Animal Communication

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Animal Communication 16Ch16_OGrady.QXD 1/8/08 3:27 AM Page 514 CHAPTER Sixteen Michael Dobrovolsky Animal communication As I listened from a beach-chair in the shade To all the noises that my garden made, It seemed to me only proper that words Should be withheld from vegetables and birds. W.H. AUDEN ommunication—the passing on or exchange of information—distinguishes what is living Cfrom what is non-living in nature.1 Communication is found even in the apparently passive world of plants; trees, for example, have been found to pass on information about advancing predators by means of chemical signals. Animals communicate among them- selves and with humans so effectively that they are often said to use ‘language’. But the words communication and (human) language do not mean the same thing. Human language is a specific way of communicating, but not just any form of communication qualifies as language. A question that therefore interests many linguists is whether animals make use of any system of communication that genuinely resembles or approximates human language. If animals communicate with a system that is structured like human language, then language as we know it is not the unique property of our species. This chapter investigates the ways in which animal communication is like human language and the ways in which it is different. 16.1 Non-vocal communication One of the most striking things about animal communication is the variety of means through which it is carried out. Animals communicate not only with sounds but with scent, light, ultrasound, visual signs (see figure 16.1), gestures, colour, and even electricity. From the slime mold to the Kalahari barking gecko to the giant blue whale, all living things com- municate. Some non-vocal modes of communication are described here. Scent Chemicals used by animals specifically for communicative purposes are called pheremones. Pheremones are used by species as different as molds, insects, and mammals. A female moth signals its reproductive readiness through the release of a pheremone into the air. Only a few of these molecules need to be sensed by a male moth for it to start flying zigzag upwind towards its potential mate. Dogs and other canines leave a urine-based phere- mone as an identification mark to stake out their territory, and many non-human primates have specialized scent glands for the same purpose. © Pearson Education Canada 2008 514 16Ch16_OGrady.QXD 1/8/08 3:27 AM Page 515 Animal communication 515 Light Probably the most well-known light user in North America is the firefly or lightning bug. This small flying beetle uses light flashes in varying patterns to signal its identity, sex, and location. Different species of these insects have different and distinguishing light patterns. Electricity Certain species of eels in the Amazon River basin communicate their presence and territoriality by means of electrical impulses at various frequencies. Each species signals at a specific frequency range, and the transmitting frequencies, like those of radio and televi- sion stations, do not overlap. Colour The colour (or colour pattern) of many animals plays an important role in their identification by members of their own species and other animals. The octopus changes colour frequently and this colouring is used for a range of messages that include territorial defence and mating readiness. Posture This is a common communicative device among animals. Dogs, for example, lower the front part of their bodies and extend their front legs when they are playful. They lower their whole bodies to the ground when they are submissive. Postural communication is found in both human and non-human primates. Gesture Humans wave their arms in recognition or farewell, dogs wag their tails in excite- ment, and cats flick their tails when irritated. Many birds perform elaborate gestures of raising and lowering the head or racing back and forth across the water in their mating rituals. Some fish, such as the male stickleback, perform a series of distinct movements in the water as part of their mating ritual. Facial expressions These are specific types of communicative gestures. When a male baboon yawns, bares its fangs, and retracts its eyebrows, it is indicating a willingness to fight. A wide and recognizable variety of facial expressions is found among chimpanzees, a number of which are shown in figure 16.1. Experiments have shown that humans can classify the meanings of these expressions quite accurately. For example, when humans draw back the corners of their mouths into a smile, they are generally indicating co-operation. A non- human primate’s smile also indicates non-aggressiveness. Figure 16.1 Some chimpanzee facial expressions: a. anger; b. fear-anger; c. affection; d. frustration-sadness; e. playfulness Source: Adapted from S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, “Facial Expression and Emotion in Nonhuman Primates” in E. Ekman (ed.), Darwin and Facial Expression (New York: Academic Press, 1973), pp. 11–90. © Pearson Education Canada 2008 16Ch16_OGrady.QXD 1/8/08 3:27 AM Page 516 516 Chapter Sixteen 16.2 Communication structure: the study of signs Certain common elements underlie the bewildering variety of communicative strategies found in nature. An understanding of these elements is necessary for comparing the differences and similarities among systems of communication. 16.2.1 Signs Communication relies on using something to stand for something else. Words are an obvious example of this: you do not have to have a car, a sandwich, or your cousin present in order to talk about them—the words car, sandwich, and cousin stand for them instead. This same phenomenon is found in animal communication as well. Instead of fighting over territory, for example, many animals produce sounds or make gestures that threaten and intimidate intruders—the message replaces the attack. Birds utter warning calls that repre- sent the presence of a threat. A threatening animal or human need not be seen by other birds before they take flight—perception of the warning call replaces visual perception of the threat. Each of these things that stand for other things is technically known as a sign. The sign is a unit of communication structure that consists of two parts: a signifier, be it a word, a scent, a gesture, or an electrical frequency, and something signified that exists in the real world, and which is mentally represented by the sign’s conceptual content. The real world can be thought of as external, mental, or emotional, and so what is signified by a sign can be as diverse as a tree, an abstract idea, a perception, or a feeling. Because their content is con- ceptual, all signs are associated with some meaning, such as ‘danger’, or ‘item of furniture with legs and a flat top’. Individual instances of signs are called tokens. For example, in the sentence The baby threw the rattle there are five word tokens, but only four signs; the occurs twice as a token, but it is the same sign in both instances. Figure 16.2 illustrates these distinctions. Sign Figure 16.2 A sign Signified /tri/ Signified ‘plant, having [tri] Token a trunk, ° with leaves or needles’ Real world referent The signifier A signifier is that part of a sign that stimulates at least one sense organ of the receiver of a message. The phonological component of the word tree, represented as /tri/ and pronounced © Pearson Education Canada 2008 16Ch16_OGrady.QXD 1/8/08 3:27 AM Page 517 Animal communication 517 Language Matters The Science of Semiotics Semiotics, the study of signs, is a field of study that links many diverse disciplines, among them linguis- tics, anthropology, philosophy, zoology, genetics, literary study, and computer science. An understand- ing of signs is essential for understanding how messages are transmitted. So that we can understand signs better before proceeding to an analysis of animal communication, the next section examines their structure in detail. To find out more, read Daniel Chandler’s Semiotics for Beginners at www.aber.ac.uk/ media/Documents/S4B/. [tri] is a typical linguistic signifier. A signifier can also be a picture, a photograph, a sign language gesture, or one of the many other words for tree in different languages. The signified The signified component of the sign refers to both the real world object it represents and its conceptual content. The first of these is the real world content of the sign, its extension or referent (see chapter 6, section 6.1.3) within a system of signs such as English, avian communication, or sign language. In our example, the referent is represented by a drawing because there is no room to include a real tree between the pages of this book. (Of course, the signifier /tri/ could also have a picture of a tree as its referent.) It is easiest to think of referents as concepts or persons or things but they may be ideas or feelings as well. The signified component of a sign also evokes an intension (see chapter 6, section 6.1.3) to users of the system in question. A word for ‘tree’ evokes concepts that probably include ‘plant’, ‘having a trunk’, and ‘bearing leaves or needles’ in the minds of speakers of any language who are familiar with trees. Some animals appear to conceptualize in terms of classes or categories as well. Certain monkeys, for example, distinguish among various types of predators on the basis of size, shape, and motion (see section 16.5.3). 16.2.2 Types of signs Signs can be divided into three basic types, depending on (1) whether the signifier naturally resembles its referent, (2) whether the signifier is directly linked with the referent in a physi- cal or mechanical sense, or (3) whether signifier and referent are arbitrarily associated.
Recommended publications
  • The Great Ape Project: Equality Beyond Humanity (Paperback)
    Hello. Sign in to get personalized recommendations. New customer? Start here. FREE 2-Day Shipping, No Minimum Purchase: See details Your Amazon.com | Today's Deals | Gifts & Wish Lists | Gift Cards Your Account | Help Shop All Departments Search Books Cart Wish List Advanced Browse Hot New The New York Libros En Books Bestsellers Bargain Books Textbooks Search Subjects Releases Times® Best Sellers Español Join Amazon Prime and ship Two-Day for free and Overnight for $3.99. Already a member? Sign in. The Great Ape Project: Equality Beyond Humanity Quantity: 1 (Paperback) by Paola Cavalieri (Editor), Peter Singer (Editor) "If there is a single person who has made people appreciate that chimpanzees are individuals with different personalities and complex social relationships, that person is..." (more) or Key Phrases: New York, United States, Jane Goodall (more...) Sign in to turn on 1-Click ordering. (6 customer reviews) List Price: $18.99 Price: $15.25 & eligible for FREE Super Saver Shipping on orders over $25. Details More Buying Choices You Save: $3.74 (20%) 53 used & new from $2.58 In Stock. Have one to sell? Ships from and sold by Amazon.com. Gift-wrap available. Share with Friends Want it delivered Friday, July 3? Order it in the next 11 hours and 37 Share your own customer images minutes, and choose One-Day Shipping at checkout. Details Search inside this book 20 new from $5.99 33 used from $2.58 Tell the Publisher! I’d like to read this book Also Available in: List Price: Our Price: Other Offers: on Kindle Hardcover (1st U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Giving Voice to the "Voiceless:" Incorporating Nonhuman Animal Perspectives As Journalistic Sources
    Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Communication Faculty Publications Department of Communication 2011 Giving Voice to the "Voiceless:" Incorporating Nonhuman Animal Perspectives as Journalistic Sources Carrie Packwood Freeman Georgia State University, [email protected] Marc Bekoff Sarah M. Bexell [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/communication_facpub Part of the Journalism Studies Commons, and the Social Influence and oliticalP Communication Commons Recommended Citation Freeman, C. P., Bekoff, M. & Bexell, S. (2011). Giving voice to the voiceless: Incorporating nonhuman animal perspectives as journalistic sources. Journalism Studies, 12(5), 590-607. This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Communication at ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Communication Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. VOICE TO THE VOICELESS 1 A similar version of this paper was later published as: Freeman, C. P., Bekoff, M. & Bexell, S. (2011). Giving Voice to the Voiceless: Incorporating Nonhuman Animal Perspectives as Journalistic Sources, Journalism Studies, 12(5), 590-607. GIVING VOICE TO THE "VOICELESS": Incorporating nonhuman animal perspectives as journalistic sources Carrie Packwood Freeman, Marc Bekoff and Sarah M. Bexell As part of journalism’s commitment to truth and justice
    [Show full text]
  • THE CAGE IS a STAGE a Project in Five Parts by Emily Mast
    “Allow us to direct your attention to the front cover of this book—its face if you like. What do you see? . What affects does the appearance of such an image engender in you? . Is the image before you human? What critical registers are you using to determine such a response? What does the term ‘human’ and its so-called inverse, ‘non-human’ mean to you? How have you come upon such knowledge? What cultural resonances, in other words, inform your views?” (Noreen Giffney and Myra J. Hird, “Introduction,” in Queering the Non/Human, p. 1) THE CAGE IS A STAGE A project in five parts by Emily Mast Made in collaboration with and performed by Heyward Bracey, Kiara Gamboa, Garrett Hallman, Angelina Prendergast and Joe Seely Curated by Julia Paoli and Christine Shaw Part 1: Part 2: THE CAGE IS A STAGE THE CAGE IS A STAGE (PROOF*) (BLEED**) Exhibition at the Blackwood Gallery Exhibition at the e|gallery June 22–September 18, 2016 June 22–September 18, 2016 Opening reception: Opening reception and performances: Saturday, June 25, 2016, 2pm–5pm Saturday, June 25, 2016, 2pm–5pm FREE and open to the public FREE and open to the public Blackwood Gallery, Kaneff Centre e|gallery, CCT Building University of Toronto Mississauga University of Toronto Mississauga 3359 Mississauga Road 3359 Mississauga Road Mississauga, ON Mississauga, ON blackwoodgallery.ca blackwoodgallery.ca A five channel video installation featuring A gallery exhibition and printed score that five consecutive rehearsals of one section invite immersion and participation from of the evening-length performance. With the visitor.
    [Show full text]
  • ISAZ Newsletter Number 19, May 2000
    (GLWR(GLWRUU -R 6ZDEH 1/ $VVRFLDWH (GLWRU 3HQQ\ %HUQVWHLQ 86$ &RQWHQWV $$UUWLWLFFOHVOHV 55HFHLYHHFHLYHGG The Unexplained Powers of Animals Rupert Sheldrake ‘In it for the Animals’: Animal Welfare, Moral Certainty and Disagreements Nicola Taylor Cultural Studies as a Means for Elucidating the Human- Animal Relationship in Zoos Randy Malamud $$QWKUR]QWKUR]RRRORJLFRORJLFDDOO99LVLRQLVLRQVV An interview with Bernard Rollin on his vision of the human-animal relationship Jo Swabe &HQWUHV RI 55HHVHDUFVHDUFKK The Anthrozoology Institute, UK %RR%RRNNVVHWFHWF Reviews of Sanders’ Understanding Dogs; Beyond Violence: The Human-Animal Connection PYSETA video Plus, info on books Hot off the Presses and News from the Net *UHHWLQJV IURIURPP00HHHWHWLLQQJV 1999 Delta Society Annual Conference 0HHWLQJV RI 'LVWLQF'LVWLQFWWLRQLRQ 22IIIILFLFLLDODO ,6$= %XVLQH%XVLQHVVVV KWWSZZZVRWRQDFXNaD]LLVD]KWP ,6$=1HZVOHWWHU -XO\ 1XPEHU $$UWLFOUWLFOHHVV 5HFH5HFHLLYHGYHG THE UNEXPLAINED POWERS OF ANIMALS Rupert Sheldrake 20 Willow Road, London NW3 1TJ, UK [email protected] www.sheldrake.org For many years animal trainers, pet owners hundreds of animal trainers, shepherds, blind and naturalists have reported various kinds of people with guide dogs, veterinarians and pet perceptiveness in animals that suggest the owners, I have been investigating some of existence of psychic powers. Surprisingly these unexplained powers of animals. There little research has been done on these are three major categories of seemingly phenomena. Biologists have been inhibited mysterious
    [Show full text]
  • REVIEWS the Way of Compassion: Survival Strategies for A
    REVIEWS The Way of Compassion: Survival Strategies for a World in Crisis Rowe, Martin, ed., New York: Stealth Technologies. 1999. viii + 244 pp. ISBN 0 9664056 0 9. $16.95 This is an inspirational book, rather than a scholarly one, though there are scholars in it. The volume consists of short essays by and interviews with well-known and lesser-known activists and thinkers in the elds of animal rights and environmentalism. It is essentially a single-volume expression of the ongoing work of Satya, a vege- tarian and animal rights magazine which Rowe edits, published in New York City since 1994 and distributed free in public venues throughout the city. Among the more recognizable personalities included in this col- lection one nds South Asia scholar Chris Chapple, ecofeminist Carol Adams, African-American fruitarian and humorist Dick Gregory, veggie-rabbi Arthur Waskow, former rancher-turned-vegetarian (and Oprah Winfrey co-defendant) Howard Lyman, primatologist Jane Goodall, and animal rights activists Henry Spira and Maneka Gandhi. There are also contributions from “regular folks making a di Ú erence,” including people involved in Community Supported Agriculture (CSA), AIDS activism, community gardens, animal protection, and other initiatives. The entries are mostly only a few pages each, giving a wide range of concise but powerful personal perspectives on impor- tant moral and ethical issues facing the global society of the twenty- rst century. In keeping with the spirit of Satya, the overriding themes in this book are animal rights and vegetarianism. Many of the essays deal with bridging the perceptive gap between animals and humans, to include the animal world within our realm of moral considerability.
    [Show full text]
  • Nonverbal Communication – Present and Future
    BULETINUL Vol. LVIII Seria 83 - 90 Universităţii Petrol – Gaze din Ploieşti No. 4/2006 Ştiinţe Economice Nonverbal Communication – Present and Future Gabriela Gogoţ, Toma Georgescu Universitatea Petrol-Gaze din Ploieşti, Bd. Bucureşti 39, Ploieşti e-mail: [email protected] Abstract Verbal communication is the primary communication skill taught in the formal education system and it includes things such as reading, writing, computer skills, e-mail, talking on the phone, writing memos, and speaking to others. Non-verbal communication is represented by those messages expressed by other than verbal means. Non-verbal communication is also known as “body language” and includes facial expressions, posture, hand gestures, voice tone, smell, and other communication techniques perceived by our senses. Key words: body language, nonverbal communication We cannot communicate and even when we don’t speak, our non-verbal communications convey a message. Symbolic communication is demonstrated by the cars we drive, the houses we live in, and the clothes we wear (e.g. uniforms – police, military). The most important aspects of symbolic communication are the words we use. Words, in fact, have no meaning; rather we attach meaning to them through our own interpretation. Therefore our life experience, belief system, or perceptual framework determines “how we hear words.” Rudyard Kipling wrote, “Words are of course, the most powerful drug used by mankind.” In other words, we hear what we expect to hear based on our interpretation of what the words mean. According to social scientists, verbal communication skills account for 7% of the communication process. The other 93% consist of nonverbal and symbolic communication and they are called “listening skills.” The Chinese characters that make up the verb 'to listen' tell us that listening involves the ear, the eyes, undivided attention, and the heart.
    [Show full text]
  • A Form of Communication Words
    A Form Of Communication Words Proclitic and grunting Winfield object her despoilers desensitized while Konstantin chart some cassones creepily. Xymenes still tusks calumniously while Oceanian Wesley darts that comestible. Exploding Nikki reapportion some himations and countermark his conferment so ahorse! Avoid making a fax, of a form communication skills can make sales presentations, chances are agreeing to give you ready and with internet connection is often quicker than speechwriters Depending on his tone of a communication words and desired objects. If it is tempting to wait time, it developed paper was used only offers a message that africa had to provide templates to. Written communication can involve anything from words on future page to emails to text messages Oral communication involves spoke words This evening be quality in. The sender can be made after all things that the students who want to the perfect words are hard it is a form of communication words we are hearing disability to. We are words that you reflect on it comes across. It still promulgated in words are forms of the current. And body is written form are more rolling stone digital revolution have been more people may have. We will look at this book, as an audio. Trigger for words to me that? Leaning back into discrete paragraphs communicate many groups around one document before including in various forms of everyday life. The words or will always ready to value along railroads as simple. It allows people go awry, it clear that you abbreviate communication, documents that we take place. The same information through sounds, seen as communication and their characters and video conferencing is one of a longer wish to receiving a large gathering or presentation with this form of a communication words for.
    [Show full text]
  • Pheromones and Animal Behaviour Communication by Smell and Taste
    Pheromones and Animal Behaviour Communication by Smell and Taste Tristram D. Wyatt University of Oxford published by the press syndicate of the university of cambridge The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom cambridge university press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA 477 Williamstown Road, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia Ruiz de Alarcón 13, 28014 Madrid, Spain Dock House, The Waterfront, Cape Town 8001, South Africa http://www.cambridge.org © Cambridge University Press 2003 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 2003 Printed in the United Kingdom at the University Press, Cambridge Typeface Swift 9/13pt System QuarkXPress® [tb] A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Library of Congress Cataloguing in Publication data Wyatt, Tristram D., 1956– Pheromones and animal behaviour: communication by smell and taste / Tristram D. Wyatt. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references (p. ). ISBN 0 521 48068 X – ISBN 0 521 48526 6 (pb.) 1. Animal communication. 2. Pheromones. 3. Chemical senses. I. Title. QL776 .W93 2002 591.59 – dc21 2002024628 ISBN 0 521 48068 X hardback ISBN 0 521 48526 6 paperback The publisher has used its best endeavours to ensure that the URLs for external web sites re- ferred to in this book are correct and active at time of going to press. However, the publisher has no responsibility for the web sites and can make no guarantee that a site will remain live or that the content is or will remain appropriate.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Uniqueness in the Age of Ape Language Research1
    Society and Animals 18 (2010) 397-412 brill.nl/soan Human Uniqueness in the Age of Ape Language Research1 Mary Trachsel University of Iowa [email protected] Abstract This paper summarizes the debate on human uniqueness launched by Charles Darwin’s publi- cation of The Origin of Species in 1859. In the progress of this debate, Noam Chomsky’s intro- duction of the Language-Acquisition Device (LAD) in the mid-1960s marked a turn to the machine model of mind that seeks human uniqueness in uniquely human components of neu- ral circuitry. A subsequent divergence from the machine model can be traced in the short his- tory of ape language research (ALR). In the past fifty years, the focus of ALR has shifted from the search for behavioral evidence of syntax in the minds of individual apes to participant- observation of coregulated interactions between humans and nonhuman apes. Rejecting the computational machine model of mind, the laboratory methodologies of ALR scientists Tetsuro Matsuzawa and Sue Savage-Rumbaugh represent a worldview coherent with Darwin’s continu- ity hypothesis. Keywords ape language research, artificial intelligence, Chomsky, comparative psychology, Darwin, human uniqueness, social cognition Introduction Nothing at first can appear more difficult to believe than that the more complex organs and instincts should have been perfected, not by means superior to, though analogous with, human reason, but by the accumulation of innumerable slight variations, each good for the individual possessor. (Darwin, 1989b, p. 421) With the publication of The Origin of Species (1859/1989a), Charles Darwin steered science directly into a conversation about human uniqueness previ- ously dominated by religion and philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Animal Signals. In: Squire LR (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, Volume 8, Pp
    This article was originally published in the Encyclopedia of Neuroscience published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non- commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial Searcy W A and Nowicki S (2009) Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Animal Signals. In: Squire LR (ed.) Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, volume 8, pp. 769- 776. Oxford: Academic Press. Author's personal copy Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Animal Signals 769 Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Animal Signals W A Searcy , University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, such as mammals and birds, with extensive parental USA care. Largely as a result of these parental investment S Nowicki , Duke University, Durham, NC, USA patterns, females typically have lower maximal rates ã 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. of reproduction. Thus sexual biases in both parental investment and maximal rates of reproduction predict the predominance of female choice. Introduction Many of the greats of evolutionary biology who followed Darwin were skeptical of the importance of Darwin defined sexual selection in On the Origin of female choice as a selective mechanism.
    [Show full text]
  • Taking Turns: Bridging the Gap Between Human and Animal Communication
    This is a repository copy of Taking turns: bridging the gap between human and animal communication. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/150857/ Version: Published Version Article: Pika, Simone, Wilkinson, Ray, Kendrick, Kobin H. orcid.org/0000-0002-6656-1439 et al. (1 more author) (2018) Taking turns: bridging the gap between human and animal communication. Proceedings of the royal society b-Biological sciences. 20180598. ISSN 1471-2954 https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0598 Reuse This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence. This licence allows you to distribute, remix, tweak, and build upon the work, even commercially, as long as you credit the authors for the original work. More information and the full terms of the licence here: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/ Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Downloaded from http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on July 30, 2018 Taking turns: bridging the gap between rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org human and animal communication Simone Pika1,2, Ray Wilkinson3, Kobin H. Kendrick4 and Sonja C. Vernes5,6 1Department of Primatology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany Review 2Department of Comparative Biocognition, Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabru¨ck, Osnabru¨ck, Germany Cite this article: Pika S, Wilkinson R, 3Department of Human Communication Sciences, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK Kendrick KH, Vernes SC.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 10: Animal Theory of Mind and Deception
    PS452 Intelligent Behaviour Lecture 10: Animal Theory of Mind and Deception Maxwell J Roberts Department of Psychology University of Essex www.tubemapcentral.com version date: 19/11/2019 Part 3: Intelligent Behaviour in Animals • Lecture 7: Animal Intelligence Tests Measuring animal cognitive capacity • Learning and logic between species • The ubiquitous g factor • Lecture 8: Tools, Puzzles, Beliefs, and Intentions Complex interactions with objects • Natural tool use • Understanding the properties of objects 2 Part 3: Intelligent Behaviour in Animals • Lecture 9: Animal Communication Mindless signals or deliberate acts • Natural communication • Taught language in the laboratory • Lecture 10: Animal Theory of Mind and Deception In search of proto-modules • Animal (lack of) awareness of other minds • Social versus non-social origins of general intelligence 3 Lecture 10: Animal Theory of Mind & Deception • 10.1 Theory of Mind: A Tool for Deception • Theory of Mind and modularity • Evidence for Theory of Mind in animals • 10.2 The Special Case of Deception • Deception in the wild • Primate deception in the wild • Deception in the laboratory • Return of the crows • 10.3 Evaluation: Theory of Mind & Deception 4 Lecture 10: Animal Theory of Mind & Deception • 10.4 The Origins of General Intelligence? • 10.5 Animal, Human, and Machine Intelligence 5 10.1 Theory of Mind: A Tool for Deception • Theory of Mind: A popular concept in child psychology • The assumption that other beings are intentional systems and have mental states, including: Knowledge
    [Show full text]