the Study of Animal Minds by Ashley Elizabeth Keefner
    How Prejudice Affects the Study of Animal Minds by Ashley Elizabeth Keefner A thesis presented to the University of Waterloo in fulfillment of the thesis requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Philosophy Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, 2017 © Ashley Elizabeth Keefner 2017 Examining Committee Membership The following served on the Examining Committee for this thesis. The decision of the Examining Committee is by majority vote. Supervisor Dr. Paul Thagard, Distinguished Professor Emeritus of Philosophy, University of Waterloo Internal Member Dr. John Turri, Canada Research Chair in Philosophy & Cognitive Science, Associate Professor of Philosophy, University of Waterloo Internal Member Dr. Katy Fulfer, Assistant Professor of Philosophy, University of Waterloo Internal-External Member Dr. Alice Kuzniar, University Research Chair, Professor of German and English, University of Waterloo External Examiner Dr. Andrew Fenton, Assistant Professor of Philosophy, Dalhousie University ii Author’s Declaration I hereby declare that I am the sole author of this thesis. This is a true copy of the thesis, including any required final revisions, as accepted by my examiners. I understand that my thesis may be made electronically available to the public. iii Abstract Humans share the planet with many wonderfully diverse animal species and human- animal interactions are part of our daily lives. An important part of understanding how humans do and should interact with other animals is understanding how humans think about other animals. In this thesis, I argue that how humans think about the minds of other animals is marked by prejudice and that this prejudice fosters epistemological, metaphysical, and ethical problems related to study of, the conception of, and the conclusions we draw about animal minds.
    [Show full text]
  • First Language
    First Language http://fla.sagepub.com/ Semiotic combinations in Pan: A comparison of communication in a chimpanzee and two bonobos Heidi Lyn, Patricia M. Greenfield and E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh First Language 2011 31: 300 originally published online 18 March 2011 DOI: 10.1177/0142723710391872 The online version of this article can be found at: http://fla.sagepub.com/content/31/3/300 Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com Additional services and information for First Language can be found at: Email Alerts: http://fla.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://fla.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Citations: http://fla.sagepub.com/content/31/3/300.refs.html >> Version of Record - Jul 26, 2011 OnlineFirst Version of Record - May 10, 2011 OnlineFirst Version of Record - Mar 18, 2011 What is This? Downloaded from fla.sagepub.com at UCLA on April 20, 2014 FIRST Article LANGUAGE First Language 31(3) 300–325 Semiotic combinations © The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permission: sagepub. in Pan: A comparison co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0142723710391872 of communication in a fla.sagepub.com chimpanzee and two bonobos Heidi Lyn University of Southern Mississippi, USA Patricia M. Greenfield University of California, Los Angeles, USA E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh Georgia State University, USA Abstract Communicative combinations of two bonobos (Pan paniscus) and a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) are compared. All three apes utilized ordering strategies for combining symbols (lexigrams) or a lexigram with a gesture to express semantic relations such as agent of action or object of action.
    [Show full text]
  • Language (Including Signjanguage)
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 385 148 FL 023 146 AUTHOR Jaramillo, James A. TITLE Can Human-Taught Primates Produce aNon-Verbal Language? PUB DATE 26 Jun 95 NOTE 83p. PUB TYPE Information Analyses (070) Reports Evaluative /Feasibility (142) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC04 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Animal Behavior; *Cognitive Processes;*Language Acquisition; *Language Processing; Language Research; Learning Processes; Linguistic Theory;*Nonverbal Communication; Primatology; Sign Language;*Visual Stimuli ABSTRACT The debate over whether primates can betaught visual language is examined, and evidence of useof nonverbal language in primate studies is compared with the languagecriteria of a number of linguistic researchers. Background information onlanguage, visual language (including signjanguage), andthe parameters of the studies is offered, including oral and human languagecriteria, conception of grammar, and use of word symbols(chip symbology). The performance of four apes in different studies, usingdifferent methods to teach visual language, is then examined in termsof these parameters. It is concluded that the apes can mentallymanipulate abstract concepts that have been defined by means of anarbitrary code, and that this manipulation involves mentally scanning a setof symbols and cognitively selecting one on the basis of itsspecific linguistic context. Ape results proved to belinguistically coded and expressed, establishing true linguistic comprehensiveproduction. Despite the fact that the ape linguistic abilities werefar below the level of adult communication,
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Communication
    16Ch16_OGrady.QXD 1/8/08 3:27 AM Page 514 CHAPTER Sixteen Michael Dobrovolsky Animal communication As I listened from a beach-chair in the shade To all the noises that my garden made, It seemed to me only proper that words Should be withheld from vegetables and birds. W.H. AUDEN ommunication—the passing on or exchange of information—distinguishes what is living Cfrom what is non-living in nature.1 Communication is found even in the apparently passive world of plants; trees, for example, have been found to pass on information about advancing predators by means of chemical signals. Animals communicate among them- selves and with humans so effectively that they are often said to use ‘language’. But the words communication and (human) language do not mean the same thing. Human language is a specific way of communicating, but not just any form of communication qualifies as language. A question that therefore interests many linguists is whether animals make use of any system of communication that genuinely resembles or approximates human language. If animals communicate with a system that is structured like human language, then language as we know it is not the unique property of our species. This chapter investigates the ways in which animal communication is like human language and the ways in which it is different. 16.1 Non-vocal communication One of the most striking things about animal communication is the variety of means through which it is carried out. Animals communicate not only with sounds but with scent, light, ultrasound, visual signs (see figure 16.1), gestures, colour, and even electricity.
    [Show full text]