Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Series Editors
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
The Roles of Client Religion, Counselor Religiosity, and Spiritual
THE ROLES OF CLIENT RELIGION, COUNSELOR RELIGIOSITY, AND SPIRITUAL COMPETENCE IN COUNSELORS’ CLINICAL JUDGMENT A DISSERTATION IN Counseling Psychology Presented to the Faculty of the University of Missouri-Kansas City in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY by DOMINICK A. SCALISE B.A., Truman State University, 2004 M.A., Truman State University, 2006 Kansas City, Missouri 2011 © 2011 DOMINICK ANTHONY SCALISE ALL RIGHTS RESERVED THE ROLES OF CLIENT RELIGION, COUNSELOR RELIGIOSITY, AND SPIRITUAL COMPETENCE IN COUNSELORS’ CLINICAL JUDGMENT Dominick Anthony Scalise, Candidate for the Doctor of Philosophy Degree University of Missouri-Kansas City, 2011 ABSTRACT The present study explored the roles that clients’ religious beliefs, therapists’ spiritual/religious beliefs, and therapists’ attitudes toward spirituality and religion may play in how therapists conceptualize a prospective client case. The study also explored the role that the construct “spiritual competence” played in moderating the relationship between therapists’ attitudes toward spirituality and religion and factors related to their clinical judgment of the client’s concerns. One hundred seventy-six therapists and doctoral students were randomly assigned to read one of four vignettes differing only on the client’s spiritual/religious beliefs and practices (Christian, Muslim, Buddhist, or an unstated religious preference). In order to account for various forms of clinical decision-making, a MANCOVA was used with dependent constructs of psychopathology, attribution for the problem, and prognosis. A separate MANCOVA was conducted in order to determine whether the interaction of client religious orientation (religious vs. unidentified religion) and counselor spiritual competence (high vs. low) would be related to different clinical judgments. -
Statsvetenskaplig Tidskrift Ny Följd, Årg 90
n112 2010/1 Innehåll Uppsatser Jan Tullberg: Stereotypfördömandet – en fördom om fördomar? . 3 Översikter och meddelanden Nya forskningsanslag från Vetenskapsrådet och Riksbankens jubileumsfond . 21 Erik Amnå: Skolornas institutionella karaktär och elevernas medborgarkompetens: en jämförelse av olika kommunala och fristående skolor över tid och rum. 23 Ludvig Beckman: Universal suffrage for real? A global index of suffrage restrictions and an explanatory framework . 33 Ludvig Beckman: Globalisation and New Political Rights. The Challenges of the Rights to Inclusion, Self-Determination and Secession . 42 Christina Bergqvist: Vem tar hand om barnen? En jämförande studie av samspelet mellan politik och kultur för beslutsfattande i familjen . 50 Sverker Gustavsson: Thick and thin constitutionalism . 56 Anna K. Jarstad: Local Peace Agreements: The Road to Peace and Democracy in KwaZulu Natal? . 60 Christer Karlsson: Hur regleras EU-medlemskapet?. 69 Johannes Lindvall: The Political Consequences of The Crisis. 79 Ulf Mörkenstam: Indigenous rights to self-determination: The Institutional Design and Policy Process of the Swedish Sami Parliament. 83 Alexa Robertson: Europe as Other: Difference in global media discourse Bo Rothstein: The Determinants of Institutional Quality. How Leaders´ Perceived Threat of Losing Power Without Compensation Affects Economic Development. 85 nStatsvetenskaplig tidskrift Ny följd, årg 90. Utgiven av Fahlbeckska stiftelsen . redaktionssekreterare Mats Sjölin (ansvarig ut- givare) bitr redaktionssekreterare Björn Badersten -
I NOT BAD, for a MAN: SHIFTING STANDARDS in the PROVISION of EMOTIONAL SUPPORT WITHIN ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS by Melinda Ciccoci
NOT BAD, FOR A MAN: SHIFTING STANDARDS IN THE PROVISION OF EMOTIONAL SUPPORT WITHIN ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS by Melinda Ciccocioppo Bachelor of Arts, Carlow University, 2002 Master of Science, University of Pittsburgh, 2008 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of The Kenneth P. Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy University of Pittsburgi h 2012 UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH DIETRICH SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES This dissertation was presented by Melinda Ciccocioppo It was presented on April 17, 2011 for approval by Elizabeth Votruba-Drzal, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology Martin Greenberg, Professor Emeritus, Department of Psychology Brooke Feeney, Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University Thesis Director/Dissertation Advisor: Irene Frieze, Professor, Department of Psychology ii NOT BAD, FOR A MAN: SHIFTING STANDARDS IN THE PROVISION OF EMOTIONAL SUPPORT WITHIN ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS Melinda Ciccocioppo, PhD University of Pittsburgh, [year] Copyright © by Melinda Ciccocioppo 2012 iii NOT BAD, FOR A MAN: SHIFTING STANDARDS IN THE PROVISION OF EMOTIONAL SUPPORT WITHIN ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS Melinda Ciccocioppo, PhD University of Pittsburgh, 2012 Previous research has found mixed results in terms of gender differences in the provision of emotional support to a relationship partner. Some studies support the popular stereotype that women are more emotionally supportive than men, while others find no gender differences in the amount of emotional support men and women provide to one another in a romantic relationship. These conflicting findings may be the result of shifting standards for men and women in terms of the amount of emotional support that is expected to be provided by each within a relationship. -
Committees and Welcomes
Committees and Welcomes SPSP Execut ive and Com m it t ees Welcome to the Fourth Annual SPSP President Jim BlascovichMeeting! Past President Claude SteeleDear SPSP Colleagues, President Elect Hazel Markus It is my great pleasure to welcome you to the annual meetings Executive Officer Harry Reis of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology. Co-Secretary Treasurers Leslie Zebrowitz Sharon Brehm This is our fourth annual meeting – a remarkably brief history Members-at-Large David Dunning for a convention that is now so firmly established. Our confer- David Funder ences in Nashville, San Antonio, and Savannah each drew crowds that were greatly in excess of expectations. In so doing, Judith Harakiewicz they rapidly established SPSP as the major national or interna- APA Council Representatives Monica Biernat tional scientific meeting for personality and social psycholo- June Tangney gists. Publication Committee John Dovidio Gifford Weary We now look forward to building on this string of success in Universal City/Los Angeles, site of our 2003 meeting. This Joanne Wood year, we once again have an exceptionally strong and diverse Convention Committee Dan Cervone chair set of scientific presentations. Our program is expanded even Lynne Cooper beyond last year’s offerings, thanks in part to funding from the Rick Hoyle Decade of Behavior initiative, which is supporting a plenary Training Committee Lisa Aspinwall address by the renowned biologist and author Jared Diamond. Kim Bartholomew We also have expanded on the lighter side of things; on Satur- Allen Omoto day night, we will end our meetings with a social event featur- PSPB Editor Fred Rhodewalt ing DJ’d music at 8 pm followed by a live performance by the PSPR Editor Eliot Smith LA rock band Lustra. -
Implicit Stereotypes and the Predictive Brain: Cognition and Culture in “Biased” Person Perception
ARTICLE Received 28 Jan 2017 | Accepted 17 Jul 2017 | Published 1 Sep 2017 DOI: 10.1057/palcomms.2017.86 OPEN Implicit stereotypes and the predictive brain: cognition and culture in “biased” person perception Perry Hinton1 ABSTRACT Over the last 30 years there has been growing research into the concept of implicit stereotypes. Particularly using the Implicit Associations Test, it has been demon- strated that experimental participants show a response bias in support of a stereotypical association, such as “young” and “good” (and “old” and “bad”) indicating evidence of an implicit age stereotype. This has been found even for people who consciously reject the use of such stereotypes, and seek to be fair in their judgement of other people. This finding has been interpreted as a “cognitive bias”, implying an implicit prejudice within the individual. This article challenges that view: it is argued that implicit stereotypical associations (like any other implicit associations) have developed through the ordinary working of “the predictive brain”. The predictive brain is assumed to operate through Bayesian principles, developing associations through experience of their prevalence in the social world of the perceiver. If the predictive brain were to sample randomly or comprehensively then stereotypical associations would not be picked up if they did not represent the state of the world. However, people are born into culture, and communicate within social networks. Thus, the implicit stereotypical associations picked up by an individual do not reflect a cognitive bias but the associations prevalent within their culture—evidence of “culture in mind”. Therefore to understand implicit stereotypes, research should examine more closely the way associations are communicated within social networks rather than focusing exclusively on an implied cognitive bias of the individual. -
Sources of Confidence Judgments in Implicit Cognition
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 367-373 Sources of confidence judgments in implicit cognition RICHARD J. TUNNEY University of Nottingham, Nottingham, England Subjective reports of confidence are frequently used as a measure of awareness in a variety of fields, including artificial grammar learning. However, little is known about what information is used to make confidence judgments and whether there are any possible sources of information used to discriminate between items that are unrelated to confidence. The data reported here replicate an earlier experiment by Vokey and Brooks (1992) and show that grammaticality decisions are based on both the grammati- cal status of items and their similarity to study exemplars. The key finding is that confidence ratings made on a continuous scale (50%–100%) are closely related to grammaticality but are unrelated to all of the measures of similarity that were tested. By contrast, confidence ratings made on a binary scale (high vs. low) are related to both grammaticality and similarity. The data confirm an earlier finding (Tunney & Shanks, 2003) that binary confidence ratings are more sensitive to low levels of awareness than continuous ratings are and suggest that participants are conscious of all the information acquired in artificial grammar learning. For many years, subjective reports of confidence have edge with low confidence may, therefore, not be reported been used to distinguish between conscious and uncon- even if, in fact, conscious. An alternative to verbal report scious cognitive processes in areas as diverse as discrim- is to ask participants to rate how confident they are that ination (Peirce & Jastrow, 1884), perception (Cheesman their judgments are correct or, alternately, to estimate & Merikle, 1984; Kunimoto, Miller, & Pashler, 2001), their overall level of accuracy. -
Gender, Emotion and Power in Work Relationships☆
Human Resource Management Review 21 (2011) 377–393 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Human Resource Management Review journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/humres Gender, emotion and power in work relationships☆ Belle Rose Ragins a,⁎, Doan E. Winkel b,1 a University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Sheldon B. Lubar School of Business, Milwaukee, WI 53201, United States b Illinois State University, College of Business, Normal, IL 61790, United States article info abstract Keywords: We offer a theoretical account of how gender and emotion combine to influence the Gender in the workplace development of power in work relationships. We document the profound impact gender has on Gender and emotion at work the display, perception and evaluation of emotion in the workplace. We illustrate the reciprocal Gender and discrimination at work relationship between emotion and power, and identify cycles of powerlessness that prevent Gender and power at work women from developing and leveraging power in their work relationships. By exploring the nexus of gender, emotion and power in work relationships, we offer new insights into how the gendering of emotion creates and perpetuates gender differences in power in organizations. Implications for research and practice are offered. © 2011 Published by Elsevier Inc. “If you get too emotional, that undercuts you. A man can cry; we know that. Lots of our leaders have cried. But a woman, it's a different kind of dynamic.” Senator Hillary Rodham Clinton (Dowd, 2008) Despite women's increasing workforce participation, the gender gap in power in organizations has not changed appreciably over the past twenty years (Catalyst, 2010; Eagly & Carli, 2007a; Ryan & Haslam, 2007). -
1 the Psychology of Implicit Intergroup Bias and the Prospect Of
1 The Psychology of Implicit Intergroup Bias and the Prospect of Change Calvin K. Lai1 & Mahzarin R. Banaji2 1 Washington University in St. Louis 2 Harvard University Last updated: June 24, 2020 Correspondence should be addressed to: Calvin Lai at [email protected]. Paper should be cited as: Lai, C. K., & Banaji, M. R. (2020). The psychology of implicit intergroup bias and the prospect of change. In D. Allen & R. Somanathan (Eds.), Difference without Domination: Pursuing Justice in Diverse Democracies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. 2 1. Introduction Over the course of evolution, human minds acquired the breathtaking quality of consciousness which gave our species the capacity to regulate behavior. Among the consequences of this capacity was the possibility of internal dialogue with oneself about the consistency between one’s intentions and actions. This facility to engage in the daily rituals of deliberative thought and action is so natural to our species that we hardly reflect on it or take stock of how effectively we are achieving the goal of intention-action consistency. We do not routinely ask at the end of each day how many of our actions were consistent with the values so many individuals hold: a belief in freedom and equality for all, in opportunity and access for all, in fairness in treatment and justice for all. Even if we wished to compute the extent to which we succeed at this task, how would we go about doing it? As William James (1904) pointed over a century ago, the difficulty of studying the human mind is that the knower is also the known, and this poses difficulties in accessing, in modestly objective fashion, the data from our own moral ledger. -
Biernat, P. 1 Curriculum Vitae Monica R
Biernat, p. 1 Curriculum Vitae Monica R. Biernat Work Address: Home Address: Department of Psychology 4801 Tempe St. Fraser Hall Lawrence, KS 66047 1415 Jayhawk Blvd. (785) 843-7597 University of Kansas Lawrence, KS 66045-7556 phone: (785) 864-9815 fax: (785) 864-5696 email: [email protected] Birth Date and Place: January 18, 1963, Detroit, MI Professional and Educational History: 2010 - present Associate Chair for Research and Graduate Studies, University of Kansas 2003 - present Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Kansas 2000 - present Director of Social Psychology Ph.D. Program, University of Kansas 1995 - 2003 Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Kansas 1992 - 1995 Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Kansas 1989 - 1992 Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Florida 2008 Visiting scholar, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 2001 Visiting scholar, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada 1989 Ph.D., University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Psychology (Social) 1986 M.A., University of Michigan, Psychology 1984 A.B., University of Michigan, Psychology and Communication Awards: APA Distinguished Scientific Award for Early Career Contribution to Psychology in the area of Social Psychology, 1998/1999 [Citation appears in American Psychologist (1999), Vol. 54, pp. 897-899.] Association for Women in Psychology, Distinguished Publication Award, 2005 W. T. Kemper Fellowship for Teaching Excellence, University of Kansas, 2004 Docking Faculty Scholar, University -
Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self-Esteem, and Stereotypes
Psychological Review Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1995, Vol. 102, No. 1,4-27 0033-295X/95/S3.00 Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self-Esteem, and Stereotypes Anthony G. Greenwald Mahzarin R. Banaji University of Washington Yale University Social behavior is ordinarily treated as being under conscious (if not always thoughtful) control. However, considerable evidence now supports the view that social behavior often operates in an implicit or unconscious fashion. The identifying feature of implicit cognition is that past experience influences judgment in a fashion not introspectively known by the actor. The present conclusion— that attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes have important implicit modes of operation—extends both the construct validity and predictive usefulness of these major theoretical constructs of social psychology. Methodologically, this review calls for increased use of indirect measures—which are imperative in studies of implicit cognition. The theorized ordinariness of implicit stereotyping is consistent with recent findings of discrimination by people who explicitly disavow prejudice. The finding that implicit cognitive effects are often reduced by focusing judges' attention on their judg- ment task provides a basis for evaluating applications (such as affirmative action) aimed at reducing such unintended discrimination. Long before they became central to other areas of psycholog- research, also has a long-established history (e.g., James, 1890; ical theory, concepts of cognitive mediation dominated the anal- see overview in Wylie, 1974, 1979). ysis of social behavior. The constructs on which this article fo- Through much of the period since the 1930s, most social psy- cuses achieved early prominence in social psychological theory chologists have assumed that attitudes, and to a lesser extent with formulations that were partly (attitude) or entirely (stereo- stereotypes, operate in a conscious mode. -
Effects of Mindfulness Meditation on Conscious and Non-Conscious Components of the Mind
applied sciences Review Effects of Mindfulness Meditation on Conscious and Non-Conscious Components of the Mind Anastasia Fabbro 1,5,*, Cristiano Crescentini 2, Alessio Matiz 3, Andrea Clarici 4 and Franco Fabbro 5 1 Department of Psychology, University of Rome La Sapienza, 00185 Rome, Italy 2 Department of Languages and Literatures, Communication, Education and Society, University of Udine, 33100 Udine, Italy; [email protected] 3 Perceptual Robotics (PERCRO) Laboratory, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, 56010 Pisa, Italy; [email protected] 4 Psychiatric Clinic, Department of Medical, Surgical and Health Sciences, University of Trieste, 34149 Trieste, Italy; [email protected] 5 Department of Medicine, University of Udine, 33100 Udine, Italy; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] Academic Editor: Peter Walla Received: 31 January 2017; Accepted: 30 March 2017; Published: 1 April 2017 Abstract: The aim of the present review is to investigate previous studies concerning the effects of meditation and dispositional mindfulness on conscious and implicit or non-conscious attitudes. First we present a brief perspective on conscious and non-conscious states of mind. Then we introduce the fundamental bases of mindfulness meditation. Third we review studies on dispositional mindfulness and meditation that employed either direct or indirect measures to assess explicit and implicit attitudes. Finally, we briefly present how meditation has been associated with the psychotherapeutic practice of psychoanalysis and, hence, as a therapeutic technique to access the unconscious. Until now, few studies have investigated the impact of meditation on non-conscious states of mind and personality; nevertheless, both scientific studies involving implicit measures and reflections from psychotherapy have underlined the importance of meditation in promoting psychological well-being, leading to de-automatization of automatic patterns of responding and to higher levels of self-awareness. -
The Blurry Boundary Between Stereotyping and Evaluation in Implicit Cognition
Alex Madva and Michael Brownstein Draft 2.6.15 Comments Welcome The Blurry Boundary between Stereotyping and Evaluation in Implicit Cognition Introduction Does the distinction between cognition and affect apply to implicit mental states? In the literature on intergroup relations, cognition and affect correspond to stereotypes— beliefs about social groups, or, more concretely, representations of traits associated with social groups (Stangor, 2009)—and prejudice—negative affective or evaluative responses toward others on the basis of social group members (McConahay & Hough, 1976; Dixon et al., 2012). While the distinction between explicit stereotypes and explicit prejudices is widely supported (Fiske & Linville, 1980; Martin & Halverson, 1981; Wol, 1982; Augoustinos & Innes, 1990; Duckitt, 1992; Dovidio et al., 1996; Fiske, 1998; Mackie & Smith, 1998; Fiske et al., 2002), its applicability to implicit mental states is controversial. Understanding the relationship between stereotypes and prejudice is important as it can help illuminate the nature of the mental representations underlying implicit mental states. While some theories propose that stereotyping and prejudice are functionally integrated (Greenwald et al., 2002; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006, 2011), others propose that the cognitive and affective components of implicit states represent separate constructs—namely implicit stereotypes (IS) and implicit evaluations (IE)—which reflect different mental processes and neural systems (Amodio & Devine, 2006, 2009; Amodio & Ratner, 2011).