And the Palatability Spectrum* (Unpalatable/Predator/Gregariousness/Prey) F

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

And the Palatability Spectrum* (Unpalatable/Predator/Gregariousness/Prey) F Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 70, No. 8, pp. 2261-2265, August 1973 Theoretical Investigations of Automimicry: Multiple Trial Learning and the Palatability Spectrum* (unpalatable/predator/gregariousness/prey) F. HARVEY POUGHt, LINCOLN P. BROWERt§, HAROLD R. MECKI, AND STEPHEN R. KESSELL§ t Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 14850; § Amherst College, Amherst, Massachusetts 01002; and I Shillington, Reading, Pennsylvania 19607 Communicated by G. E. Hutchinson, May 3, 1973 ABSTRACT We previously explored automimicry as- Case 1 and develop two additional models. Case 2 will con- suming that a species of prey was so unpalatable as to sider a situation in which palatable individuals of the prey promote conditioned avoidance for a period of time after a predator encountered a single individual (Case 1). In this species can intervene between the two unpalatables, and paper, we assume that the prey is less noxious and that Case 3, the most restrictive, requires that the predator must two encounters are required. Case 2 allows the two en- encounter the two unpalatables consecutively. counters with unpalatables to be separated by any num- ber of palatables, while in Case 3 the predator must en- counter two unpalatables, consecutively. MATHEMATICAL MODELS The general relationships in the three cases are similar, The mathematical models for all these cases assume that but the automimetic advantage is reduced moderately in en- Case 2 and greatly in Case 3. To attain the same auto- palatable alternative prey are available in the natural mimetic advantage as in Case I requires an increase in the vironment of the predator and that an attack by a predator proportion of unpalatables, or in the induced rejection on an individual of the automimetic species is lethal for the period, or both. Consequently, selection will tend to prey. increase the unpalatability so that Cases 2 and 3 converge The variables we consider are the following: n is the number to Case 1. Species that are uniformly and highly unpalatable can of prey that each predator would eat if none of the prey afford to be more dispersed than automimetic species. were unpalatable, m is the number of prey available per Case-2 and -3 automimetic species will benefit greatly from predator, and k' is the frequency of unpalatable prey in a gregariousness, while in Case-i automimicry situations predator's sample. We have chosen n values ranging from 2 this is less important. to 100 and m values from 0.05 to 10,000. The limits upon and Brower, Pough, and Meck (1) explored mathematically relationship between the variables were discussed in our the theory of automimicry resulting from the discovery previous paper (1). that some monarch butterflies (Danaus plexippus L.) are The mathematics of Case 2 follow from Eqs. 1-3 in our palatable and others are not (2). Because the palatable and previous paper (1). unpalatable individuals are members of the same species they At the beginning of the ith predation the fraction of preda- are visually identical and predators cannot distinguish them tors that have not yet had an emetic experience is (1 - k')i-i. on the basis of sight. Our mathematical model assumed that The fraction of predators that have had one emetic experience an unpleasant experience with a single unpalatable individual is (i - 1)k'(1 - k') -2. Hence, the fraction of predators still would be sufficient to condition a predator to avoid several eating at the beginning of the ith period is subsequent individuals. The analysis led to the conclusion that automimicry enables a remarkably low proportion of un- (1 kl)'-' + (i 1)kl(l kl) i-2 [4] palatable prey to confer a substantial immunity from preda- tion to the entire population. and this expression is equal to the average number of prey A recent study (3) has shown that monarch butterflies eaten by one predator during the ith period. The average from natural populations exhibit palatability spectra and total number of prey eaten by one predator in n prey rejection include individuals that are only moderately unpalatable. A periods is predator might have to experience more than one of these before it was conditioned to avoid subsequent individuals. n n 1: (1 -k')'- 1 + k' E (i -1)(1 -k')i [5] In this paper, we explore the extent to which the auto- i=l i=l mimetic advantage accruing to a population is changed when each predator must encounter two unpalatable prey instead The first series is the same geometric series that appeared of one to avoid subsequent prey on sight alone. We shall refer in the analysis of Case 1. The summation is repeated below: to the former mathematical model, which involved prey sufficiently unpalatable to produce single trial learning, as n 1 (1 - kl')-' = - [1 - (1 - kt)n]. * This is paper no. II of the series. The first paper is ref. 1. si=1o I Requests for reprints may be addressed to this author, Depart- ment of Biology, Amherst College, Amherst, Mass. 01002. The sum of the second series can be found by differentiating 2261 2262 Zoology: Pough et al. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 70 (1973) 1.0 PREY TOO COMMON PREY TOO RARoEthe foregoing equation with respect to k'. Thus -,CASE 2 + 1 W K [7 0 1 (k+)2-[l-(1-ka)k].O.Oa/ K F~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~(\)-n( - -'k-+-[1 - (1 - k l)n-1 o/.I The expression for the average total number of prey eaten by one predator in n rejection periods now becomes 4~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ W 04 k 0.5000 § W W jut = 1 _ [2 - (nk' + 2 - 2k')(1 - k')"-']. [7] 0.2~~~~~~~~.9 XZ 05 al QSOL 1.0 5S 10 50 100 mk' 1.0- PREY TOO COMMON K'9s PREY TOO RARE, Case S. In this case the direct method of solution used n/m n/m Tpreviouslyfr co cannot be applied. A finite difference equation 0.50004 must be used. The symbol xi will be used to denote the 0.8- number of predators that still retain their appetites after < CASAE22 the ith eating period. The number of predators susceptible to - b 0.2500 a)thloss of appetite in the (ixi+ period is minus the number predators that did not get an unpalatable prey in the ith z 0-6- l\ period, i.e., (\k'10 = 1--, [2-k)X].)xf1.+2-2k)(1- [9] 0.48 The number of predators that stop eating during the 0~~~~~~~~~)0b 0.2500of/aploss(i + 1)thpetitperiodinisthequal to Eq. 9 multiplied by k'. This result risequaltoxt-xf+n.Therefore, 0.0 1\\\ - - (1 - k')x1..i = xi+, (1~~~~~~~~~~~~~~x [9])j- 0.2- ;!11 -tso~~~~~~~~~~of xi f-X+lx -= k'[x - (1 k')xi-l]. [10] Li l a \ \ \ ~~~~~~~~~~Afterrearranging terms, the finite difference equation for xi 0~~~~~~~~050 °'01 05 01 0.5 1. 5 10 50 100 X~-1k)fk(-'x_=° [l K' A trial solution of the form 0o PREY TOO COMMON PREY TOO RARE n/m < I o0sffoO°/2\ n/m >1 xi =co,f [12] 0.2500\ is assumed where C is an arbitrary constant. It is found by 0.8- 0.1000-isubstitutione t x of- the trial solution into the finite difference W C ||\ \ \\equation that A is given by the quadratic equation <0.62 x|j\\(1- - k') -k'(1 - k') = 0. [13] 0.0~ AfThisequationhastheroots / + 2[(1-kl)(1 + 3k')]l/ [14] 0.22D/ \ X X X 2 = -~~~~~~~~~~~~22 [(1 - k)(1 + 3k')]1' ///o\ The equation for xf now becomes ooL~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~X=\X 1 tctC202iC11 [1s5 .01 05 0.1 0.5 1.0 5 10 50 0 abundance (m) andfrequency of unpalatable prey in a predator's PREDATION POTENTIAL n/m sample (k') on the automimetic advantage (Aj). The induced FIG. 1. (a) Autom2m cry, Case 2: Two unpalatable prey are prey rejection period (n) is 5. (b) Same as (a), except n = 25. required to condition subsequent avoidance. Effect of prey (c) Same as (a), except n = 100. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 70 (1973) Theoretical Investigations of Automimicry 2263 1.0- PREY TOO COMMON PREY TOO RARE The constants C1 and C2 are evaluated from the conditions that xi = Xowhen i is equal to zero or 1. Then, it is found that n/rn <I n/rn al the number of predators that are still eating at the end of the ith eating period is CASE 3 J = [16] w Xi [(1 - 2)31'- (1-1)1i] a K' 0 -0 1- 0.9999~~~~~~K.9 Z z0.6-0.6~ ~ ~ ~ ~ This is equal to the number of prey eaten during the (i + 1)th period. The number of prey eaten in the ith eating period is 2 \ found by rewriting the foregoing results with i replaced by W 0 (i- 1). The result is 0 X a- 0.4-_/_05030 13i - 132 [(1 - 132)13I - (1 - 131)021 1. [17] < / a~~~~~~~~~500>011- 2 0.2- The total number of prey eaten during n trials is 0.2500air [ -132) jal l (1 -1) j132'-]. [18] 0.0-I F .01 .05 0l 0.5 1.0 5 10 56 I0 When the two geometric series are summed, this becomes 1.0 PREY TOO COMMON K' PREY TOO RARE). - 132)(1 - ), - - < 0~~~.99 xo #2i - I')l (1 13)(1 - 132n)1 1] n/m <1' n/m >1 132L 1-1 [19] 0-8 CAE 3 0 The fraction of prey eaten is equal to this result divided by \ the initial number of prey: b 1 ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~[(1-#2)2(l _-11 Z 0.6- / (1 - $2)(1 - 1)(1 - 32) ll- (1 - #1)2(1 - d32n)] [20] 2 o/.4X and the fraction of the population surviving is 0<,IA m(k')2(1 [- 12) 0.2- -(1- 1)2(1-_ 2)] [21] 000 0// The equations for Cases 1-3 break down when k' = 0.
Recommended publications
  • Mimicry - Ecology - Oxford Bibliographies 12/13/12 7:29 PM
    Mimicry - Ecology - Oxford Bibliographies 12/13/12 7:29 PM Mimicry David W. Kikuchi, David W. Pfennig Introduction Among nature’s most exquisite adaptations are examples in which natural selection has favored a species (the mimic) to resemble a second, often unrelated species (the model) because it confuses a third species (the receiver). For example, the individual members of a nontoxic species that happen to resemble a toxic species may dupe any predators by behaving as if they are also dangerous and should therefore be avoided. In this way, adaptive resemblances can evolve via natural selection. When this phenomenon—dubbed “mimicry”—was first outlined by Henry Walter Bates in the middle of the 19th century, its intuitive appeal was so great that Charles Darwin immediately seized upon it as one of the finest examples of evolution by means of natural selection. Even today, mimicry is often used as a prime example in textbooks and in the popular press as a superlative example of natural selection’s efficacy. Moreover, mimicry remains an active area of research, and studies of mimicry have helped illuminate such diverse topics as how novel, complex traits arise; how new species form; and how animals make complex decisions. General Overviews Since Henry Walter Bates first published his theories of mimicry in 1862 (see Bates 1862, cited under Historical Background), there have been periodic reviews of our knowledge in the subject area. Cott 1940 was mainly concerned with animal coloration. Subsequent reviews, such as Edmunds 1974 and Ruxton, et al. 2004, have focused on types of mimicry associated with defense from predators.
    [Show full text]
  • Rapid Evolution of Mimicry Following Local Model Extinction Rsbl.Royalsocietypublishing.Org Christopher K
    Evolutionary biology Rapid evolution of mimicry following local model extinction rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org Christopher K. Akcali and David W. Pfennig Department of Biology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3280, USA Research Batesian mimicry evolves when individuals of a palatable species gain the selective advantage of reduced predation because they resemble a toxic Cite this article: Akcali CK, Pfennig DW. 2014 species that predators avoid. Here, we evaluated whether—and in which Rapid evolution of mimicry following local direction—Batesian mimicry has evolved in a natural population of model extinction. Biol. Lett. 10: 20140304. mimics following extirpation of their model. We specifically asked whether http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2014.0304 the precision of coral snake mimicry has evolved among kingsnakes from a region where coral snakes recently (1960) went locally extinct. We found that these kingsnakes have evolved more precise mimicry; by contrast, no such change occurred in a sympatric non-mimetic species or in conspecifics Received: 9 April 2014 from a region where coral snakes remain abundant. Presumably, more pre- cise mimicry has continued to evolve after model extirpation, because Accepted: 22 May 2014 relatively few predator generations have passed, and the fitness costs incurred by predators that mistook a deadly coral snake for a kingsnake were historically much greater than those incurred by predators that mistook a kingsnake for a coral snake. Indeed, these results are consistent with prior Subject Areas: theoretical and empirical studies, which revealed that only the most precise evolution, ecology, behaviour mimics are favoured as their model becomes increasingly rare.
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptations for Survival: Symbioses, Camouflage & Mimicry
    Adaptations for Survival: Symbioses, Camouflage & Mimicry OCN 201 Biology Lecture 11 http://www.berkeley.edu/news/media/releases/2005/03/24_octopus.shtml Symbiosis • Parasitism - negative effect on host • Commensalism - no effect on host • Mutualism - both parties benefit Often involves food but benefits may also include protection from predators, dispersal, or habitat Parasitism Leeches (Segmented Worms) Tongue Louse (Crustacean) Nematodes (Roundworms) Commensalism or Mutualism? Anemone shrimp http://magma.nationalgeographic.com/ Anemone fish http://www.scuba-equipment-usa.com/marine/APR04/ Mutualism Cleaner Shrimp and Eel http://magma.nationalgeographic.com/ Whale Barnacles & Lice What kinds of symbioses are these? Commensal Parasite Camouflage • Often important for predators and prey to avoid being seen • Predators to catch their prey and prey to hide from their predators • Camouflage: Passive or adaptive Passive Camouflage Countershading Sharks Birds Countershading coloration of the Caribbean reef shark © George Ryschkewitsch Fish JONATHAN CHESTER Mammals shiftingbaselines.org/blog/big_tuna.jpg http://www.nmfs.noaa.gov/pr/images/cetaceans/orca_spyhopping-noaa.jpg Passive Camouflage http://www.cspangler.com/images/photos/aquarium/weedy-sea-dragon2.jpg Adaptive Camouflage Camouflage by Accessorizing Decorator crab Friday Harbor Marine Health Observatory http://www.projectnoah.org/ Camouflage by Mimicry http://www.berkeley.edu/news/media/releases/2005/03/24_octopus.shtml Mimicry • Animals can gain protection (or even access to prey) by looking
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptations for Survival: Symbioses, Camouflage
    Adaptations for Survival: Symbioses, Camouflage & Mimicry OCN 201 Biology Lecture 11 http://www.oceanfootage.com/stockfootage/Cleaning_Station_Fish/ http://www.berkeley.edu/news/media/releases/2005/03/24_octopus.shtml Symbiosis • Parasitism - negative effect on host • Commensalism - no effect on host • Mutualism - both parties benefit Often involves food but benefits may also include protection from predators, dispersal, or habitat Parasites Leeches (Segmented Worms) Tongue Louse (Crustacean) Nematodes (Roundworms) Whale Barnacles & Lice Commensalism or Parasitism? Commensalism or Mutualism? http://magma.nationalgeographic.com/ http://www.scuba-equipment-usa.com/marine/APR04/ Mutualism Cleaner Shrimp http://magma.nationalgeographic.com/ Anemone Hermit Crab http://www.scuba-equipment-usa.com/marine/APR04/ Camouflage Countershading Sharks Birds Countershading coloration of the Caribbean reef shark © George Ryschkewitsch Fish JONATHAN CHESTER Mammals shiftingbaselines.org/blog/big_tuna.jpg http://www.nmfs.noaa.gov/pr/images/cetaceans/orca_spyhopping-noaa.jpg Adaptive Camouflage Camouflage http://www.cspangler.com/images/photos/aquarium/weedy-sea-dragon2.jpg Camouflage by Mimicry Mimicry • Batesian: an edible species evolves to look similar to an inedible species to avoid predation • Mullerian: two or more inedible species all evolve to look similar maximizing efficiency with which predators learn to avoid them Batesian Mimicry An edible species evolves to resemble an inedible species to avoid predators Pufferfish (poisonous) Filefish (non-poisonous)
    [Show full text]
  • (Lepidoptera: Pieridae) Butterflies Are Palatable to Avian Predators
    insects Article Evaluating an Alleged Mimic of the Monarch Butterfly: Neophasia (Lepidoptera: Pieridae) Butterflies are Palatable to Avian Predators Dale A. Halbritter 1,2,* , Johnalyn M. Gordon 3, Kandy L. Keacher 4, Michael L. Avery 4,5 and Jaret C. Daniels 2,6 1 USDA-ARS Invasive Plant Research Laboratory, 3225 College Ave, Fort Lauderdale, FL 33314, USA 2 Entomology and Nematology Department, University of Florida, 1881 Natural Area Dr, Steinmetz Hall, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA; jdaniels@flmnh.ufl.edu 3 Fort Lauderdale Research and Education Center, University of Florida, 3205 College Ave, Davie, FL 33314, USA; johnalynmgordon@ufl.edu 4 Florida Field Station, USDA-APHIS National Wildlife Research Center, 2820 E University Ave, Gainesville, FL 32641, USA; [email protected] (K.L.K.); [email protected] (M.L.A.) 5 2906 NW 14th Pl., Gainesville, FL 32605, USA 6 McGuire Center for Lepidoptera and Biodiversity, Florida Museum of Natural History, 3215 Hull Road, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA * Correspondence: dhalb001@ufl.edu or [email protected]; Tel.: +1-661-406-8932 Received: 28 September 2018; Accepted: 22 October 2018; Published: 29 October 2018 Abstract: Some taxa have adopted the strategy of mimicry to protect themselves from predation. Butterflies are some of the best representatives used to study mimicry, with the monarch butterfly, Danaus plexippus (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae) a well-known model. We are the first to empirically investigate a proposed mimic of the monarch butterfly: Neophasia terlooii, the Mexican pine white butterfly (Lepidoptera: Pieridae). We used captive birds to assess the palatability of N. terlooii and its sister species, N.
    [Show full text]
  • Mimicry: Ecology, Evolution, and Development
    Editorial Mimicry: Ecology, evolution, and development David PFENNIG, Guest Editor Department of Biology, University of North Carolina, Coker Hall, CB#3280, Chapel Hill, NC 27599 USA, [email protected] 1 Introduction 1879), multiple undesirable species (e.g., toxic species) converge on the same warning signal, thereby sharing Mimicry occurs when one species (the “mimic”) the cost of educating predators about their undesirabil- evolves to resemble a second species (the “model”) be- ity. cause of the selective benefits associated with confusing Mimicry is among the most active research areas in a third species (the “receiver”). For example, natural all of evolutionary biology, in part because of the highly selection can favor phenotypic convergence between integrative nature that the study of mimicry necessarily completely unrelated species when an edible species entails. Mimicry involves asking both functional ques- receives the benefit of reduced predation by resembling tions (it involves investigating, for example, the adap- an inedible species that predators avoid. tive significance of more versus less precise resem- Research into mimicry has a rich history that traces blance between models and mimics) and mechanistic back to the beginnings of modern evolutionary biology. ones (it also involves investigating, for example, how In 1862––a scant three years after Darwin had published mimetic phenotypes are produced). Thus, mimicry re- The Origin of Species––Henry Walter Bates (1862), an search draws on diverse fields, many of which are on English explorer and naturalist, first suggested that close the cutting edge of biological research. Indeed, as Bro- resemblances between unrelated species could evolve as die and Brodie (2004, p.
    [Show full text]
  • Model Toxin Level Does Not Directly Influence the Evolution of Mimicry In
    Evol Ecol (2015) 29:511–523 DOI 10.1007/s10682-015-9765-8 ORIGINAL PAPER Model toxin level does not directly influence the evolution of mimicry in the salamander Plethodon cinereus 1,2 1 1 Andrew C. Kraemer • Jeanne M. Serb • Dean C. Adams Received: 16 January 2015 / Accepted: 8 May 2015 / Published online: 14 May 2015 Ó Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015 Abstract The resemblance between palatable mimics and unpalatable models in Bate- sian mimicry systems is tempered by many factors, including the toxicity of the model species. Model toxicity is thought to influence both the occurrence of mimicry and the evolution of mimetic phenotypes, such that mimicry is most likely to persist when models are particularly toxic. Additionally, model toxicity may influence the evolution of mimetic phenotype by allowing inaccurate mimicry to evolve through a mechanism termed ‘relaxed selection’. We tested these hypotheses in a salamander mimicry system between the model Notophthalmus viridescens and the mimic Plethodon cinereus, in which N. viridescens toxicity takes the form of tetrodotoxin. Surprisingly, though we discovered geographic variation in model toxin level, we found no support for the hypotheses that model toxicity directly influences either the occurrence of mimicry or the evolution of mimic phenotype. Instead, a link between N. viridescens size and toxicity may indirectly lead to relaxed selection in this mimicry system. Additionally, limitations of predator perception or var- iation in the rate of phenotypic evolution of models and mimics may account for the evolution of imperfect mimicry in this salamander species. Finally, variation in predator communities among localities or modern changes in environmental conditions may con- tribute to the patchy occurrence of mimicry in P.
    [Show full text]
  • Deceptive Coloration - Natureworks 01/04/20, 11:58 AM
    Deceptive Coloration - NatureWorks 01/04/20, 11:58 AM Deceptive Coloration Deceptive coloration is when an organism's color Mimicry fools either its predators or its prey. There are two Some animals and plants look like other things -- types of deceptive coloration: camouflage and they mimic them. Mimicry is another type of mimicry. deceptive coloration. It can protect the mimic from Camouflage predators or hide the mimic from prey. If mimicry was a play, there would be three characters. The Model - the species or object that is copied. The Mimic - looks and acts like another species or object. The Dupe- the tricked predator or prey. The poisonous Camouflage helps an organism blend in with its coral snake surroundings. Camouflage can be colors or and the patterns or both. When organisms are harmless camouflaged, they are harder to find. This means king snake predators have to spend a longer time finding can look a them. That's a waste of energy! When a predator is lot alike. camouflaged, it makes it easier to sneak up on or Predators surprise its prey. will avoid Blending In: Stripes or Solids? the king snake because they think it is poisonous. This type There are of mimicry is called Batesian mimicry. In lots of Batesian mimicry a harmless species mimics a different toxic or dangerous species. examples of The viceroy butterfly and monarch butterfly were once thought to camouflage. Some colors and patterns help exhibit animals blend into areas with light and shadow. Batesian The tiger's stripes help it blend into tall grass. Its mimicry golden brown strips blend in with the grass and the where a dark brown and black stripes merge with darker harmless shadows.
    [Show full text]
  • The Origins of Mimicry Rings
    in Artificial Life VIII, Standish, Abbass, Bedau (eds)(MIT Press) 2002. pp 186{191 1 The Origins of Mimicry Rings Daniel W. Franks and Jason Noble School of Computing, University of Leeds Email: [dwfranks|jasonn]@comp.leeds.ac.uk Abstract Although beneficial to the mimic, Batesian mimicry is Mutualistic Mullerian¨ mimicry and parasitic Batesian detrimental to the survival of the model, as the presence mimicry can co-exist in mimicry rings, i.e., mimetic re- of tasty individuals dilutes the honesty of the warning lationships between multiple species. Theory suggests coloration. This means that Batesian mimicry is a par- that all Mullerian¨ mimics in an ecosystem should con- asitic relationship between mimic and model (see e.g., verge into one large ring. Potentially, the presence of Wickler, 1968) . Batesian mimics will encourage convergence. It has been suggested that rare species should seek out common Predators have fallible sensory systems and they gen- species as models. Mimicry rings have not previously eralize from their predation experiences. Thus, although been modelled; we present an evolutionary simulation to they are much more likely to mistake a perfect mimic investigate the above questions. Complete convergence for its model, they can still mistake approximate resem- is not observed although Batesian mimicry is shown to blances for the real thing. This gives a greater level of be important factor in the origin of mimicry rings. protection to a mimic the more accurate its mimicry be- Bees and wasps both possess yellow and black striped comes. However, if the appearances of two prey species warning colourations, making an obvious display of their are sufficiently distinct, the predator will never mistake unpleasantness.
    [Show full text]
  • (Thorn) Automimicry and Mimicry of Aposematic Colorful Thorns
    ARTICLE IN PRESS Journal of Theoretical Biology 224 (2003) 183–188 Weapon (thorn) automimicry and mimicry of aposematic colorful thorns in plants Simcha Lev-Yadun* Department of Biology, Faculty of Science and Science Education, University of Haifa-Oranim, Tivon 36006, Israel Received 29 April 2002; received in revised form 21 March 2003; accepted 4 April 2003 Abstract In order to further characterize the function of coloration in plants as defense against herbivory, two types of thorn mimicry are described: (1) A unique type of weapon (thorn) automimicry (within the same individual) that was previously known only in animals, and (2) mimicry of aposematic colorful thorns, by colorful elongated and pointed plant organs (buds, leaves and fruit) that, despite their appearance, are not sharp. Some thorny plants including dozens of species of Agave, one species of Aloe and a palm species have thorn-like imprints or colorations on their leaves, constituting thorn automimicry by giving the impression of more extensive thorns. The mimicry of aposematic colorful thorns is a typical case of Batesian mimicry, but the thorn automimicry is a special intra-organismic Batesian mimicry. I propose that both types of mimicry serve as anti-herbivore mechanisms. r 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Agave; Aloe; Aposematic coloration; Automimicry; Batesian mimicry; Herbivory; Thorns 1. Introduction Several authors have proposed mimicry in plants as an anti-herbivore mechanism. Wiens (1978) estimated Cases of automimicry, i.e. mimicry of some parts in that about 5% of the land plants are mimetic, listing other parts of the same individual, have rarely been several types of protective plant mimicry.
    [Show full text]
  • The Origin of Mimicry
    The Origin of Mimicry Deception or merely coincidence Bram Wiggers, s2485834, [email protected], Supervisor: Dr. Harmen de Weerd Abstract: One of the most remarkable phenomena in nature is mimicry, the ability of one species (mimics) to imitate the phenotype of another species (models). Several reasons for the origin of mimicry have been proposed, but no definitive conclusion has been found yet. In this paper, different hypotheses will be tested by an agent based co-evolution model. We consider an adaptive plane with a camouflage and toxic gradient. Animal populations can move over this adaptive plane by evolving. Two options will be compared: (1) Deception, which assumes that mimics evolve to take advantage of the warning color of the models. (2) Coincidence, where models evolve a warning color that coincidentally benefits the mimics. We found that both the hypotheses are a plausible reason for the origin of mimicry, but that deception takes place in the coincidence environment as well. When the model loses its camouflage, the mimics do not move toward the model, but stay in their own place, which supports the idea of coincidence. 1 Introduction (Rabosky, Cox, Rabosky, Title, Holmes, Feldman, and McGuire, 2016) and other animals (Maan and One of the most remarkable phenomena in nature Cummings, 2011). Various research has been done is mimicry, the ability of animals (mimics) to mimic (Mallet and Joron, 1999), but no clear answer has the phenotype of other animals (models). Typically, been found for the origin of mimicry. the effect is called mimicry when the mimic imitates a phenotype of a model which can be dangerous We distinguish Batesian mimicry and M¨ullerian for a predator.
    [Show full text]
  • Mimicry: an Interface Between Psychology and Evolution
    Commentary Mimicry: An interface between psychology and evolution James Mallet* Department of Biology, Galton Laboratory, University College London, 4 Stephenson Way, London NW1 2HE, United Kingdom very now and then, a new technique Bates also noticed that some Heliconius species. Even though Wallace was proba- Eallows major breakthroughs to be (in the ‘‘Acraeoid Heliconidae,’’ now bly correct about the ultimate benefits of made on a hitherto intractable problem. Nymphalidae: Heliconiinae) also mim- warning color to the population, warning In this issue of PNAS, Leena Lindstro¨m icked Ithomiinae, even though they clearly signals could not simply evolve from cryp- and colleagues (1) have shed light on had the aromatic smells of distasteful spe- sis, unless rare aposematic morphs within long-standing controversies in warning cies, and even though other Heliconius species could overcome the disadvantage color and mimicry. They have achieved were themselves the objects of mimicry of rarity that causes Mu¨llerian mimicry this through experiments on the behavior (3). Essentially, Bates discovered an early (6). A variety of hypotheses were pro- of wild-caught birds (great tits, Parus ma- version of Mu¨llerian mimicry, or mimicry posed in which grouping, particularly of jor), using their ingenious ‘‘novel world’’ between pairs of distasteful species, al- close kin (6, 7), would allow the local setup, a modification though he attrib- frequency to rise above a frequency- of that originally intro- uted this mainly to dependent selection threshold. Others re- duced by Rauno Ala- parallel adaptation garded individual selection as more likely, talo and Johanna Leena Lindstro¨m and colleagues by both species to and proposed that the threshold fre- Mappes in 1996 (2).
    [Show full text]