The Language Bioprogram Hypothesis

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Language Bioprogram Hypothesis THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1984) 7, 173-221 Printed in the United States of America The language bioprogram hypothesis Derek Bickerton Department of Linguistics, University of Hawaii at Manoa, Honolulu, Hawaii 96822 Abstract: It is hypothesized that Creole languages are largely invented by children and show fundamental similarities, which derive from a biological program for language. The structures of Hawaiian Pidgin and Hawaiian Creole are contrasted, and evidence is provided to show that the latter derived from the former in a single generation. A realistic model of the processes of Creole formation shows how several specific historical and demographic factors interacted to restrict, in varying degrees, the access of pidgin speakers to the dominant language, and hence the nature of input to the children of those speakers. It is shown that the resulting similarities of Creole languages derive from a single grammar with a restricted list of categories and operations. However, grammars of individual Creoles will differ from this grammar to a varying extent: The degree of difference will correlate very closely with the quantity of dominant-language input, which in turn is controlled by extralinguistic factors. Alternative explanations of the above phenomena are surveyed, in particular, substratum theory and monogenesis: Both are found inadequate to account for the facts. Primary acquisition is examined in light of the general hypothesis, and it is suggested that the bioprogram provides a skeletal model of language which the child can then readily convert into the target language. Cases of systematic error and precocious learning provide indirect support for the hypothesis. Some conjectures are made concerning the evolutionary origins of the bioprogram and what study of Creoles and related topics might reveal about language origins. Keywords: child language; Creole; evolution; glottogenesis; grammar; language; language acquisition; language origins; learnability; linguistic universals; pidgin; psycholinguistics 1.0 The basic hypothesis their native language; theoretically this process can occur at any stage in a pidgin's history, but for reasons that will A central issue with respect to human language, and one become apparent, we shall be dealing only with Creoles that is far from resolved, concerns the extent and specific- that have come into existence very early in the develop- ity of the mechanisms that underlie it. At one extreme lie ment of their antecedent pidgins. It has long been recog- the views of Chomsky [see "Rules and Representations," nized by creolists that Creoles somehow "expand" and BBS 3(1) 1980] and his associates, who posit a "mental render more complex the pidgin grammar that precedes organ" that is as modular and as functionally specialized as them (Hall 1966), but until recently there was no clear the human heart or lungs. At the other extreme lie those picture of what constituted this expansion, and no indica- of many empiricists who hold that the human mind is a tion as to how the expansion was achieved. The LBH general-purpose problem-solving device, no particular claims that the innovative aspects of Creole grammar are portion of which is specifically devoted to language. For inventions on the part of the first generation of children the past two decades at least, debate has ranged over who have a pidgin as their linguistic input, rather than fairly well known territory: the extent to which language features transmitted from preexisting languages. The is like or unlike other objects of human learning, the role LBH claims, further, that such inventions show a degree of input in the normal acquisition of language by the of similarity, across wide variations in linguistic back- child, the ontological status of innate knowledge, and so ground, that is too great to be attributed to chance. forth. In the sections that follow, I examine evidence from Finally, the LBH claims that the most cogent explanation a relatively little known area of human language which of this similarity is that it derives from the structure of a clearly bears on this issue; I argue in favor of a language species-specific program for language, genetically coded bioprogram hypothesis (henceforth LBH) that suggests and expressed, in ways still largely mysterious, in the that the infrastructure of language is specified at least as structures and modes of operation of the human brain. narrowly as Chomsky has claimed. This general argument, at varying levels of specificity The languages to be examined are known as Creoles, and detail, has been developed in previous publications which in turn have derived from pidgin languages. A (Bickerton 1974; 1977; 1979; and especially 1981). How- pidgin is an auxiliary language that arises when speakers ever, since the appearance of these versions, there have of several mutually unintelligible languages are in close been a number of developments that permit a more contact; by definition, it has no native speakers. A Creole sharply focused and explicit presentation of the LBH. comes into existence when children acquire a pidgin as These developments include studies by Philip Baker on © 7984 Cambridge University Press 0140-525X1841020173-49IS06.00 THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1984) 7:2 173 Bickerton: Language bioprogram hypothesis the origins of Mauritius Creole (Baker 1976; 1982; Baker In the case of Creole speakers, there are two pos- & Come 1982), studies of Haitian syntax by Claire sibilities. The structures that distinguish them from Lefebvre and her associates (Lefebvre, Magloire-Holly & pidgin speakers were acquired either in childhood or in Piou 1982), extensive fieldwork on Saramaccan syntax by adulthood. If Creole speakers acquired the structures as Frank Byrne (Byrne 1982; forthcoming), and an ongoing adults, they must have done so by means of acquisitiona. longitudinal study of the linguistic development of a blind processes equally available to pidgin speakers, who were two-year-old by Robert Wilson. also adults. Yet we find structures that are shared by all locally born (i.e. Creole) speakers and no immigrant (i.e pidgin) speakers. Such structures can only have been 2.0 The arguments for invention acquired by processes inaccessible to pidgin speakers. The only plausible candidates for such processes are the In order to support the LBH it is necessary to show that ones peculiar to children, which, according to the so- all, or at least a substantial part, of the grammar of a called critical period hypothesis (Krashen 1973; Len- language can be produced in the absence of the genera- neberg 1967; Scovell 1981), are inaccessible to adults. tion-to-generation transmission of particular languages Therefore, forms unique to locally born speakers must that is a normal characteristic of our species. Note that have been acquired by them as children. although evidence for this point would still fall short of Given that the present speech of pidgin speakers is as demonstrating the LBH as a whole, failure to find support rudimentary as (or less rudimentary than) their speech for it would be fatal to the hypothesis. It is not necessary, shortly after arrival in Hawaii, we can illustrate the nature however, to demonstrate that the LBH specifies the only of 1900—1920 pidgin by examples drawn from speakers means through which novel linguistic structures can who arrived in Hawaii during that period (in each case the arise, as some contributors to Hill's (1979) volume seem speaker's native language and date of arrival are given in to have assumed. parentheses). Even the lexicon of this pidgin was highly Creole languages arise where large numbers of people unstable, as (l)-(3) show: speaking mutually unintelligible languages are forced to 1. kote, motete, awl frend giv, no? (Japanese, 1918). associate on a permanent basis but have no preexisting buy, take-back, all friend give, INTERROGATIVE. language in common. Such conditions were produced par "[They] buy [presents], take [them] back, and give excellence by European colonialism in the period [them] to all their friends, don't they?" 1500-1900, and were most recently satisfied in Hawaii in 2. insai lepo aen hanapa aen blaenket, pau (Visayan, the period 1876-1920. Hawaii is therefore the only place 1916). in the world where it is still possible to study the linguistic inside dirt and cover and blanket, finish phenomena produced under these conditions by direct "[They put the body] in the ground and cover [it examination of surviving speakers from the relevant with a] blanket, that's all." period. 3. mi onli chachi-chachi go palei, tarin gonon naega In evaluating the Hawaii evidence, one caveat must be bisanis ani (Korean, 1916). borne in mind. This evidence is drawn from a study made I only church go pray, other things I business is-not during 1973-75, of speakers then in their seventies, "I just went to church to pray; other things were not eighties, or even (in a couple of cases) nineties. It can be my business." interpreted as evidence for what happened in the period (The non-English lexical items are Japanese in [1], Ha- 1900-1920 only on the assumption that the speech of waiian in [2] - a form of pidginized Hawaiian was the most individuals does not change appreciably after adulthood widespread means of communication prior to 1900; see is reached. Since this assumption, although common- Bickerton & Wilson (1984) - and Korean in [3]. place in linguistics, may not obtain in the unusual condi- Examples such as (l)-(3) have no recognizable syntax. tions dealt with here, we need to see how it might fail and What fragments of syntax can be recognized are heavily what the consequences of such failure might be. influenced by native-language grammar; thus, in (4), Take first the speech of pidgin speakers, which will be objects precede verbs, as in Japanese, whereas in (5), the shown to be extremely rudimentary in structure.
Recommended publications
  • Language Creativity: a Sociolinguistic Reading of Linguistic Change in Lebanon
    BAU Journal - Society, Culture and Human Behavior Volume 1 Issue 1 ISSN: 2663-9122 Article 2 August 2019 LANGUAGE CREATIVITY: A SOCIOLINGUISTIC READING OF LINGUISTIC CHANGE IN LEBANON Sawsan Tohme English Department, Faculty of Human Sciences, Beirut Arab University, Lebanon, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.bau.edu.lb/schbjournal Part of the Architecture Commons, Arts and Humanities Commons, Education Commons, and the Law Commons Recommended Citation Tohme, Sawsan (2019) "LANGUAGE CREATIVITY: A SOCIOLINGUISTIC READING OF LINGUISTIC CHANGE IN LEBANON," BAU Journal - Society, Culture and Human Behavior: Vol. 1 : Iss. 1 , Article 2. Available at: https://digitalcommons.bau.edu.lb/schbjournal/vol1/iss1/2 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Digital Commons @ BAU. It has been accepted for inclusion in BAU Journal - Society, Culture and Human Behavior by an authorized editor of Digital Commons @ BAU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. LANGUAGE CREATIVITY: A SOCIOLINGUISTIC READING OF LINGUISTIC CHANGE IN LEBANON Abstract Language creativity in incorporated in everyday conversations and language behaviour. It is present in everyday expression although it might sometimes be invisible, looked down on or disdained. Regardless of whether we are in favour of or against this creativity, it is worth being recognised. As far as Lebanon is concerned, Arabic (i.e. Standard Arabic) is the official language, while Lebanese abic,Ar along with English and French, are the main languages used by the Lebanese. This language diversity can be explained in the light of a number of factors and sociolinguistic functions.
    [Show full text]
  • Making Sense of "Bad English"
    MAKING SENSE OF “BAD ENGLISH” Why is it that some ways of using English are considered “good” and others are considered “bad”? Why are certain forms of language termed elegant, eloquent, or refined, whereas others are deemed uneducated, coarse, or inappropriate? Making Sense of “Bad English” is an accessible introduction to attitudes and ideologies towards the use of English in different settings around the world. Outlining how perceptions about what constitutes “good” and “bad” English have been shaped, this book shows how these principles are based on social factors rather than linguistic issues and highlights some of the real-life consequences of these perceptions. Features include: • an overview of attitudes towards English and how they came about, as well as real-life consequences and benefits of using “bad” English; • explicit links between different English language systems, including child’s English, English as a lingua franca, African American English, Singlish, and New Delhi English; • examples taken from classic names in the field of sociolinguistics, including Labov, Trudgill, Baugh, and Lambert, as well as rising stars and more recent cutting-edge research; • links to relevant social parallels, including cultural outputs such as holiday myths, to help readers engage in a new way with the notion of Standard English; • supporting online material for students which features worksheets, links to audio and news files, further examples and discussion questions, and background on key issues from the book. Making Sense of “Bad English” provides an engaging and thought-provoking overview of this topic and is essential reading for any student studying sociolinguistics within a global setting.
    [Show full text]
  • 'Speaking Singlish' Comic Strips
    International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net Volume 12, Issue 12, 2020 The Use of Colloquial Singaporean English in ‘Speaking Singlish’ Comic Strips: A Syntactic Analysis Delianaa*, Felicia Oscarb, a,bEnglish department, Faculty of Cultural Studies, Universitas Sumatera Utara, Email: a*[email protected] This study explores the sentence structure of Colloquial Singaporean English (CSE) and how it differs from Standard English (SE). A descriptive qualitative method is employed as the research design. The data source is the dialogue of five comic strips which are purposively chosen from Speaking Singlish comic strips. Data is in the form of sentences totalling 34 declaratives, 20 wh- interrogatives, 14 yes -no interrogatives, 3 imperatives and 1 exclamative. The results present the sentence structure of CSE found in the data generally constructed by one subject, one predicate, and occasionally one discourse element. The subject is a noun phrase while the predicate varies amongst noun, adjective, adverb, and verb phrases– particularly in copula deletion. On the other hand, there are several differences between the sentence structure of CSE and SE in the data including the use of copula, topic sentence, discourse elements, adverbs, unmarked plural noun and past tense. Key words: Colloquial Singaporean English (CSE), Speaking Singlish, sentence structure. Introduction Standard English (SE) is a variety of the English language. This view is perhaps more acceptable in the case of Non-Standard English (NSE). The classification of SE being a dialect goes against the lay understanding that a dialect is a subset of a language, usually with a geographical restriction regarding its distribution (Kerswill, 2006).
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Introduction
    Introduction 1 1 Introduction SILVIA KOUWENBERG AND JOHN VICTOR SINGLER 1 Background That there is a reason – and a need – for The Handbook of Pidgin and Creole Studies in linguistics today comes from the recognition of the challenges that pidgin and creole languages pose for linguistics as a field. At the same time, an assessment of the state of pidgin and creole studies is timely for those who work within it. The questions that drive pidgin and creole studies today revolve around the nature and interaction of the forces that have shaped these languages. These questions are not new; they are what motivated the study of these languages in the first place. However, in recent years insights from other branches of lin- guistics as well as careful sociohistorical studies have moved the field forward. Creole studies has attained this point within linguistics, it can be argued, as a logical progression from three events that occurred half a century ago: • the publication in 1957 of Chomsky’s Syntactic Structures, • the publication four years earlier of Weinreich’s Languages in Contact, and • the convening in 1959 of the first international conference on creole studies at Mona, Jamaica. 1.1 The Chomskyan paradigm The publication of Syntactic Structures – a defining moment in the history of modern linguistics – has, additionally, special relevance for creole studies. The Chomskyan paradigm emphasized the interaction of language and the mind. The structure of the brain for language provides the basis for universals – the properties that characterize and, indeed, define all human language. Further, linguists working within the Chomskyan paradigm sought to understand the nature of the acquisition of language, the way in which the intersection of universal principles with language-particular input resulted in children’s acquisition of language.
    [Show full text]
  • Thai Code-Mixing in Thai Healthmagazines
    ENGLISH–THAI CODE-MIXING IN THAI HEALTH MAGAZINES A MASTER’S PROJECT BY WATCHAREE JANHOM Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Arts Degree in Business English for International Communication at Srinakharinwirot University October 2011 ENGLISH–THAI CODE-MIXING IN THAI HEALTH MAGAZINES A MASTER’S PROJECT BY WATCHAREE JANHOM Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Arts Degree in Business English for International Communication at Srinakharinwirot University October 2011 Copyright 2011 Srinakharinwirot University ENGLISH–THAI CODE-MIXING IN THAI HEALTH MAGAZINES AN ABSTRACT BY WATCHAREE JANHOM Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Arts Degree in Business English for International Communication at Srinakharinwirot University October 2011 Copyright 2011 Srinakharinwirot University Watcharee Janhom. (2011) English-Thai Code-Mixing in Thai Health Magazines Master’s Project, M.A. (Business English for International Communication). Bangkok: Graduate School, Srinkharinwirot University. Project Advisor: Assistant Professor Sirinna Boonyasaquan This study aimed to investigate the extent of English-Thai code-mixing and analyze its patterns found in seven Thai health magazines, from the cover page to the back page. Advertisements were not included in the study. The analysis was based on two classification frameworks applied by Ho (2007) and Kannaovakun (2003). Ho’s classification framework comprised letters of the alphabet, short forms, proper nouns, lexical words, phrases, incomplete sentences, and single complete sentences. Meanwhile, Kannaovakun’s classification framework consisted of truncation, hybridization, conversion, semantic shift, reduplication, and word order shift. Based on Ho’s classification framework, the findings showed that the highest occurrence of linguistic pattern of code-mixing was proper nouns (36.27%), followed by lexical words (28.29%), phrases (25.04%), letters of the alphabet (8.27%), short forms (1.05 %) and single full sentences (0.60%), respectively.
    [Show full text]
  • A Brief Descriptive Grammar of Pijal Media Lengua and an Acoustic Vowel Space Analysis of Pijal Media Lengua and Imbabura Quichua
    A Brief Descriptive Grammar of Pijal Media Lengua and an Acoustic Vowel Space Analysis of Pijal Media Lengua and Imbabura Quichua by Jesse Stewart A Thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies of The University of Manitoba in partial fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Linguistics University of Manitoba Winnipeg Copyright © 2011 by Jesse Stewart Abstract This thesis presents an acoustic vowel space analysis of F1 and F2 frequencies from 10 speakers of a newly documented variety of Media Lengua, called Pijal Media Lengua (PML) and 10 speakers of Imbabura Quichua (IQ). This thesis also provides a brief grammatical discription of PML with insights into contrasts and similarities between Spanish, Quichua and other documented varieties of Media Lengua, namely, Salcedo Media Lengua (Muysken 1997) and Angla Media Lengua (Gómez-Rendón 2005). Media Lengua is typically described as a mixed language with a Quichua morphosyntactic structure wherein almost all content words are replaced by their Spanish-derived counterparts through the process of relexification. I use mixed effects models to test Spanish- derived vowels against their Quichua-derived counterparts in PML for statistical significance followed by separate mixed effects models to test Spanish-derived /i/ vs. /e/ and /u/ vs. /o/ for statistical significance. The results of this thesis provide suggestive data for (1) co-existing vowel systems in moderate contact situations such as that of Quichua and (2) moderate evidence for co-exsiting vowel systems in extreme contact situations such as mixed languages. Results also show that (3) PML may be manipulating as many as eight vowels wherein Spanish-derived high vowels and low vowels co-exist as extreme mergers with their Quichua- dervied counterparts, while high vowel and mid vowels co-exist as partial mergers; and (4) IQ may be manipulating as many as six vowels instead of the traditional view of three wherein Spanish-derived high vowels have completely merged with their native Quichua counterparts.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 2 Sign Language Types
    Chapter 2 Sign language types This chapter defines four different sign language types, based on the infor- mation available in the respective sources. Before introducing the types of sign languages, I first report on the diachronic developments in the field of typological sign language research that gave rise to the distinction of the various sign language types. Sign language research started about five decades ago in the United States of America mainly due to the pioneering work of Stokoe (2005 [1960]), Klima and Bellugi (1979), and Poizner, Klima and Bellugi (1987) on American Sign Language (ASL). Gradually linguists in other countries, mainly in Europe, became interested in sign language research and started analyzing European sign languages e.g. British Sign Language (BSL), Swedish Sign Language (SSL), Sign Language of the Netherlands (NGT) and German Sign Language (DGS). Most of the in-depth linguistic descrip- tions have been based on Western sign languages. Therefore, it has long been assumed that some fundamental levels of linguistic structure, such as spatial morphology and syntax, operate identically in all sign languages. Recent studies, however, have discovered some important variations in spatial organization in some previously unknown sign languages (Washabaugh, 1986; Nyst, 2007; Marsaja, 2008; Padden, Meir, Aronoff, & Sandler, 2010). In the context of growing interest in non-Western sign languages towards the end of the 1990s and more recently, there have been efforts towards developing a typology of sign languages (Zeshan, 2004ab, 2008, 2011b; Schuit, Baker, & Pfau, 2011). Although it has been repeatedly emphasized in the literature that the sign language research still has too little data on sign languages other than those of national deaf communities, based in Western or Asian cultures (Zeshan, 2008).
    [Show full text]
  • Pidgins and Creoles
    Chapter 7: Contact Languages I: Pidgins and Creoles `The Negroes who established themselves on the Djuka Creek two centuries ago found Trio Indians living on the Tapanahoni. They maintained continuing re- lations with them....The trade dialect shows clear traces of these circumstances. It consists almost entirely of words borrowed from Trio or from Negro English' (Verslag der Toemoekhoemak-expeditie, by C.H. De Goeje, 1908). `The Nez Perces used two distinct languages, the proper and the Jargon, which differ so much that, knowing one, a stranger could not understand the other. The Jargon is the slave language, originating with the prisoners of war, who are captured in battle from the various neighboring tribes and who were made slaves; their different languages, mixing with that of their masters, formed a jargon....The Jargon in this tribe was used in conversing with the servants and the court language on all other occasions' (Ka-Mi-Akin: Last Hero of the Yakimas, 2nd edn., by A.J. Splawn, 1944, p. 490). The Delaware Indians `rather design to conceal their language from us than to properly communicate it, except in things which happen in daily trade; saying that it is sufficient for us to understand them in that; and then they speak only half sentences, shortened words...; and all things which have only a rude resemblance to each other, they frequently call by the same name' (Narratives of New Netherland 1609-1664, by J. Franklin Jameson, 1909, p. 128, quoting a comment made by the Dutch missionary Jonas Micha¨eliusin August 1628). The list of language contact typologies at the beginning of Chapter 4 had three entries under the heading `extreme language mixture': pidgins, creoles, and bilingual mixed lan- guages.
    [Show full text]
  • Nativization of English Among Bantu Language Speakers in South Africa
    UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics Title Nativization of English among Bantu Language Speakers in South Africa Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9748v75z Journal Issues in Applied Linguistics, 15(2) ISSN 1050-4273 Author Makalela, Leketi Publication Date 2007 DOI 10.5070/L4152005082 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Nativization of English among Bantu Language Speakers in South Africa Leketi Makalela University of Limpopo 7KLVVWXG\LQYHVWLJDWHVFKDUDFWHULVWLFIHDWXUHVRI%ODFN6RXWK$IULFDQ(QJOLVK %6$( paying attention to the role of the Bantu language substrate system in the nativization proc- HVVRIWKHYDULHW\8VLQJSURWRW\SLFDOIHDWXUHVLGHQWLÀHGLQSUHYLRXVVWXGLHVDQGDGGLWLRQDO GDWDIURPVSHDNHUVRIDQRWKHU%DQWXODQJXDJH6HSHGLWKLVVWXG\H[DPLQHVWKHLQÁXHQFHRI ÀUVWODQJXDJHIHDWXUHVRQPRUSKRV\QWDFWLFSKRQRORJLFDODQGGLVFRXUVHDQGSUDJPDWLF IHDWXUHV7KHUHVXOWVRIWKHVWXG\VKRZWKDW%DQWXODQJXDJHORJLFSOD\VDSLYRWDOUROHLQ IUDPLQJWKHUXOHVDQGV\VWHPDWLFSURGXFWLRQRIWKH%6$(IHDWXUHV,WLVWKHUHIRUHDUJXHGWKDW GHYHORSPHQWVLQ%6$(VKRZWKDWLWKDVHYROYHGLQWRDQHQGRQRUPDWLYHYDULHW\LQLWVRZQ right and that it has future prospects for standardization due to the demographic strength DQGLPSURYHGVRFLDOUDQNRILWVVSHDNHUV5HFRPPHQGDWLRQVIRUODQJXDJHSODQQLQJDUHRI- IHUHGLQWKHHQGIRUDGDSWDWLRQWRRWKHUFRPSDUDEOHVLWXDWLRQV Research on the spread of English as an international language has shown WKDWRILWVVSHDNHUVDUHQRQWUDGLWLRQDOPRWKHUWRQJXHVSHDNHUV %UXWW*ULIÁHU 7KH:RUOG(QJOLVKHV :( SDUDGLJPZKLFKKDVIRUWKHSDVW\HDUVEHFRPH one of the frameworks
    [Show full text]
  • Sociolinguistics and Language Education
    Sociolinguistics and Language Education 1790.indb i 5/13/2010 3:43:18 PM NEW PERSPECTIVES ON LANGUAGE AND EDUCATION Series Editor: Professor Viv Edwards, University of Reading, Reading, Great Britain Series Advisor: Professor Allan Luke, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia Two decades of research and development in language and literacy education have yielded a broad, multidisciplinary focus. Yet education systems face constant economic and technological change, with attendant issues of identity and power, community and culture. This series will feature critical and interpretive, disciplinary and multidisciplinary perspectives on teaching and learning, language and literacy in new times. Full details of all the books in this series and of all our other publications can be found on http://www.multilingual-matters.com, or by writing to Multilingual Matters, St Nicholas House, 31–34 High Street, Bristol BS1 2AW, UK. 1790.indb ii 5/13/2010 3:43:19 PM NEW PERSPECTIVES ON LANGUAGE AND EDUCATION Series Editor: Professor Viv Edwards Sociolinguistics and Language Education Edited by Nancy H. Hornberger and Sandra Lee McKay MULTILINGUAL MATTERS Bristol • Buffalo • Toronto 1790.indb iii 5/13/2010 3:43:19 PM Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data A catalog record for this book is available from the Library of Congress. Sociolinguistics and Language Education/Edited by Nancy H. Hornberger and Sandra Lee McKay. New Perspectives on Language and Education: 18 Includes bibliographical references and index. 1. Sociolinguistics. 2. Language and education. 3. Language and culture. I. Hornberger, Nancy H. II. McKay, Sandra. P40.S784 2010 306.44–dc22 2010018315 British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data A catalogue entry for this book is available from the British Library.
    [Show full text]
  • Nativization of Dissertation Acknowledgements and Private Letters in Cameroon1 DANIEL A
    Nordic Journal of African Studies 15(2): 166–184 (2006) Nativization of Dissertation Acknowledgements and Private Letters in Cameroon1 DANIEL A. NKEMLEKE University of Yaounde I, Cameroon ABSTRACT One very important area of research on English in the “Outer Circle” has been on the extent to which text types have become nativized in the home culture. Yet, there seems to be a dearth of interest in this domain of study in Cameroon. With ample evidence from dissertation acknowledgements and private letters, this study assesses the cultural-specific indicators that make these text types distinctive. Because these texts are culturally and situationally highly bound, the investigation relies not only on the Sociolinguistic Approach generally used in such analyses, but also on the Cultural-based Approach and Grice’s Co-operation Principle. Two hundred dissertation acknowledgements and 222 private letters have been used. And findings reveal that both text types show a greater degree of adaptation to the “culture of community” characteristic of traditional African lifestyle. For example, the general configuration of dissertation acknowledgements, and to a certain extent private letters, reflect the “good manners” of deference to elders and those who have played an important role in the writer’s life and success. Other contextually determined features such as the transfer of native devices for personalizing speech interaction are frequent. The study concludes that while text- type research remains a rich area of study in view of the establishment of a profile of Cameroon-specific features of new Englishes, it is equally a good basis on which to investigate the ethnography of culture.
    [Show full text]
  • Nativization Processes in L1 Esperanto
    J. Child Lang. (), –. # Cambridge University Press DOI: .\S Printed in the United Kingdom Nativization processes in L Esperanto* BENJAMIN K. BERGEN Linguistics Department, University of California, Berkeley (Received December . Revised January ) The artificial language Esperanto is spoken not only as a second language, by its proponents, but also as a native language by children of some of those proponents. The present study is a preliminary description of some characteristics of the Native Esperanto (NE) of eight speakers, ranging in age from six to fourteen years. As such, it is the first of its kind – previous works on NE are either theoretical treatises or individual case studies. We find, at least for the eight subjects studied, both bilingual- ism and nativization effects, differentiating native from non-native Esperanto speech. Among these effects are loss or modification of the accusative case, phonological reduction, attrition of the tense\aspect system, and pronominal cliticization. The theoretical ramifications are discussed, particularly with regard to universals of language acquisition and the effects of expressive requirements of language. Esperanto is an artificial language, created by L. L. Zamenhof, at the end of the nineteenth century. Zamenhof, who was born in Bialystok (at the time in Lithuania) in , was a polyglot, by all accounts able to speak fluent Polish, Yiddish, and Russian, in addition to having a good command of German, Hebrew, Latin, and French (Boulton, ). His language, the result of years of work, was published in and displays an immense lexical influence from Romance languages. By some estimates as many as %of Esperanto words are from Romance sources.
    [Show full text]