PronounsinKumykDiscourse:ACognitivePerspective ADISSERTATION SUBMITTEDTOTHEFACULTYOFTHEGRADUATESCHOOL OFTHEUNIVERSITYOFMINNESOTA BY LindaAnneHumnick INPARTIALFULFILLMENTOFTHEREQUIREMENTS FORTHEDEGREEOF DOCTOROFPHILOSOPHY JeanetteGundel,Adviser March2009

©LindaAnneHumnick,March2009 Acknowledgements JustasthisresearchbeganwithmyinitialfieldworkamongtheKumykpeopleof ,,Iwouldliketobeginbyexpressingmygratitudeforthegenerous hospitalityandlinguisticcontributionsofmyKumykfriendsandcolleagues.First,I wouldliketothankseveralKumykscholarswhocontributedtomyresearch.Thelate ZhangishiKhangishievwasmyfirstpatienttutorintheintricaciesofKumyk andmorphology.ThelateFaridaMuratchaevainspiredmewithherscholarshiponthe Kumyklanguageandencouragedmeinmyownresearch.RuhaniyatMusaeva,adear friend,beganasmytutorinspokenKumyk,assistedinthecollectionofdata,and generouslygavememanyopportunitiestosharethebeautyofherfamilyandculture. Finally,AgaragimSultanmuradovhasbeeninvaluableasacolleagueinscholarship, languageconsultant,contributortothetextdatabase,andastheadministratorofthe questionnaireonsiteinDagestan.Ivaluehisfriendship,thepatienceandinsightwith whichhehasansweredallmyquestionsoverthepast5years,andhismodelof scholarshipinKumyklanguageresearch. Iamgratefultothemembersofmyexaminingcommittee:NancyStenson,with whomIshareapassionforfieldresearch,HooiLingSoh,whosetsawonderfulexample instrivingforexcellenceinresearch,andMichaelKac,whosecreativeandanalytical thinkinghavebeenaninspiration.IalsoappreciatethecontributionofBruceDowning, whoservedonmycommitteeatanearlierstageandsharesmyenthusiasmfortheTurkic world.Iamdeeplyindebttomyadviser,JeanetteGundel,whonotonlyhassharedher insightandexpertiseinresearchandtaughtmemuchabouttheprocessofwriting,but hasalsoopenedherhomeandherfriendshiptomeovertheseyears,mentoringmeina numberofareasoflifeandscholarship. Iwouldnothavebeenabletocompleteaworkofthisscopewithoutthe encouragementoffriends.ThoughIcannotbegintonameallthefriendswhohave encouragedmewiththeirprayersandactsofkindnessinthesebusyyears,Iwouldat leastliketomentiontwofellowgraduatestudentswhohavesupportedmealongtheway. i Inparticular,IthankMichikoBuchanan,whoseencouragementandfriendshipreminded methatthebulbslyingdormantinthewinterwilleventuallysproutandbloom.A specialprivilegeduringmygraduatestudyhasbeenhavingmyfriendAmelKhalfaouias afellowresearcher,andasecondarycoderonsomeofmytexts.Moreimportantly,we havesharedmanyareasoffriendshipbeyondtheGivennessHierarchy—including dreamsforourfamiliesandcareers,andevenanappreciationofgoodfood. Finally,Iacknowledgethatthisworkhasonlybeenpossiblethroughthesupport andsacrificesofmyfamily.Iowemuchappreciationtomylovinggrandparents,Iva DeanWinklerandthelateGordonWinklerforteachingmethevalueofeducationand formakingprovisionsformycollegeandpostgraduateeducationevenbeforeIwasold enoughtogotocollege.Iespeciallywanttothankmygrandmother,forbeingoneofmy biggestfanswhileIwasingraduateschool.Iwanttoacknowledgemydeepappreciation formyparents,BobandBarbaraDunnigan,fortheirlove,prayers,wordsof encouragement,andunconditionalsupport.Theyhavealwaysencouragedmetodomy best,buthavealsoremindedmeto“taketimetosmelltheroses.”Ideeplyappreciatethe loveandsupportofmyhusband,Paul,andmychildren,AbrahamandKaty,andIthank themfortheirpatiencewithmeasIinvestedtimeandenergyinthisresearch.Onthe practicalside,IbenefittedgreatlyfromPaul’stechnicalcomputersupportandwillingness tohelpwithmanyaspectsofourfamilylife,andIamalsogratefultohavePaulasa colleagueinfieldresearch.Histravelsduringthelastfiveyearswereaninvaluable assistanceintheprocessofgatheringdata,andhismutualinterestandwillingnessto listentomyideashavebeenagreatencouragementtome.WithKatyandAbraham, manyhugsandmomentsoffunhavekeptmegoingduringthelonghoursofresearchand writing.Forthethreeofyouwhohavewalkedmostcloselywithmethroughthis process,yourlovehasmadeallthedifference.Finally,Igivemythanksandpraisetomy HeavenlyFather,whocreatedmewiththisinterestinlanguage,whoprovidesforallmy needs,andwhohasguidedmyfootalongthispath.

ii Dedication

TothegloryoftheCreatoroflanguageandthehonoroftheKumykpeople

iii Abstract

Thisdissertationinvestigatespronominalformsofreferringexpressionsin Kumyk,aTurkiclanguagespokenprimarilyintheDagestanregionofRussia.The Kumyklanguagehassixthirdpersonpronominals,includingnullarguments, demonstratives,andreflexives.Morphologically,eachoftheseformsisunmarkedfor genderoranimacy.Thisworkprovidesanexplanatoryaccountofthedistributionand interpretationofdifferentpronominalformsinKumykprimarilyintermsofwhatthese formscommunicateaboutthestatusoftheirreferentsinthemindsofthespeech participants,specificallyclaimingthatdifferentpronominalformssignaldifferencesin thecognitivestatusoftheirreferents,followingtheGivennessHierarchymodelof Gundel,Hedberg,andZacharski(1993).Theanalysisisbasedprimarilyondatafroma corpusoforalandwrittenKumyktextswithsupportingevidencefromgrammaticality judgmentsofconstructedexamplesinquestionnaires.Accordingtotheanalysis,null argumentsandreflexivessignalthestatus,‘infocus’,whiledemonstrativessignalthe status,‘activated’.Particularattentionisgiventotheroleofscalarimplicatureswhich arisefromtheunidirectionalentailmentofstatusesontheGivennessHierarchyandthe factthatthedemonstrative sho ,whichsignalsactivation,hasaparticularassociationwith thisimplicature.Auniquecontributionoftheanalysisistheevidenceforthefactthat sho notonlygivesrisetoascalarimplicatureincontextswheretworeferentshave differentmaximalcognitivestatus(e.g.oneinfocusversusoneatmostactivated),but alsoincontextswheretworeferentshavethesamemaximalcognitivestatus,afactwhich leadstotheconclusionthatthisformspecializesinindicatingthelesssalientoftwoor moreentities.Thestudyalsoprovidesevidencethatthedemonstrative bu specializesin indicatingthemoreprominentoftwoormoreentitiesthatareatleastactivated.Finally, inadditiontotheroleofpronominalsinsignalingcognitivestatusandcommunicating therelativeprominenceofmultiplereferents,thestudyexplorescontextualeffectssuch asimposedsalience,pointofview,empathy,orcontrastivefocusthatareassociatedwith particularforms. iv Table of Contents

Acknowledgements...... i Dedication…………………………………………………………………..iii Abstract……………………………………………………………………..iv TableofContents...... v ListofTables ...... ix ListofFigures...... x Chapter1: Introduction...... 1 1.0. Kumyklanguagefacts...... 3 1.1. Kumykpronouns...... 5 1.2. Organizationofthestudy...... 8 Chapter2: TheoreticalFramework...... 10 2.0. Whatisacognitivemodel?...... 10 2.1. Givenness,topiccontinuity,centeringandaccessibility ...... 12 2.2. TheGivennessHierarchymodel...... 17 2.3. CodingprocedurefortheGHmodel ...... 22 2.3.1. Somecodingchoicesinthisstudy...... 24 2.3.2. Codingclausallyintroducedandhigherorderentities ...... 27 2.4. TheGHmodelandotherpragmaticfactorsinreferentialchoice...... 31 2.5. Methodologyinthisstudy ...... 34 2.5.1. Descriptionofcorpus...... 34 2.6. Descriptionofquestionnaireandgrammaticalityjudgments ...... 35 Chapter3: KumykPronounsandTheirProperties...... 44 3.0. Generalcomments ...... 44 3.1. Personalpronounsordemonstratives ...... 47 3.2. Nullpronouns...... 53

v 3.3. Reflexives ...... 58 3.3.1. Generalproperties...... 58 3.3.2. Syntacticproperties...... 60 3.4. Summary...... 64 Chapter4: CorpusStudy...... 65 4.0. Generalcomments ...... 65 4.1. Nullsinthecorpus ...... 68 4.1.1. Codingthereferentsofnullarguments...... 68 4.1.2. Discussionofundeterminedcases ...... 72 4.1.3. Discussionofactivatedcases...... 74 4.1.4. Higherleveltopics...... 80 4.1.5. Clausallyintroducedentities ...... 83 4.2. Reflexivesinthecorpus...... 84 4.2.1. Discussionofundeterminedcases ...... 84 4.2.2. Referentsofreflexivesand‘antecedent’types ...... 86 4.2.2.1. Sameclauseantecedent...... 87

4.2.2.2. Longdistanceantecedent...... 89

4.2.3. Contextualeffectsofreflexives ...... 95 4.3. Thepronounbuinthecorpus ...... 100 4.3.1. Codingthereferentsof bu ...... 102 4.3.2. Discussionofundetermined/cataphoracases...... 104 4.3.3. Discussionofexamplesthatweredifficulttocode ...... 105 4.3.4. Anactivatedentitynotintroducedbyanominal...... 107 4.3.5. Contextualeffectsassociatedwith bu ...... 108 4.4. Thepronoun ointhecorpus ...... 114 4.4.1. Codingreferentsof o ...... 115 4.4.2. Discussionofnoteworthycases...... 117 4.4.3. Referencetoclausallyintroducedentities...... 119 4.5. Thepronoun sho inthecorpus...... 122 vi 4.5.1. Referentsof sho introducedbynominals...... 124 4.5.2. Referencetoentitiesnotintroducedbynominals...... 128 4.6. Thepronoun shu inthecorpus...... 133 4.7. Summaryandinterpretationofcorpusanalysis...... 134 4.7.1. Formstatuscorrelations:Wheretogofromhere...... 134 4.7.2. Otherdistributionalfeatures ...... 136 4.7.3. Evidenceofcontextualeffects...... 139 4.7.4. Methodologicalissues...... 140 Chapter5: QuestionnaireResults ...... 144 5.0. Goalsofthequestionnaire ...... 144 5.1. Usingthequestionnairetotestpredictions...... 147 5.2. Questionnairedataonnullarguments...... 150 5.2.1. Commentsonpotentialcategoryrestrictions...... 151 5.2.2. Useofnullswithreferentscodedasactivated...... 153 5.3. Reflexives ...... 160 5.4. Questionnairedataon bu ...... 162 5.4.1. Useofbu withreferentscodedasactivated...... 163 5.4.2. Useof bu withclausalentities ...... 171 5.5. Questionnairedataon o ...... 174 5.6. Questionnairedataon sho ...... 177 5.7. Questionnairedataon shu ...... 185 5.8. Evidenceforimplicatures ...... 187 5.8.1. Overtpronouns,topicshift,andimplicatures...... 190 5.8.2. Implicatures,frequency,andspecialization...... 195 5.8.3. Rankingdata ...... 204 5.9. Conclusions...... 218 Chapter6: ImplicationsoftheAnalysis ...... 223 6.0. FormstatuscorrelationsandpredictionsoftheGHmodel...... 223 6.1. AdequacyoftheGHmodelinthisanalysis...... 226

vii 6.2. Degreesofrestrictiveness ...... 230 6.3. Cognitivestatusandrankingeffects...... 237 6.4. Inherentversusimposedsalience...... 241 6.5. Questionsofwiderinterestandareasforfurtherstudy...... 244 References 248 Appendix1:GeographicalDistributionoftheLanguagesofDagestan ... 257 Appendix2:Textcorpus ...... 258 Appendix3:SampleText...... 259 Appendix4:Questionnaire...... 282

viii List of Tables

Table1 : Distributitonofpronounsintextcorpus ……………………...………………35

Table2: Morphologicaldeclensionsofpronominalforms ……………………………..49

Table3: Theinflectedreflexive ……………………………………………...………….60

Table4: Casedeclensionsof3 rd personreflexives ……………………………………..60

Table5: CodingResultsforPronominalFormsintheTextCorpus …………………...66 Table6 : Percentageofcodedreferentswithaspecificcognitivestatus ……………….66 Table7: Bu andrankedentities ……………………………………………………….202

ix List of Figures

Figure1: TheGivennessHierarchy …………………………………………………….18 Figure2: Codingprotocolforinfocusstatus(Gundel2004) ………………………..…23 Figure3:Codingprotocolforactivatedstatus(Gundel2004)………..……………..…24 Figure4: Pronounsinlogicalconnectives …………………………………………..…51 Figure5 : FormStatusCorrelations …………………………………………………….67 Figure6 : Rankedpairs ………………………………………………………………...207 Figure7 : Final FormStatusCorrelations …………………………………………….224 Figure8 : DegreeofRestrictiveness ……………………………………………….…..235 Figure9 : Referencetonominallyintroducedentities ………………………………….236 Figure10 :Referencetoentitiesintroducedbyaclause …………………………...….236 Figure11 : Percentofcorpusreferentsinfocus ……………………………………….237

x

Chapter 1: Introduction

Thegoalofthisdissertationistoprovideanexplanatoryaccountofthe

distributionandinterpretationofdifferentpronominalformsinKumykdiscourse primarilyintermsofwhattheseformscommunicateaboutthestatusoftheirreferentsin

themindsofthespeechparticipants.Thebasisforthisanalysisisevidencefromatext

corpus,withsupportingdatafromgrammaticalityjudgmentsandconstructedexamplesin

questionnaires. 1

ThisstudyfollowstheGivennessHierarchyframeworkofGundel,Hedberg,and

Zacharski1993,whoclaimthatpronounsandothertypesofreferringformssignalthe cognitivestatusoftheirreferentsinthemindoftheaddresseeaspartoftheirlexical meaning.Crosslinguisticstudieswithinthismodelshowthatpronominalsreferto entitiesthatthespeakerexpectstoberepresentedintheworkingmemoryofthe addressee(activated)andasubsetofpronominalsarerestrictedtoreferencetoentities thatareatthecenteroftheaddressee’sattention(infocus)(Gundeletal.1993,Gundel,

Bassene,Gordon,HumnickandKhalfaoui2007).Thisstudylikewisefindsthattheuse ofKumykpronominalscorrelateswithactivatedreferentsandthattwotypesofforms, nullsandthepronominalreflexive o’ziu arerestrictedtoreferentsinfocus.Myresearch alsoprovidesevidencetosupporttheGivennessHierarchymodel’sclaimthatthe

1AsignificantpartofthisresearchiscarriedoutunderNSFgrant#0519890toJeanetteGundel.

1 distinctionbetweenthesetofformsthatsignalthatareferentisactivatedinthemindof theaddresseeandthesetthatsignalsthatareferentisinfocusisthebasisforscalar implicaturesthatplayaroleinreferentdisambiguationthroughtheindicationofrelative salience.

Inadditiontocommunicatinginformationaboutthecognitivestatusofareferent, thechoiceofpronominalmaycommunicateotherinformationaboutareferent,suchas thespeaker’sempathytowardstheentityorthefactthatoneentityisbeingcontrasted withanother.InKumyk,differencesinthesetypesoffeaturesoccuramongformsthat signalthesamecognitivestatus.Asignificantaspectofthisstudy,moreover,isthe evidencethat,amongtheformsthatsignal‘activated’,twoformsappeartohavea specializedfunctionofindicatinghowthespeakerwishesthehearertointerpretthe relativesalienceoftheentityincomparisontootherentitiesinthecontext—withone oftenindicatingthemostprominentofasetofentities,whiletheotherindicatestheleast prominent.

Whiletheprimarysignificanceofthisresearchistosupportthecrosslinguistic validityoftheorieswhichcorrelatetheuseofrestrictedformswithattentionstate,the dissertationwillalsobeasignificantcontributiontothegeneralknowledgeofKumyk,a languageaboutwhichrelativelylittlehasbeenwritteningeneralandevenlessin

English.Thedissertationprovidesanoverviewofpronominalformsinthelanguage, includingaspectsofmorphology,andacharacterizationoftheuseoftheseformsin discourse.Inparticular,thediscussionofnullargumentsandpronominalreflexives providesinformationofwiderinteresttosyntactictheoryandlinguistictypology.

2 1.0. facts

TheKumyklanguageisamemberoftheWestgroupoftheKipchak(or

Western)branchofTurkiclanguages.Themajorityoftheapproximately450,000 speakersofthislanguageliveintheRepublicofDagestanintheRussianFederation. 2

Kumykisoneofapproximately40differentlanguagesspokeninthisregion,whichlies

northofAzerbaijanbetweentheMountainsandtheCaspianSea.Smaller

groupsofKumykspeakersarealsoreportedinotherpartsoftheCaucasusregion,

Turkey,and(Gordon2005).

ThemajorityofKumykspeakersarebilingualinRussianandKumyk.While

Russianisboththenationallanguageandtheprimarylanguageofeducationforthe

region,KumykisavitalandwelldevelopedlanguagewithinDagestan.Kumykhasbeen

aliterarylanguageformorethanacentury.Thefirstorthography,basedon

script,wasuseduntil1928,andthenreplacedbyaLatinbasedscript.Since1938,

KumykhasusedaCyrillicorthography.

Kumykiscurrentlyalanguageofeducation,butthedegreeofuseineducation

variesfromcommunitytocommunityaccordingtothecompositionofthelocal population.InpredominatelyKumykcommunities,Kumykisalanguageofinstruction

attheprimarylevel,whileinsecondaryschoolsandinhighereducation,thelanguageis

taughtonlyasasubject.Inmultiethniccommunities,Russianisthelanguageof

2Thepopulationestimatereflectsinformationfromthe2002Russiancensus.Previousinformationreports approximately280,000speakers(cf.Gordon2005).Amapshowingthegeographicaldistributionof KumykspeakersinrelationtothedistributionofspeakersofotherlanguagesinDagestanappearsas Appendix1.

3 instruction,andKumykliteratureisofferedasasubjectonlyinareaswithasignificant percentageofKumykspeakers.Forthisreason,whileKumykspeakerswholivein predominantlyKumykcommunitiestendtobeliterateinKumykaswellasRussian,the majorityofKumykspeakersinmultilingualcommunitiessuchas,the capitalofDagestan,useKumykonlyasanorallanguage.

WithintheDagestanregion,Kumykisusedinboththemediaandthearts,asis evidentfromtheexistenceofKumyktheatre,journals,newspaper,radioprogramsand

TVbroadcasts.Manylocalscholarsaffiliatedwithinstitutionsofhighereducationand researchinstitutionssuchastheDagestanbranchoftheRussianAcademyofSciencesare activeinlanguagerelatedresearch.

VerylittledocumentationoftheKumyklanguageexistsinEnglish,buttherearea significantnumberofdescriptiveworksinRussianandonenotablecomprehensivework inKumyk.Dimitriev1940providestheearliestcomprehensivegrammaroftheKumyk languageinRussian.Khangishiev1995providesamorerecentoverviewofthe morphologyandgrammarwrittenintheKumyklanguage. 3Inadditiontothese comprehensiveworks,anumberofarticlesandtheseshavebeenwritteninRussianon variousaspectsofthelanguage.Amongthesearedescriptionsoftheverbalsystem

(Djanmavov1967,Hangishiev1985,Gadzhiakhmedov1987,Sultanmuradov1997inter alia),morphophonology(Ol’mesov1994),historicalandderivationalmorphology

(Gadzhiakhmedov1998,Kadyradzhiev1999,Abdurakmanova2005),andcomplex

3AnunpublishedgrammaroverviewinEnglishbasedontheseworkswascompiledbyClinton1997.

4 clauseconstructions(Abdulaeva1973). 4Ofparticularrelevancetothisworkisabrief

descriptionofdemonstrativesbyMuratchaeva(2001).

Tothispoint,littleattentionhasbeengiventothecomprehensivedescriptionof

theuseofreferringexpressionsinKumykdiscourse,thoughabriefoverviewisoffered

inHumnick2002andsmallerscaleanalysesofcertainaspectsoftheuseofpronounsand

nullargumentsareofferedinHumnick2005,2007and2008.

1.1. Kumyk pronouns

TheKumyklanguagehasavarietyofthirdpersonpronominalforms.These

includenullarguments,theovertproforms, bu , o, sho ,and shu ,andtheinflected3rd personreflexive, o’ziu .Morphologically,eachoftheseformsisunmarkedforgenderor animacy.Ingrammaticaldescriptions, oisdescribedasapersonalpronoun,with o’ziu as

itsreflexivecounterpart,while bu , o, sho ,and shu aredescribedasdemonstratives

(Dimitriev1940,Hangishiev1995).Inspiteofthefactthattheseproformsareassociated

withdifferentlabelssuchas‘reflexive’,‘demonstrative’,or‘personalpronoun’,toalarge

extenttheysharethesamesyntacticdistribution,asillustratedinexamples(1)(4).

Examples(1)(3)exemplifythealternationbetweenanullargument,theovertpronoun

o, andtheinflectedreflexive o’ziuinthesecondsentenceofeachexample.Example(4)

illustratesanalternationbetween oand bu inthetwoparallelsentencesoftheexample. 5

4ForamorecompletedescriptionoflinguisticworksontheKumyklanguage,seeGjulmagomedov,A.G., I.X.Abdullaev,M.Sh.Khalilov,andF.A.Muratchaeva,eds.1998.Bibliographiiapodagestanskomu iazykaznaniiu.Makhachkala:IzdatelsvoInstitutaIazyka,LiteraturyIiskusstvaD.N.Ts.R.A.N. 5ThevernaculartexthasbeentransliteratedaccordingtotheALALC1997standards.Thefollowing abbreviationsareusedinglossing: ABL =Ablative, ACC =Accusative, COND =Conditional,CON =Conjunction,

5 (1) Salimaterine ash ete. Song,Øsavutsabazhuĭe. Salimathusband.3POSS .DAT foodmake. PR later dishes wash. PR ‘Salimatfixesherhusbandameal.Later(s)hewashesthedishes.’

(2) Salimaterine ash ete. Song,osavutsabazhuĭe. Salimathusband.3POSS .DAT foodmake. PR later3.2 dishes wash. PR ‘Salimatfixesherhusbandameal.Later(s)hewashesthedishes.’

(3) Salimatk”yzyny yashyiuretdi. Salimat daughter.3POSS .ACC well teach. PST Xali o’ziu savutsaba zhumaĭ; k’zyny da zhuĭe. Now self.3POSSdishes wash. NEG .PR daughter.3POSS EMPH wash. PR ‘Salimattaughtherdaughterwell.Now(s)heherselfdoesn’twashthedishes; herdaughterdoes.’

(4) O savutsaba zhuĭmady. Bu zhuĭdy. 3.2 dishes wash. NEG .PST 3.1 wash. PST ‘Thatonedidn’twashthedishes.Thisonedid.’

Theexistenceofsuchadiversityofproformssharingthesamesyntacticdistributionbut lackinggrammaticaldistinctionsofgenderoranimacystronglysuggeststhatdifferent formsareusedtosignalothercharacteristicsofthereferent.

COP =Copula,DAT =Dative,EMPH =Emphatic, FUT =Future, GEN =Genitive,GER =Gerund/Converb, INF =Infinitive/VerbalNoun, LOC =Locative, MOD =Modifier, NOM =Nominalizer, PASS =Passive, PL=Plural, POSS =Possessive, PR =Present, PRT =Participal, PST =Past, RFLX =Reflexive(verbal), S=Singular, SPEC =Speculative.3.1,3.2,3.3and3.4indicatethefourdistinctthirdpersonpronouns, bu , o, sho ,and shu , respectively.

6 Anumberoflinguistssuggestthattheexplanationoftheuseofdifferent pronominalformsliesprimarilywithintherealmofpragmaticsandinformationstructure.

Forexample,theuseofnullargumentsincontrasttoovertpronounsisoftenassociated withthetopicalityorsalienceofthereferent(Givón1983,Ariel1988,Gundel,Hedberg, andZacharski1993,interalia).Intheliteratureonreflexives,whichpredominantly focusesonthesyntacticpropertiesofcoreferencerelations,anumberofstudiesassociate certainusesofsuchformswithnonsyntacticpropertiesofthereferentssuchasdiscourse prominence,contrast,orempathy(Kuno1987,Baker1995,ZribiHertz1995).For demonstratives,differencesindistributionareassociatedwithdifferencesinthe referent’slocationinrelationtothespeaker/hearer(Diessel1999,Dixon2003,interalia) ordifferentcommunicationfunctionsrelatedtoestablishingjointattention(Özyürekand

Kita2001(citedinEnfield2003),Enfield2003,Diessel2006).Finally,someapproaches correlatetheuseofallthreetypesofproforms,inadditiontootherformsofreferring expressions,withdifferencesintheattentionstateoftheaddresseeinrelationtothe referent(Chafe1976,Ariel1988,Gundeletal.1993,interalia).Theseapproaches, whicharenotnecessarilyincontradictiontotheothertypesofapproachesmentioned here,differinthesensethattheyaddressthedistributionofproformsaspartofaunified explanationofthedistributionofreferringformsinlanguageandthecognitiveprocesses whichdeterminereferentialchoice.

Reflectingsuchunifiedapproaches,thisstudyproposesthatthecharacteristicsof referentswhicharesignaledbydifferentproformsinKumykrelatetohowthereferentis representedinthemindofthehearer.Morespecifically,Iclaimthatdifferent

7 pronominalformssignaldifferencesinthecognitivestatusoftheirreferents,following theGivennessHierarchymodelofGundeletal.(1993).Proposingsixcognitivestatuses whichrepresentdifferentlevelsinthehearer’smemoryandattentionstate,Gundeletal. claimthatdifferentformsofreferringexpressionscanbeusedtosignaldifferent cognitivestatuses.Inotherwordsthechoicebetweenformsinagivenlanguage,suchas thechoicebetweenanullformversusthepronoun oinKumyk,representsadifferencein

howthespeakerassumesthereferentoftheformisrepresentedinthehearer’smind.

Thesignalsthatthehearerreceivesviatheformofreferringexpression,inturn,guidethe

hearerintheprocessofreferenceresolutionbyconstrainingreferencetothesetof possiblereferentswithaparticularstatus.Particularlyinthecaseofreducedformswith

minimaldescriptiveorconceptualcontent,suchconstraintssignificantlyreducethe processingeffortrequiredforthehearertomaketheassociationbetweenaformandits

referent.

1.2. Organization of the study

InChapter2ofthisdissertation,Iprovideacomprehensivediscussionoftheories

relatedtothedistributionofreferringformsindiscourseandtheparticularfeaturesofthe

cognitivemodelofGundeletal.1993.Thesecondpartofthechapterdescribesthe

methodologyusedtoanalyzereferringformswithinthisparticularframework,in particular,thecodingprotocolusedtodeterminethecognitivestatusofthereferentofa

referringexpression.Chapter3definesthescopeoftheterm‘pronominal’forthe

Kumyklanguageandprovidesgeneralmorphologicaldataforthesixdistinctforms

consideredinthisresearch.Chapters4presentsananalysisofpronountokensandtheir

8 referentswithinacorpusstudy,inparticular,thecodingofthecognitivestatusofthe referentsandthehypothesizedcorrelationsbetweenaparticularformandaparticular cognitivestatuswithintheGivennessHierarchy.Thischapteralsodescribesdistinct featuesofeachform,includingspecificcontextualeffectsassociatedwiththeiruse.

Chapter5furtherexploresthehypothesizedformstatuscorrelationsonthebasisof grammaticalityjudgmentsandothertypesofquestionnairedata.InthischapterIalso explorethewayinwhichdifferentformsareusedtoindicatetherelativeprominenceof entities,boththroughscalarimplicaturesandotherrankingeffects.Finally,Chapter6 summarizestheconclusionsandtheoreticalimplicationsoftheintegratedanalysis.

9 Chapter 2: Theoretical Framework

2.0. What is a cognitive model?

Associatingtheformofareferringexpressionwithcharacteristicsofareferentis basictotheactofreference.Whilecontentnounssuchas tree maybesaidtobe associatedwithreferentswhichhavetreelikequalities,thedifferencebetweenthe expression atree andtheexpression thetree—and,evenmoreso,thedifferencebetween expressionssuchas it or that —arenotrelatedtotheinherentqualitiesofthetreeentity, buttoqualitiesassociatedwiththerepresentationofthatentityinthespeechcontext,in particularwhatthespeakerbelievesaboutthewayinwhichthereferentofaparticular expressionisrepresentedinthemindoftheaddresseeatagivenmoment(Chafe1976,

Prince1981,Ariel1988,Gundeletal.1993,Lambrecht1994,interalia).

Inordertoillustratetherelationshipbetweentheformofareferringexpression andhowanentityisrepresentedintheaddressee’smind,considerthefollowingexample.

SupposethatAnnecallsBobonthetelephoneandAnneuttersthespeechrepresentedin

(5).

(5) Anne(toBob): RememberthewomanwemetattheChristmasparty? She cametoseemeyesterday.

ThefirstutterancebringsarepresentationofaparticularwomantothecenterofBob’s attention.InthesecondutteranceAnneuses she torefertothesamewoman.Anumber ofpotentialexpressionscouldhavereferredtothesameindividual—forexample, the

womanwiththeredhair,thatwoman,awoman ,amongothers.ThefactthatAnneuses

10 she indicatesthatsheexpectsthewomanfromthepartytobeatthecenterofBob’s attention(duetothefactthatsheistheentitybroughtintofocusbytheprevious utterance).Bob,inturn,caneasilyinterpretthisformeventhoughithasverylimited semanticcontentbecauseheassociatesitwiththewomanwhoisatthecenterofhis attention.

Furtherevidencethataspeakercommunicatessomethingaboutareferenttothe hearerviathechoiceofreferringexpressioncanbedemonstratedinthefollowing scenario.SupposethatAnnecommunicatestheutterancein(6)toBob.

(6) Anne(toBob): RememberthewomanwemetattheChristmasparty? Awoman cametoseemeyesterday…

Byusingtheform awoman AnnecommunicatestoBobthatshebelievesthereferentis notatthecenterofhisattentionbecauseitissomeonewhoisnotevenidentifiableby

Bobbyvirtueofbeinginhislongorshorttermmemoryoronthebasisofthedescriptive contentencodedinthephraseitself.6SinceAnnejustreferredtoawomanfromthe

Christmasparty(whoisnowatthecenterofBob’sattention),BobassumesthatAnne

6WithintheGivennessHierarchymodelproposedbyGundeletal.1993,thisisduetothescalar implicaturesinherentinahierarchicalmodeloftheuseofreferringformsinterpretedinlightofGrice’s MaximofQuantity,whichareexplainedmorefullyinsection2.2.Theimplicaturethatthereferentisnot morethantypeidentifiable,arisesfromthefactthatthedeterminer aisinterpretedassignalingthestatus ‘typeidentifiable’.Sincethespeakerdoesnotuseastrongerformthatindicatestheaddresseeshouldbe abletoidentifythereferent—thatis, thewoman , thatwoman ,or she ,asshownin(i)below—theuseof a woman implicatesthatthereferentisnotuniquelyidentifiablebyBob.Whenthestrongerformisused,on theotherhand,asinexample(i)below,thehearerwouldhavetosearchmemorytofindauniquely identifiablereferent,andthewomanfromtheChristmaspartywouldsatisfythatsearchwithminimial processingeffort. (i)Anne(toBob):RememberthewomanwemetattheChristmasparty? Thewoman/thatwoman/she cametoseemeyesterday.

11 mustbetalkingaboutawomanwhoisdifferentfromthewomanfromtheChristmas party.

Reducedformssuchasthepronoun she haveextremelylimitedinformation content,yettheyareeffectiveincommunicationbecausetheyareconstrainedtousewith highlysalientreferents.FromtheexchangebetweenAnneandBob,weseethat associationsbetweentheformofareferringexpressionandsalienceofpotentialreferents allowBobtoidentifythereferentsofthevariousreferringexpressionsinAnne’sspeech.

Thenextsectionpresentstheoreticalmaterialwhichdescribestheconstraintsonreferring expressionsandtheireffectsincommunicationmoreprecisely.

2.1. Givenness, topic continuity, centering and accessibility

Inalandmarkarticle,Chafe1976isoneofthefirsttotalkabouttheinformation statusofreferentsofnounphrases.Citingstatuseslikegivennew,focusofcontrast, definiteindefinite,subject,topic,andpointofview,Chafepreparesthewayfortheories whichlinktheuseofdistinctformsofreferringexpressionswithpropertiesofthe referentinthemindsoftheinterlocutors.Subsequentstudiesfocusondescribingthese propertiesintermsofgivenness/familiarity(Prince1981),topiccontinuity(Givón1983), accessibility(Ariel1988,1990),centerofattention(Grosz,Joshi,andWeinstein1983,

1995,Walker,Joshi,andPrince1998),orcognitivestatus(Gundeletal.1993).While theGivennessHierarchy(henceforthGH)modelofGundeletal.providesthemost comprehensiveframeworkfordescribingthecognitivepropertiesofreferentsofreferring expressions,itisusefultolookatthekeycontributionsofearlierworksandthe

12 differencesbetweentheGHmodelandotherframeworkswhichassociateformsof referringexpressionswithmentalproperties.

Prince1981introducesacomprehensiveviewof“givenness”,unifyingvarious

typeswithinasingletaxonomyandintroducingdistinctionssuchas“new”,“inferrable”,

and“evoked”.Thisworkrecognizestwotypesofgivennesswhicharedirectlyrelatedto

thewayinwhichareferentisrepresentedinthemindoftheaddressee:1)givenness

whichisaresultofthefactthatareferentisassumedtobeintheconsciousnessofthe

addresseebecauseithaseitherbeenintroducedinthediscourseorispresentinthe physicalcontextoftheutterance;and2)givennesswhichresultsfromsharedcultural

knowledge,whichiscrucialtotheconceptthatareferentis“inferrable”,whichmeans

thatitcanbelogicallyinferredfromotherdiscourseentitiesinthecontext(1981:236).7

AthirdtypeofgivennessdescribedbyPrincerelatestopropertiessuchasparallelism, predictability,andrecoverability.AccordingtoPrince,thistypeofgivennessrefersto theassumptionthat“thehearercanpredictorcouldhavepredictedthataparticular linguisticitemwillorwouldoccurinaparticularpositionwithinasentence”(1981:226).

ThoughtheroleofthistypeofgivennessisnotaddressedspecificallybytheGHmodel, myanalysisindicatesthatsyntacticparallelismplaysaroleinthedistributionofreduced formsofreferringexpressionsinKumyk.

7Intheutterance,‘Iwenttothehouse,butthedoorwaslocked,’theentityrepresentedby thedoor is inferredfromtheexistenceofthehouseentity(activatedinthepreviousclause)basedonculturalor encyclopedicknowledgeaboutthestructureofahouse.

13 Givón(1983)establishescorrelationsbetweenvariousdegreesoftopiccontinuity indiscourseandahierarchyofformsofreferringexpressionsbasedontheamountof codingmaterial.Workingwithinthedomainoffunctional,Givónclaimsthat different“topics”,ordiscourseparticipants,areassociatedwithdifferentdegreesof accessibilityforprocessingbasedontheirpredictabilityor“continuity”(1983:7).Forms ofreferringexpressions,inturn,correlatewiththedegreeofcontinuityofthetopic.For example,inmanylanguages,anullformisassociatedwithacontinuingtopic,whilean overtpronounsignalsachangeintopic.Givónproposesacrosslinguisticgeneralization thatahierarchyofformsbasedonphonologicalsize(zeroanaphora>unstressed/bound pronounsorgrammaticalagreement,stressed/independentpronouns,fullNP’s)correlates withthedegreeoftopiccontinuity(1983:18).Toalargeextent,theorderingofformsin thehierarchyisreflectedinthecorrelationssuggestedbytheGHmodel;however,the lattermodelisahierarchyofcognitivestates,notofforms.ThoughGivón’sclaims accountforaspectsofthedistributionofformsindiscourse,hemakesnoclaimsabout theuseofreferringexpressionsforentitieswhicharenotaccessibleviathelinguistic context(thatis,viaculturalknowledgeorthephysicalcontext).Givón’spredictions, moreover,representprobablecorrelations,incontrasttotheabsolutepredictionsofthe

GHmodelthatresultfromthestructureofunidirectionalentailment.

BuildingonPrince’sfamiliarityscaleandGivón’shierarchyofforms,Ariel

(1988,1990)proposesthatthedistributionofdifferenttypesofreferringexpressionscan beaccountedforbythedegreeofaccessibilityoftheirreferentsintermsofahierarchyof high,mid,andlowaccessibilitylevels.AccordingtoAriel,areferentisaccessiblevia

14 threehierarchicallyorderedmeans(fromlowesttohighestaccessibility):“general knowledge,physicalsurroundingsandpreviouslinguisticmaterial”(1988:68).Ateach oftheselevelsofaccessibility,differentformsrepresentdifferentrankingsonascaleof lowtohighaccessibility.Forexample,withinthelevelofaccessibilityviageneral knowledge,aformsuchas JoanSmiththepresident representsloweraccessibilitythan theform Joan ,andwithinthelevelofaccessibilityviathelinguisticcontext,areflexive anaphorsuchas herself representshigheraccessibilitythanthepronoun she (1988:81).

Ariel’stheoryiscomprehensiveinthesensethatitclaimstoaccount,notonlyfora varietyofformsofexpressions,butalsoforthedistinctionbetweenpronominaland anaphor(inthebindingtheorysense);however,shedoesnotsuggestamethodologyby whichonecantestformstatuscorrelationsincrosslinguisticcorpusstudies.Also,like

Givón,herpredictionsareprobabalisticanddonotdistinguishthosecaseswhere pronounsarecompletelydisallowedfromcaseswhereapronounorfullNPisequally appropriate.

CenteringTheory(Groszetal.,1983,1995,Walker,Joshi,andPrince1998)is anotherapproachwhichclaimsthattherelativesalienceofdiscourseentitiesplaces constraintsontheformofreferringexpressionwhichmaybeused.WhiletheGivenness

Hierarchyisacomprehensivemodelofformstatuscorrelationsandhowtheyderive frombothdiscourseandaspectsoftheextralinguisticcontext(includingencyclopedic knowledge,asforinferrables),CenteringTheoryfocusesontherelationshipbetweenthe formofreferringexpressionandthecoherenceofdiscourse,describingfocusofattention atpointsinadiscourseandhowchangesinattentionstatetakeplace(Groszetal.1995:4).

15 Thistheorydescribesattentionstateasaproductofthediscoursealone,primarilyrelating ittoprevioussegments,butalsosuggestingthathigherleveldiscourseunitsplayarole.

Oneofitsprimarygoalsistopredictviaa“centeringalgorithm”howtheconceptof focuscanbeusedtoidentifythereferentsofanaphoricexpressionssuchaspronouns, ellipsis,anddefinitenounphrases(Groszetal.1995:5).Asitsnamesuggests,Centering

Theoryconcentratesonidentifyingthefocusorcenterofattention—thatis,themost salientfromamongthesetofreferentsofreferringformsrepresentedataparticularpoint inthediscourse.Fromthisset,thetheorypredictsthehighestrankingreferentprimarily onthebasisofsyntacticrole,butalsoconsideringpropertiessuchaspronominalization andsurfaceposition(wordorder).AsMulkern2003pointsout,thisconceptualization reducessaliencetothelevelofthelocalutteranceanddoesnotallowameansof consideringfactorssuchasfrequencyofmentionoutsideofthelocalutterance,overall importanceofthetopictothediscourseasawhole,participantsintheextralinguistic context,orhigherlevelentitiesevokedbythediscourse(suchaspropositions)(37).

ConsideringthatthecenteroffocusistheemphasisofCenteringTheory,itisnot surprisingthatoneofitslargestcontributionshasbeeninthedescriptionoftheuseof reducedformsofreferringexpressionssuchasnullargumentsandpronouns.Of particularrelevancetothepresentstudyisthedescriptionofTurkishbyTuran

(1998:140),whichdescribesoneaspectofthedistributionofnullargumentsaccordingto thefollowingRuleofCentering:“TheCb(U n)isthehighestrankedentityoftheC f(U n

1);ifanyentityisrealizedwithanullsubject,itistheC b.”Whilesucharuleonly

16 addressesaspecificsubsetofnullarguments,Turan’sstudyprovidesevidencefor specificfactorswhichcontributetothesalienceofreferentsinTurkish(cf.section2.3).

2.2. The Givenness Hierarchy model

WhileArieldescribesstatisticalcorrelationbetweencategoriesofformsand levelsofaccessibilityofthereferent,Gundeletal.assumethattheformthataspeaker choosesfromamongmultiplepossiblecategoriessignalsthespeaker’sassumptionsabout thereferent’smemoryorattentionstatusinthehearer’smind—thatis,its‘cognitive status’(1993:2745).AsignificantdifferencebetweenAriel’smodelandtheGHmodel isthattheformeraddressesdegreesofaccessibilitywhilethelatterprimarilyaddresses mannerofaccessibility(Gundel,NtelitheosandKowalsky2007). 8Thismodelproposes sixcognitivestatuseswhichareassumedtobepartoftheconventionalmeaningof lexicalitemslikepronounsanddeterminers:infocus,activated,familiar,uniquely identifiable,referential,andtypeidentifiable.Foreachofthesestatuses,thetheory proposesspecificminimalcriteriabywhichanentitymaybeassociatedwiththatstatus

(seesection2.3).

UnlikeinAriel’swork,intheGivennessHierarchymodel,thesetofcognitive statusesisimplicationallyrelatedandhierarchicallyorderedfrommostrestricted(in

8Oneshouldnote,however,that,whilethedifferencebetweenthecognitivestatusoffamiliaranduniquely identifiableprimarilyliesinhowoneaccessesthereferent,thedifferencebetweeninfocusandactivatedis, indeed,oneofbothmanneranddegreeofaccessibility.

17 focus)toleastrestricted(typeidentifiable),asshowninFigure1,alongwiththe correspondingreferringexpressionsinEnglish(1993:2745). 9

Status InFocus> Activated> Familiar> Uniquely> Referential> Type> Identifiable Identifiable Form it, this,that, that N the N indefinite aN unstressed stressed this N (s)he (S)HE, this N

Figure1: TheGivennessHierarchy

Correlatingatypeofreferringexpressionwithacognitivestatusinthehierarchymeans thatitsreferentsmustalwayshaveatleastthatstatus.Forexample,inEnglish,the pronoun it isanalyzedassignalling‘infocus’andmaynotbeusedtorepresentareferent whichisatmostactivated.Theunidirectionalentailmentrelationshipbetweenthe statusesmeansthatanyformthatisinfocusisalsoactivated,familiar(andsoforth).In otherwords,thestatusesarenotmutuallyexclusive.

Theentailmentofstatusesinthehierarchyalsopredicts,asGundel2003explains,

“aonetomanymappingbetweenstatusesandformsinlanguageuse,sinceformsare underspecifiedforhigherstatuses,ratherthanexcludingthem”(134).Thismeansthata formthatsignalsonestatusmaybeusedtorepresentreferentsofahigherstatus.For example,inEnglish,thepronoun that canrefertoanentitythatisatmostactivatedoran

9Notethatpronounsreferringtospeechactparticipantsarenotgenerallyincludedindiscussionsoftheory aboutthedistributionoftypesofreferringformsbecauseofthespecialcontextualfeaturethatthereferents arealwaysimplicitinthespeechactcontext.

18 entitythatisinfocus.Italsofollowsthatareferentthatisatmostactivatedcanbe representedbyanyoneofthefollowingforms: this , that ,astressedpronoun, this N, that

N, the N,(indefinite) this Nor aN(butnot it ).Oneoftheimplicationsofthisclaimis

thattheuseofaparticularformisconstrainedtoreferentswhichhaveaparticularstatus, butwithintheseconstraintstheuseofoneformoveranotherisnotpredictableby

cognitivestatusalone.Thechoiceofoneformoveranotherisassumedtobegoverned bypragmaticprinciplessuchasGrice’sMaximofQuantityandSperberandWilson’s

Relevanceprinciple(Grice1975,SperberandWilson1986/1995,2004).Factorswhich

mayaffectthischoiceincludethenumberofcompetingreferents,higherleveldiscourse

structure,therelativesalienceofparticipants,andthedesiredcontextualeffects.

Examples(7)and(8)illustratehowthesecontextualfactorsmayinfluencethechoiceof

referringexpression.

(7) a. JohnhasaveryenergeticGoldenRetrievernamedSam. b. EveryafternoonheandSamgoforawalk. c. Usuallyafteranhour,Johnisreadytocomehome,butthedogisn’t.

Inthisexample,accordingtotheGHmodel,thereferentsofboth thedog and John arein

focusatthebeginningofutterance(c);however,neitherentityisreferredtowitha pronoun.Imaginethesameutterancewithpronouns,asshownin(c2).AsRelevance

Theorypredicts,speakersdonotuseanutteranceofthisformbecausetheambiguityof thepronounsincreasestheprocessingeffortwithoutadequatecognitiveeffects(Sperber andWilson2004:38).

c2. Usuallyafteranhour,heisreadytocomehome,butheisn’t.

19 Ariel1990describesaninterestingtypeofcontextualeffectcreatedbytheuseof alowerstatusform—inthiscase,adefiniteNP—inreferencetoanentitywithhigh activationstatus(infocusstatus,accordingtotheGHmodel).Herexampleis reproducedhereasexample(8).

(8) AfterI’dprepared oureveningmeal ,Icooked oureveningmeal andwashedup after oureveningmeal .Ididn’teatmuchof oureveningmeal becauseIwasthen sickofthesightofit. (D.Harpwood, TheDiaryofaHappyHousewife ,ascitedinAriel1990:200).

Inthiscase,thespeaker’suseofthedefiniteNPisnotrelatedtotheneedtoavoid ambiguity,but,asArieldescribesit,relatedto“thewishtoinvokeintheaddresseethe samefeelingofrepugnancewith‘theeveningmeal’”(200).

AnotheruniqueclaimoftheGHmodelisthattheformstatuscorrelation representedinthehierarchyispartoftheconventionallexicalmeaningofaform,and thattheuseoftheseformscreatesconversationalimplicatures—morespecifically,scalar implicatures—accordingtoGrice’sQuantityMaxim(“Makeyourcontributionas informativeasrequired/Donotmakeyourcontributionmoreinformativethanis required”(1975:162)).Accordingtothisprinciple,theuseofaformwhichsignalsthata referentisactivatedcouldcreatetheimplicaturethatthereferentisnotinfocusinthe samewaythat some implicates notall(Gundeletal.1993:294299).AsGundel

(2003:136)explainsthismeansthat,forexampleinEnglish,thedemonstrativepronoun that is“typicallyusedonlywhenconditionsforusingthemorerestrictive(hencemore informative)pronoun, it ,arenotmet”.ThispointisillustratedinGundeletal.1993and

20 Gundel2003throughthecontrastinthefollowingexamples,representedhereas(9)and

(10).AccordingtoGundeletal.,atthetimethepronounisuttered,thekitchenisin focus,buttheclosetisatmostactivated;hencethehearerassociates itinexample(9)

withthekitchen,but thatinexample(10)withthecloset.

(9) Anyway,goingbackfromthekitchenthenisalittlehallwayleadingtoawindow. Acrossfromthekitchenisabigwalkthroughclosetandnextto it …

(10) Anyway,goingbackfromthekitchenthenisalittlehallwayleadingtoawindow. Acrossfromthekitchenisabigwalkthroughclosetandnextto that … (Gundeletal.1993:298,ex.56;Gundel2003:136,ex.2425,italicsmine)

TosummarizetheeffectsoftheimplicationalnatureoftheGivennessHierarchy,

theformsinthehierarchysignalthatareferentisatleastaparticularstatus,and,atthe

sametime,oftenimplicatethatthereferentisatmostthatstatus(i.e.notahigherstatus).

Thesescalarimplicaturescreatearankingeffectwhichcanplayaroleindisambiguating

referents,asintheexamplesabove.AccordingtoGundeletal.1993andGundeland

Mulkern1998:26,theimplicaturecreatedbytheuseofademonstrativealsoexplains

whytheseformsareassociatedwithfocusshift.Inadditiontothedisambiguating

function,therankingeffectcreatedbyscalarimplicatureshasotherdiscourseeffects.

Forexample,Mulkern2003notesthatiftwoentitieshavethesamecognitivestatusat

somepointinthediscourse,thechoiceofreferringformcanindicatethatthespeakeris

imposingahigherlevelofsalienceforoneentityinrelationtoanother(25).As

explainedbyMulkern,this“imposedsalience”contrastswiththe“inherentsalienceof

theentitiesrepresentedbytheiractualcognitivestatus.

21 Aremainingquestionrelatedtopragmaticprinciplesguidingtheuseofformsto

signalcognitivestatusiswhetherornotcertainusescanrepresent“flouting”—or blatantlynotfollowingtheircognitivestatuscorrelationsinasenseparalleltothe

floutingofmaximsdescribedbyGrice(1975).Whilethisideaisnotspecifically

expressedintheliteraturerelatedtotheGHmodel,itcouldexplaintheexceptionalcase

oftheuseofdemonstrativepronounsincataphoricreferences.Ademonstrativepronoun

shouldnotnormallybeusedinreferencetoanentitywhichisnotevenactivated,butis

occasionallyusedinsuchawayinEnglish,forexample,asin(11).

(11) Waituntilyouhearthis:BilljustproposedtoMary!

Sucha“misuse”oftheactivatedformmaycreateextraprocessingeffort,asthehearer

mustanticipatethecontinuationoftheutteranceinordertoresolvethereference,butit

seemstoproduceanaddedeffectofemphasisviasuchanticipation.

2.3. Coding procedure for the GH model

TheclaimsoftheGivennessHierarchyarebasedoncrosslinguisticstudiesof

referringformsinnaturaldiscourseaswellasconstructedexamplestestedwithspeakers.

Inbothcases,theanalysisisbasedonaprocessofcodingthereferentofareferring

expressioninordertodeterminethecognitivestatusofthatreferent.Inorderto

determinethisstatus,theGivennessHierarchymodelusesacodingprotocolwhich

describessufficientconditionsforassigningaparticularcognitivestatus(Gundeletal.

1993,Gundel2004).Thecriteriainthecodingprotocolrefertofeaturessuchasthe

following:thesyntacticandlinearpositioninwhichareferentwaspreviously

22 mentioned,whetherornotareferentisahigherleveltopicinthediscourse,whethera referentisaneventorstateintroduceddirectlybythecontentofaclause,oraspeechact orhigherorderentityintroducedmoreindirectly,whetherornottheintroductionofan entityisaccompaniedbygestureoreyegaze,andwhetherauniquerepresentationofan entitycanbecreatedviathelinguisticformbywhichitisintroduced.

Themostrelevantcognitivestatusesinastudyofpronominalsare‘infocus’and

‘activated’,asitishighlyunlikelyforareducedformofreferringexpressiontobeused withareferentwhosestatusislowerthanactivated.AccordingtoGundeletal.,a referentisinfocusifitis“atthecenter”ofanindividual’sattention(2003:284).Specific criteriaforcodingthisstatusarelistedinFigure2.

AreferentisINFOCUSifitmeetsatleastoneofthefollowingcriteria: 1. Itismentionedinsubjectpositionintheimmediatelyprecedingsentence/clause. 2. Itismentionedearlierinthesamesentence. 3. Itismentionedinsyntacticfocuspositionoftheimmediatelyprecedingclause(postcopular positionofacleftorexistentialsentenceinEnglish). 4. Itisahigherleveltopicthatispartoftheinterpretationoftheprecedingclause(whetheritis overtlymentionedthereornot). 5. Itismentionedinthetwoimmediatelyprecedingclauses. 6. Itistheeventdenotedbytheimmediatelyprecedingsentence.

Figure2: Codingprotocolforinfocusstatus(Gundel2004)

Thestatus‘activated’describesentitieswhicharerepresentedinworking

memory,asopposedtolongtermmemory(Gundeletal.2003:284).Whileanyofthe

criteriamentionedinFigure2aresufficientforcodingareferentasactivated(sincethe

status‘infocus’entailsthestatus‘activated’),Figure3showsadditionalsufficient

criteriaforactivatedstatusaccordingtoGundel2004.

23 AreferentisACTIVATEDifitmeetsatleastoneofthefollowingcriteria: 1. Itismentionedinoneoftheimmediatelyprecedingtwosentences. 2. Itissomethingintheimmediatespatiotemporalcontextthatisactivatedbymeansofa simultaneousgestureoreyegaze. 3.Itisaproposition,fact,orspeechactassociatedwiththeeventuality(eventorstate)denoted bytheimmediatelyprecedingsentence(s).

Figure3: Codingprotocolforactivatedstatus(Gundel2004)

2.3.1. Somecodingchoicesinthisstudy Whilethecriteriaofthecodingprotocolhavebeentestedcrosslinguistically,the theoreticalmodeldoesnotexcludethepossibilityofcertainlanguagespecific adjustmentstothecriteriawhichareincharacterwiththeirpurpose.Inthisstudy,there areatleasttwoareasworthnotingwhereIhaveinterpretedthecriteriainwayswhich are,perhaps,languagespecific.

First,thecriteriawhichreferto‘sentence’and‘clause’necessitatealanguage specificdefinitionoftheterms.Intermsofsurfacesyntacticfeatures,Idefine‘sentence’ asanyclausewithaverbwhichincludesbothtense/aspectandnumbermarkers

(regardlessofpunctuation),whileIdefine‘clause’asacosubordinateorsubordinate structurewithanonfiniteverb(onewhichlackstense/aspectornumberfeatures).Based ondiscussionwithJeanetteGundelaboutcurrentrevisionstothecodingprotocolunder consideration,Iinterpretcriterion5asapplyingtofiniteclauses(hence‘sentences’). 10

10 Currentdiscussionoftherevisionsofthecodingprotocolsuggeststhatrevisingcriterion5toreplace ‘clauses’with‘sentences’mayactuallybemoreaccuratecrosslinguistically.Toillustratethedifference, considertheexamplein(i).

24 Secondly,Iinterpretthecategoryof‘subject’asincludingnonsubjectarguments whicharetheexperiencersofpsychologicalverbs.Thisdecisionisbasedonevidence fromTurkishinTuran(1998:153)thattheexperiencerobject,particularlyinthecaseofa psychologicalverb,isofasimilarlevelofsaliencetogrammaticalsubjects. 11 Itshould benotedthatcurrentrevisionsofthecodingprotocolbyGundelreplacespecificmention asagrammaticalsubjectwiththemoregeneralcriterion,mentioninapositionof syntacticprominence.Anumberofotherquestionsconcerningtheapplicationofthe codingwhichariseintheanalysis,suchasthecodingofpluralandcompositeentitiesor thetreatmentofcontextualfeaturessuchaspicturesandvideorepresentationofreferents, arediscussedinChapter4.

(i)Italkedtomymotherearlythismorning.AfterIwenttowork,Idecidedtocallheragain. But she wasn’tathome. Inthtisexample,ifcriterion5appliestoalltypesofclauses—includingsubordinateandnonfiniteclauses, thenthereferentofthepronoun she (bold)inthelastsentencewouldnotbeautomaticallycodedasin focus,asthereferentismentionedinthepreviousmainclausebytheform her ,butnotinthesubordinate clausewhichprecedesit(thatis, afterIwenttowork ).Ifcriterion5isformulatedtoapplytosentences, thenthereferentof she inthelastsentencewouldbecodedasinfocusbecauseitismentionedbytheform her intheprevioussentenceandbytheexpression mymother inthesentencebeforethat.Theapplication ofcriterion1alsomeritssomeelaborationgiventhediscussionoftheclausesentencedistinction.Forthe sakeofclarification,considerthefollowingexample: 1. Bob drovethecarbecause Anne asked him to. 2. She wanted him tohavesomepracticedrivingonthehighway. Whencodingthereferentof she insentence2,notethatAnneismentionedasthesubjectoftheprevious clause,butnotasthesubjectofthemainclauseoftheprevioussentence.Thecriterionisintendedtoapply inawaythatincludesthesecasesasinfocus.Moreover,thereferentof him inthesecondsentenceisBob. Bobismentionedinthepreviousclause,butnotasthesubject;however, Bob isthesubjectoftheprevious sentence.Thecriterionshouldbeappliedinawaysuchthatthiscaseisalsoinfocus. 11AnexampleofanexperiencerobjectinKumykisillustratedintheexpressionpaĭkhammarnyesinegele, which translatesliterally,‘itcametotheprophet’smind(that…)’.

25 AsanexampleofhowthecriteriaofthecodingprotocolareappliedinKumykin ordertodeterminethecognitivestatusofthereferentofareferringexpression,consider thetextexcerptin(12),whichconsistsofthefirstthreesentences(ac)ofanarrativetext.

Inthisillustration,thereferringexpressionsselectedforcodingareunderlined,while othermentionsofthesamereferents(whicharerelevanttotheapplicationofthecoding criteria)aremarkedinboldtype.Clauseboundariesaremarkedwithbrackets.

(12) a.[Birpaĭkhammarybir k"yzy , biresheki, bir one prophet. GEN one girl.3. POSS onedonkey.3. POSS one mishiki,bir de iti bolg"an.] cat.3. POSS oneEMPH dog.3. POSS be.3 S.PST ‘Aprophethadadaughter,adonkey,acatandadog.’ b. [K"yzny erge berme iaraĭg"an vak"tisi girl. ACC husband. DAT give. INF possible. PST .PRT time.3. POSS gelip] [bug"ar gelechi gele.] come. GER 3.1. DAT suitor come.3 S.PR ‘Whenitcomestimeforthedaughtertobegiveninmarriage,asuitorcomes toher.’ c. [K"yznyatasy darazi bolup,] girl. GEN father.3. POSS andagreed be. GER [Ø k"yzyn berezhek bola.] girl.3. POSS .ACC give. FUT be.3 S.PR ‘Thegirl'sfatheragreeing,(he)willgivethegirl.’

Thereferentoftheexpression k"yzny ‘hisdaughter’inthefirstclauseof(b)isthe daughteroftheprophet.Thesamereferentismentionedintheprevioussentence/clause

26 in(a),butnotastheuniquegrammaticalsubject;hencenocriterionforinfocusis satisfied. 12Thereferentofthepronoun bug”ar in(b)isalsothedaughter.Inthiscase,

thereferentisevaluatedasinfocusbycriterion2,“mentionedearlierinthesame

sentence.”Criterion5,“mentionedinthetwoimmediatelyprecedingclauses”would

alsoapplytothiscase.In(c), k"yznyatasy “girl’sfather” referstotheprophet,whois

mentionedin(a),butnotineitherclauseoftheprevioussentencein(b);thusthereferent

failstosatisfyacriterionforinfocus.Theprophetisalsothereferentforthenull

argumentin(c).Sincetheprophetismentionedearlierinthesamesentence,thisreferent

isinfocusbyCriterion2.Criterion1wouldalsoapply,sincethereferentismentionedin

subjectpositioninthepreviousclause.

2.3.2. Codingclausallyintroducedandhigherorderentities Thetypesofreferentsforanycategoryofreferringexpressionmayincludenot

onlyentitiessuchaspeople,places,orthingsintroducedbynominals,butalsoentities

introducedbyclauses,suchasevents,facts,andpropositions.Fortheexamplein(13), possiblereferentsintroducedintheinitialsentenceincludeAnsar,hismother,theapple,

theeventofAnsargivinganappletohismother,orthefactthatAnsargaveanappleto

hismother.Sentences(a)–(e)illustratepossiblecontinuationsofthetextwitheachof

thesereferents.

12 Thereferentof k”yzny‘hisdaughter’in(b)ismentionedpreviouslyaspartofthecomplexsubject, bir paĭkhammarybirk"yzy,biresheki,birmishiki,birdeiti‘aprophet’sonedaughter,onedonkey,onecat, andonedog’.Thoughthecomplexentitysetwouldbebroughtintofocusviamentioninsubjectposition, anindividualentitywouldnotnecessarilybebroughtintofocusbythementionofthesetinsubject position;thusthereferentof k”yzny iscodedas‘activated’.

27 (13) Ansargavehismotheranapple. a. Heisaniceboy. b. Shewaspleased. c. Itwasnotripeenough. d. Ithappenedthismorning. e. Thatsurprisedeveryone. In(a),(b),and(c),thepersonalpronounsrefertoentitiesintroducedbythenominalsin

(13), Ansar , hismother ,and anapple ,respectively.In(d)and(e),ontheotherhand,the referentsof it and that areentitiesintroducedbythewholeclause.Thereferentof it isan event,whilethereferentof that isafact.

Theidentificationandcodingofentitiesintroducedbyclauses(andverbphrases) hasbeenestablishedinGundel’scodingprotocolbasedoncorpusevidencefromEnglish andNorwegian(Gundel,BorthenandFretheim1999,Hegarty,Gundel,andBorthen

2001,Gundel,HegartyandBorthen2003,Hedberg,GundelandZacharski2007)and supportingclaimsabouttherankingofabstractentitiesindiscourse(Asher1993,Cornish

1999). 13 Themostcompletedocumentationofthedistributionofreferringformsin referencetoclausallyintroducedentitiesinEnglishappearsinGundeletal.2003.Based onpreviousevidencethatthepronoun it requiresitsreferenttobeinfocuswhilethe pronoun that onlyrequiresactivation(Gundeletal.1993),theauthorsproposethat clausallyintroducedentitieswhichmaysubsequentlybereferredtobythepronoun it are infocus,whilethosethatcannotareatmostactivated.Theyarguethateventsare

13 Cornish(1999:4748),baseshisdistinctiononLyons’(1977)3tieredclassificationofentityorderwhich issimilartothedistinctionsmadebyAsher.

28 immediatelybroughtintofocusbyanutterance,sinceaclausedirectlydenotestheevent itdescribes,andtheeventcanthusbebroughtintofocussimplybyvirtueofprocessing theclause.Factsandpropositionsassociatedwithaclause,ontheotherhand,are typicallyatmostactivated(cf.(13)).Inmakingthisclaim,theyalsorefertotheworkof

Asher(1993)whosuggestsarankingofabstractentitiesintermsof“worldimmanence”, witheventshavinggreaterworldimmanenceduetotheirpropertiesofindividuationand spatiotemperalfeatures. 14 SubsequentworkofGundeletal.(2005,2007)describesa

majordifferencebetweenhigherorderentitiesintermsofadistinctionbetweenadirect

versusindirectrelationshipbetweenareferringformanditsantecedenttrigger. 15

AccordingtoGundeletal.,events,activities,orstatesaredirectlyintroducedbyclauses orverbphrases,whilefacts,situations,propositionstypicallymustbeinferred(orinthe caseofspeechactsareactivatedbutrarelybroughtintofocus)and,therefore,involve furtherprocessing(2005:356,2007:31). 16

Asignificantconcerninresearchmethodologyistheproperidentificationof

higherorderentitytypes.Hegarty(2007),followingVendler(1967)proposesthatthese

typescanusuallybeidentifiedbasedontheselectionalpropertiesofverbs(171173).

Forexample,thecomplementsofverbssuchas believe and think areusually

14 Fraurud1996alsosupportstheroleofindividuationinentitycategories,thoughthisisnotspecifically linkedtodistinctionsbetweenclausallyintroducedentities. 15 Notethattheconceptof‘indirectanaphor’wasfirstdiscussedbyErküandGundel1987butthe applicationtoentitiesintroducedbyclausesisdiscussedinlaterworks. 16 Speechactsperformedbyvirtueofanutterancearealwaysatmostactivated,sincethefocusofattention willbeonthecontentofthespeechact,notthefactthatithasbeenperformed(cf.Gundel,Hegarty,and Borthen2003formoredetail).Cornish(1999)mentionsthatDik(1997),likeGundeletal.considers speechactstobeevenlessaccessiblethanfacts,situationsandpropositions.

29 propositions,accordingtoHegarty,andfactsarecomplementsofverbslike notice and pointout inEnglish.Followingsimilarlogic,aspeechact,wouldberecognizableby virtueofthefactthatthereferringformisthecomplementofaspeakingverb,asin John said that .

Eventsandstatesareconsideredtohavespatiotemporalfeaturesandoftenoccur withverbsofperception(e.g. see hear , feel ),orwithverbsmeaning“happen,”“repeat” or“do”(Asher1993,Gundeletal.2003,Hegarty2007).Hegartyalsoclarifiesthe distinctionbetweenaneventandasituation.Whileaneventhasspecifictemporal boundaries,asituationisacompositeofaneventanditsconsequencesand,thus,hasan initialtemporalpointwhichcoincideswiththatoftheevent,butanundeterminedend point(2007:171172).Referencestosituationsareoftenassociatedwithverbsor adjectiveswhichexpressemotion,suchasintheexpressions, thatembarrassedme .

Activitiesalsofallwithinthecategoryofentitiesnotintroducedbynominals,but theyareconsideredwithintheGivennessHierarchyliteraturetobeintroduceddirectlyby averbphraseratherthanafullclause(Hedbergetal.2007). 17 Likeevents,activities havespatiotemporalfeatures,buttheydonotincludereferencetotheagent/experiencer.

Inreferencetotheexamplein(13),asentencecontinuationwhichincludesapronominal referencetoanactivitycouldbe, Didyouseehimdo it ?—where it referstotheactivity

of(Ansar)givinganappletohismother.Oneissuethatariseswithinthisstudyis

referencetoanactivitytyperatherthanatokenactivitywithspecificspatiotemporal

17 Notethatthisdescriptionofactivitiesversuseventsdiffersfromliteratureonthesemanticclassification ofevents,whichconsidersactivitiestobeoneoffoursubcategoriesofevents(Vendler1967,interalia).

30 features.Aswithreferencetoindividualsversusentitytypes,referencetospecific activitiesactivates,butdoesnotnecessarilybringintofocustheactivitytype.

Subsequentreferencetothetypecanthereforebemadeonlywithaformthatdoesnot requireinfocusstatus.Forexample,intheutterance thatisnotallowed inresponseto

theexamplein(13), that refersnottoaspecificinstanceofgivinganapplebuttoan abstractedtypeofactivitythatdoesnotinvolveaspecificagent,recipient,time,orplace.

Thefactthat itisnotallowed soundsoddinthiscontextindicatesthatthetypeisnotin focus;inotherwords, it ,whichisrestrictedtoentitiesinfocus,cannotbeusedwithan activitytypethatisatmostactivated.

Intheanalysisofclausallyintroducedentitiesandotherentitiesnotintroducedby nominals,onefactorrelevanttothediscussionisthedegreeofnominalizationofthe clause/verb.Theverbtonounscaleisacontinuum,whichincludesvariousdegreesof nominalizationinKumyk,suchasvarioustypesofnonfinitesuffixes(participle/verbal noun),nominalizingsuffixes,andcasesuffixes.Inthisstudyentitiesintroducedbya nonfiniteclausewithsomedegreeofnominalizationareconsideredtobemoreeasily accessiblethanthoseintroducedbyafiniteclause,eventhoughthisisnotspecifiedinthe codingprotocol.

2.4. The GH model and other pragmatic factors in referential choice

BoththetheoreticalframeworkandthecodingproceduresoftheGHmodel provideanexcellentmeansofexplainingthedistributionandinterpretationofreferring expressionsindiscourse.Followingthisframework,thefocusofthedissertationisto describethedegreetowhichchoicesbetweenreducedformsofreferringexpressionscan

31 bedescribedintermsofthecontrastbetweentwolevelsofcognitivestatus:1)infocus and2)activated.Iassumethatthecontrastsrepresentedbytheassociationbetweenform andcognitivestatusarebothinherenttoreferentialchoiceandalsousedtoconstrainthe setofreferentsforthehearer.

BeyondtheGHframework,giventhefactthattherearesixreducedformsin

Kumyk,itislikelythatthedifferencesinformrepresentfunctionaldifferencesin additiontothetwowaydistinctioninthecognitivestatusofreferents.Basedonthe1993 research,itisnotunusualtohavemorethanoneformsignalingaparticularcognitive status,evenwhentheseformsaregrammaticallyinterchangeableintermsofgenderor animacy.AllfivelanguagesresearchedinGundeletal.1993havetwoormore interchangeableformsrepresentingthestatus,‘activated’.Threeofthefourlanguages whichusenullpronounstorepresententitiesinfocusalsohaveanovertpronounwhich signals‘infocus’.Asecondaryinterestofthisdissertationistoconsiderdistinctionsin theuseofproformswhosereferentsrequirethesamecognitivestatus.Thissecondary taskcouldbeapproachedfromtwoperspectives:1)consideringfurtherdistinctionsin theupperendoftheGHhierarchy—thatis,statuseswhichliebetweenthedistinction betweenactivatedandinfocus,and2)considering“referencespecific”functionsofthe multiplereferringformswhichrepresentthesamelevelofcognitivestatus. 18 Thefirst

18 AccordingtoBrownSchmidtetal.2005,the“referencespecific”frameworkimpliesthat“themapping betweenreferentialformsandreferentsismediatedbymultiplefactors,withdifferentreferentialforms beingmoresensitivetosomefactorsthanothers.”Theyfurtherstatethatthistypeofframeworkassumes thatatheoryofreferencemustspecifythesetofconstraintsandtheirrelativeweightsforeachreferring form.

32 possibilityisnotincompatiblewiththeGHmodel,thoughitwouldrequirerevisionsin thestructureofthehierarchy;however,Ifocusonthelatterperspectivewhichconsiders somespecializedfunctionsofsomereferringforms.Somerecentworksviewevidence forreferencespecificfunctionsasachallengetowhattheyviewasthe“unidimensional salienceapproach”oftheGHmodel(KaiserandTrueswell2004,Kaiser2005,Brown

Schmidt,ByronandTanenhaus2005).Myview,however,isthatsuchfunctionscanbe identifiedwithoutcontradictingclaimsrelatedtocognitivestatus.

Referentialdifferencessuggestedbytheliteraturethatarediscussedinthisstudy includedifferencesinreferencetosingularversuscompositeentities,assuggestedinthe workofBrownSchmidtetal.(2005),differencesinreferencetodirectlyintroduced versusindirectlyintroducedentities(Cornish1999,Gundeletal.2007),orforms specializedformarkingcontrast(Mulkern2003,KaiserandTrueswell2004).Another possibilitysuggestedbytheliteratureondemonstrativesandreflexivesisthatforms mightbespecializedformarkingspeakerpointofvieworempathy(Kuno1987,Cole,

Hermon,andHuang2001). 19

Finally,apossiblesourceofmultipleformsrepresentingthesamecognitivestatus

isthetensionbetweenelidingredundantformsandtheneedtorepresenthighlysalient

entitieswithformswhichbearphonologicalstress.Amarkedorstressbearingform

mightbenecessaryinanemphaticcontext,suchascontextwhichcontraststwoentities

inthephysicalcontext.Amarkedform(versusanullform)wouldalsobenecessaryina

19 Notethatothers,suchasKibrik1999andGrüningandKibrik2005includepointofviewasacriterion thatcontributestosalience,notasanindependentfactor.

33 contextinwhichthespeakerdesirestoimposesalienceorinfocuscognitivestatusfora referentwhichdoesnotinherentlyhavethatstatus(Mulkern2003).

2.5. Methodology in this study

FollowingthepatternofpreviouscrosslinguisticstudyusingtheGivenness

Hierarchymodel,thisanalysisisbasedonthedistributionofreferringformsinacorpus studysupplementedbyananalysisofconstructedexamplesthroughquestionnairesand grammaticalityjudgments.Inthiscase,thegreateremphasisisgiventocorpusstudy.

2.5.1. Descriptionofcorpus ThetextcorpusforthisstudyconsistsoftwelveKumyktexts,totaling

approximately100interlinearizedpages.Ofthesetexts,fouraretranscriptionsoforal

elicitations,andeightarewrittentexts.Intermsofpercentageofthetotalnumberof pages,27.3%ofthecorpusconsistsoforalmaterialand72.7%iswritten.Thewritten

textsincludeoneunpublishedessay,threenewspaperarticles,twoexcerptsfroma publishednovelandtwocompletetextsfrompublishedcollectionsofanecdotesand

essays.Oneoraltextisadescriptionofashortvideo,oneisanarrativeofawordless picturebook,andtheremainingtwoarefreeaccountsofculturalorhistoricalevents.

Thetextsinthecorpusincludeavarietyofgenres,includingnarrative,hortatory,and

expositorydiscourse.Mostofthetextsrepresentcontemporarystyle,whileonetext

datesfromtheearly20 th Century.Appendix2listsallthetextsinthecorpusbyname alongwithinformationaboutlength,mode,genre,andstyleforeachtext.Appendix3 displaysasampletextwithcodedforms.

34 Inthiscorpus,thereferentsof193tokensoftheovertpronouns bu , o, shu , sho , and o’ziu andthereferentsof229nullargumentsareanalyzedandcodedforcognitive status. 20 ThedistributionofeachoftheseformswithinthecorpusisshowninTable1.

Notethatfornullsandreflexives,theanalysisislimitedtothoseformswhichrepresent

thirdpersonentities.Theanalysisalsoexcludespronounswhichoccurwithinquoted

speechinnarrativetexts,duetothedifficultyofdeterminingthecognitivestatusofthe

referent. 21

Form Numberof Tokens Ø 229 o'ziu 36 bu 33 o 90 sho 33 shu 1

Table1: Distributitonofpronounsintextcorpus

2.6. Description of questionnaire and grammaticality judgments

Whilecorpusstudiesprovideanexcellentmeansofformingandtesting correlationsbetweenreferringformsandcognitivestatus,itisalsoimportanttotest

20 SeeChapter3,section3.2formyexplanationofhownullpronounsaredefinedinthisstudy. 21 Whiletherearecasesinwhichthereisnotenoughmaterialtocodethereferentofapronounfound withinaquotation,recentresearchbyWattersandGundel(2007),showsthatfortheEnglishpronoun it , thereisenoughmaterialinthemajorityofcasestocodethereferentsofpronounsfoundwithinquotations.

35 whetherornotgrammaticalityjudgmentsofconstructedexamplesindicatethesame constraintsontheuseofreferringforms.Thechallengeoftestingaproposed correlation/constraintliesintheimportanceofprovidingadequatetextualor extralinguisticcontextinwhichthecognitivestatusofareferentisevidentasrepresented bythecriteriaofthecodingprotocol. 22 Whilepreliminaryresearchteststhesubstitution offormswithinanaturallyoccurringtext,thissectionprimarilydescribestheuseofa questionnairetotesttheacceptabilityofpronounuseinconstructedsentencepairs.

Thequestionnairealsoprovidesanopportunitytoelicitdatarelatedtoanumber ofotherissuesofinteresttothisstudy:correlationsbetweenpronounchoiceand resolvingambiguityinreferenceresolution,rankingeffectsinsentenceswithmultiple pronouns,andcorrelationofpronounswithanimateversusinanimateentities.Some samplesarespecificallydesignedtoprovideevidenceforconstraintsinreferringto certaintypesofclausallyintroducedentities.Inthesectionsbelow,moreinformation willbeprovidedaboutthemethodologyofthequestionnaireinrelationtothesegoals.

Thequestionnaire,consistingof5parts,wasadministeredinwrittenformto5 respondentsinDagestan,Russia.23 Therespondentsrepresentbothmalesandfemales

22Iinitiallyexperimentedwithtestingpredictionsbysubstitutingcertainformswithinthetextsofmy naturaltextcorpus.Forexample,inagiventext,Iwouldsubstituteaformwhichisrestrictedtousewith entitiesinfocus(thatis, bu , o’ziu or Ø) foraforminthetextthatreferstoanentitywhichisatmost activatedbythecriteriaofthecodingprotocoloftheGHmodel.IwouldthenaskaKumykspeakertoread thetextandmarkanyformsthatappeartobeawkwardorunnatural.Insuchaprocedure,itisalso importanttoaskthespeakerstonotewhichentitytheythinktheformrefersto.ThescopeofthingsIcould testwiththismethod,however,wassomewhatlimited. 23 Thedifficultyofaccesstotheregionbyforeignersdidnotallowmetoadministerthequestionnairesin person,butthewrittenquestionnaireswereadministeredbyalocalproxywhohadbeentrainedto administerthem.

36 betweentheagesof19and64,fromseveraldifferentdialectalregions.Inorderto eliminatesomepotentialbiasrelatedtotheorderofpresentationofexamplesfor judgment(seeSchütze1996),Iusetwodifferentversionsofthequestionnairewhich primarilydifferinthepresentationorderofthegrammaticalityjudgments.(Thereare alsoacoupleofexampleswhichonlyappearinoneversionortheother,whichallowed metohaveagreatersamplingofmaterialwithoutmakingthequestionnairetoolong.)

VersionAofthequestionnaireisdisplayedasAppendix4.

Inthefirstpartofthequestionnaire,respondentsaregiven8sentences,eachof whichisfollowedbyademonstrativepronouninparentheses(nonullsorreflexives).

Foreachsentence,therespondentisaskedtowriteaonesentencecontinuationusinga particularpronoun.Anexampleofthistypeofexerciseisshownin(14).

(14) Given: Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. (bu) teacherstudent. DAT book give.3 ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ Response: Bushossahatokhumabashlady. 3.1immediatelyread. INF begin.3PST ‘(S)hestartedtoreadrightaway.’

ThemethodologyforthissectionisbasedonKaiser2005:272,whosegoalistotest predictionsthatthechoiceofreferringforminfluencestherespondenttoselecta particularreferentfromtheprecedingsentence.Whilemyprimarygoalinthissectionis toelicitanaturaluseofthegivenpronoun,thissectionalsointendstoprovideevidence thatcontrastsinformguideanaphoraresolutionandhelpresolveambiguity.Kaiserand

Trueswell2004suggestthatasentencecompletionmethodcanprovideevidencethatthe

37 choiceofproform(fromamongapresumedsetofforms)allowsthehearertoidentifythe referentofthatform(fromamongasetofmultiplereferents).Forexample,providedthe

Kumykcontextin(14),thesubjectisaskedtocontinuethetextusingaspecific pronominalform( bu ).Thechoiceofformisassumedtoinfluencethesubject’schoiceof

topicofthecontinuingtextfromamongpossiblereferentsintroducedintheoriginal

sentence.Inresponseto(14),mostrespondentsuse bu torefertothestudent,nottothe bookorahigherorderentitylikeaneventorfact.Thistypeofexerciseisparticularly

interestinginKumyk,wherethereisnomorphologicaldistinctionbetweenanimateand

inanimateentities,and,thus,cognitivestatuscouldbetheonlypropertythatwould

determinethepossibilityofreferencewithaparticularform.(Seealsothedescriptionof

section4ofthequestionnaireforfurtherdiscussionofhowthechoiceofpronounguides

thehearerinselectingfromamongmultiplepossiblereferents.)

Inpart2ofthequestionnaire,respondentsaregiven17pairsofsentencesand

askedtofillinablankinthesecondsentencewithoneofthepossiblepronominalforms

listed: bu,o,sho,shu,ando’ziu (first9pairs); bu,o,sho,shu,o’ziu +Ø(next8pairs).

Therespondentisalsogiventhechoicethat,ifnopronounispossible,(s)heshould indicatethetypeofformwhichcouldbeusedtofillintheblank.Unlikepart1,the blanksaresituatedinsententialcontextsbiasedforaparticularreferent,andtheresponse elicitsaformthatispossibleforthatreferent.Forexample,in(15),thecontextis constructedsothattherespondentwillchooseapronounthatrefersthereferentof kitap

‘book’inthefirstsentence.

38 (15) Given: Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. __Tangcholpanedi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST Tangcholpanbe.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.__wasTangcholpan.’ Response: Ol Tangcholpan edi. 3.2 Tangcholpanbe.3PST ‘ItwasTangcholpan.’

Theresponsesinthissectionindicatewhichformscanbeusedwithcertaintypesof

referents.Sincetherespondentchoosesonlyoneformtofillintheblank,theresponses

alsogiveanindicationofpreferencesforpronounuse.Thelackofuseofaparticular

formwithacertaintypeofreferent,however,doesnotprovidedirectevidenceof

constraintsontheuseofaparticularform,sincethatformmaybeapossiblebutnota preferredchoice.Formostofthesesentences,theacceptabilityofvariousformsistested

separatelyinthegrammaticalityjudgmentportion

Part3(13pairsofsentences)isstructuredinthesamewayaspart2,exceptthat,

inallbut3cases,thesecondsentenceofthepairhasmorethanoneblank,and

respondentsarealsoaskedtoidentifythereferentsofthepronounstheyusedtofillinthe blanks(4caseshave3blanks,and6cases,2blanks).Theidentificationofreferentsis

importantinexampleswherethereismorethanonepossiblereferent,asin(16),where

thepronouninthethirdblankcouldrefertoeitherPatimatortheteacher. 24

24 Ihadtoexcludedatafromonequestionnaireinthissectionbecausetherespondentdidnotidentifythe referentsofthepronouns.

39 (16) A. MuallimPatimatg”akitapberdi. teacherPatimat. DAT book give.3PST ‘TheteachergaveabooktoPatimat.’ B.…1……2…këp ushatdy. Song, …3…u’ĭge getdi. muchplease.3PST later home. DAT go.3PST ‘Itpleasedher.Latershewenthome.’ Response: 1= O(Patimat);2= shonu (book);3= Ø(Patimat) Section3notonlyprovidesinformationaboutwhichtypesofpronounscanbeusedfor

whichtypesofreferents,butalsoillustratestendenciesintheuseofpronounsin

sentenceswithmorethanonepronominalform.Someanalysisofa“rankingaffect”is possiblefromthisdata.

Thepurposeofsection4ofthequestionnaireistotesttherespondents’intuitions

aboutwhethertheuseofaparticularpronominalformsignalssomethingtothehearer

aboutthechoiceofreferentincontextswithmultiplepossiblereferents.Thissectionis

constructedspecificallytotestaprevioushypothesisthat oand sho ,thoughsignalingthe samecognitivestatus,varyinthedegreeorfrequencywithwhichtheygiverisetoa rankingeffectviascalarimplicatures. 25 Forthispart,pairsofsentencesweregiven,asin

(17)(A)and(B).

(17) A. Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’

25 Theform bu wasnotincludedinthissection,asitwashypothesizedthatthe“rankingeffectforthis pronouninrelationto oor sho isduetothefactthatitsignals‘infocus’whiletheothertwosignal ‘activated’.

40 B1.Og”ar bolg”an zat boldumu? 3.2.DAT happen.3PST .PRT thing happen.3PST ‘DidanythinghappentoPRO?’ Response: og”ar =___ B2.Shog”ar bolg”an zat boldumu? 3.3.DAT happen.3PST .PRT thing happen.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.DidanythinghappentoPRO?’ Response: shog”ar =___ Thesecondsentenceofthepairallowsforambiguityinpronominalreference.For example,in(17)(B 1),thepronoun og”ar inthesecondsentencecouldrefereitherto

Bolatortothehorse.Therespondentisaskedtoidentifythereferentoftheunderlined pronoun.Foreachsentencepairinthissection,therearetwoversions,illustratedhereas

(17)(B 1)and(B 2),whichdifferonlyinwhether oor sho isusedinthesecondsentence.

Thetwonearlyidenticalpairsarenotgivensidebyside,but,rather,interspersedwith

othersentencesofasimilartype.

Finally,section5providesgrammaticalityjudgmentsof4142sentencepairs

containingpronominalformsinthesecondsentence,asillustratedin(18).Each

respondentisaskedtojudgethesecondsentenceinthepairaccordingtothefollowing

scale:1=soundsgreat;2=soundsgood;3=possible,butpeopledon’tusuallysayitthat

way;4=noKumykpersonwouldsayitthatway. 26

26 Idecidedtoexcludeonesetofdatainwhichtherespondentonlyratedonesentenceas3andnoneas4. (fromBabayurt,notMakhachkala)

41 (18) Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. Sho Tangcholpan edi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST 3.3 Tangcholpanbe.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.ItwasTangcholpan.’ Response: 1234(circleone) Inordertoprovidemoretypesofsentences,Ivaryasubsetofsentencepairsfor2ofthe questionnaires,whichmeansthatforthatsubsetoftheexamples(16sentences),only2 respondentsprovidedata,whiletheotherexampleshavedatafrom4respondents.

Inthissection,negativejudgmentsforcertainformswhichrepresentreferents codedasactivatedareinterpretedasevidencethattheseformsarerestrictedtoreferents whichareinfocus.Likewise,positivejudgmentsofpronounsusedtorefertoentities consideredtobeatmostactivatedareinterpretedasevidencethattheformsarenot constrainedtoreferentsinfocus. 27 Oneofthecaveatsforthistypeofmethodologyis thattherestrictionsontheuseofdifferentformsofreferringexpressionmightrepresent

‘softconstraints’asopposedto‘hardconstraints’.Whilethecognitivestatusconstraints mightbestrongenoughtoguidetheproductionofreferringforms(andhenceexplainthe distributionofformsinacorpus),theymightnotbestrongenoughtoproducejudgments ofunacceptability.Thedatafromthissectionalsoprovideevidenceforotherpossible constraintsontheuseofpronominals,suchaswhethercertainformscanbeusedwith indirectlyconstructedreferentsorarerestrictedtoreferentswithparticularanimacy

27 Sincethecodingcriteriaaresufficientbutnotnecessarycriteria,somereferentsthatdonotmeetanyof thecriteriaforbeinginfocusmayactuallybeinfocus.Decisionsaboutwhichtypesofreferentsare consideredtobeatmostactivatedarediscussedfurtherinChapter5onthebasisofparameterssuggested bythequestionnaireresults.

42 features,orwhethertherearerestrictionswhichcorrelatewithpreviousmentionin subjectposition.

Tosummarizethepartsofthequestionnaire,Sections13provideevidencefor thesetofpronounswhicharepossibleincertaincontexts,butdonotprovideclear evidenceforwhichpronounsarenotpossible,astheabsenceofaparticularpronounin thesecontextsmaybeduetochanceratherthanduetorestrictions.Section3provides additionalevidenceabouttendenciesforthedistributionofformsandtheirreferentsin sentenceswithmultiplepronominals.Section4primarilyprovidesevidencefor differencesbetween oand sho inrelationtoscalarimplicatures.Finally,Section5is

intendedtoprovideevidenceonthecognitivestatusconstraintsandotherpossible

constraintsontheuseofpronouns.

43

Chapter 3: Kumyk Pronouns and Their Properties

3.0. General comments

Withintherangeofpossibleformsofreferringexpressions,thisstudyfocuseson reducedforms,orthosewhichhaveminimalsemanticcontent. 28 Intheliterature,these

formsareknownasindexicals,deicticforms,proformsoranaphors.Withinthisclass,

variousdistinctionshavebeenmadeintheliterature.HankamerandSag(1976)

distinguishbetweensurfaceanaphorsanddeepanaphors,theformerrepresentingreduced

formswhichrequireasyntacticantecedent,andthelatterrepresentingthosewitha pragmaticallycontrolledantecedent.Latersyntacticliteratureformalizesadistinction betweenpronominalsandanaphorsintermsofbindingconditions(Chomsky19801982,

1986,1995).Anaphorsrepresenttheclassofproformswhicharesyntacticallybound

withintheirlocaldomain(clause),whichmeanstheseformshaveaccommanding

antecedentwithinthatdomain.Pronounsrepresentformswhicharelocallyfree—or

formswhichcannothaveanantecedentinthelocaldomainbutcanhaveanantecedentin

thesentencebutoutsidethelocaldomainorinthediscoursecontext(viaprevious

mentionormutualknowledge).Thedistinctionbetweenanaphorandpronounasdefined byHankamerandSagandChomsky,amongothers,suggeststhattheantecedentforan

28 Inthisstudy,asisthecaseinmostcomparitivestudiesofreferringexpressions,thefocusisonthird personpronouns,asthereferentsoftheseformscanbecomparedtothereferentsofothercategoriesof referringforms,suchasnounphrases.Also,firstandsecondpersonpronouns,whichrefertospeechact participants,areoflimitedinterest,sincetheirreferentsarealwaysinfocusinthespeechcontext.

44 anaphorisdeterminedthroughasyntacticprocess,whiletheantecedentforapronounis determinedsemanticallyorpragmatically.

Theworkinaccessibilityorcognitivestatusneitherreliesonnorrefutesthe distinctionbetweenanaphorandpronoun.Inonerespect,itisarguable,asAriel1990 suggests,thatapparentsyntacticrestrictionsarebasedoninherentaccessibility.Inother words,sinceananaphor’santecedentisconstrainedtothelocaldomain,theantecedentis alwayshighlyaccessible.Inthesameway,withintheGivennessHierarchymodel,any formwhichfitsthesyntacticdefinitionofanaphorwouldnormallybeinfocus.Inspite ofthepotentialforcognitivemodelstoaccountforthedistributionofanaphors,the currentstudyfocusesondescribingthedistributionofformswhichcannotbedescribed solelyintermsofbindingconditionsandcontrol—thatis,pronominal(asopposedto anaphoric—inthebindingsense)usesofnullargumentsandreflexives,andovertnon reflexivepronouns.Sections3.2and3.3provideevidencethatsometypesofnull argumentsandreflexives,whichareanaphorsinmanyotherlanguagesarepronominals inKumykand,therefore,meritconsiderationinthisstudy.

Amongovertpronominalforms,severalcategoricaldistinctionsaremade

(primarilyinrelationtotheuseofdemonstratives),anditisimportanttoclarifytherole ofsuchdistinctionsinthisstudy.HallidayandHasan(1976:5776)makeadistinction betweenexophoricusesandendophoricuses.Exophoricuses,morecommonlycalled deicticuses,representreferencetoentitiesinthephysicalcontextofthespeechsituation,

45 whileendophoricusesincludeallotheruses. 29 ThoughtheGHframeworkapplies equallytobothendophoricandexophoricuses,mostofmydatacomesfromendophoric uses.Someworksfurthersubdivideendophoricusesintotwoormorecategoriesbased onwhethertheyrefertodiscourseparticipantsortohigherlevelentitiessuchas propositionsorfacts(Diessel1999:93).Thisstudy,however,doesnotmakeanyfurther distinctionsamongendophoricuses,applyingtheframeworktoalltypesofreferents, whetherindividuals,objects,events,propositions,orothertypesofentities.

FollowingtheGHframework,butincontrasttomuchoftheliteratureon demonstratives,differencesbetweenmultipledemonstrativeformsarenotdescribedin termsofdistancefeaturessuchasproximal,medial,ordistal,butintermsofdifferences incognitivestatus.ItisworthnotingthatrecentliteratureoutsidetheGHframework alsopreferstodescribedemonstrativesintermsoftheircommunicativeorcognitive functionratherthanintermsofdistancefeatures(Diessel2006:12).Theseapproaches proposethataprimaryfunctionofdemonstratives,whetherendophoricorexophoric,isto establishjointfocusofattention(ÖzyürekandKita2001(ascitedinEnfield2003),

KüntayandÖzyürek2002,Enfield2003,Levinson2004,Diessel2006).

ThischapterdescribessixthirdpersonpronominalsinKumyk,providingdetailsof form,generalaspectsofuse,andasummaryofhowtheseformsaredescribedinthe literatureonKumyk,aswellassomecommentsonTurkish,acloselyrelatedandmore widelyanalyzedTurkiclanguage.Thedescriptionoffourovertnonreflexiveformsalso

29 Thisstudydoesnotaddresstheissueofwhetheronetypeofusage—exophoricorendophoric—ismore primitive.

46 raisesthequestionofwhetheradistinctionshouldbemadebetweendemonstrativesand personalpronounsinKumyk,andthesectionsonreflexivesandnullargumentsprovide justificationforincludingtheinflectedreflexiveandasubsetofnullargumentsinKumyk amongpronominals.

3.1. Personal pronouns or demonstratives

TheKumyklanguagehasfourthirdpersonpronouns,occurringintheirroot formsas bu,o,shu and sho .30 Morphologicallyunmarkedforgenderoranimacy,these fourformsreferbothexophoricallyandendophoricallytovarioustypesofanimateand inanimateentities,includingbothhumanornonhumanindividuals,objects,abstract concepts,andclausallyintroducedentitiessuchasevents,propositions,andfacts.

InhisearlydescriptionofKumykgrammar,Dimitriev(1940)classifies o asa neuterpersonalpronoun,while bu,o,sho, and shu areclassifiedasdemonstrative pronouns. 31 Thereis,however,nosyntacticbasisformakingadistinctionbetween personalpronounsanddemonstrativesinKumyk,eitherinmorphologicalformorin syntacticdistribution.Moreover,sincemanywouldexpecttheterm‘demonstrative’to beassociatedwithdistancefeatures(whichisincongruentwiththeframeworkIam using),thisstudychoosestousethemoregeneralterm‘pronoun’inreferencetothese fournonreflexiveformsandtostudythemcomparativelyasoneclass.Itisuseful, nevertheless,tobeawareofhowlinguistshavedescribedthedifferencesinmeaning

30 Ol alsooccursasadialectalvariation of o. 31TherootornominativepronominalformsinKumykareidenticaltothedemonstrativedeterminers bu , o, shu and sho ,asintheexpression, shoadam ‘thatman’.

47 betweenthefourdemonstratives.Dimitriev(1940:79)and,likewise,Muratchaeva

(2001)describethedifferencesinmeaningintermsofdistanceand“knownness”as follows: bu representsnearandknown, shu representsnearandnotknown, orepresents

distant,and sho representsthemedialdistance(furtherthan shu ,nearerthan o). 32 The distinctionbetween shu and sho isofparticularinterest,astheform shu israrein discourse,andanumberofotherTurkiclanguagesexhibita3way,ratherthana4way demonstrativesystem.Turkish,forexample,encodesa3waydistinctionbetweenthe forms bu , u,and o.Lewis(1967)describes uasamedialdemonstrative,whileothers claimthatthesameformencodesareferentthattheaddresseeisnotattendingto

(ÖzyürekandKita2001,ascitedinEnfield2003:109).Whileitisimportanttobeaware ofhowTurkicpronounsaredescribedintheliterature,thisstudysuggests,rather,thatthe differenceinmeaningbetweenthefourformsinKumykcanbedescribedmore effectivelywithreferencetothecognitivestatusofthereferentstheyrepresent.

Anoverviewofthemorphologyofthirdpersonpronounsprovidesinformation aboutvariouscaseformsandthetypesofentitiestowhichtheyrefer.Thebasicforms bu,o,shu and sho representthenominativesingular.Asisthecasewithothernounsin

Kumyk,pluralpronounsareformedbytheadditionofthesuffix –lar tothebasicform, asillustratedinthefirstcolumnofTable2.33

32 NeitherDimitriev(1940)norMuratchaeva(2001)clearlystateswhether oor sho represententities whichareknownorunknowntothehearer;howeverIassumethatbothformsrepresententitieswhichare known. 33 Insomecases,thesingularformcanbeusedtorepresentapluralentity,aswiththeadverbial/adjective formssuchas sholaĭ(not sholarlaĭ).

48 Pronominalcaseformsotherthannominativereflectthecombinationofoneof thefollowingcasesuffixeswiththesingularorpluralform:genitive (nI),dative (gA), accusative (nI),locative(dA),ablative(dAn ),andpossessive(( sIi),asshownin(19).34

ThefulldeclensionofsingularandpluralpronounsisdisplayedinTable2.

(19) o + lar +nI = olany root + plural+accusative

NOMINATIVE GENITIVE DATIVE ACCUSATIVE LOCATIVE ABLATIVE POSSESSIVE

bu munu bug”ar munu munda mundan busu * shu shunu shug”ar shunu shunda shundan shusu SING . o onu og”ar onu onda ondan osu sho shonu shog”ar shonu shonda shondan bular bulany bulag”a bulany bularda bulardan shular shulany shulag”a shulany shularda shulardan PLURAL olar olany olag”a olany olarda olardan sholar sholany sholag”a sholany sholarda sholardan

Table2: Morphologicaldeclensionsofpronominalforms

(*Thepossessivecaseisonlydocumentedwiththeform bu inthisdata. Theothertwoformsarelistedby Dimitriev1940:79.)

34 Capitallettersinsuffixforms,suchas mA representvowelharmonyalternationsrepresentedinthe orthography.

49 Pronounsplusthelocative(dA)orablative(dAn)suffixesareusedinthesense

oftheEnglishlocationaldeictics here or there inreferringtoplaces,asillustratedin(20),

where onda referstothemarket.

(20) Murat bazarg”a gete. Onda Øalma aldy. Murat market. DAT go.3S.PR there apple buy.3S.PST ‘Muratwenttothemarket.(He)boughtapplesthere.’

Classchangingsuffixessuchas –laĭ(modifier)combinewithpronouns,asinthe adverbial bulaĭshownin(21).Semantically,theseformsareroughlyequivalenttothe forms thus or soor like that inEnglish,andtheyprimarilyrefertoentitiesintroducedby clausesorverbphrases.Interestinglyenough, laĭadverbialsmayalsobenominalized, asintheform olaĭlar ‘oneslikethat’in(22).

(21) Kuranda bulaĭ aĭtila: Koran. LOC 3S. MOD say. PASS .PR

“katingishige atasynyag”lusunukollari oman. DAT father.3POSS .GEN brother.3POSS .GEN hand. PL .3POSS tiĭsegunakhtugulbashg”a adamlartiĭsegunakh.” touch.3COND sin NEG other man. PL touch.3COND sin ‘IntheKoranitissaidlikethis:“Ifawoman’sfather’sorbrother’shandstouch her,itisnotasin;ifothermentouch(her),itissin.’

(22) Bibliotekag”abardym – cheber kitaplarda okhumaĭeken. library .DAT go.1 S.PST – literature book. PL EMPH read. INF COP

Klassda olaĭlar dag”ydabolg”an. lass. LOC 3.2. MOD .PL more be. PST .PRT ‘Iwenttothelibrary—toreadliterature.Intheclasstherewereotherslikethat.’ [Marian.045]

50 Pronominalformsinvariouscasesoccurwithanumberofpostpositions,also labeledsubordinatingconjunctionsorlogicalconnectives,asin osaialy ‘becauseof

3S’/’therefore’/‘accordingto’or shog”argere ,literally,‘seeingthat’,butconveyingthe meaning‘therefore’/‘becauseof’.Alistofconnectivesincorporatingpronounsis displayedinFigure4.

Nominativeform Pronouns(+Case)+Postposition bu munuuchun ‘for3S’ shu shug”argëre ‘accordingto3S’ shundansong ‘after3S’ shuunubirge ‘togetherwith3S’ o onuuchun ‘for3S’ osaialy ‘becauseof3S’ osebepli ‘accordingto3S’ og”argëre ‘accordingto3S’ sho sho(nu)saialy ‘becauseof3S’ shosebepli ‘accordingto3S’ shosebepden ‘becauseof3S’ shonuuchun ‘for3S’ shondansong ‘after3S’ shondantaba ‘throughthat’ shog”argëre ‘accordingto3S’ shog”ardekaramayly‘notconsisdering3S’

Figure4: Pronounsinlogicalconnectives

Thepronounswithintheseexpressionsmostoftenrefertoevents,facts,propositionsor

otherhigherorderentities,asillustratedin(23),where o saialy referstothefactthatthe

motherinlawwantstoinvitepeopletoafeast.

(23) Yashbolnitsadanchykg”an songbirzhumany ichindekaĭnana childhospital. ABL exit. PST .PRT afteroneweek. GEN inside.of mother.in.law 51 takhanaetip adamlany chakyrmabaĭram etme suĭe. takana make. GER man.3PL .ACC invite.INF feastdo.INF want.PR Osaialy bu chakyryvlar ĭiberip kyzny adamlaryn 3.2according.to3.1 invitation.PL send.GER girl. GEN man. PL .3 POSS .ACC o’ziunu adamlaryn chakyra. self–3POSS .GEN man.PL .3 POSS .ACC invite.PR ‘Afterthechildleavesthehospital,withinoneweek,themotherinlawwantsto holdafeast,makingtaxanaandinvitingpeople.Becauseofthis,shesends invitationsandinvitesdaughter(inlaw)’speople,(self’s)ownpeople.’ [Birth1.0089]

Inafewcases,itisdifficulttodetermineifderivationalformsof bu,o,shu and sho aretrulyreferentialpronouns.Onesetofexamplesinvolvestheform shonchaky

‘thatmuch’/’somuch’,inwhichthepronounrootcombineswiththemorpheme cha/

chaky ,whichcarriesthemeaningof‘amount’,asshownin(24).

(24) sho + chak”y = shonchak”y root + amount ‘thatmuch’/‘somuch’

Insomecases shonchak”yappearstobereferential,referringtoaspecificamount

mentionedinthediscoursecontext(25),whileinothercasesthesameformfunctionsas

anintensifier,asin(26).

(25) Muratny on rubil’bar. Mennike shonchak”yak”cha ëk”. Murat. GEN ten ruble exist.3 S my 3.3.muchmoney exist. NEG ‘Murathas10rubles.Idon’thavethatmuchmoney.’

(26) Nege_tiugiulde shonchakyuzak"zamanishlegen because EMPH 3.3.much distanttime work.PST .PRT

52 ishibizni natizhasyn gërmek work.1PL .POSS .GEN result.3POSS .ACC see.INF ullusiuiunchulluish. greatjoyful greatwork ‘Sincetheresultofourworkhadtakensuchalongtimetosee,itwasagreatjoy, abigdeal.’[NT.012.013]

Nonreferentialcasesinwhich shonchak”y servesasanintensifierarenotconsideredin

thisstudy.

3.2. Null pronouns

Withinbindingtheory(Chomsky19801982,1986,1995)adistinctionismade betweensyntacticallycontrollednullarguments(anaphors)andnullargumentswhichare

notsyntacticallycontrolled(pronominals);thisstudyfocusesonthelatter.Thestudyof pronominalnullargumentsismoreinterestingtocognitiveapproachesbecause,unlike

anaphors,whichareobligatory,pronominalnullsalternatewithotherformsofreferring

expressions.Muchattentionhasbeengiventoexplainingelidedsubjectsor‘prodrop’in

termsofsyntacticparameterssuchasrecoverability(seeHuang2000fora

comprehensiveoverview);however,itiswidelyacceptedthat,crosslinguistically,the

occurrenceofpronominalnullargumentsversusfullerformsintheirvariousargument positionsdependsonnonsyntacticparameters.Nonsyntacticparametersproposedin

theliteratureincludetopiccontinuity(Givón1983),centering(Groszetal.,1983,1995,

Walker,Joshi,andPrince1998),accessibility(Ariel1988,1990),andcognitivestatus

(Gundeletal.1993),withtheapproachofthisstudyfocusingonthelastofthese.Most

oftheliteraturewhichanalyzesnullargumentsbasedonnonsyntacticparameterssuchas

53 accessibilityorcognitivestatusappliesmorebroadlytoalltypesofnullarguments(not justsubjects)andisassumedbymanytoapplyequallytobothanaphorsandpronouns(in thebindingsense).Theorieswhichassociatetheformofreferringexpressionwith attentionstate,mentalaccessibility,orthecognitivestatusofthereferentproposethat nullargumentsareassociatedwiththehighestlevelofaccessibilityorthemostrestricted levelofcognitivestatus–thatis,locatedatthecenterofthehearer’sattention. 35 Several ofthesetheoriesalsoassociatenullformswiththeconceptsofcontinuity,parallelismor predictability(Prince1981,Givón1983),afactorwhichappearstoplayaroleinthe distributionofnullargumentsinKumyk,asdiscussedinChapter4).

InKumyk,nullpronounsoccurinavarietyofsyntacticcontexts:nullsubjectsof finiteclauses,asillustratedin(27),nullsubjectsofadverbialclausesasin(28)andnon subjectargumentssuchasobjectsorpossessors,asillustratedin(29).36

(27) Muratbazarg”a gete. Onda Øalma aldy. Murat market. DAT go.3S.PR there apple buy.3S.PST ‘Muratwenttothemarket.(He)boughtapplesthere.’

(28) Muratbazarg”a gete. Onda Ø alma alyp, Murat market. DAT go.3 S.PR there apple buy. GER o bizge geldi. 3S1PL .DAT come.3 S.PST ‘Muratwenttothemarket.Havingboughtapplesthere,hecametous.’

35 Itshouldbenotedthatthistypeofcharacterizationofnullargumentsappliesonlytoreferentialcases, andnottoexpletiveorgenericusesofnullarguments. 36 Humnick2006providesamorecompletedescriptionofsyntacticallyrestricted(anaphoric)versus unrestricted(pronominal)nullargumentsinKumyk.

54 (29) K"yznyatasyi darazi bolup, girl. GEN father.3. POSS andagree be. GER

Øi[subject] Øi[possessor] k"yzyn j berezhekbola. girl.3. POSS .ACC give. FUT be. PR ‘Thegirl’sfather,agreeing,willgive(his)daughter.’

Inexample(27)thenullargumentisthesubjectofthesecondmatrixclause.Asisthe caseinmanyprodroplanguages,thefiniteverb aldy ‘bought’ismorphologically markedforsubjectagreementinpersonandnumber.Inthiscase,themorpheme –dy indicatesthirdpersonsingular.Inexample(28)thenullformoccursinanadverbial clauseasthesubjectofthegerund, alyp .37 Inthistypeofcomplexclauseconstruction,

thenullsubjectcouldoccureitherwiththegerundorwiththematrixverb(asinthefirst

sentenceof(29)),orwithboth.Though,inmostcases,itwouldbeinfelicitoustohave

overtsubjectsinbothclausesitispossibletohaveanovertsubjectinbothclausesifone

isareflexive. 38

Thejustificationforthecategoryofnullpossessivepronounsillustratedin(29)is basedontheofpossessiveconstructionsinKumyk.Inapossessiveconstruction

37 Nonfiniteverbswiththesuffix–Ip arealsodescribedas‘converbs’inTurkicliterature(seealso Haspelmath1995). 38 Whilethegeneraluseofareflexivepronounwithacoreferentovertsubjectappearstobeinfelicitous, caseswhichareclearlyemphaticusagesareacceptable.Example(i)occursintheextendedtextcorpus. Sincethereflexivehereismarkedwitha1 st personsuffix,thereisnoquestionthatitiscoreferentialwith thematrixsubject, men“I”. (i) O’ziumde bilmeĭ, men bir k”yzny betine tiklenip turg”anman. self.1S EMPH know.NEG .GER 1S one girl’s face.at stare. GER remain.PST .1 S ‘Notevenrealizingitmyself,Ikeptstaringatonegirl’sface.’

55 inwhichallelementsareovert,theNPrepresentingthepossessorexhibitsagenitive suffix,whilethepossesseeexhibitsapossessivesuffixwhichagreesinpersonand numberwiththepossessor. 39 Thestructureoftheovertpossessiveconstructionis illustratedin(30),where(a)isafirstpersonpossessiveand(b)isathirdperson possessive.

(30) a. meni ashym 1S.GEN food1S.POSS ‘myfood’

b. mishikni ashy cat3. GEN food3.POSS ‘thecat’sfood’

Incontrastto(30),inwhichthethirdpersonpossessorisovert,inexample(29)above, thepossessorisrealizedasanullargument,whilethepossesseeisrealizedasanovert form, k"yzyn ‘hisdaughter3POSS ACC ’that exhibitsagreementinpersonandnumber

withthepossessor(thefather)viathesuffix,y.

Thisstudyexcludestwotypesofsyntacticallycontrollednullarguments:1)null

subjectsinnonfinitecomplementclausesthataresyntacticallyrestrictedtocoreference

withaparticularargumentofthematrixclausebythecategoryoftheverbalsuffixand2)

nullsubjectsofadnominalclauses(Humnick2008).Forexample,inacomplement

clausethatendswiththesuffix –mAor mAg”A ,asshownin(31),thenullsubjectmust

39 Thethirdpersonsingularandthirdpersonpluralforms,however,areindistinguishable.

56 beinterpretedascoreferentwiththematrixsubject, k"atyn ‘woman’.40 (Thenonfinite

clauseisenclosedinbrackets,withitssubjectinbold,andthematrixsubjectis

underlined.)

(31) K"atyn i [baĭramg”a Øi adamlany chak"yrma] siue. womanfeast. DAT man.3PL .ACC invite. INF want.3 S.PR ‘Thewomanwantstoinvitepeopletothefeast.’

Asshownin(32)(a)and(b),inthistypeofnonfiniteclause,itisungrammaticalforthe

subjecttooccurasanovertargument,whethercoreferentwiththematrixsubjectornot.

(32) a. *K"atyn i [gelin j adamlany baĭramg”a chak"yrma] siue. womanbride man.3PL .ACC feast. DAT invite.INF want.3S.PR ‘Thewomanwantsthebridetoinvitepeopletothefeast.’

b. *K"atyn i [o’ziu i adamlany baĭramg”a chak"yrma]siue. womanself man.3PL .ACC feast. DAT invite. INF want.3 S.PR ‘Thewomanwantsherselftoinvitepeopletothefeast.’

Thesecondtypeofnullargumentsexcludedinthisstudyarethosewhichoccuras

argumentswithinadnominalparticipialclauseswiththesuffixes–Ag”An (present)or –

g”An (past)—thatistheequivalentoftherelativeclause.Suchclauses,asillustratedin

(33)(a)and(b),canmodifyanyargumentofthematrixclause,andthenullargumentof

theparticipialclauseisalwaysinterpretedascoreferentwiththehead.

40 Therearealsosemantictypesofverbstakingthesamesuffixinwhichthenullargumentiscontrolledby theobject,aswiththecaseofthephrasalverblike ixtiiarbere‘givestherightto’.

57 (33) a. [Ø i ash etegen] k”atyn igele. foodmake. PR .PRT woman come.3 S.PR ‘Thewomanwhomakesthefoodcoming.’ b. [K”atynØietegen] ash i iakhshy. womanmake. PR .PRT food good ‘Thefoodthatthewomanmakesisgood.’

In(33)(a),theparticipialclause(inbrackets)modifies k”atyn ‘woman’,andthenull subjectofthenonfiniteverb etegen ‘making’mustbeinterpretedascoreferentwith k”atyny .Similarly,in(33)(b),thenullargumentwhichistheobjectoftheadnominal mustbeinterpretedascoreferentwith ash ‘food’.Forthetypesofnullargumentsinboth

(31)and(33),thereferentissyntacticallydetermined,andthenullargumentcannot alternatewithotherformsofreferringexpressions,thussuchexampleshave comparativelylittleinteresttothisstudy.

3.3. Reflexives

3.3.1. Generalproperties Kumykexhibitstwotypesofnominalreflexives(inadditiontoverbal reflexivization):theuninflectedreflexive o’z andtheinflectedformofthesameroot morpheme.Theuninflectedform,whichisnotincludedinthisstudy,servesprimarilyas amodifieranddoesnotnecessarilyimplyspecificreference,asinexample(34).

(34) Anam o’z k”olu bulanash etgen. mother.1S.POSS self hand.3POSS with bread make.3PST ‘Mymothermadehomemadebread.’(Lit.‘Mymothermadebreadbyown hand.’)

58 Theinflectedreflexiveiscomposedofthenounroot o’z ‘self’followedbya possessivesuffixwhich,inagenitiveconstruction,indicatesagreementinpersonand numberwiththepossessor.Example(35)illustratestheparallelstructurebetweenafirst personsingulargenitiveconstruction(a)andtheinflectedfirstpersonsingularreflexive

(b).

(35) a. meni gëzium 1s. GEN eye–1S.POSS ‘myeye’ b. meni o’zium 41 1s. GEN self–1S.(POSS ) ‘myself’

Reflexivepronouns,likeotherKumykpronouns,donotindicateanimacyorgender,but aremarkedforpersonandnumber,asshowninTable3.42 (Although1 st and2 nd person formsaregivenhereforreferencepurposes,thefocusofthisresearchis3 rd person forms.)Likeallnouns,reflexivesexhibitcasesuffixesagreeingwiththeirargument function.Table4showsthecasedeclensionfor3 rd personsingularandpluralforms.

41 IntheremainderofthispaperIhavedroppedthe‘ POSS ’portionoftheglossandhavechosentorepresent theglossesforreflexivessimplyas‘self.1S,self.2 S,self.3 S,self.1 P,self.2 P,self.3 P’. 42 Whenthereferentofareflexiveisthirdpersonplural,oftenasingularreflexiveformisused.Unlike thirdpersonsingularforms,thirdpersonpluralreflexiveformsdonotexhibitthepossessivesuffix,butare declinedforcaselikeotherpluralnouns. 44 Coleetal.describethreetypesoflongdistancereflexives:1)“longdistanceboundanaphors”,2)“forms whichareusedasreflexiveslocallyandaspronominalsnonlocally”,and3)“formsthatare“primarily” boundanaphorreflexives,butwhichcanbeusednonlocallyinspecificsyntacticanddiscoursecontexts” (2001:xviii).ForlanguageslikeKumyk,thefactthatthesameformbehaveslocallylikeananaphorand nonlocallylikeapronominalisstillsomewhatproblematic.ForsimilardataontheMalayreflexivediri + pronoun,ColeandHermon2005offertheexplanationthatthefeatures‘anaphor’or‘pronominal’arenot

59

1st person o’zium Singular 2nd person o’ziung 3rd person o’ziu 1st person o’ziubiuz Plural 2nd person o’ziugiuz 3rd person o'zler

Table3: Theinflectedreflexive

Nominative Genitive Dative Accusative Locative Ablative Singular o’ziu o’ziuniu o’ziune o’ziun o’ziunde o’ziunden

Plural o’zler o’zleni o’zlege o’zleni o’zlerde o’zlerden

Table4: Casedeclensionsof3 rd personreflexives

3.3.2. Syntacticproperties Kumykreflexivesdonothavetheprototypicalpropertiesofaformthatisstrictly ananaphorinthebindingtheorysense,thatis,aformwhoseantecedentisac commandingelementfoundwithinthesamelocaldomain,orapronominal,aformwhich cannothavealocalccommandingantecedent.Theambiguousinterpretationofthe sentenceillustratedin(36)showsthattheantecedentofareflexivemaybeeitherac

60 commandingelementwithinthelocaldomain(Murat)orasyntacticelementoutsidethe localdomain(Ali).

(36) Ali i Murat j o’ziun i/j siuegenine inanaĭ. Ali Murat self.3S.ACC like.PR .PRT .3 POSS .DAT believe. PR ‘Ali ibelievesthatMurat jlikeshim i/himself j.’

Theevidenceoflongdistanceantecedentsraisesthequestionofwhetherthereflexivein

Kumykismorelikealongdistanceboundanaphororapronominal. 44 Cole,Hermon,

andHuang2001demonstratethatlongdistancereflexivesinavarietyoftypologically

diverselanguagesvaryintheirresemblancetopronounsorboundanaphors.For

example,basedonevidenceprovidedbyKornfilt(2001)inthesamevolume,Coleetal.

categorizetheTurkishinflectedreflexive kendi asapronominalinitsusebeyondthe localdomain(2001:xx).Theircategorizationcitesthefollowingtypesofevidencethata longdistancereflexiveisapronominalratherthanananaphor:1)lackofrestrictionto certaintypesofcomplexclausestructures,2)lackofrestrictiontosubjectantecedents,3) thepossibilityofusewithextrasententialantecedentsand4)thelackofrestrictionto logophoriccontexts(Pica1987,Coleetal.2001). 45

AnanalysisofKumykreflexivesincomplexclausestructuresdemonstratesthat

longdistancereflexivizationisfoundinalltypesofclausecombinations(Humnick

45 Coleetal.2001alsopointoutthatboundanaphorsrequiresloppyreadingsunderVPellipsis,while pronounsallowforbothstrictandsloppyreadingsunderVPellipsis;however,Kornfilt’sanalysisof Turkishdoesnotprovidedataonthisaspectofinflectedreflexives.

61 2007).Examples(37)–(38)illustratetwoofthesetypes:complementclause constructionsandrelativeclauses.

(37) Iug’an i [[Pavel j o’ziun i/j siuegenine] John Paul self.3 S.ACC like. PR .PRT .3 POSS .DAT men inanag”anymny] bile. 1S believe. PR .PRT .1 S.POSS .ACC know.3 S.PR ‘John iknowsthatIbelievethatPaul jlikeshimself j/him i.’

(38) Ibrag'im i [o'ziu i(Øj) tizip getgen] zatlag”aj k"arap Abrahamself.3 S build. GER leave. PST .PRT thing. PL .DAT look. GER bir zatlar aĭtdi. onething. PL say.3S.PST ‘Abraham i,lookingatthethings jwhich(he)himself ibuilt,saidsomethings.’

Asillustratedin(37)reflexiveswithinnominalclausecomplementscanhaveeitherthe

localclausesubjectorthematrixclausesubjectasanantecedent,andthereisnoblocking

effectfromintermediatesubjects. 46 Inthisexample, o’ziun ,theobjectoftheverb, siuegenine ‘like’canrefereitherto Pavel ,thelocalsubjectorto Iug’an ,thematrix

subject,andtheinterveningsubject men ‘I’doesnotblockthelongdistancereference.

Example(38)demonstratesareflexiveoccurringasanargumentofarelativeclause.In

thiscase,thereflexive o’ziu iscoreferentwiththematrixsubject, Ibrag’im .InKumyk

relativeclauses,theheadisrepresentedasagap(inotherwords,notovertlyrepresented).

46 Insomelanguages,suchasChinese,thescopeoflongdistancereflexivizationcannotcrossaclausein whichthesubjecthasdifferentpersonandnumberfeaturesfromthereflexive,aphenomenonwhichis knownasa‘blockingeffect’(Coleetal.2001:xiv,Huang2000:98).

62 Reflexivesarenot(usually)coreferentwiththehead,butarecoreferentwiththematrix subject.

Thoughthemajorityofreflexiveshavesubjectantecedents,thereisevidencethat longdistancereflexivizationallowsfornonsubjectantecedentsinsomecases,as demonstratedin(39).

(39) O’ziu i/j biblioteka gelse, Iug’an i Pavela j gërezhek. self library. DAT come. COND John Paul. DAT see. FUT ‘Ifselfcomestothelibrary,JohnwillseePaul.’

Inthisexample,thereflexiveoccursinthenonfiniteconditionalclause,anditcanbe coreferenteitherwiththesubjectofthematrixclause( Iug’an )oranonsubjectargument

ofthematrixclause( Pavela ).Inotherwords,thisexamplecanmeaneither“If Paul

comestothelibrary,JohnwillseePaul”or“If John comestothelibraryJohnwillsee

Paul”.

Longdistancereflexivizationalsoextendstoextrasententialantecedents,as

shownin(40),wherethereflexive o’ziu ,thesubjectofthesecondsentence,isunderstood

tobecoreferentwith muallim ‘teacher’intheprevioussentence. 47

(40) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. Teacherstudent. DAT book give. PST .

47 Thisexampleisfromsection3ofthequestionnaireresponses.Both o’ziu and onu areformsusedtofill intheblankintwoblanksinthisexample.

63 Song o’ziu onu eksameni bulan k”utlady. Later self.3S 3S.ACC exam. ACC with congratulate. PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.Thenhe( o’ziu =teacher)congratulated him( onu =student)withtheexam.’

TheevidencethatlongdistancereflexivizationinKumykisnotrestrictedtocertain clausetypesandallowsfornonsubjectandextrasententialantecedentssupportsthe claimthattheKumykinflectedreflexiveisapronominalwhosedistributioncannotbe characterizedonapurelysyntacticbasis(thatis,onthebasisoflocalityandantecedent typeconstraints).Itisappropriate,therefore,toconsiderthedistributionofthisformin termsofnonsyntacticparameterssuchasthecognitivestatusofthereferent.

3.4. Summary

Tosummarize,theprimaryinterestofthisstudyistheendophoricuseof pronominalformswhicharenotconstrainedtoanantecedentbasedpurelyonsyntactic

constraintssuchasbindingconditions.Thisclassincludesfiveovertforms, bu , shu , o, sho ,and o’ziuandasubsetofnullarguments.Thischapterprovidesevidencethatthe

differencesinthedistributionofthesefiveformscannotbedescribedsolelyintermsof

syntax,andcategoriessuchas‘demonstrative’orperhapseven‘reflexive’(totheextent

thatitpresumesananaphoricdistribution)arenotverymeaningfulindescribingtheuse

oftheseforms.Thepurposeofthisstudyisprimarilytodescribetheuseoftheseforms

intermsofacorrelationwiththecognitivepropertiesoftheirreferents.

64 Chapter 4: Corpus Study

4.0. General comments

ThegoalofthecorpusanalysisistocomparethesixpronominalformsinKumyk onthebasisoffeaturesoftheirreferents—mostimportantlyhowthereferentineachuse oftheseformscorrespondstooneofthecognitivestatusesontheGivennessHierarchy.

Table5displaysthenumericalresultsofthiscodingofreferentsaccordingtothehighest statusforwhichareferentcouldbecoded.48 Forexample,fortheform bu ,30tokens haveareferentthatiscodedasinfocus,whileonereferentiscodedasatmostactivated.

Sincethestatus‘infocus’entailsthatareferentisalsoactivated,all31codedformshave referentsthatareatleastactivated,butonlyonehasareferentthatiscodedasatmost activated.Thistablealsolists12formsforwhichthecognitivestatusofthereferent remainsundetermined.Theseareprimarilycaseswhichfallwithinthescopeofthe study,butforwhichthereisaparticularquestionrelatedtotheapplicationofthecoding protocol.Table6displaysthedistributionoftokensofagivenformintermsof percentagesofthetotalnumberofreferentscodedforthatform.Inotherwords,forthe form bu ,96.8%ofthe31codedreferentsareinfocus.Notethat,asnoreferentofa pronominaliscodedasastatuslowerthanfamiliarinthisstudy,onlythethreerelevant statuses,infocus,activated,andfamiliar,arerepresentedinbothofthesetables.

48 SincethestatusesoftheGivennessHierarchyareinarelationshipofunidirectionalentailment,ifa referentiscodedasinfocus,thereferentisalsoactivated,familiar,andsoforth.Inthetablethereferentof agiventokenisonlylistedunderthehigheststatusforwhichitiscodedandnottheotherstatuseswhichit entails.

65 Form Numberof Referent Referent Referent Referent occurrences infocus activated familiar undetermined Ø 229 213 8 8 o'ziu 36 35 1 bu 33 30 1 2 o 90 64 24 1 1 sho 33 4 29 shu 1 1

Table5: CodingResultsforPronominalFormsintheTextCorpus Form Totalcoded %infocus %activated %familiar referents Ø 221 96.4% 3.6% o'ziu 35 100% bu 31 96.8% 3.2% o 89 71.9% 27.0% 1.1% sho 33 12.1% 87.9% shu 1 0% 100% TOTAL 410 84.4% 15.4 0.2% Table6 : Percentageofcodedreferentswithaspecificcognitivestatus AccordingtotheGivennessHierarchymodel,ifthereferentsofagivenformare

distributedamongmorethanonestatusonthehierarchy,theformissaidtoexplicitly

encodetheloweststatus,i.e.tosignalthatthereferentoftheformisatleastthatstatus.

Forexample,ifthereferentsofaformaredistributedbetweenthestatuses‘infocus’and

‘activated’,asforthepronoun sho intable5,where29referentsarecodedasactivated and4referentsarecodedasinfocus,theformissaidtosignalthatthereferentis activated.Insomecases,however,whenthenumberofreferentscorrespondingtothe loweststatusislessthan5%ofthecodedforms,theseformsmaybeconsidered 66 exceptionalormiscoded,andtheformisanalyzedascorrelatingwiththehigherstatus.

Forexample,fortheform o,thetotalnumberofcodedformsinthecorpusis89,andthe

referentsaredistributedasfollows:64(71.9%)infocus,24(27.0%)activated,and1

(1.1%)familiar.Sincethenumberoffamiliarreferentsislessthan5.0%theform ois analyzedassignalingthatthereferentisactivated.Accordingtotheseprinciples,the corpusdataresultsinthehypothesizedformstatuscorrelations:nulls, o’ziu and bu signal‘infocus’,while o, sho ,and shu signal‘activated’(seeFigure5).Notethat,since thereisonlyonetokenofthepronoun shu ,thehypothesizedformstatuscorrelationisat bestpreliminary.

FOC ACT Ø,o'ziu,bu o,sho,shu* (*insufficientdata)

Figure5 : FormStatusCorrelations Theremainderofthischapterdiscussesthedistributionofeachformprimarilyin

termsofthecognitivestatusofthereferent.Myanalysisalsonotescorrelationsbetween particularformsandothercharacteristicsofthereferent,includinganimacyfeatures

(human,nonhuman,inanimate),previousmentionasasubject(asubsetofthecoding

criteriaforinfocus)andwhetherareferentisintroducedpreviouslybymeansofanoun phraseorawholeclause.Withinthediscussionofeachform,Iprovidesamplesaswell

asadiscussionofexceptionalorundeterminedcases,casesofparticularinterest,difficult

67 codingdecisions,anduniquefeaturesofthedistribution,includingcontextualeffects associatedwiththeform.

4.1. Nulls in the corpus

Nullargumentsarethemostfrequentformofpronominaloccurringinthis corpus,withatotalof229tokens.Ofthistotal,eighttokensarecasesofbackwards anaphorawhichareclassifiedasundetermined.Themajorityofcodedreferentsofnull argumentsareinfocus,withonlyeightreferentscodedasactivated.Sincethenumberof activatedreferentsissuchasmallportionofthetotalnumberofreferents(3.6%),Iposit thattheseareexceptionsandthatnullargumentssignalthattheirreferentsareinfocus. 49

Accordingtothecorpusdata,theuseofnullargumentsisneitherrestrictedto referencetoprevioussubjectsnorrestrictedintermsofanimacyfeatures.Whiletheuse ofnullargumentswithclausallyintroducedentitiesisrare,thereisoneexampleinthe corpuswhichisdiscussedinsection4.1.5. 50

4.1.1. Codingthereferentsofnullarguments Inexample(41),anarrativetextexcerptfromthefirstchapterofanovel,I illustrateanumberofdifferentcasesofthecodingofnullarguments. 51

49Theterm‘exception’herecouldincludebothcaseswhicharemiscoded—thatis,casesforwhichthereis nocodingcriterionfromthecodingprotocolwhichapplies,butthereferentisactuallyinfocus—orspeech performanceerrors.Thenumericalpercentageof‘exceptions’inmyresearchissimilartoresultsfoundin Gundeletal.1993,who,forChinese,positthatnullformssignal‘infocus’,though1outof25formsis codedasactivated(291). 50 Therearealsotwoambiguouscaseswhichpossiblyrefertoaclausallyintroducedentity,butwhichI haveinterpretedasreferringtoaperson. 51 Inordertofacilitatethediscussionofalargerunitoftext,Ipresentonlythevernaculartextline,inwhich nullargumentsareidentifiedwiththesymbolØ,accompaniedbythefreetranslation.

68 (41) Excerptfrom“IAmGuilty,Marian.” a. Biziniurtnyiag”asynda u’chak” baredi. b. Ullular olag”a “iurtbulanbiriashly”depaĭtag”anedi. c. Shogertimiiadaialg”anmy,bilmeĭmen. d. Amma shoterekler iurtumnyiakhshysynadaiamanadashag’at. e. Elnik”aĭg”ylygiunlerinde olar ,bukharibërklerĭimik,Øbashlarynsabur kiuĭdeo’rgegëterip, Ømaslag”atetgenler, f. Shatlygiunlerindebusa, Ø butak”laryn, Øiaprak”larynoĭnatyp Ø k”uvanchg”ak”uvanhk”oshg”anlar. g. Ullutamashatiugiulmiu…

h. K’aravulchularĭimik, olar u’cheviudeiurtnuu’chiag”ynda io’sgen(Ø i dërtiunchiu,giunbatar,iag”yndabusao’zenag”aedi). i. Ø K”ybladag”ysy k”aragiunlenikëpgërgen, j. Øk”aragiĭgenk”atyndaĭkëpkerenlermungaĭg”an, k. ØGëziashornunachyk”tëkgen. l. Negetiugiuldeol k”aburlag”abarag”ansok”mak”nyu’stiundeo’sgenedi. m. Iurtdabolg”anchak”yo’liuonu tiubiundeno’tgen. n. Shozatlag”aØtalchyg”ypbusaiaraĭ,onu bashytezk”urudu. o. [Øgiuntybyshdag”ysy]bashellenialdyndatok”tap,[Ø]elgeik”lyk”etgen; p. [Ø]k”abuellerdenk”uvang”an(“Bashelbalag’geltirer,k”abuelk”ap tolturar”).

a. ‘Attheedgeofourvillagewerethreewhitetrees. b. Theelderssaidthattheywerethesameageasourvillage. c. Whetherthisistrueorfalse,Idon’tknow. d. Butthosetreeswerewitnessestothegoodandbadinmyvillage. e. Inthemournfuldaysofwindthey,likesheepskincoats,pointed(their)heads upsupportively,(they)gaveadvice. f. Inthehappydays(their)branches,(their)leavesplayed,(they)addedjoyto joy. g. It’snotabigsurprise…

69 h. Likeguardians,theygrewonthethreesidesofthevillage(onthefourth,the westside,flowedariver). i. Thesouthernone(ofthem)sawmanyblackdays, j. likeawomanclothedinblack,(it)mournedmanytimes, k. insteadoftears,(it)crieddew. l. That’sbecauseitgrewonthepathtothegraveyard. m.Whoeverdiedinthevillagepassedbyit. n. Maybe(it)worriedoverthosethings,anditstop(lit.head)driedupquickly. o. Theeasternone(ofthem)restedinfrontoftheheadwinds,(it)madeshadefor thepeople; p. (it)rejoicedinthewestwinds(“Theheadwindwillbringdisaster,thewest windwillfillthesack”).’ Inthefirstsectionoftext,sentences(a)–(i),therearesixnullargumentsfor

whichthereferentisasetofthreetreeswhichareimportanttoavillage(allmentionsof

thisreferent,whetherovertornullforms,areinbold).Thefirstnullargumentwhich

referstothetreesoccursinsentence(e)asthepossessorinthepossessivephrase, Ø

bashlaryn ‘theirheads’.Thisnulliscoreferentwiththeovertpronoun olar ,thesubject

ofthepreviousclause,and,sincethereferentismentionedovertlyearlierinthesame

sentence,itisinfocusaccordingtocriterion2ofthecodingprotocol.Thesecondnull

argumentin(e)isthesubjectofthesecondclauseandisalsoinfocusbycriterion2.

Sentence(f)containsthreenullarguments,twoofwhicharepartofthepossessive phrases, Øbutak”laryn‘theirbranches’and Øiaprak”laryn ‘theirleaves’.Forthefirst

nullargumentin(f),thefactthatthereferent(thethreetrees)ismentionedasthesubject

oftheprevioussentencesatisfiesthefirstcriterionthestatus,‘infocus’.Forthenexttwo

cases,thereferentisinfocusduetoitspreviousmentioninthesamesentence.In

sentence(i)thenullargumentispartofthepossessivephrase, Øk”ybladag”ysy ‘the

70 southernoneofthem’,anditsreferentiscodedasinfocusaccordingtocriterion1

(mentionedassubjectofprevioussentence).

Insentences(j)–(n),thediscoursetopicisthesoutherntree,whichisfirst mentionedasthesubjectofsentence(i),andallthenullargumentsrefertothisentity.

(Allreferencestothesoutherntreeareunderlined.)In(j)and(k)eachofthenull argumentsmaybecodedasinfocusbecauseeachoftheirreferentsismentionedasthe subjectoftheprevioussentence.In(n),thereferentofthenullargumentismentionedin theprevioussentence,butnotasthesubject;however,sinceitisalsomentionedinthe sentencebeforethat,itisinfocusbycriterion5.

Thecodingofthefirstnullargumentinsentence(o),whichreferstothegroupof trees,isofparticularinterest.Whilesentences(j)–(n)clearlymentiononeofthetrees, thereisnoovertmentionofthegroupoftreesinthislocalsection.However,thisplural entityhasbeenmentionedovertlyasasetatthebeginningofthetextasahigherlevel topicforthisunitofdiscourse.Eachmentionofanindividualmemberoftheset—thatis eachmentionofanindividualtree—isenoughtomaintainthepluralentityasahigher leveltopicwhichis“partoftheinterpretationoftheprecedingclause(whetheritis overtlymentionedthereornot)”(criterion4)and,thus,itisinfocus.Thesecondnull argumentin(o)andtheonenullargumentin(p)bothrefertotheeasterntree,whichis introducedasthesubjectofsentence(o).(Eachofthesereferencestotheeasterntreeis markedinbrackets).Thereferentofeachofthesenullformscanbecodedasbeingin focusstraightforwardlyaccordingtocriteria2and1,respectively.

71 4.1.2. Discussionofundeterminedcases Alleightcasesinthisstudywherethecognitivestatusofthereferentofanull argumentremainsundeterminedarecasesofbackwardsanaphora,orcasesinwhicha nullorovertpronominalwithinanonfiniteclauselinearlyprecedesanovertmentionof thesamereferentinthemainclause.Thesearespecialcaseswherethereferentisnot resolveduntilthewholesentenceisprocessed,andtheyoccuronlyunderrestricted syntacticconditions.Casesofbackwardsanaphoraarenotconsideredtobe counterexamplestotheclaimthatnullargumentsrequirereferentsinfocus,butmerely casesinwhichthereferentcannotbecodedbythecurrentformulationofthecriteria.

UnlikeEnglish,whereasubordinateclausemayprecedeorfollowthemain clause,inKumyk,asubordinateornonfiniteclausealwaysprecedesthemainclause.In acomplexclauseconstruction,coreferentsubjectscanonlyoccuronceasanovert argument—eitherinthemainclauseorthenonfiniteclause,butnotinboth.Previous researchonnullargumentsinKumyk(Humnick2008)indicatesthat,whenanullsubject occursinanonfiniteadverbialclause,itsreferentisusuallyalreadyinfocusfrom previousdiscourse.Theeightundeterminedcasesinthiscorpusstudyrepresentonly casesofnullargumentsinanonfiniteadverbialclauseforwhichthereferentcannotbe codedasinfocusbecauseitcannotberesolvedatthepointwherethenullisencountered

(asopposedtothemajorityofnullargumentsofnonfiniteclausesforwhichthereferent isinfocusduetomentionintheprevioussentence(s)).Fiveoftheseoccurinnonfinite clausesendingwithan –Ipconverb/gerund,andthreeoccurinnonfiniteclausesending withthegerund,gEndE.Thetextexcerptin(42)illustratesacaseofbackwards anaphorainvolvinga–gende clauseinthefinalsentenceofthetext.Thenonfinite 72 clause, Menigjorgende‘when(they)sawme’containsanullargumentthatiscoreferent withtheexpression doskanyaldybulanereturg”ank”yzlar ‘thegirlsstandinginfrontof theblackboard’.

(42) Bir giun akhsham darslardan song, vozhatyĭ k”yz Oneday evening lesson. PL .ABL after lead girl o’ziuniu klassyn k”aldyrg”an. self.3POSS .GEN class.3POSS stay. CAUS .PST .PRT ‘Onedayaftertheafternoonlessons,theleadgirlaskedherclasstostaylate.’ Mende, baiag”ytergeme girdim. 1S and well watch. INF come.in.1S.PST ‘AndI,well,cametowatch.’ Okhuvchular baĭramg”a ĭyrlar u’ĭrene bolg”an eken. student. PL holiday. DATsong.PL learn.PR be.PST COP ‘Thestudentshadbeenlearningsongsforacelebration.’ Ø Meni gërgende, doskany aldy bulanereturg”an k”yzlar 1S.ACC see. PST .PRT .LOC board. GEN front with stand. PST .PRT girl.PL uialdy. become.embarrassed. PST ‘When(they)sawme,thegirlsstandingattheblackboardbecameembarrassed.’ [Marian.026.029]

Consideredtobeaspecialtypeofcataphora,casesofbackwardsanaphorarequire moreefforttoprocessandmaycreateparticularcontextualeffects.Onepossibleeffect, knownas inmediasres ,isanartificiallycreatedimpressionatthebeginningofa

narrativetextthatareferent/participantisalreadyatthecenterofattention.Inother

cases,asin(42),backwardsanaphoraisaresultoforderingconstituentsaccordingto principlesofcoherence.Inthelastsentenceofthisexample,eitherthelocativeclause

menigërgende ‘when(they)sawme’ortheovertsubject, doskanyaldybulanereturg”an

73 k”yzlar ‘thegirlsstandingattheblackboard’,couldoccurinsentenceinitialposition; however,theplacementoftheobject, meni ,atthebeginningofthesentenceprovides greatercoherencewiththepreviouseventlinesentence, Mende,baiag”ytergemegirdim

‘AndI,well,cametowatch.’

4.1.3. Discussionofactivatedcases Oftheeightcasesinthedatainwhichanullargumentisassociatedwitha referentcodedasatmostactivated,fourrepresentcasesinwhichtheentitiesarevery comparabletoreferentsinfocus,butdonotfitanyparticularcriterionofthecoding protocol,whilefourappeartobeexceptionslicensedbyaformofparallelism.Thefour casesinwhichthereferentiscomparabletoinfocusincludetworeferentsmentionedas syntacticallyprominentnonsubjectelementsoftheprevioussentenceandtwocasesofa pluralreferentforwhichatleastonememberofthepluralentityisinfocusatthetimeof mention.

Example(43)displaysonecaseofanullargumentforwhichthereferentisa syntacticallyprominentnonsubjectargumentoftheprevioussentence,butnotonethatis specificallymentionedinthecodingprotocolasbringingareferentintofocus.

(43) Khoziaĭstvoda ëlbashchy alyshyng"an. farm. LOC leader change. PST ‘Theheadofthefarmhasbeenreplaced.’ Iangy directoru Mag"ammat Khamavovnu tezden tanyĭman. new director. ACC Magamet Khamavov. ACC from.long.agoknow. PR .1 S ‘Ihaveknownthenewdirector,MagametKhamavovalongtime.’ Ø G'arakatchy ëldash. expert friend ‘He'sanexpert.’[Agr.053055]

74 Inthiscase,thenullargumentinthethirdsentencereferstoMagametKhamavov, mentionedinthesecondsentenceasthedirectobject, iangydirectoruMag"ammat

Khamavovnu ‘thenewdirectorMagametKhamavov’.Theobjectoccursinsentence

initialpositionandmostlikelyintopicposition—inotherwords,inaplaceofsyntactic prominenceequivalenttothatofthesubjectinothertypesofsentenceconstructions.It

is,thus,understandablethatinrecentrevisionsofthecodingprotocolthecriteriaare

revisedtogeneralizethecriterion,“mentionedasthesubject”or“mentionedinsyntactic

focusposition”to“mentionedinapositionofsyntacticprominence”(Gundel2006).

Thecriterionofareferentbeingmentionedinapositionofsyntacticprominence

isalsorelevanttoanalysisofthefirstoftwonullargumentsdisplayedinthesecond

sentenceof(44).

(44) Ullualim iaryk”landyryvchu AbusupiyanAkayevni i eki k”yzy j greatscholar holy.one Absupian Akaev two girl.3poss bolg”an. be.3pst ‘Thegreatscholar,theenlightenedone,AbsupianAkaev,hadtwodaughters.’ Sho eki de k”yznyu’ĭlenmege zamany etishgende 3.3two EMPH girl.GEN get.married.INF time.3POSS arrive. PST .PRT .LOC

ØiØj u’ĭlendirgen. get.married.CAUS .3 PST ‘Whenthetimecameforthetwogirlstogetmarriedhegavetheminmarriage.’ [Editorial.025.026]

Thereferentsofthesetwonullsarementionedaspartofapossessivephrasewhich occursasthegrammaticalsubjectofthefirstsentence: ullualimyaryk”landyryvchu

75 AbusupiyanAkayevniekik”yzy ‘thegreatscholar,theenlightenedone,Absupian

Akaev’stwodaughters’.Thedaughtersarementionedagainatthebeginningofthe secondsentence.Basedonthecodingcriteria,thedaughterscanbecodedasinfocus basedoncriteria1or2,butAbsupianAkaeviscodedasactivatedbecausethisreferentis notmentionedasthesyntacticsubjectinthefirstsentence;however,itisinaprominent positionasthepossessorinthepossessivenounphraseinsubjectposition.

Anothertypeofcasewhichisverysimilartothatofareferentinfocusisthecase ofapluralentityforwhichallmembersareactivatedandatleastonememberoftheset isinfocusatthetimeofmention.Therearetwocasesoftheuseofnullargumentswith suchentities,oneofwhichisillustratedinexample(45).

(45) Bularda geche de Øi(prophet) k"almaĭ mishigine Øi bara. 3.1. PL .LOC night EMPH stay. NEG .PR cat.3.POSS .DAT go. PR ‘Notstayingwiththemforthenight,(he)wenttothecat.’ Hoshbesh Ø etip sorashypashg"a Ø olturalar. greeting do. GER greet. GER food. DAT sit.3PL .PR ‘Greetingeachother,(they)sitdowntoeat.’[Prophet.002627] Ofthefournullargumentsinthistext,theformofinteresttothisdiscussionistheone

firstmentionedinthesecondsentence(representedinbold).Thetwonullargumentsin

thefirstsentencerefertotheprophet,whoisinfocusbasedonthepreviouscontextnot

shownhere.Anotherimportantreferentwhichismentionedinthefirstsentenceisthe

cat(thatis,thecatwhoturnedintoagirlearlierinthenarrative),mentionedovertlyby

theform mishigine‘tothecat’.Thetwonullargumentsinthesecondsentencebothhave

apluralreferentconsistingofatleasttheprophetandthecatgirl,andperhapsother

76 membersofthehousehold(e.g.thecatgirl’shusband).Accordingtothemethodologyof

thisstudy,thefirstmentionofapluralreferentisthefirsttimeallthemembersoftheset

arementionedaspartofthesamenounphrase,sotheindividualmentionsoftheprophet

andthecatgirlintheprevioussentencearenotenoughtobringthepluralreferentinto

focusatthetimeofmentionofthefirstnullargumentofthesecondsentence(however,

thesecondnullargumentofthesamesentenceisinfocusbycriterion2).This

exceptionaluseofthenullformwithanatmostactivatedreferentappearstobe

acceptableduetothefactthatatleastoneofthemembersofthepluralsetisinfocusat

thetimeofmention.

Infourcasesinwhichnullargumentsareusedinreferencetoatmostactivated

entities,theiruseappearstobelicensedbyaformofparallelisminthesyntacticstructure

oreventstructure.Allofthese(representedinbold)occurintheexcerptfromthetext

called“TheProphet”,whichisdisplayedin(46).

(46) Excerptfrom“TheProphet” a. Erten Øi (prophet)turg"anda tilegenler jgelip morning get.up.PST .PRT .LOC requester. PL come. GER Øj k"yzny elteler. girl. ACC lead.3 PL .PR ‘Inthemorning,when[theprophet]getsup,therequesterscomeandtake awaythegirl.’ b. K"yz getip gechesinde paĭkhammarnyi u'ĭunde girl leave. GER night.3POSS .LOC prophet. GEN home.3POSS .LOC o'ziuniui k"yzyndan da isbaĭy k"yz bola. self.3POSS girl.3POSS .ABL and slender girl be. PR ‘Afterthegirlleaves,thatevening,intheprohet'shome,besideshisown daughterthereappearsaslendergirl.’

77 c. O Øiu'ĭdegi mishigi bolg"an. 3.2S home. LOC .MOD cat.3POSS be. PST ‘Shehadbeen(his)housecat.’ 52 d. Erten Økgelip geleshegenler k onu da elte. morning come. GER ask.for.in.marriage. PR .PRT .PL 3.2.ACC EMPH lead. PR ‘Cominginthemorning,theonesasking[forher]inmarriageleadher [away],too.’ e. U'chiunchiu geche Øiiti k"yzbola, third night dog.3POSS girlbecome. PR ‘Thethirdnight(his)dogbecomesagirl,…’ f. erteninde Øl gelip o k"yznyda morning.3POSS .LOC come. GER 3.2. DET girl. ACC EMPH Øl alyp gete. take. GER leave. PR ‘…inthemorning[they]comeandtakethatgirl,too.’ g. Aradan bireki giun getip bir geche interval. ABL one two day pass. GER one night Øiesheki k"yz bola. donkey.3POSS girlbecome. PR ‘Afteracoupleofdayspass,onenight(his)donkeybecomesagirl.’ h. Erteninde onu da Øm elteler. morning.3POSS .LOC 3.2S. ACC EMPH lead.3PL .PR ‘Inthemorning,[they]takeher,too.’ [Prophet.000915]

Inthisexcerpt,threesentencescontainpossessivephrasesinwhichthepossessor

iselided(underlinedinthetext): Øiu'ĭdegimishigi‘(his)housecat’, Øiiti‘(his)dog’

and Øiesheki‘(his)donkey’.Ineachcasethepossessoristheprophet,themain

52 Thenullargumentinthefirstclauseofthissentenceisacaseofbackwardsanaphoraandis,thus,listed amongtheundeterminedcasesofcoding.

78 participantinthenarrative.Thenullargumentrepresentingthepossessorin(c)iscoded asinfocus,astheprophetismentionedinbothsentence(a)and(b)–thoughnotasthe subjectof(b)–thuscriterion5applies.Inthecaseofboth Øiiti‘hisdog’in(e)and Øi

esheki‘hisdonkey’(g),thereferentofthenullarguments,thatis,theprophet,isnot mentionedintheprevioussentence;thereforeitisnotcodedasinfocus,butasactivated.

InthelattertwocasesIproposethattheparallelisminargumentstructurebetweenthe threecasesisenoughtorendertheuseofanullargumentacceptableeventhoughthe referentiscodedasactivated. 53

Twoothernullargumentsusedwithactivatedentitiesinthesametextinvolvea slightlydifferentformofparallelism.Thesecases,whichoccurinsentences(f)and(h), areparticularlyinterestinginthesensethattheyinvolvedparallelismatthelevelof discoursestructure.Inthistexttherearefourgroupsofgroom’srepresentatives(suitors) thatcometotakeawayeachoftheprophet’sfourdaughters,threeofwhichwere previouslyhouseholdanimals.In(a),therepresentativesofthefirstgroomare mentionedviatheovertform tilegenler jinthefirstclauseandanullargumentinthe secondclause,thereferentofwhichisinfocusbycriterion2.Therepresentativesofthe secondgroomarementionedin(d)viaanullargumentinthefirstclauseandtheform, geleshegenler kinthesecondclause.Inthiscasethenullargumentisconsideredacaseof backwardsanaphora(listedasundetermined).Bythetimethethirdgroom’s representativesarementionedin(f),nullformsareusedinboththefirstandsecond

53 Alternatively,itcouldbearguedthatthereferent(theprophet)isinfocusbecauseitisahigherlevel topicthatispartoftheinterpretationofthewholeunitoftextrepresentedintheexample.

79 clause,eventhoughthereferentoftheseformswasnotmentionedinthepreceding sentence(e).Similarly,inthesingleclauseof(h),anullargumentisusedinreferenceto thefourthgroom’srepresentatives,thoughnorepresentativesarementionedinthe previoussentence.Inthesecases,theacceptabilityofthenullformsin(f)and(h) appearstobejustifiedbyatypeofparallelismIcallscriptrepetition,referringtothe repetitionofaseriesofeventsinthenarrativewhichinvolvesparallelsentenceswiththe sameargumentstructure(Humnick2008:57).Theevidencefromthisexamplesuggests that“scriptrepetition”givesrisetoaparticularpatternandtheexpectationthatthis patternwillbecontinued.Ibelievethatdegreeofexpectednessorpredictabilityisa

factorwhichcontributestotheinherentsalienceorcognitivestatusofanentity(cf.

Prince’s“parallelismprinciple”(1981:228)).

4.1.4. Higherleveltopics Oneofthemoresubjectiveareasofthecodingprocessistheapplicationof criterion4forcodingareferentinfocus:“Itisahigherleveltopicthatispartofthe interpretationoftheprecedingclause(whetheritisovertlymentionedthereornot).”

WhileIapplythiscriterionconservativelyintheanalysis,itisworthwhiletopresenta coupleofcaseswherethiscriterionclearlyappliestothecodingofanullreferent.

Inthefirstofthesecases,theuseofnullformsisassociatedwithashiftin narrativepointofviewintheAihalaytext,presentedin(47).(Lines(a)–(d)ofthistext arepresentedintheformofanEnglishtranslation,whileline(e),wherethenull argumentoccurs,ispresentedasglossedtext.)

80 (47) a.‘ForseveralweeksAihalayworeherslippersonherdaysoffwhentherewere meetings,gatheringsintheclub,orwhenshevisitedsomeone.

b.Loandbehold,atthattime,thesolestartedcomingofftheslippersshewore towork. c.Itwassaidthatthebootmakerwasgoingarounddrinkinglikeevil;toputit briefly,therewasnowayoutexcepteithertostayhomeandnottogotowork ortogotoworkbarefoot. d.Thatdaytherewereespeciallyalotofmeetings—themeetingofthecleanup commission,themeetingofthetownsoviet.’ e.Bulany barysyn_da uzak" zamansëĭlediler erishdiler, 3.1. PL .GEN every.one long time say.3 PL .PST argue.3PL .PST u'ĭge busa Ø gechesag'at birde kaĭtmag"a tiushdiu. home. DAT however nighthour one. LOC return. INF descend.3PST ‘Ateveryoneofthem(people)talkedalongtime,andargued,andsheended upcominghomeatoneo'clockatnight.’[Aihalay.07.011]

Inlines(a)–(b),thenarrativepointofviewisthatofathirdpersonomniscientnarrator.

Lines(c)–(e)representashifttonarrationfromthepointofviewofAihalay’sthoughts,

expressingherevaluationofthesituationandheronlyoptions:“therewasnowayout

excepteithertostayhomeandnottogotoworkortogotoworkbarefoot(line(c)).”

SinceAihalay’sthoughtsarebeingrepresented,itfollowsthatsheisimplicitly

representedinthislineoftext,eventhoughsheisnotovertlymentioned.Moreover,the

factthatAihalayisnotmentionedovertlyhasthedramaticeffectofallowingthehearer

(reader)toempathize—toputherselfinAihalay’splace.Inline(d),themeetings

discussedarethemeetingsAihalayattended,andthewaythemeetingsaredescribed

representsherevaluation.SinceAihalayispartoftheinterpretationofbothsentence(c)

andsentence(d),sheisarguablya‘higherleveltopic,andthenullargumentin(e)canbe

codedasinfocus. 81 Anothercaseinwhichareferentofanullargumentiscodedonthebasisofbeing ahigherleveltopicinthepreviouscontextoccursintheAgriculturetextinadescription ofhowonefarmtriestonegotiateadealtousethelandofanotherfarm(48).

(48) ‘Whenweknewthatourneighbors'landwasempty,wewantedtosowcropsfor common(profit),"saidSoltanyangiyurtGPU'sagriculturalexpert,Abusayit Gamzatov.Whereisthat?(idiom)[Soltanyangiyurt'sleaders didn'ttakeitclose (didn’tlikeit).Maybe they didn'taccepttheconditions.Howcouldthisbe?The seedswouldhavebeenfromus,andthetechnology,andwewouldhavetilledand evenwhenthewheatwasripewewouldhavegatheredit.’ Alyng"an tiushiumnu tengeteng etme khyialybyz be.taken. PST .PRT crop. ACC equally do. INF thought.1PL .POSS baredi. ] exist3.PST .AUX ØØ54 Razibolmadilar… (they)(conditions) agreebe. NEG .3 PL .PST ‘Weplannedtodividetheharvestevenly.Theydidn'tagreetoit...’ [Agr.032039]

ThehigherleveltopicintheunitoftextmarkedwithbracketsistheSoltanyangiyurt’s

leaders’responsetotheconditionsofthedeal.Soltanyangiyurt’sleadersareovertly

mentionedseveraltimesinthepreviouscontext,butnotinthesentenceprecedingthe

nullargument(inbold).Inthissentence,however,theSoltanyangiyurtleadersare

implicitlypartofthesemanticsoftheverb tengetengetme ‘divide’byvirtueofbeingthe peopletotheharvestwouldbedividedwith.

54 Thesetofconditionsarementionedovertlyasapluralnominalearlierinthisexcerptandthespecific detailsoftheconditionsarementionedovertlyinthetwosentencespriortotheoccurrenceofthisnull argument;thusitisinfocusbycriterion5.

82 4.1.5. Clausallyintroducedentities FollowingtheassumptionoftheGHmodelthateventsorstatesdenotedbya precedingsentencearebroughtintofocus,onewouldexpectthatnullargumentscouldbe usedtorefertoeventsorstates.Inthecorpusdata,however,thereisonlyoneclearcase inwhichanullargumentreferstoeventorstateintroducedbyaclause,whichisshown in(49).

(49) BalikiØ shartlaryn k"abul_etmegendir. maybe(they) condition.3PL .ACC accept.NEG .PR .PRT .SPEC ‘Maybetheydidn'taccepttheconditions.’ Ø Nechik bola? how be.PR ‘Howcouldthisbe?’ [Agr.035.036]

Inthisexample,thenullsubjectofthesecondsentencereferstoapotentialstate

introducedbytheprevioussentence.Notethattheverbismarkedwiththesuffix –dir ,

whichisaparticleexpressingconjectureorspeculation(Clintonms:28).

Thelackofotherexamplesofnullreferencetoclausallyintroducedentities

suggeststhatnullsmayactuallyberestrictedtoreferencetonominallyintroducedentities

andthatexample(49)isanexceptionalcase.Informaltestingsuggeststhatcertain

exclamationsorexpressionsofpropositionalattitudesuchas nechikbola ‘howcouldthis be’and anlamaĭman ‘Idon’tunderstand’and bilmedim ‘Ididn’tknow’areacceptable

83 withoutovertobjects,whetherthereferentisanevent,state,orfact. 55 Suchcasesmay

actuallybeconventionalizedexpressionsthatcouldbeconsideredtobenonreferential.

4.2. Reflexives in the corpus

All35codedreferentsofthereflexivepronoun o’ziu ,areinfocus.Whilethe

codingofreferentsof o’ziu isstraightforwardinmostcases,onecaseremains

undetermined—acaseofbackwardsanaphorawhichisdiscussedinsection4.2.1.The

remainderofthissectionpresentsdataaboutthereferentsofreflexiveswhichfocuses

primarilyonthelineardistanceandargumentpositionofpreviousmention,assuchdata

isusefulinsupportingmyanalysisof o’ziu asapronominal.

Asforotherfeaturesofthedistributionofreferents,whilemostreferentsof o’ziu

arehuman,thereisonecaseinwhichthisformisusedwithanonhumanreferent(cf.

example(57),section4.2.2.2). O’ziu isnot,however,usedwithentitiesintroduced

directlyorindirectlybyfullclauses,suchaseventsandfacts.

4.2.1. Discussionofundeterminedcases Theonereferentofthereflexive o’ziu forwhichthecodingremainsundetermined

isacaseofbackwardsanaphorainvolvingareflexiveusedwithinarelativeclause,as

illustratedin(50).

(50) Milletleni g’alklany arasynda bulaĭ aĭtyvlarbar: nationspeoples among 3.1. MOD sayingsexist.PR

55 NotethatafactdenotedbyaprevioussentenceisassumedtobeatmostactivatedbytheGHmodel, henceonewouldnotexpectreferencewithaformwhichsignals‘infocus’tobeacceptable.

84 “o’ziuniu i milletin g’alkyn siuiup self. GEN nation.3POSS .ACC people.3POSS .ACC love.GER bazharmaĭg”anadamlari o’zge milletleni de know.how. NEG .PRT man.PL other nation.PL .ACC EMPH g’alklany da siuiup bazharmas”. people.PL .ACC EMPH love.GER know.how. NEG .FUT ‘Amongthenationsandpeoplesthereisasayinglikethis:"Menwhodon'tknow howtolovetheirownnationandpeoplewillnotknowhowtoloveothernations andpeople.’[Editorial.07]

Inthisexample,thereflexiveiscoreferentwiththematrixsubject adamlar ‘men’,but

linearlyprecedesit.(And,sincethereisnoothermentioninthepreviouscontext,there

isnoothercriterionbywhichthereferentcanbeinfocus.)Asinthecaseofnulls,

backwardsanaphorawithreflexivesisrestrictedtocasesinwhichtheformlinearly

precedesamentionofthesamereferentinmatrixsubjectposition.Thoughsuch

referentscannotbecodedasinfocusunderthecurrentformulationofthecoding

protocol,aformulationofthecodingprotocolwhichdefinesthecriteriaintermsof

syntactichierarchyratherthanlinearorderwouldresultinthecodingoftheseformsasin

focus.Ontheotherhand,thefactthatsuchcasesareexceptionstogeneralizationsabout

linearordermayresultinaddedcontextualeffectsand,thus,itwouldbemoreinsightful

nottocodethemconventionallyinthesamewayasotherreferentsinfocus.

Anotherinterestingcaseinthecorpuslookslikeacaseofbackwardsanaphora

involvingalocalpossessivereflexive,butthereferentofthereflexiveisactuallycoded

asinfocusbyvirtueofbeingahigherleveltopic(criterion4).Asshownin(51), o’zleni

(plural,genitive)iscoreferentwith khoziaĭstvo ‘the(collective)farm’.Thementionof

thepossessivereflexivelinearlyprecedesthementionofthefarminthesubjectposition

85 oftheclause;however,itishighlylikelythatthereferentofthereflexiveisalreadyin focusatthetimeofthatmention,asitiscoreferentwiththepronominalform munda

‘here’intheprevioussentence,andthefarmisthehigherleveltopicofalargersection ofthetextfromwhichthisexcerptistaken

(51) Mundaashlyk" tarlavlanymaĭdanyn 3.1. LOC crops field.PL .GEN area.3POSS .ACC dag"y_da gengleshdirme umut ete. even.more widen. INF hopedo.3PR ‘Here,itishopedthattheareaofthegrainfieldswillbewidenedevenmore.’ O'zleni beshkombaĭny bulankhoziaĭstvotiushium self.3PL .GEN fivecombine withfarm crop k"aĭtaryvnuchalt wak"urumlu kiuĭde o'tgere. harvest. ACC quicklyandorganized state. LOC carry.out.3 PR ‘Withtheirfivecombinesthefarmcarriedouttheharvestinaquickand organizedmanner.’[Agr.016017]

4.2.2. Referentsofreflexivesand‘antecedent’types ThoughananalysiswithintheGHmodelfocusesonthecognitivefeaturesofthe referentofaform,manyreferentsarecodedonthebasisofhowthesyntacticfeaturesof previousmentionsofthereferent—featuressuchasargumentpositionandlinear distance—contributetocognitivestatus.Theevaluationoffeaturesofpreviousmention is,toalargedegree,paralleltothewayinwhichsyntacticianscategorizereflexivesin termsoftheirantecedents.Inusingtheterm antecedent here,Iamusingaconventional

shortcuttorefertothelinguisticformwhichconstitutesamentionofthesamereferentas

theformbeingcoded.Inthisanalysis,Idividethetokensofreflexivesintotwomain

categories:thosewhichhaveanantecedentinthesamesimpleclause(18tokens)versus

86 thosewhichhavealongdistanceantecedentoutsideofthesimpleclause(17tokens). 56

Section4.2.2.1discussesreflexiveswithsameclauseantecedents.Mostoftheseare possessives,buttherearealsoafewcasesofnonpossessivereflexiveswithlocalclause antecedentswhicharediscussedseparately.Amongcaseswhereareflexivehasan antecedentinthelocalclause,thereisonlyonecaseofaformthathasanonsubject antecedent(andnootherantecedentinthepreviousclauseorsentence).Section4.2.2.2 presentsreflexiveswithlongdistanceantecedents.Longdistancereflexivization,by

definition,involvesformsthatdonothaveasubjectantecedentwithinthelocalclause.

Inthiscorpusmostofthelongdistanceantecedentsinthiscorpusaresubjectsofthe

matrixclause(or,inonecase,anadjacentnonmatrixclause),thoughtherearetwocases

inwhichreflexiveshavealongdistancenonsubjectantecedent.Thissectionalso

discussestwocasesoflongdistancereflexivizationthatareextrasentential.Though

infrequent,thesecasesstronglysupportofthecategorizationof o’ziu asapronominal

ratherthanananaphor.

4.2.2.1. Sameclauseantecedent Themostcommonuseoftheform o’ziu inthecorpusisasalocalpossessive

reflexive,whereitiscoreferentwiththelocalclausesubject(whetherexpressedasan

overtorimplicitargument).Nearlyhalf(15outof35codedcases)ofthetokensof o’ziu

arelocalpossessives,mostofwhichareinfocusbycriterion2.Theexamplein(52)

56 Byusingthedistinctionbetweenantecedentswithinthesameclauseasopposedtothoseoutsidethe clauseIfollowthetypeofcriteriainthecodingprotocol(e.g.linearandhierarchicaldistance)without capturingallofthedistinctionsbetweenlocalandnonlocaldomainswhicharerepresentedwithinbinding theory.

87 illustratesalocalpossessivereflexivewhere o’zleni (plural,genitive)hasthesame referentas iurtdaingarivgiĭinegenkatynlar‘themostbeautifullydressedwomeninthe village,’mentionedpreviouslyinthesameclauseasthesubject.

(52) Iurtda ing ariv giĭinegen katynlar i village. LOC very pretty be.dressed. PR .PRT wife. PL o'zleni i machilerin Aĭkhalaĭg"a bichdire;… self.3 PL .GEN slipper. PL .3. POSS .ACC Aihalay. DAT cut.out. CAUS .PR ‘Themostbeautifullydressedwomeninthevillagehavetheirslippersmadeby Aihalay;…’[Aihalay.003]

Thereisonecase,shownin(53),inwhichapossessivereflexivehasanon subjectantecedentinthesameclause(inbrackets).Thoughthiscaseisdifferentinterms ofsyntacticrelations,thecodingofthereferentof o’ziuniu —thatis,theprophet—is straightforward,astheprophetisovertlymentionedearlierinthesentence,and,thus,is infocusbycriterion2.

(53) K"yz getip [gechesinde paĭkhammarnyi u'ĭunde daughter.3poss leave. GER that.evening prophet. GEN home.3POSS .LOC o'ziuniui k"yzyndan da isbaĭy k"yz bola.] self.3S.GEN girl.3POSS .ABL EMPH slender girl be. PR ‘Afterthegirl/daughter ileaves,thatevening,intheprophet’s jhome,besideshis j owndaughterthereappearsaslendergirl k..’[Prophet.0010]

Amongreflexiveswhichhaveanantecedentinthelocalclause,thereareonlytwo thatarenotpossessives.Thisinfrequencyisdue,inpart,tothefactthatreflexive patientsaremostoftenexpressedbyaverbalmorpheme(whichrenderstheovert mentionofthepatientunnecessary)ratherthanapronoun.Oneexampleofanon

88 possessivereflexivewithalocalclauseantecedentisillustratedinthesecondsentenceof

(54).

(54) Oramda bary_dag’alk o'ziune k"arap kiulegendir street. LOC every people self.3S.DAT look. GEN laugh. PR .PRT .COP dep esine gelip Aĭkhalaĭi ĭylamag"a COMP mind.3 POSS .DAT come. GER Aihalay cry. INF k"alyp takhtamekni u’stiunde olturdu. littlestay. GER bench. ACC on sit.3S.PST ‘Aihalay isatdownonthebenchonthevergeoftears,thinkingthatallthepeople onthestreetwouldlookather iandlaugh.’ Tek birden_eki o'ziuniu i aldinda tashlanyp butsuddenly self.3S.GEN in.front.of be.thrown. GER turag"anmachiĭni Øi gëriup k"aldi. stay. PR .PRT slipper. ACC see. GER stay.3 S.PST ‘Butallofasuddenshesawinfrontofheraslipperthathadbeenthrowndown.’ [Aihalay.028.029]

Thereferentofthereflexive o’ziuniu isAihalay,whoismentionedovertlyinthefirst

sentenceoftheexample,butisalsomentionedinthesecondsentenceasanullsubject

argument.

4.2.2.2. Longdistanceantecedent Ofthe17tokensof o’ziu withalongdistanceantecedent,themostcommontype

consistsofthe7casesofreflexiveswhichoccurinarelativeclauseandhaveamatrix

clausesubjectantecedent.InKumyk,relativeclausesconsistofaheadprecededbya

clausalmodifierinwhichtheheadisrepresentedbyagapornull.Forexample,in(38),

thereflexiveoccursintherelativeclause o'ziu tizipgetgen ‘whichselfbuilt’,which

modifiesthehead, zatlag”a‘things’.

89 (55) Ibrag'im i [o'ziu i(Øj)tizip getgen] zatlag”aj k"arap Abrahamself.3 S build. GER leave. PST .PRT thing. PL .DAT look. GER bir zatlar aĭtdi. onething. PL say.3S.PST ‘Abraham i,lookingatthethings jwhich(he)himself ibuilt,saidsomethings.’ [Video1.014] (ExamplerepeatedfromChapter3)

Inthisexample, o’ziu referstothereferentof Ibrag’im ‘Abraham’,thematrixclause subject.

Inadditiontorelativeclausereflexiveswithsubjectantecedents,therearetwo casesofrelativeclausereflexiveswithnonsubjectantecedents,asillustratedin(56)and

(57).57 Example(56)illustratesacaseinwhichareflexivewithinarelativeclausehasa referentthatismentioned,notasthematrixsubject,but,still,asthemostprominent argumentofthematrixclause.

(56) [Adamlag”ai[ o'ziu i (Øj)siuiegen] adamyndan j man. PL .DAT self.3S love. PR .PRT man.3. POSS .ACC .ABL aĭrylmag"a] bek k"yĭyn; be.separated.from. INF very difficult ‘Itishardforpeopletobeseparatedfromsomeonetheylove;…’[Obituary.006]

57 Thisistheonlytypeoflongdistancenonsubjectantecedentillustratedinthecorpus;however,thereare otherpossibletypesoflongdistance,nonsubjectantecedents.Forexample,inthefollowingobject controllednonfiniteclause,areflexiveinthenonfiniteclausecanbecoreferentwitheithertheobjector thesubjectofthematrixclause. Murat i Arsenge j [o’ziuniu i/j topurag”y satmag”a] ikhtiĭar bergen. Murat Arsen. DAT self.3S.GEN land.3POSS sell.INF right give.3 S.PST ‘Murat igaveArsen jtherighttosellhis i/j land.’

90 Inthisexample, o’ziu hasthesamereferentas adamlag”a‘people’,whichoccursinthe dativecaseinthematrixclause adamlag”aadamyndanaĭrylmag"abekk”yĭyn… ‘Itis

hardforpeopletobeseparatedfromaman…’ 58

Example(57)isanunusualcaseinwhichareflexiveinanounphrasehasasits antecedentanounphraseembeddedwithinthesamenounphrase.

(57) Adabiiat–inche saniiatny bir tarmag”y. literature fine arts. GEN one area.3POSS ‘Literatureisoneareaofthefinearts.’ [[Inche saniiatny g’ar tarmag”ny i busa [(Øj) o’ziune i fine arts. GEN every area.3POSS .GEN however self.3S.DAT (58) khas_bolg”an] k”uraly j]] bola. belong.PST .PRT material.3 POSS exist.3 S.PR ‘Eachareaofthefinearts(however)hasmaterialswhichbelongtoit.’(Lit.‘Fine arts’eacharea’s[(they)belongtoit(area)]materialsexist.’)[MotherTongue.05]

In(57)therelativeclauseinwhich o’ziune occursmodifies k”uraly ‘material’theheadof thesubjectnounphrase.Interestingly,therelativeclause(singlebrackets)occursinthe middleoftheextendednounphrase(doublebrackets)thatservesasthesyntacticsubject.

Thereflexivewithintheclausalmodifiercorefersnotwiththesubjectnounphrase,but withtheheadofthepossessivephrase inchesaniiatnyg’artarmag”ny ‘eachareaofthe finearts’thatisthegenitivecomplement(possessor)of o’ziune khasbolg”ank”uraly

‘materialswhichbelongtoit’.

58 InKumykpluralagreementissometimesomittedinthirdperson;thusinthiscaseitisacceptableforthe thirdpersonsingularform o’ziu tobeusedforthereferentof adamlaxa ,apluralnoun.

91 Whilereflexiveswhichserveasrelativeclauseargumentsarethemostcommon typeoflongdistancereflexive,thereareonlytwooccurrencesoflongdistance possessivereflexives,oneofwhichisshownin(59).

(59) [Karimulla iiashadansholaĭ tez geter] dep Karimullalife. ABL 3.3. ADV early go.3 S.FUT COMP ia o'ziuniu i iadabizin onu i iuvuk"laryny nor self. 3S.GEN orour 3.2. ACC near.one. PL .3. POSS .GEN birde esibizge gelmeĭ edi. no.one memory.1PL .DAT come. NEG .PR 3S.PST .AUX ‘Neitherhenorwe,hisdearones,noneofusthoughtthatKarimullawouldleave thislifesoearly.’[Obituary.011]

Interestingly,theantecedentofthisreflexive(whichoccursinthematrixclause)isthe subjectofthecomplementclausewhichlinearlyprecedesthereflexiveratherthanthe subjectofthematrixclause.

Thesecondlongdistancepossessivereflexive,shownin(60),occurswithinsemi directspeechandiscoreferentwiththesubjectofthematrixclause.Thistypeofcase, whichinvolveslogophoricity,willbediscussedfurtherinsection4.2.3.

(60) Ø Ashamag"atok"tag"an zamandaAĭkhalaĭi bary_da eat. INF stop. PST .PRT time. LOC Aihalay all ishleĭgenleni aĭlanyp olardan “ëlda o'ziuniu i work. PR .PRT .PL .ACC bother. GER 3.2. PL .ABL road. LOC self.3POSS .GEN machiĭin tapmag"anmyken” depsorady. slipper.3POSS .ACC find. NEG .PST .INTER .beingCOMP ask.3. PST ‘Whenitwastimetostoptoeat,Aihalaybotheredalltheworkers,askingthem, “haveyoufoundmyslipperontheroad?”.’[Aihalay.021]

92 AnothertypeoflongdistancereflexiveIcategorizeasanemphaticreflexive.

Thistypeofreflexiveoccursasthesubjectofoneclauseandiscoreferentwiththe subject(eitherovertorimplicit)ofanotherclauseinacomplexclauseconstruction.

Sincethesereflexivesalternatewithnullargumentsingivingacoreferentreadinginthis typeofcontext,theuseoftheovertformisinterpretedashavinganemphaticeffect.

Notethatinthesamecontext,anyothertypeofovertpronounwouldbeinterpretedas notcoreferentwithanovertsubjectoftheadjoiningclause(cf.example(66)foracasein whichanemphaticreflexiveiscoreferentwithanovertsubjectinanadjoiningclause).

(61) GecheØi(prophet)iatg"anda o'ziui etgen ters night lie.down. PST .PRT .LOC self..3 S do. PST .PRT wrong ishge o'ziui g'ëkiuniup tobag"a Øi tiushe turup work. DAT self.3 S regret. GER repentance. DAT descend. PR stay. GER Øibirdag"y namaz k"yla. one.moreprayer do. PR ‘[That]nightwhen[he]laydown,[he]regretsthewrongthingthathehaddone, fallsintorepentance,[and]praysonemoreprayer.’[Prophet.0006]

Inexample(61),fromthenarrativeabouttheprophet,theprophetisinfocusfromthe previouscontextandisthereferentofallthenullsandreflexives.Notethatthefirst

occurrenceofthereflexiveisnotemphatic,butispartofarelativeclauseandisneeded because ters ish ‘difficultwork’isnotthesubjectintheclausalmodifier(thusthesubject

withinthemodifyingclausemustbeexpressedovertly).Inthisexample,theantecedent

ofthesecondreflexiveisthenullsubjectofthesentenceinitialnonfiniteclause.

Intwocases,emphaticreflexiveshaveextrasententialsubjectantecedents(where

sentenceisassumedtobeequivalenttofiniteclauseandindependentofpunctuation).In

93 theexamplein(62),thereflexive o’ziu occursinthesecondfiniteclauseandiscoreferent

with o,thesubjectofthefiniteclauserepresentedinthefirstline.(Bothformsreferto

Aihalay,themajorparticipantofthetext.)

(62) Geche oi iaman tiushler gëriup chyk"g"an; night3.2 bad dream. PL see. GER exit.3S.PST nechik tiushler ekenin chi o’ziu i de bilmeĭ; what.kind dream. PL COP .ACC EMPH self.3SEMPH know. NEG .3 S.PR nege_tiugiulu'iuiu tiubiunden o'tiup ishge because home.3POSS .GEN from.underpass.by. GER work. DAT barag"anarbalany k"avg"alaryonu i birden–ekiuiatg"an… go. PR .PRT carriage. PL .GEN noise.PL .3 POSS 3.2. ACC suddenly wake.3S.PST ‘Thatnightshe ihadbaddreams;howbad,(she)herself ididn'tevenknow;that’s becausethenoiseofcarriagesgoingtoworkpassingbyunderneath(her)room wokeher isuddenly…’[Aihalay.017]

Thereflexivein(62)isemphatic,bothinthesensethatanullargumentwouldhavebeen sufficientinthiscontextandinthesensethattheform o’ziu isfollowedbytheemphatic particle, de .

In(63)thereflexive o’zler occursinthefinalsentenceoftheexample,andthe

referentisthenarrator’stownmateswhoareinchargeofaparticularfarm.

(63) (Translationofprecedingcontext): ‘Truly,mytownmatesdidn'tagree.Youdon'twanttosayit.Butwhenyoutell aboutthebeginning,howcanyounotreporttheend?Themainreason,why shouldwegive(our)landtooutsiders?Inotherwords,letthegrassgrowthere, butwedon'tgiveittoanyoneelse.’[Agr.041.045] Øi Anglashylmaĭ. be.understand. PASS .NEG .PR ‘Theycan’tbeunderstood.’ 94 O'zler i de k"ollamaĭ, Øi bazharag"anlag”a da bermeĭ. self.3PL EMPH use .NEG .PR know.how. PR .PRT .PL .DAT EMPH give.NEG .PR ‘Theyarenotusingitthemselves,andtheydon'tgiveittothosewhoknowhow.’ [Agr.046.047]

Inthiscasethereferentisinfocus,andtheemphaticuseofthereflexiveformallowsfor thecontrastivefocusbetweenthepeoplewhoarenotusingthelandandthosewhowould liketouseit.Suchpragmaticorcontextualeffectswillbediscussedfurtherinthenext section.

4.2.3. Contextualeffectsofreflexives Inanumberofsyntacticstudies,thelongdistanceuseofreflexivesisassociated withpropertiessuchascontrast,emphaticusage,andlogophoricity(Baker1995,Zribi

Hertz1995,Coleetal.2001).Inclassificationsoflongdistancereflexives,moreover,

Coleetal.pointoutthatlongdistanceboundanaphorsareoftenrestrictedtospecific typesofdiscoursecontexts,suchaslogophoricconstructions(2001:xixxx).WhileI proposethattheKumykform o’ziuisapronominalwhichisrestrictedtoreferentsin focusbutnotrestrictedtocertaintypesofdiscoursecontexts,thecorpusanalysisdoes provideevidencethatdiscoursepropertiesorcontextualeffectssuchaslogophoricityand

imposedsalienceareoftenassociatedwiththisform.Ontheotherhand,therearecases,

suchasthelongdistanceuseofreflexivesinrelativeclauses,wherereflexivesmerely

representobligatoryargumentsandarefreeofaddedcontextualeffects.Inthisanalysis,

Iproposethatinanycasewhereareflexivecanalternatewithanullargument(both

syntacticallyandbecausethereferentisinfocus)theuseofareflexivehasadded

contextualeffects.FirstIdiscussexampleswherethecontrastbetweenthereflexive

95 form o’ziu andotherpossibleforms(e.g.nullsordemonstrativepronouns)emphasizesa

contrastbetweenparticularentitiesreferredtoinatext.Reflexivescanalsobeused

emphaticallyincaseswhichexpresssometypeofunexpectedelement.Finally,previous

researchproposesacorrelationbetweenthelongdistanceuseofreflexivesand

logophoriccontexts,wherelogophoricitycanbedefinedasthepropertyofreferring

expressionwhichsignifiescoreferencewiththespeaker/thinker—forexample,ina

complexclauseinvolvingaverbofspeakingorcognitiveprocess(Coleetal.2001). 59 In

Kumyk,thoughnotalllongdistancereflexivesareassociatedwithlogophoriccontexts,

corpusevidencesuggeststhatlogophoricconstructionsrequiretheuseofreflexive pronouns.

Inthecorpusdatathereareseveralnotablecaseswhereareflexivepossessiveis

repeatedlyusedinacontextwhereanullpossessivepronounwouldbeadequate.Note

that,sincethepersonandnumberofthepossessorisrecoverablefromthepossessive

suffixontheheadnouninapossessivephrase,nullpossessivesareadequateinmany

cases.(Infact,thereareaboutfourtimesasmanynullpossessivesasreflexive possessivesinthecorpus.)Inanumberofsuchcases,theform o’ziu isusedtoexpressa contrastbetweentwoormoreentities.Forexample,intheAihalaytext,Aihalay,a womanwhomakesalivingbyembroideringslippers,hastwopairsofslippers:her everydaypair,whichshewearstowork,andanewintricatelyembroideredpairwhich

59 Coleetal.2001givecredittoHagège1974forcoiningtheterm logophoric .

96 sheonlywearsonspecialoccasions.Oncebothpairsofslippershavebeenintroducedin thetext,continuingreferencestotheeverydaypairdonotcontainovertpossessive reflexives,butanumberofreferencestotheholidayslipperscontainthepossessive marker.Thiscontrastcanbeseeninexample(64).

(64) MaĭnybirindeAĭkhalaĭbirgiĭimmachiĭgematerialbulansavg”atlang”an. Aĭkhalaĭshomachiĭlenibekaryvetip,tiurliuomuzlarvak”yzylsak”tiiandan beshmiuiushliuiuldyzdatutuptikgenedi.Machiĭlerdesengmachiĭler bolg”anmudag”y,k”arapturmag”abirzatlar,o’zliugiundeniuriupgetezhek. BirekizhumalarAĭkhalaĭo’ziuniumachiĭlerin vyxodnoĭgiunlerdesobraniialar bolg”angeziklerde,klubdag”yzhĭynlag”aiadak”onak”laĭbarag”andagiĭip iuriudiu.Munashovak”tide onuishgegiĭegenmachiĭlerini ultanyaĭrylyp k”aldy. ‘OnthefirstofMayAihalayrewardedherselfwithmaterialtomakeslippers. Aihalaymadetheseslippersverybeautifully,embroideringthemwithvarious designsandsewingonaredleatherfivepointedstar.Ifyoucansayitabout slippers,it'sasiftheycouldwalkbythemselves.ForseveralweeksAihalaywore herown( o’ziu )slippers onherdaysoffwhenthereweremeetings,gatheringsin theclub,orwhenshevisitedsomeone.Loandbehold,atthattime,thesole startedcomingoff her( onu )slippers thatsheworetowork .’[Aihalay.0048]

InmanyofthecontextsoftheAihalaytextinwhichreflexivepossessivesare

used,anullargumentwouldhavebeenunambiguouslyinterpreted,asinexample(65).

(65) Ø Iukhlamag"a iatag"anda Aĭkhalaĭ oĭlashdy sleep. INF lie.down. PR .PRT .LOC Aihalay think.3PST wa erten ishge o'ziuniu/Ø baĭram machileri andmorning work. DAT self.3 S.GEN holiday slipper. PL .3 POSS

97 bulan barazhak"g"a Ø tok"tashdi. with go. FUT .DAT decide.3PST ‘Whenshelaydowntosleep,Aihalaythought,andshedecidedthatshewouldgo [towork]inthemorningwithher(veryown)specialoccasionslippers.’ [Aihalay.016]

Insuchcases,theuseofthereflexivewiththespecialslippersincontrastwiththelackof usefortheeverydayslippers,however,producestheeffectofconveyingtheimportance ofthespecialslipperstoAihalayand/ortothenarrativeasawhole.Onecouldalsoclaim thattheuseofthisform,notonlyconveystheimportanceofthereferent,butalso requiresthehearer/readertoviewthisreferentwithempathy.

Inthenarrativecalled“TheProphet’sDaughters”,theprophethasonebiological daughterandthreehouseholdpetswhichbecomedaughtersatalaterpointinthestory.

Thereflexiveisusuallyusedinreferencetothebiologicaldaughtertoemphasizethe contrastbetweenhisrealdaughterandtheonesthatwereformerlyhouseholdanimals.

Theuseofthereflexiveinpossessiveexpressionsreferringtohisowndaughteris notable,asovertpossessivesarerarelyusedinconjunctionwithkinshipterms.

Emphaticsubjectreflexivesarealsousedforcontrastandtoindicate unexpectedness.Inexample(63)intheprevioussection,thereflexivesubjectisusedto contrasttheownersofonefarmwhoarenotusingtheirlandwiththeownersofthe neighboringfarmwhohaveaskedtousethefirstfarm’sland.Thecontrastivefocusin thisutteranceconveysthenarrator’sfeelingsofsurpriseandlackofunderstandingofthe firstfarmowners’decisionnottoleasetheirland.

98 Inexample(66),areflexiveintheexpression o’ziumdebilmeĭ‘notknowing myself’isusedtoconveytheunexpectednessthatthespeakerwouldnotknowwhathe himselfisdoing.

(66) O’zium i de bilmeĭ, men i bir k”yzny self.1 S EMPH know. NEG .3S.PR 1S onegirl’s betine tiklenip turg”anman. face. DAT stare. GER remain.1S.PST ‘Notevenrealizingitmyself,Ikeptstaringatonegirl’sface.’[Marian.037]

Inanothercase,foundinanewspaperobituary,areflexiveisusedtoconveythe

unexpectednessofthedeceased’suntimelydeath,asdisplayedin(67).

(67) Buĭylny avgust aĭyny 31nde iuvuk"ubuz 3.1 year.GEN August month.3POSS .GEN 31th near.one.1PL .POSS Karimulla Dag'irovg"a i 45ĭyl bitme gerek edi. Karimulla Dagirovga 45year finish. INF must3. PST .AUX ‘OnAugust31stofthisyear,ourdearfriendKarimullaDagirovkhashouldhave been45yearsold.’ Tek ol i o'ziuniu i sho chak"ynaetishmedi. but 3.2self.3S.GEN 3.3 amount. DAT arrive. INF .3 PST ‘Buthedidn'treachthisage(ofhis).’[Obituary.001.002]

Theform o’ziuniu ,whichreferstoKarimullaDagirov,isusedasagenitivemodifierof shochak”yna ‘thisage’.Since,grammatically, shochak”yna ‘thisage’wouldhavebeen

sufficient,theadditionofthereflexivemodifierisinterpretedashavinganemphatic

function—probablyassociatedwiththeshockofhisuntimelydeath.

Finally,inmanylanguages,aparticularsetofpronounsisusedforlogophoric

referencewhichmaybedistinctfromothertypesofthirdpersonreference.Forexample,

99 inKumyk,inasentencewhichtranslates,‘Zuchrathoughtthateveryonewouldlaughat her,’asshownin(68)(basedonthecorpusexampleshownin(54)),theuseoftheform o’ziu in(a)isinterpretedascoreferentwithZuchra,whiletheuseoftheform og”ar in

(b)isinterpretedasnotcoreferentwithZuchra.

(68) a. Zukhrag”a i bary_da_g’alk o'ziune i k"arap kiulegendir Zuchra everyone self.3S.DAT look. GEN laugh. PR .PRT .COP dep esine geldi. COMP mind.3 POSS .DAT come.PST ‘Zuchrathoughtthateveryonewouldlaughather(Zuchra).’ b. Zukhrag”a i bary_da_g’alk og”ar j/*i k"arap kiulegendir Zuchra everyone 3S.DAT look. GEN laugh. PR .PRT .SPEC dep esine geldi. COMP mind.3 POSS .DAT come. PST ‘Zuchrathoughtthateveryonewouldlaughather(notZuchra).’

Thecorpusdataincludestwocasesofthelogophoricuseofreflexivessimilarto

example(68),oneofwhichoccursinanindirectspeechclause,andtheotherofwhich

occurswithinsemidirectspeech(cf.examples(54)and(60)respectively).Inthese

cases,thechoiceofareflexiveasopposedtoanovertnonreflexivepronounseemstobe

relatedtocoreferentialityrestrictionsspecifictologophoriccontextsand,therefore,does

nothavecontextualeffectsbeyondthatofprovidingtheinformationneededto

disambiguatethereferent.

4.3. The pronoun bu in the corpus

Thecorpusanalysisof bu isbasedon33tokens,31ofwhichhavereferentswhich

couldbecodedforcognitivestatus.Thetwoundeterminedcasesarebothcasesof 100 cataphoricreference.Ofthe31codedreferents,30arecodedasinfocus,withthe remainingreferentbeingcodedasatmostactivated.Theonereferentcodedasactivated alsohappenstobetheonlycodedreferentwhichisintroducedbyaverbphrase(cf. section4.3.4).Withonlyoneactivatedreferent,representing3.2%ofitsreferents,the form bu isanalyzedassignalinginfocus.

Someothernoteworthycharacteristicsofthecorpusdistributionof bu are1)that itsoccurrenceissomewhathigherinoralversuswrittentexts(14casesin27pagesof oraltextversus19casesin72pagesofwrittentext,2)thatthemajorityofreferentsof bu arehumans,3)thatthemajoritybutnotanoverwhelmingpercentageofreferentsof bu arepreviouslymentionedassyntacticsubjects,and4)thattheuseof bu with clausallyintroducedentitiesisrareandlimitedtocasesofcataphoricreference.The frequencyof bu inoralversuswrittentextsisaninterestingphenomenon,butonewhich doesnotfallwithinthedirectscopeofthisstudy.Thefrequencyofhumanversusnon humanreferentsof bu mostlikelyreflectsthefrequencyofhumansversusothertypesof entitiesasparticipantsintextsingeneralratherthananytypeofrestrictionortendency relatedtotheuseofthespecificform, bu .Insupportofthisconclusion,thelistofnon humanreferentsof bu includesavarietyoftypesofinanimateentities:villages,farm, tree,meetings,time,speech,slander,hypocrisy,thusthisfactorisnotdiscussedfurtherin thisstudy.Thedataon bu showsthatathirdofreferentswerenotpreviouslymentioned assubjects,confirmingthatsubjecthoodoftheantecedentisnottheprimaryfactor affectingthedistributionofthisform.Ofthefourgeneralizationsmentionedabove,the lastseemstobethemostsignificant.Theform bu refersonlythreetimestoanentitynot

101 introducedbyanominal.Oneofthesecasesisanactivityintroducedbyaverbphrase thatispartofaclausalargument.Theothertwoarecasesofcataphoricreferenceto speechsuchastheexpression, bulaĭaĭtyvlar ‘thiskindofsaying’withtheparticular

featurethatthespeechitselfisintroducedbyanounphrase.Theapparentrestrictiveness

of bu inrelationtoentitiesnotintroducedbynominalsmeritsfurtherdiscussion.

4.3.1. Codingthereferentsof bu Thecodingofthemajorityofreferentsof bu isstraightforward,andtheprimary purposeofthissectionistoillustratethecodingandtoprovideexamplesofanimateand inanimatereferents.In(69),anexcerptfromthebeginningofanoralnarrative,the referentof munu (accusative/genitiveformof bu )isahumannamedLayla.

(69) Laĭla degen kumuk k"yz ulan tapdi. Laylasay. PST .PRT Kumyk girl son give.birth.3PST ‘AKumykgirlcalledLaylagavebirthtoason.’ Bir zhuma getip o bolnitsadan chyk"dy. oneweek leave. GER 3.2 hospital. ABL exit.3PST ‘Oneweekhavingpassed,sheleftthehospital.’ Bolnitsany aldina munu k"arshylama hospital. GEN before.2POSS .DAT 3.1. ACC meet. INF munu absunlary k"aĭynk"yzlary geldi. 3.1S.GEN sister.in.law. PL .3 POSS sister.in.law. PL .3 POSS come.3PST ‘Hersistersinlaw(brother'swives)andsistersinlaw(husband'ssisters)cameto meetherinfrontofthehospital.’ [Birth1.0013]

Previousreferencestothisparticipant(inbold)occurinthefirstandsecondsentences.

Atthetimeofthefirstoccurrenceof munu inthethirdsentence,thereferentiscodedas infocus(eitherbycriterion1,asitwasmentionedasthesubjectoftheprevioussentence orbycriterion5,asitismentionedinthetwoprecedingclauses).Atthetimeofthe 102 seconduseof munu ,thereferentisclearlystillinfocusbycriteria1and5oralsoby criterion2(mentionedearlierinthesamesentence).

Example(70)illustratesreferencetoaninanimateentity—inthiscase,acharacter trait.

(70) Adamny aldynda bir tiurliu, Øartynda bir man.GEN front.3. POSS .LOC one various back.3. POSS .LOC one tiurliu sëĭleĭgen gishige munafik” dep aĭtyla. various say.PR .PRT person.DAT faith.betrayer say. PRT be.called.PR ‘Apersonwhosaysonethinginfrontofapersonandanotherbehindissaidtobe abetrayeroffaith.’ Munafik”leni g’ech_kimni iag”ynda k”yĭmaty ikhtibary faith.betrayer. PL noone on.the.part.ofworth.3POSS trust.3POSS bolmas. be. NEG .FUT ‘Betrayersoffaithdon'thaveworth,trustinanyone'seyes.’ G’ak”yllybalalar bulaĭ iamank”ylyk”danbek wise young.ones.PL 3.1. MOD bad trait.ABL much sak”lanmag”a tiĭishli. keep. INF suitable ‘Wisechildrendeservetobeprotectedfromabadcharactertraitlikethis.’ [Character.01.03]

Inthisexample,thecharactertraitisintroducedinthefirstsentencebythedescription thatisgiventhelabel, munafik” ,then munafik”is repeatedinthesecondsentence.By

thetimeofitsmentioninthethirdsentence,therefore,thereferentisinfocusbycriterion

5.

103 4.3.2. Discussionofundetermined/cataphoracases Thetwouncodedreferentsof bu arecasesofcataphora.Likebackwards

anaphora,cataphoricreferencesarecasesinwhichthereferentofapronouncannotbe

resolvedbecausethereisnomentionofthereferentinthecontext(linearly)precedingthe

occurrenceofthepronoun,but,rather,thereferentofthepronounisovertlymentionedin

thecontextfollowingthepronoun.Cataphoraislessspecificthanbackwardsanaphora,

whichisrestrictedtoparticularsyntacticcontexts(cf.section4.1.2).In(71)the

expression bulaĭaĭtyvlar ‘thiskindofsaying’ referstothecontentofthequotedspeech

whichfollows.

(71) Milletlenig’alklanyarasynda bulaĭ aĭtyvlarbar: nationspeoples among 3.1. MOD sayingsexist.PR “o’ziuniu milletin g’alkyn siuiup self. GEN nation.3POSS .ACC people.3POSS .ACC love.GER bazharmaĭg”anadamlar o’zge milletleni de know.how. NEG .PRT man.PL other nation.PL .ACC EMPH g’alklany da siuiup bazharmas”. people.PL .ACC EMPH love.GER know.how. NEG .FUT ‘Amongthenationsandpeoplesthereisasayinglikethis:"Menwhodon't knowhowtolovetheirownnationandpeoplewillnotknowhowtoloveother nationsandpeople.’[Editorial.07(alsoshownasex.(50)]

Itisinterestingthatbothcasesofcataphorainvolveexpressionswhichintroducespeech

withanounphrasethatreferstoaspeechact( bulaĭaĭtylar ‘sayingslikethis’and bulaĭ

zhawap‘ananswerlikethis’).Thecontextualuseofcataphora,particularlytointroduce

speechisdescribedbyLevinsohnasameansof‘highlighting’orimposingprominence

104 (2008:82).Interestingly,inthiscorpus, bu istheonlyovertpronounthatisused cataphorically.

4.3.3. Discussionofexamplesthatweredifficulttocode Inoneparticulartext,severaltokensofthepronoun bu aredifficulttocode becauseofcontextualfactorsrelatedtothemethodofelicitation.Theelicitationofthis textinvolvedaprocessofshowingthespeakeraseriesofpicturesinawordlesspicture book.Thespeakernarratedthestorywhilelookingateachpictureinturn,thusthe referentswerewithintheeyegazeofthespeakerandhearerwhenevertheywere portrayedinthatparticularpicture(orpageofpictures).Whilethecodingcriteriaforan activatedreferentrefertoeyegazeviacriterion2,“Itissomethingintheimmediate spatiotemporalcontextthatisactivatedbymeansofasimultaneousgestureoreyegaze,” thiscontextualfactorisonlymentionedasameansofactivatingareferent,andtherole ofeyegazeincontinuingreferencetoanactivatedparticipantisnotdiscussedinthe codingprotocol.AccordingtoJeanetteGundel(personalcommunication),however,a pictureofanactivatedreferentcanbeconsideredtheequivalentofanovertmentionof thereferent(oranimplicitmentionofahigherleveltopic),andIthereforechooseto codeentitiesasinfocusiftheyappearinmorethanonecontiguouspicture.Example

(72)illustratestwotokensof bu whichcanbeexplainedinconnectionwithpictures whichareinterpretedasmentionsofareferent.

(72) Ø suratlag”a k"arag"anda bu iangy ĭylny aldyndag”y picture.PL .DAT look.PR .PRT .LOC 3.1new year. GEN in.front.of. MOD giunlerIsany tuvg"an giuniu bulan baĭlavlu day.PL Jesus.GEN be.born.PST .PRT day.3POSS with connected. ADJ 105 baĭramlany vak"tisi. holiday.PL .GEN time.3POSS ‘Lookingatthepictures,thisisthetimebeforeNewYear's,thetimeofthe holidayconnectedwithJesus'birth(Christmas).’ [inthepicturethewomaniswalkingonastreetdecoratedforChristmas] Adamlar baĭramlag”a g'azirlik gëriup man.PL holiday.PL .DAT ready.NOM see.GER aĭlanag"any gëriune. go.around.PR .PRT .3 POSS appear.3PR ‘Peopleappeartobegoingaroundasifgettingreadyfortheholidays.’ [inthepicturethewomanisonthesideofthestreetplayingtheaccordianwhile peoplearepassingbywithChristmastreesandpackages] Song bu tiukengegire. then 3.1 store.DAT enter.3PR ‘Laterthisone(she)goesintoastore.’[WordlessBook.021.022] [inthepicturethewomanisgoingintoastore]

Inthisnarrative,anoldwomanistheVIPparticipant,andintheseriesofpictureswith

whichthistextwaselicited,thewomanappearsinallbutonepicture(theexceptionisa

landscapepictureearlyinthestory—thoughtherearepicturesofthewomanonthesame page).Thefirsttokenof bu inexample(72)occursinthefirstsentence,anditsreferent

isnotthewoman,but,rather,thepictureitself.Sincethepictureisovertlymentioned

earlierinthesentence,thisreferentcanbecodedasinfocuswithoutreferencetothefact

thatthepictureiswithintheeyegazeofthespeakerandhearer.Forthesecondtokenof

bu ,however,itisnecessarytorefertotheroleofthepictureasamentionofthereferent.

Thesecondtokenof bu referstothewomanwhoistheVIPparticipant.Sheisnot

overtlymentionedinthepreviousthreesentences,butsheiswithintheeyegazeof

speakerandhearerviathepicturesand,therefore,codedasinfocus.

106 4.3.4. Anactivatedentitynotintroducedbyanominal Theform bu onlyhasoneinstanceinthecorpuswherethereferentiscodedasat mostactivated,andthiscasealsohappenstobetheonlycasewhere bu referstoanentity introducedpreviouslybyasyntacticunitotherthananominal.Inthecaseshownin(73), thereferentof bu is ëk”zatnysëĭlemekge‘sayingsomethingthatdoesnotexist’,an activitythatisintroducedbyaverbphrasewithinanominalclauseinthedativecase.

(73) G’ibat degen iaman zat bolsa da slander say.PST .PTCP bad thing be. COND EMPH Ø gishini artyndan bar zatny sëĭlemekdir. person.GEN back.3POSS .ABL exist thing say. INF .COP ‘Althoughslanderisbad,itistosaybehindsomeone’sbacksomethingthat exists.’ Ëk” zatny sëĭlemekge – bug’tan dep aĭtyla. exist. NEG thing.ACC say.INF .DAT – defamation COMP be.said.3 PST ‘Sayingsomethingthatdoesnotexistiscalleddefamation.’[Literally,‘(The word) bug’tan issaidto[sayingsomethingthatdoesnotexist.’] Bu busa g’ibatdan da iaman. this however slander. ABL EMPH bad ‘Thisisworsethanslander.’ [Character.015.017]

Whileactivitiesintroducedbyverbphrasesaretreatedasinfocus,Iapplythisonlyin caseswheretheactivityisintroducedbythefiniteverbphraseofthematrixclauseorasa subjectcomplementofthematrixclause.Since,inthiscase,theactivityisintroducedby averbphraseembeddedinanonsubjectnominalclause,Icodetheentityasactivated; however,itcouldpotentiallybeinfocusbasedonthefactthatitis,semantically,the mostprominentargumentoftheclause.Consideringthatthisistheonlyreferentof bu notintroducedbyanounphrase,itissignificantthattheverbphrasewhichintroducesthe

107 referentinvolvesaformofnominalization,whichisevidentfromthenonfiniteverb formandthedativecasesuffixinsëĭlemekge(‘say’+participialsuffix+dativesuffix).

Thisis,perhaps,anindicationthattheuseof bu withclausallyintroducedentities(or entitiesintroducedbyaverbphrase)isrestrictedtocasesinwhichthereissomedegree ofnominalization. 60

4.3.5. Contextualeffectsassociatedwith bu Basedonthecorpusanalysis,anumberofinterestingcontextualeffectsare potentiallyassociatedwiththeform bu .Section4.3.2abovediscussesitscataphoricuse

asahighlightingdevice.Othercontextualeffectsassociatedwith bu includesignalingan

establishedtopicatapointoftopicswitchanduseinelaboration.

Inmanycasesinthecorpus,theuseof bucorrelateswithanestablishedtopic,or

thethematicreferentforaunitoftext,andanumberoftheseinvolvecontinuing

referencetoapreviousestablishedtopic(subject)incasesoftopicshift(subjectshift).

Asshowninanumberofcorpusexamples,areferent(infocus)mentionedinsubject position(astopic)inonesentenceislikelytobementionedasanullargumentifit

continuestobementionedinsubjectposition,butnotlikelytobementionedasanull

argumentifitoccursinanotherargumentpositioninasentenceinwhichanewsubject

(topic)isintroduced.Insuchsituations,thepronoun bu seemstobethepreferredformof

60 Inthetwouncodedcasesofcataphora,oneofwhichisdisplayedasexample(71), bu referstoaspeech actintroducedbyafollowingclause;however,ineachofthesecases,thespeechactitselfisfirst introducedbyanominal bulaĭaĭtyvlarbar ‘thereisasayinglikethis’and bulaĭzhawapbergen ‘gavean answerlikethis’.Thisfactcanbeinterpretedassupportingtheideathattheuseof bu withclausally introducedentitiesrequiressomedegreeofnominalization.

108 overtreference.InGHmodelterms, bu isoftenusedtorefertoanestablishedentityin focusinanutteranceinwhichanewentityisbroughtintofocus.

Thetext“IAmGuiltyMarian”providesaclearexampleofthecontrastinguseof bu incasesoftopicshiftasopposedtotheuseofnullsforcontinuingtopics.Inthistext, theauthorwritesaboutthreetreeswhichstandguardoverthethreesidesofthevillage.

Eachtree,inturn,isthetopicofaparagraphwhichgivesfurtherdetailsaboutthenature ofthattree.Intheparagraphsdescribingthefirsttwotrees,displayedasexample(41)in

Section4.1.1,overtnounphrasesareusedininitialreferencetothetrees,whilenull argumentsareusedforcontinuingsubjectreference.Thetextexcerptaboutthethirdtree, displayedasexample(74),likewise,beginswithaninitialnounphrasereference tëbendegisi ‘thelowerone’(mentionedassubjectofthematrixclause),followedbythree nullsubjectreferencesinthenextthreematrixclauses.

(74) Tëbendegisibusaiurtg”aiakhshylyk”bulangiregenlegek”ychak” iaĭypbashekgen, ‘Thelowerone,however,lowersitshead,openingitsarmstothoseenteringthe villagewithgoodintentions; Ønamartkhyialbulangelegenlegek”ashtiugiulk”opaĭg”an. ‘tothoseenteringwithevilthoughts,doesn’tblinkaneye.’ Øiurtdangetegenulanlag”aiakhshyëletgen, ‘Totheyoungmenleavingthevillage,wishesgoodtravels;’ Ø61 gëriunmeĭgenbolg”unchaalg”yshaĭtyp,Øartyndank”arag”an. ‘until(they)disappear,sayingpraises,lookingafterthem.’

61 Thisisareferencetotheyoungmenleavingthevillage,notthetree.

109 Saparg”achyg”ag”anlar bug”arsavboletgen, ‘Thoseleavingonajourneysaidgoodbyetoit,’ k”aĭtag”anlarØsalambergen,sorashg”an. ‘thosereturninggreeted(it).’ Iurtdangetegendeiurekgesyĭynmaĭg”ank”aĭg”y,ichimtolg”andertbulanmen de munu tiubiunegeldim. ‘WhenIleftthevillage,inmydistress,myheartfilledwithsorrow,Ialsocameto thefootofit.’

Inthefifthfiniteclause,however,anewsubjectsaparg”achyg”ag”anlar ‘departing

ones’isintroduced,whilethetreecontinuesitsstatusasareferentinfocus.Inthis

sentence,theform bug”ar referstothetree.Inthefollowingfiniteclause,anothernew

subject k”aĭtag”anlar‘returningones’ isintroduced,butanullargumentisusedforthe

treebecauseitismentionedinthesameargumentpositionasthepreviousclause.Inthe

nextsentenceanothernewsubjectisintroduced—men ,referringtothefirstperson

narrator—andthetreeisagainreferredtowiththegenitiveformof bu ( munu )—probably becauseitoccursinadifferentnonsubjectargumentposition.Thislineisparticularly

interesting,asthefirstpersonnarratorofthenovelisintroducedinconnectionwithhis

comingtothefootofthistreeatthetimeofhisdeparturefromthevillage.Onemay

assume,therefore,thatthethirdtreehasparticularsaliencefortheauthorandthe

narrative,andthatatleastthesecondinstanceof bu (ifnotboth)couldalsobeinterpreted

asameansofimposinghigherrelativesalienceonthethirdtreecomparedtotheother

treesmentionedinthepreviouscontext.

Anothercasewhere bu appearstobeassociatedwithtopicshiftisillustratedin

example(69)above.Inthiscase,areferentismentionedbytheovertpronoun ointhe

110 subjectpositionofoneclause,thenbytheaccusativeformof bu ( munu )inanonsubject positioninthefollowingclause.Inasimilarcaseshownin(75),thetopicofthefirst clause,thatis,thegirlthatisthereferentof k”yzny ,isnotthesyntacticsubject,butisin animportantsyntacticpositionasananimatepossessor(PoesioandNissim2001).

(75) K"yznyerge bermeiaraĭg"an vak"tisi gelip girl. ACC husband. DAT give. INF possible. PST .PRT time.3. POSS come. GER bug"ar gelechi gele. 3.1. DAT one.who.requests.engagement come. PR ‘Whenitcomestimeforthedaughtertobegiveninmarriage,apersonrequesting engagementcomestoher.’[Prophet.0002]

Inthesecondclauseofthissentence,thesubjectis gelechi ‘suitor’,andthegirlis mentionedby bug”ar ,adativepronoun.Thisexampleisparticularlyinteresting,asthe topicshiftoccursbetweenanadverbialclauseandmatrixclauseofthesamesentence.

Therearealsocaseswheretheuseof bu isassociatedwithamoregeneralized formoftopicshift,asinthevariouschangesoflocationin“TheProphet’sDaughters”.

Eachanimal/daughter’shomeisalocalhigherleveltopic,andashiftoflocationusually takestheformofanutteranceinwhich bu isusedtorefertothelocationwhichhasbeen thecenterofattentionandeitheranotherformofdemonstrativeoranounphraseisused torefertothenewlocation.Example(76)illustratestheuseof bu inacontextwitha changeinlocation.(ThetextassociatedwiththefirstlocationispresentedasanEnglish translation,whilethetextcontainingtheactualpronominalformsisfullypresented.)

(76) ‘(He)beginsbygoingtothedonkey.Intheevening,havingeatenandtaken drink,beginningtheconversationaboutthisandthat,theprophetaskshissonin lawabouthislifesituation. 111 ‘Soninlaw:“Everything’sokay,butonethingisbad–enoughtimehaspassed toteachananimal.Whetheryousaysomethingtoherornot,it’sthesame;very peacefulinattitudetowardswork.”“Adonkeywillnotknowhowtowork,”the prophetthought.’ Geche Ø bularda k"onak" da bolup erten night 3.1. PL .LOC guest thusbe. GER morning itine Ø bara. dog.3POSS .DAT go.3PR ‘Stayingthenightasaguestattheirplace,inthemorning(he)goestothedog.’ Shunda da sholaĭ gech bolg”uncha 3.4. LOC EMPH 3.3. ADV late become.before. GER iashav g'aldan Ø Ø sorashyp… life situation. ABL greet. GER ‘Atthatplace,thus(asbefore),beforeitgotlate,(he)asksaboutthesituationof life,…’[Prophet.001723]

Inthefinalpartofthistext,thelocationthathasbeenthetopicofthissection,thatis,the donkeydaughter’shome,isreferredtowiththepronoun bularda ,whilethenewlocation, thedogdaughter’shomeisreferredtowiththepronoun shunda .

Likethereflexive,theuseofthepronoun bu issometimesassociatedwith emphaticprominence—inparticularemphaticprominencethatisassociatedwith unexpectedness. InKumyk,emphaticprominenceisoftenmarkedbytheparticle de/da .

Insomecases,suchastheoneillustratedinexample(77),theuseoftheform bularda

(plurallocativeform)combineswiththeemphaticparticle da toexpressunexpectedness orsurprise.

(77) Tiurliutiurliumilletler iashaĭg”an shag”arlarda iurtlarda different. INT nation. PL live. PR .PRT city. PL .LOC village. PL .LOC rus til asluernitutma bashlag”an,sholardag”y Russiantongue importantplace. ACC take. INF begin. PST 3PL .LOC .ADJ 112 o’siup gelegen naslu ana tiliniartyiurtu grow.up. GER come. PR .PRT generation mother tongue.ACC mishmash iada birdok”dabilmeĭgen bolup bara. ornot.at.all know. NEG .PST .PRT be. GER go.3PR ‘Incitiesandvillagesinwhichmanydifferentnationslive,theRussianlanguage hasbeguntotakeanimportantplace;thegenerationgrowingupin 3PL has beguntoknowthemothertonguepoorlyornotatall.’ Iangyz bir milletnivakilleri iashaĭg”aniurtlardada only one nation. GEN member.PL .3 POSS live. PR .PRT village. PL .LOC EMPH g’al onchak”ymaktardaĭtiugiul,nege_tiugiulde bularda da state so.much better NEG because EMPH 3.PL .LOC EMPH rus tilnietegen ta”siri bashg”a az tiugiul. Russian tongue. GEN do. PR .PRT influence other little not ‘Thestateinvillagesinwhichmembersofonlyonenationliveisnotmuchbetter, becauseeveninthese,theinfluenceoftheRussianlanguageisnotlittle.’ [MotherTongue.012.013]

Inthecaseofthisexample,thespeakerassumesthatthehearer/readerexpectsRussianto beusedasalinguafrancainvillageswheremorethanonelocallanguageisspoken,but

expectsminimaluseofRussianinvillagesthatarepredominantlyKumyk.Theuseof bu

withtheemphaticparticletorefertovillagesthatarepredominantlyKumykcoincides

withtheexpressionofafactthatiscountertoexpectations:thattheinfluenceofRussian

issignificantinvillagesthatarepredominatelyKumyk.

Finally,anotherfunctionwhichseemstobeassociatedprimarilywiththeform bu

iselaboration.Inelaboration,thespeakerisgivingmoredetailedinformationaboutthe

topicratherthanactuallyaddinginformation/continuingthenarrative.Oneexampleis

illustratedin(78),wherethepronoun bu referstothepeoplegatheredatthecelebration

113 andintroducesfurtherdetailsabouttheactualcategoriesofrelativeswhocometosuch celebrations.

(78) Baĭramg"a zhyĭylg"an g’alk këbiusiu g'alda feast. DAT be.gathered. PST .PRT people most situation. LOC bu k"atyngishiler, k"yzny kardashlary, ulanny kardashlary, 3.1 woman. PL girl. GEN relative. PL .3 POSS son. GEN relative. PL .3 POSS bular bari_daiashg"a uzak" o'miur 3.1. PL all child. DAT distant lifetime nasipli yashavëraĭ. fortunate life wish.3PR ‘Thepeoplegatheredforthecelebration,usuallythesearewomen,thegirl’s relatives,theboy’srelatives,allofthesewishthechild‘longlife’,‘happylife’.’ [Birth1.018]

Asinthecaseoftopicshift,theuseof bu seemstomarkunexpectedness.Ratherthanthe

unexpectednessofparticipantchange,theunexpectednessinthiscaseisthefactthatthe

narrativeisbeinginterruptedbytheadditionofmorespecificinformationaboutthetopic.

Callingsuchcases‘renewal’ofthetopic,Levinsohnprimarilyassociatesthiswiththe

introductionofbackgroundmaterial(2008:45). 62

4.4. The pronoun o in the corpus

Oisthemostcommonlyoccurringovertpronounandoccurs90timesinthe corpus.Thereferentsof oincludebothanimateandinanimateentities,aswellasabstract

62 ThoughIcreditLevinsohnfortheoriginalidea,Iclaimthatelaborationisasubsetofbackground informationandthattheform bu isspecificallyassociatedwiththissubset,whileotherovertpronounsare alsoassociatedwiththeintroductionofothertypesofbackgroundinformation.

114 entitiessuchastimesandfacts.Asamatteroffact,approximately25%ofreferentsare inanimateentities.

Outof90tokensinthecorpus,thereferentsofallbutoneoftheseformscouldbe codedforcognitivestatus.Theonereferentwhichisundeterminedisanindirectly constructedentityandisdiscussedinsection4.4.2.Ofthe89referentscoded,64arein focus,24areatmostactivated,andoneiscodedasatmostfamiliar.Sincethenumberof atmostfamiliarcasesisonly1.1%ofthetotalcasescoded,Ihypothesizethat osignals thatthereferentisactivatedandtheonecaseinwhichthereferentiscodedasfamiliar

(1.1%ofreferentscodedforthisform)iswithintheproposedrangeofpossible exceptions(<5.0%).

4.4.1. Codingreferentsof o Thissectionprovidessomesampletokensof ointhecorpusandthecodingofthe referentsintheseexamples.Theexcerptin(79)contains3occurrencesof o–oneinthe secondsentenceandtwointhethirdsentence.

(79) Ibrag'im girp geldi. Abraham enter. GER come.3 S.PST ‘Abrahamcamein.’ Onu k"olunda bir sumka bar. 3.2. GEN arm.3POSS .LOC one suitcase exist.PR ‘Thereisasuitcaseinhishand.’ Ondan o bir zatlar boshatdy. 3.2.ABL 3.2 one thing.PL empty.3S.PST ‘Fromitheemptiedsomethings.’[Video1.003]

115 Inthisexample,thefirsttoken, onu ,referstoAbraham,whoismentionedasthesubject oftheprevioussentenceand,thus,isinfocusbycriterion1.Justoveronethird(34/89) ofthereferentsof ointhecorpusareinfocusbycriterion1.Thesecondtoken, ondan ,

referstothesuitcase/bag,whichismentionedinasyntacticfocuspositionoftheprevious

sentence—theprecopularpositionofanexistentialsentence—andisinfocusbycriterion

3.63 Thethirdtoken, o,referstoAbrahamagain.Thoughthisentityisnotmentionedina

subjectorfocuspositionintheprevioussentence,itismentionedineachofthe

immediatelyprecedingclauses,and,thus,isinfocusbycriterion5.

Inanumberofcases,includingthetextportionrepresentedin(80),areferentof o

ismentionedintheprecedingsentencebutnotinoneofthetwopositionsautomatically

codedasinfocus—thatis,asthesubjectorinfocusposition.

(80) Ishge erli segiz kilometr ël bar; work.DAT untileight kilometerroad exist.PR onda akhsham giunbatg”ynchag”aØ turmag"ada gerek… 3.2.LOC evening sununtil.setting stay.INF .DAT EMPH necessary ‘Itwasnearly8kilometerstowork,anditwouldbenecessarytostaythereuntil thesunset,buttheslipperswereinastateofdisrepair.’[Aihalay.014]

Ifthereferentisnotmentionedinthepreprevioussentence,thenitmustbecodedas activated(criterion1).Inexample(80),thereferentoftheform onda ‘atthat(place)’is theplaceofwork,whichismentionedforthefirsttimeintheprevioussentenceby ishge

63 Notethewordorderdifferencefromthecodingprotocol(precopularversuspostcopular).Asistypical forSOVlanguages,inKumykthefocalconstituentinanexistentialsentenceispreverbal.

116 ‘towork’,butnotasthesubjectorinfocusposition,and,thus,itsreferentiscodedas activated. 64

Oneinterestingcaseinvolvesapluralreferentthatiscodedasactivated.The pluralentityrepresentingtwochildrenismentionedovertlyasapluralpronounatone pointinthetext.Thisisfollowedbysixsentencesinwhichthechildrenarenot

mentionedasapluralentity,butoneofthetwochildrenismentionedineachofthe

sentencesofthisspan.Asforthecasesmentionedinsection4.1.3onnullarguments,I

considerthat,oncethepluralreferenthasbeenactivated,thementionofanindividual

memberofthesetmaintainsthepluralentityasatleastactivated,ifnotinfocus;

however,thisaspectofpluralsisnotspecifiedinthecodingprotocol.

4.4.2. Discussionofnoteworthycases Ofthe90referentsof o,oneisnoteworthybecauseitiscodedasatleastfamiliar butdoesnotmeetanycriteriaforbeingactivatedfromthecodingprotocol,andoneis

undeterminedbecauseitisonlyindirectlyconstructedfromanentityovertlymentioned

inthecorpus.Thetextexcerptshownin(81)illustratestheundeterminedcaseandthe

casecodedasfamiliar(lines(i)and(j),respectively).

(81) a. Shogiundensongmenonuesetegenboldum. b. Songokhuvuntergepdek”aradym: c. baryzatdandërtbeshk”yĭmatbulaniuriuĭ,

64 Thefactthat ish combineswiththedative/directionalmorpheme–ge constitutesanovertrefererencetoa place.Thereissomedegreeofuncertainty,however,aboutwhetherornotatimeorplaceassociatedwith aneventisactivatedorbroughtintofocusbythementionofanevent.

117 d. teknetesen,ruschaiazyvdanosaledi. e. Bibliotekg”abardym– f. cheberkitaplardaokhumaĭeken. g. Klassda olaĭlardag”ydabolg”an. h. Shosaĭalyda,vozhatyĭbulandaoĭlashyp,spor etmetok”tashdyk”… i. Bizerten, olar busatiushdensongokhuĭedi. j. Shosaĭalydamenchyg”ag”anda ol geliptiupdetabulaedi. k. Esimdebar:Mar’iankoridornybag”anasynadataianypturag”anedi. a.‘Fromthatdayforward,Irememberedher. b.Lateron,Ilookedatmygrades c.everythingwasgoingabout45, d.but,whattodo….Russianlanguagewaslookingbad. e.Iwenttothelibrary f.–itwastoreadliterature. g.Intheclass,therewereotherslikethat. h.Forthisreason,Italkedtotheleaderandwedecidedtoformagroup… i.Westudiedinthemorning,andtheystudiedintheafternoon. j.Becauseofthat,whenIwasleaving,shewascomingin. k.Inmymind,Marianstoodoutamongthemanyinthecorridor.’ [Marian.040.049]

Theentityforwhichcodingisundeterminedisrelatedtotwospecificentitiesmentioned

overtlyinlines(g)and(h)ofthetext: klassda ,referringtothenarrator’sclassand sbor ,

referringtoagroupofstudentshavingtroublewithRussianwriting/literature.The

referentof olar inline(i),however,refersneithertothewholeclassnortotheremedial

group,buttothestudentsintheclasswhowerenotpartoftheremedialgroup.This

entityisapluralentitywhichmustbeconstructedbysubtractingcertainmembersofthe

setofindividualsintheclass.Thoughsuchanentityisnotspecificallymentionedinthe

text,itisacceptabletorefertoitwithapronoun.Sincetheclassasawholehasalready

beenmentioned,thereconstructionofthegroupseemstofunctioninasimilarwaytoan

118 activatedentity,allowingforpronominalreference.Notethat,thisissomewhatsimilar toacaseinwhichapluralentityisindirectlyintroducedviathementionoftwoormore individuals,whichIalsoconsidertobeanactivatedentity.Thoughthecodingof indirectlyintroducedentitiesofthistypeisnotspecificallymentionedinthecoding protocol,Iwouldconsiderthisentitytobeactivated.

Theonereferentof ocodedasfamiliaroccurswithinthesametextinreferenceto

Marian,agirlintheclasswhomthenarratorisparticularlyinterestedin.(Asamatterof fact,thewholeintroductorysectionoftextisaboutthemomentwhenhefirstsaw

Marian.)Thetextportionin(81)beginswiththewords, shogiundensongmenonues etegenboldum ‘fromthatdayforwardIrememberedher’.Apronounisinline(j),even thoughMarianhasnotbeenmentionedovertlyinthepreviouseightfiniteclauses.This seemstobeanexceptionaluseofaformthatsignals‘activated’whichislikelyusedfora particularliteraryeffect—thatis,toportraythecognitiveprominencethatMarianhasfor thenarrator.

4.4.3. Referencetoclausallyintroducedentities Thepronoun oisusedfivetimesinthecorpusinreferencetoanentityintroduced byaclause.Infourcases, oreferstoafactorpropositionintroducedbythepreceding clause(s),whichiscodedasatmostactivated.Twoofthesecases(onefactandone proposition)areillustratedin(82)and(83).

(82) Iashbolnitsadan chyk”g”an songbirzhumanyichinde kaĭnana childhospital. ABL exit. PST .PRT afteroneweek. GEN inside.of mother.in.law takhanaetip adamlanychak”yrmabayrametmesuĭe. takana make. GER man.3PL .ACC invite.INF feastdo.INF want.PR 119 O saĭalybu chak”yryvlarĭiberipk”yzny 3.2 Saccording.to3.1 S invitation.PL send.GER girl. GEN adamlaryn o’ziuniuadamlaryn chak”yra. man. PL .3 POSS .ACC self.3POSS .GEN man.PL .3 POSS .ACC invite.PR ‘Afterthechildleavesthehospital,withinoneweek,themotherinlawwantsto holdafeast,makingtaxanaandinvitingpeople.Becauseofthis,shesends invitationsandinvitesdaughter(inlaw)’speople,(self’s)ownpeople.’ [Birth.008]

(83) Gelezhekde Karimulla iuvug”ubuznu ullu ishleni come. FUT .LOC Karimulla near.one.1PL .POSS .GEN great work.3PL .POSS bashyn tutup gërmegeØ umut ete edik. beginning.3POSS .ACC hold. GER see. INF hope do. PR 1P.PST .AUX ‘Inthetimetocome,wehopethatourdearKarimulla’sgreatworkswillbeseen fromthebeginning(leadingtheway?).’ Olsavlaĭg’alkny bashyn tutma da 3Swholepeople. GEN head. 3POSS .ACC hold. INF EMPH bazharazhak" edi. know.how. FUT 3. PST .AUX ‘Hewillleadallthepeople.’ Og"ar bir de sheklik etmeĭbiz. 3.2. DAT one EMPH doubt do. NEG .3 P.PR ‘Wedon’tdoubtit.’ [Obituary.015.017]

In(82), o,whichoccursinthesecondsentence,referstothefactthatthemotherinlaw wantstoinvitepeopletoacelebration.In(83),og”ar ,thedativeformofthepronounis anargumentoftheverb shekliketmeĭbiz ‘wedonotdoubt’.Becausedoubtingrelatesto truthconditions,thecomplementoftheverbisconsideredtobeaproposition—the propositionthatKarimullawillleadallthepeople(intheafterlife)ingoodworks.

120 Inonecase,theform oisusedtorefertoafactintroducedbymultiplesentences,

whichisalsocodedasactivated.Inthefollowingexample,presentedintranslation,the pronoun ooccursintheexpressionondank”aĭryda ‘besidesthis’.

(84) ‘Invillageswhererepresentativesofonlyonenationlive,thesituationisnotso praiseworthy,becausetherealsotheinfluenceoftheRussianlanguageisnoless. Invillageschoolschildrenstudythemothertongueonlyinthebeginninggrades; inuppergrades,however,allcoursesarecarriedoutinRussian;mothertongue languageandliteratureareonlytaughtasseparatesubjects.Thenumberofstudy hoursgiventotheselessonsisevensometimesreduced.Besidesthis( ondan k”aĭryda ),inthepresentage,justlikeeverywhere,invillagestheinfluenceof televisionandotherinstruments/formsoftechnologyonthepeople,especiallythe youngergeneration,ispowerful.’[MotherTongue.012015]

Inthisexample,thefacttowhich ondan refersisconsideredtobethefact(inferredfrom

fourconsecutivefiniteclauses)thatRussianhasabiginfluenceineducationbecause

mothertongueeducationislimitedtotheearlygradesandRussianisusedintheupper

gradesforallsubjectsexceptmothertonguelanguageandliterature,whichcan

sometimeshavereducedhours.

Thelastcaseinwhichareferentof oisintroducedbyaclauseisacaseinwhich o referstoatimeassociatedwithastatethathasbeenmentionedintheprevioussentence, asinthelastclauseofexample(85).65

(85) Ish iak"dan alg"anda da Karimulla bazharyvly work side. ABL take.PST .PRT .LOC and Karimullasuccessful

65 Whileaneventorstateisconsideredtobeintroduceddirectlybyaclause,thetimeoftheevent/statemay beintroducedindirectlyandcouldbeconsideredtobeatmostactivated.

121 ulanedi. son3. PST .AUX ‘Withregardtowork,Karimullawasasuccessfulman.’ Ol on ĭyldanda artyk"Babaiurt raĭonsudnu 3S ten year.ABL and moreBabayurtregioncourt.GEN predsedateli bolup, song busa Ø Mag'achk"ala chairman.3POSS be. GER later however Makhachkala shag'arnyLenin raĭonsud'iasu bolup ishledi; city. GEN Lenin region judge.3POSS be.GER work.3PST ondan alda Øsegiz ĭyllar prokuraturadazhavaplu 3.2. ABL beforehand eight year.PL prosecutor.LOC answer.MOD k"ulluk"larda ishledi. service.PL .LOC work.3PST ‘FormorethantenyearsheservedaschairmanoftheBabayurtcourt;laterhe servedasajudgeforMakhachkala'sLeninregion;beforethathehadworkedfor eightyearsinaresponsiblepositioninthepublicprosecutor'soffice.’ [Obituary.012.013]

Itisnoteworthythat oisusedmuchlessfrequentlythan sho forentities

introducedindirectlybyclauses;however,basedonthecorpus,itisdifficulttodetermine

anydifferencesinthedistributionofthesetwoformswithclausallyintroducedentities.

Thefactthat oisnotusedatallinreferencetoaneventorstateinthecorpusis

consideredtobeduetotheinfrequencyofsuchtypesofreferencesindiscourserather

thanarestriction,since ocanbeusedwithclausallyintroducedentitiesthathavealower

cognitivestatus.

4.5. The pronoun sho in the corpus

Thedistributionof sho inrelationtofeaturesotherthancognitivestatus,as

summarizedinTable6,meritssomediscussion.Thisstudyidentifies33casesof

122 pronominal sho withinthetextcorpus.Inallthesecases,thereferentsarecodedaseither activatedorinfocus,withthelattercategorymuchlessfrequent.Onlyfourtokensare referencestoentitieswhicharecodedasinfocus,anditisnoteworthythatonlyoneof thesereferstoanentitymentionedasthesubjectoftheprevioussentence(oneofthe codingcriteriaforinfocus).

Amongthereferentsof sho ,23casesareentitiesintroducedbyaclauseorverb phrase,whiletencasesarereferencestoentitiesintroducedbyanominal.Ofthelatter category,onlytworefertohumansandbothofthesereferencesaretogroups,not individuals.Inthesecases,thegroupsare‘Kumykreaders’and‘minoritypeoples’(cf.

(88)).Thoughnoreferentsof sho arehumanindividuals, sho doesrefertoinanimate individualsinthecorpus.Ofthenonhumanreferentsintroducedbynominals,fourare individualentitiesandfourarepluralentities. 66 Sincethedistributionof sho intermsof

entitytypeisunique,thediscussionofcodingisdividedaccordingtoentitiesintroduced bynominals,whetherinfocusoractivated(section4.5.1)versusentitiesintroducedby

clausesandVPs,allofwhichareactivated(section4.5.2).

66 Underpluralentities,Iincludeonecasewhereasingularformof sho referstoanamountwhichcanbe construedasacompositeentityconstructedindirectlyfromaplural:667hectares.(seeByron).

123 Human Human Animate Animate Inanimate Inanimate Introduced singular plural (non (non singular plural byclause human) human) orVP singular plural 2 4 4 23

Table6: Typesofreferentsofsho

4.5.1. Referentsof sho introducedbynominals Ofthetenreferentsof sho introducedbynominals,fourareinfocusandsixareat mostactivated.Examples(86)and(87)illustratetwostraightforwardcasesinwhichthe referentsareinfocus.In(86)shonu referstoanewspaperarticlewhichisintroducedin theprevioussentenceasthesubjectofthepassiveverb jerleshdirilgen ‘islocated’—that is,theextendednounphrase(markedbybrackets)ofwhich makalasyisthehead.

(86) Ëldashgazetni buĭyl 19nchuIanvarda chyk”g”an nomerinde friendnewpaper.GEN 3.1year19th January. LOC go.out.PST .PRT edition.LOC [K”yzyljurtshag’ardaniazag”an muxbir IzmullaG’adzhievi Khasilyurt city.ABL write. PR .PRT correspondentIzmullaGadjiyev’s “g’alk”ybyzny aburun ër_etmeĭik” degen mak”alasy] people.1PL .GEN regard.3POSS .ACC lose.NEG .1 PL .IMP say. PST .PRT article.3POSS ërleshdirilgen . be.found.3PST ‘Intheeditionof"TheFriend"newspaperpublishedonJanuary19thofthisyear, isfoundanarticlecalled,"Letusnotloseregardforourpeople"bythe correspondent,IzmullahGadjiev,whowritesfromKhasilyurt.’ Menshonu tergevliu kiuide okhudum. 1S 3.3. ACC careful way.LOC read.1S.PST ‘Ireadthiscarefully.’[Editorial.0102]

124 Sincethenewspaperarticleismentionedinsubjectpositionintheprevioussentence,itis infocusbycriterion1.Thisistheonlycaseinthecorpusinwhich sho referstoanentity

introducedasthesyntacticsubject.

In(87)theformofinterestisthepronoun sho whichoccursinthelastlineofthe example(notthedeterminer sho inthethirdline).

(87) Bu kaĭtyp Ø ibadatkhanag"a gire. 3.1 return. GER church. DAT enter.PR ‘Onthewayback,shegoesintothechurch.’ Ø Ibadatkhanag"agirip gelegende church.DAT enter.GER come.PR .PRT .LOC sho 67 onu k"olundan akchany 3.3 3.2. GEN hand.3POSS .ABL money. ACC alyp getgen iash mototsikly bulang”y take. GER leave.PST .PRT child motorcycle.3POSS with. MOD shonu ichinden chyg”yp gele. 3.3. GEN inside.3PSS .ABL go.out. GER come. PR ‘Assheisgoingintothechurch,thekidwhotookthemoneyoutofherhandthe onewiththemotorcycleiscomingfrominside(thechurch).’ [WordlessBook.02930]

Inthisexample sho referstoaplace:achurch.Thechurchismentionedintwo

consecutivesentencesbytheform ibadatkhanag"a ,whichbringsitintofocuseven

thoughitisnotmentionedinaprominentsyntacticpositionintheprevioussentence.

67 Sho isanalyzedasadeterminerinthisphrase,whichtranslatesas‘thatkidwhotookthemoneyfromher hand’.

125 Intwonotablecasesinwhich sho referstoanentityinfocus,theformisused withinanounphrasetorefertoanentitymentionedwithinthesamenounphrase.Oneof theseisillustratedin(88),wheres holany referstothereferentof azg’alklany ‘minority peoples’intheexpression, sholanyarasindakumuklanyda ‘amongtheseare’.

(88) Artdag”y vak”tilerde sanav iak”dan az g’alklany previous. LOC .MOD time. PL .LOC number side. ABL minority people. PL .GEN (sholany arasynda Kumuklany da) tilleri (3.3. PL .GEN midst.3POSS .LOC Kumyk. PL .GEN EMPH language.3PL .POSS avur k”ysmatg”a taryp tura. heavy fate. DAT undergo. GER stay. PR ‘Inrecenttimes,intermsofnumbers,minoritylanguages(amongthemKumyk) havebeensubjecttoa“heavy”fate.’[MotherTongue.09]

Inthiscase sho isusedinanexpressionwhichgivesanexampleofonememberofaset.

Thoughthisissimilartoelaboration,Imakeadistinctionherebetweenexemplification andelaboration.

Examples(89)and(90)illustratetwoofthesixactivatedentitiesintroducedby nominals.In(89)thereferentof shonda isthenounphrase o’zleniu’ĭundeLinda iashlarynyg’ak”yndaalg”anbirgichirekkinoĭimikzatni ‘somethinglikeasmallmovie thatLindahadtakenintheirhomeofthekids’.

(89) Paul mag”a kompiuterdeo'zleni u'ĭunde Linda Paul 1S.DAT computer. LOC self.PL .GEN home.3POSS .LOC Linda iashlaryny g'ak"ynda alg"an birgichchirek kino child.PL .3 POSS .ACC about take.PST .PRT one tiny movie

126 ĭimikzatni gërsetdi. likething.ACC show.3PST ‘Onhiscomputer,PaulshowedsomethinglikeasmallmoviethatLindahad takenintheirhomeof/aboutthekids.’ Shonda Paulnu da Lindanyda avletleri Ibrag'imni 3.3. LOC Paul. GEN and Linda.GEN and child.PL .3POSS Abraham. GEN deKatiany da g'ak"ynda gërsetilgen. andKaty. GEN andabout be.shown.3PST ‘There(inthevideo),(something)wasshownaboutPaulandLinda'schildren, AbrahamandKaty.’[Video2.001.002]

Inthiscase shonda referstothemovie,mentionedintheprevioussentence.In(90)the referentof sho istheamountrepresentedby 667hectares .Inthiscase,theamountisa compositesingularentitywhichisindirectlyconstructedfromthepluralnounphrase 667 hectares .

(90) Bu ĭyl munda667 gektarda giuzliuklege k"ulluk"etildi. 3.1 year 3.1. LOC 667 hectare. LOC autumn. PL .DAT servicebe.done.3PST ‘Thisyear667hectaresofautumnones(wheat)wereservicedhere.’ Sho da getgen ĭyl chachylg"anyndan 3.3 EMPH leave.PST .PRT year be.scattered.PST .PRT .3POSS .ABL ese 150 gektarg"aartyk"bola. than150 hectare. DAT morebe.PR ‘Thatis150hectaresmorethanwhatwasplantedlastyear.’ [Agr.014.015]

Inanothercase,shownin(91)alocation—inthiscaseastore—ismentioned

directlyinatext,thennotmentioneddirectlyinthenexttwosentences.However,the

127 eventdenotedbythesecondtolastsentenceindirectlyintroducesthelocationofthe event,and,therefore,thestoreisatleastactivated. 68

(91) Song bu tiukengegire. Then 3.1store.DAT enter.PR ‘Laterthisone(she)goesintoastore.’ Ø Tiukenchi bulan sëĭleĭ. storekeeper with speak. PR ‘(She)talkstotheshopkeeper.’ Ø Tiukenchige birzatlar… 69 store.keeper.DAT onething.PL ak”chalar berip, ØØ arg"anyn sata. money.PL give.GER accordion.3POSS .ACC sell.PR ‘Totheshopkeepersomething…givesmoney…(she)sells(him)heraccordion.’ Ø Satyp Ø ak”chany da sell.GER money.ACC and alyp, Ø shondan chyg”yp bara. take. GER 3.3. ABL go.out. GER go. PR ‘Havingsoldit,takingthemoney,shegoesout(ofthere).’ [WordlessBook.023.026]

4.5.2. Referencetoentitiesnotintroducedbynominals OfallthepronominalformsinKumyk, sho istheformmostoftenusedwith entitiesintroducedbyclausesandverbphrases.Asamatteroffact,23outof33or

69.7%ofitsreferentsareentitiesintroduceddirectlyorindirectlybyclauses/VPs—as

68 Thementionof tiukenchi ‘storekeeper’twotimescouldalsobeconsideredtobeanindirectmentionof thestore,inwhichcaseonemightcodethereferentof shondan asinfocus;however,Ichooseamore conservativeinterpretationofthecodinghere. 69 Thisisinterpretedasarepair,wheretherespondentsaysbirzatlar‘somethings’,thenrealizesthatthisis asaleandmoneyisprobablybeingexchanged;however,thespeakerdoesnotcorrectthetexttosaythat theshopkeeperratherthanthewomanisgivingthemoney.

128 comparedto5outof90(5.6%)for o,1outof31(3.2%)forbu (restricted)and1outof

221(.4%)fornulls(restricted).Interestingly,thepercentageofKumykreferentsnot

introducedbynominalsfor sho issimilartopercentagesreportedfortheEnglish

demonstratives this and that inpreviousresearch:72%inGundel,Hedberg,and

Zacharski2002/2005and82.1%inHedberg,Gundel,andZacharski2007:33) 70 .

Themajorityofclausallyintroducedreferentsof sho arefactsorpropositions,as illustratedin(92)and(93).Inexample(92)sho occursintheexpression sho saialy

‘therefore,becauseofthat’andreferstothefactmentionedinthepreviousclause:the

factthatAihalay’ssquadwasinacompetitionwithUmazhat’ssquad.

(92) Aĭkhalĭini zvenosu Umuzhatny zvenosu Aihalay.GEN squad.3POSS Umuzhat.GEN squad.3POSS bulaniaryshg"a chyk"g"anedi. withcompetition.DAT exit.PST 3. PST .AUX ‘Aihalay'ssquadwasinacompetitionwithUmazhat'ssquad.’ Sho saialy da ol erten tangdan turup 3.3 therefore EMPH 3.2morningdawn.ABL get.up.GER Ø k"apusta ornatmak" uchun barmag"a gerek edi. cabbageplant. INF in.order.to go. INF necessary3. PST .AUX ‘Thereforeshehadtogetupinthemorningandgoplantcabbage.’ [Aihalay.012013]

Amongcorpusexamplesofparticularinterest,thereisonecaseinwhich sho

referstoapropositionwhichistheconclusiondeducedfromasetofpremisesexpressed

70 Thelatterfigureonlyincludesactivatedentitiesnotintroducedbynominals,astherewerenotentitiesin focusnonintroducedbynominalsreportedinthiscorpus.

129 inaseriesofpropositions.Theillustrationin(93)showsthesentenceswhichexpressthe premises(intheformofanEnglishtranslation)followedbythesentenceexpressingthe deductioninwhichtheform sholaĭoccursaspartoftheexpression sholaĭbolg”ansong

‘itfollowsthat’.

(93) ‘Literatureisoneoftheareasofthefinearts.Eachareaofthearts,however,has materialswhichbelongtoit.Forexample,ifthematerialsofpaintingarepaints, literature’s(materials)wouldbewords.’ Sholaĭ bolg”an song, adabiiat tuvradantuvra g’alk”ny tili 3.3. MOD be. PST .PRT afterliteraturealong.with people. GEN language bulan sëz baĭlyg”y bulan baĭlavlu. with word richness with connected ‘Itfollowsthatliterature,alongwiththelanguageofthepeople,isdirectly connectedwithitsrichnessofvocabulary.’[MotherTongue.04.07]

Anotherinterestingcategoryofreferentsnotintroducedbynominalsinvolves othertypesofentitiesreferredtobytheform sholaĭ.Kumykpronounsmarkedbythe suffix –laĭserveasmodifiersofbothnounsandverbs,andcanrefertoentitiesofvarious types,includingindividuals,activities,andstates,aswellascertainfeaturesof individuals,activitiesandstates.Inseveralcasesinthiscorpusinwhich sholaĭmodifies averbornominalizedverb,thereferentisdefinedastheentityintroducedbyan adverbialconstituentintheprevioussentence.Forexample,in(94)thefirsttokenof sholaĭ(whichmodifiesthenominalizedverb biĭivniu‘dancing’)referstoanentity introducedbytheembeddedadverbialclausemodifierof biĭimek ‘dance’: biribiribulan k”uchak”lashyp ‘huggingoneanother’.

130 (94) Masala busurmandinde ulanda kyzda biribiri bulan for.exampleMuslim religion. LOC boyandgirl andone.anotherwith k”uchak”lashyp biĭimek geriurula. hugging.each.other dance.INF be.forbidden.PR ‘Forexample,intheMuslimreligionitisforbiddenforaboyandagirltodance huggingoneanother.’ Meno’zium de sholaĭ biĭivniu bekershi gëremen. 1S self.1 S EMPH 3.3. MOD dance. NOM .ACC veryugly consider.1PR ‘I,myself,consideritveryugly(inpoortaste)todancelikethat.’ Ulanda k”yzda birge biĭiĭgende, ulank”olunzamanzaman boyandgirl andtogetherdance. PR .PRT .LOC boyhand.3POSS time.to.time k”yzny boĭnunatiĭdirip biĭiĭgen gezikler de k”arshylasha, girl. GEN neck touchingdance. PR .PRT occasion.PL EMPH come.across.PR sholaĭulannymenlap osal k”ylyk”lyulandepg’isap_etemen. suchboy1S veryweak character. MOD boyCOMP consider.1PR ‘Onecomesacrossoccasionswhenaboyandgirldancetogether,thatfromtime totimetheboy'shandtouchesthegirl'sneck;Iconsiderthatkindofboytobea boyofveryweakcharacter.’[Editorial.021.023]

AsHedbergetal.(2007:34)suggestthatanentityintroducedbyaclausalunitotherthan thematrixclauseshouldnotbecodedasinfocus,Iassumethattheentityintroducedby theadverbialclauseisnotinfocus.

Thesecondtokenof sholaĭin(94),whichoccursinthephrase, sholaĭulan ‘aboy

likethat’,referstotheboyintroducedintheparticipialphrase, ulank”olunzamanzaman

k”yznyboĭnunatiĭdiripbiĭiĭgen ‘aboydancingwith(his)handfromtimetotime

touchingthegirl’sneck’whichmodifiesthenoun gezikler ‘times’.Forthiscase,the referentisconsideredtobe‘aboywhodanceswithhishandtouchingthegirl’sneck’,an individualentitywhichisindirectlyconstructedfromtheinterpretationoftheparticipial phrase(ratherthananevent,state,fact,orproposition).

131 Onetypeofclausallyintroducedentitywhichislessfrequentlymentionedinthe literatureisasituation.AccordingtoHegarty(2007),asituationisacomplexentity whichinvolveseventsandstatestogetherwiththeirconsequences(2007:171).The exampleshownin(95)hasapronominalformwhichreferstoasituation.

(95) Bu ĭyl da khoziaĭstvosunu avlak"chylary 1ming 3.1yearEMPH farm.GEN field.worker.PL .3 POSS 1thousand tonnag"a iuvuk" urluk"satmag"a khyĭal ete. ton. DAT near seedsell.INF thoughtdo.3PR ‘Thisyearfarmworkersplantosellnearlyonethousandtonsofseed.’ Demek sholluk”da anadash khoziaĭstvosunu ekonomikasyn say.INF 3.3.means mother.tongue farm.3POSS .GEN economy.3POSS .ACC dag"y_da bekleshdirmeumutlu. even.more strengthen.INF hope.MOD ‘Thatis,bysuchmeans,itishopedthatthelocal/mothertonguefarmeconomy willbestrengthenedevenmore.’[Agriculture.021022]

Theform sholluk”da inthisexampleisaderivationalformof sho whichcontainsthe

nominalaffix luk” (primarilyusedtoformabstractnouns)andislexicallydefinedas‘by

thismeans’.Thereferentinthiscaseisthesituationthatwouldresultfromthefarm

workerssellingathousandtonsofseed.FollowingHedberg,Gundel,andZacharski

(2007:35),Icodethisreferentasactivated.

Inthediscussionofclausallyintroducedentities,itisworthnotingthatthecorpus

datacontainsnoclearcasesinwhich sho referstoaneventorstatedirectlyintroducedby

aprevioussentence,afactwhichismostlikelyrelatedtothenaturalinfrequencyofthis

referenttyperatherthanarestrictionforaparticularform.

132 4.6. The pronoun shu in the corpus

Thepronoun shu israreinthecorpusandoccursonlythreetimes,twoofwhich

areexcludedfromthestudyduetothefactthattheyoccurwithindirectspeech. 71 The

oneexampleanalyzed,whichisdisplayedin(96),hasareferentcodedasatmost

activated.

(96) GecheØ bularda k"onak"da bolup erten night3.1. PL .LOC guest EMPH be. GER morning itine Øbara. dog.3POSS .DAT go.3PR ‘Stayingthenightasaguestattheirplace,inthemorning[he]goestothedog.’ Shunda da sholaĭ gechbolg”uncha 3.4. LOC EMPH 3.3. MOD late become.before. GER yashav g'aldan Ø Ø sorashyp life situation. ABL greet. GER gieviu de bug"arkant ete… son.in.law.3POSS EMPH 3.1. DAT complaintdo. PR ‘Atthatplace,thus(asbefore),beforeitgotlate,(he)asksaboutthesituationof life,andhissoninlawanswershim…’[Prophet.002223(cf.(76))]

Inthisexamplethereferentof shunda (literally,‘atthat’)isunderstoodasthedog.One

canthinkofthiscolloquiallyas“atthedog’splace”.Sincethereferentofthepronounis

mentionedpreviouslybytheform itine ‘tothedog’,anonsubjectargumentinthe

previoussentence,itiscodedasactivated.Thoughitisnotreasonabletomakea

71 Theexampleswhichoccurwithindirectspeechoccurinoneparticularspeechportion,displayedas sentences.0041.0046inAppendix3. 73 Inprinciple, shu couldbetestedinthesameway,butthemorefundamentalquestion,giventhe infrequencyofthisform,isitsgeneralacceptability.

133 hypothesisaboutwhatstatus shu signalsbasedononeform,itismorelikelythatthis formsignals‘activated’ratherthan‘infocus’.

Aninterestingpointnotdirectlyrelatedtotheanalysisinthischapteristheway

inwhichthecontrastbetween bularda(literally,‘atthose’)inthefirstsentenceof(96)

and shundainthesecondsentenceisusedtoexpressthemovementofthemain participant(theprophet)fromonelocationtothenext–thatis,fromthehomeofthe

donkeydaughterandherhusband(accordingtopreviouscontext)tothehomeofthedog

daughter(andherhusband).Itisinterestingtolookathowthiscontrastcorrespondsto

descriptionsofthedemonstrativesintermsofdeicticmeaningdescribedintermsof

distanceandknownness(cf.Chapter3). AccordingtoDimitriev1940andMuratchaeva

2001, bu representssomethingthatisbothnearandknown,while shu represents

somethingthatisnearandnotknown(presumably,totheaddressee,thoughthisisnot

specifiedbyMuratchaeva),sothisdistinctionisnotdefinedintermsofdistancefromthe

deicticcenter,butratherbytheparameterofknownness.Inthisexample,thedog

cannotbedescribedasunknown,sinceitismentionedintheprevioussentence.Ifone

thinksofthecontrastintermsofcognitivestatus,however,thedifferencebetweenthe

tworeferentsisonebetweenareferentinfocus(thedonkeydaughterandherhusband)

andareferentthatisactivated,butnotnecessarilyinfocus(thedogdaughter).

4.7. Summary and interpretation of corpus analysis

4.7.1. Formstatuscorrelations:Wheretogofromhere Thecorpusanalysisindicatesthattheovertpronoun bu ,nullpronounsand pronominalreflexivesallsignal“infocus”status,whilethepronouns, oand sho areboth

134 associatedwiththestatus“activated”.Whilethecorpusprovidesinsufficientdataonthe form shu ,thefactthattheonereferentcodedinthiscorpusisactivatedsuggeststhat shu ismorelikelytofallwithinthegroupofformssignalingactivation.

Itisimportanttokeepinmindtheneedtocomparehypothesesaboutformstatus

correlationsresultingfromcorpusanalysiswithhypothesesresultingfromtheanalysisof

questionnairedata.Morespecifically,fortwooftheformshypothesizedtosignal‘in

focus’, o’ziu andnullarguments,additionaltestingshouldrevealwhetherthesmall

numbersintheloweststatusareexceptionsoranindicationthattheformactuallysignals

thelowerstatus.Also,giventherelativelylownumberoftokensof bu (33)in

comparisonto o(91)andnulls(229),thefactthatallbutonereferentisinfocuscould

representlimitsinthecorpusratherthanconstraintsontheuseoftheform.(Thesame principlewouldapplyto o’ziu ,whichhas36tokens,butislesslikelytoapplytonulls,

with229tokens.)

Thesmallnumberofreferentsinfocusfor sho –incomparisontothedistribution

ofinfocusversusactivatedentitiesfor o–isaninterestingaspectofthedistributionof pronominals.Animportantresultofformstatuscorrelationsistheideathat,following principlesofRelevanceandGrice’sMaxim’sofQuantity(cf.Chapter2,section3)form

statuscorrelationsareusedtocreatescalarimplicatures.Morespecifically,theuseofa

formwhichsignalsthatthereferentisatleastactivatedmayalsobeusedtoimplicatethat

thereferentisatmostactivated.(Thisisenoughtoexplainthedistributionof sho —and

comparabletoEnglishdataon this/that fromGundeletal.1993.)Thedataon sho are

consistentwithastrongscalarimplicature,‘notinfocus’,asthereareonlyfourinfocus

135 referents;however,since sho isnotusedtorefertoindividualhumansinthecorpus,this couldbeanissueoftypeofentityratherthancognitivestatus.Thequestionnairetests whetherrestrictionson sho arerelatedtocognitivestatusissuesaloneoralsoto restrictionsontypesofentities(e.g.that sho cannotrefertoindividualhumanentities).

Also,eventhoughtheunidirectionalentailmentofformsintheGHmodelpredictsthat therewillnotbeaformthatsignals‘notinfocus’(justasnolanguagehasaformthat means‘notall’)thesmallnumberofreferentsof sho thatareinfocus(4tokens)suggests thatthiscouldbeapossiblecounterexampleandshouldbeinvestigatedfurther.

4.7.2. Otherdistributionalfeatures Inanalyzingthedistributionofreferentsinagivencorpus,itbecomesevidentthat certaintypesofreferentsaremorefrequentthanothers.Inparticular,insomegenres, certaintypesofreferentsaremorecommonthanothers.Forexample,innarrativetexts, individualanimatereferents(particularlyhumans)arementionedmorefrequentlythan inanimateentitiesandpluralentities.Inexpositorytexts,ontheotherhand,higherorder entitiessuchasfactsandpropositionsaremorecommonthaninothertypesoftexts.Any analysisofthedistributionofreferringformsshouldtakeintoconsiderationthe underlying(in)frequencyofcertaintypesofreferents,andfurtherresearchshouldinvolve expandedanalysisofinfrequentreferenttypes.Forexample,thefactthatagivenform referstoanimateentitiesmorefrequentlythaninanimateentitiesismorelikelyto representtheunderlyingfrequencyofentitytypesratherthananyparticularcorrelation betweenformandentitytype.Onenotablefeatureofthedistributionofreferentsinthis

Kumykcorpusisthatthereisonlyoneclearreferencetoaclausallyintroducedeventor

136 state(whichisreferredtobyanullargument),thusitisdifficulttodetermineifthereisa distinctioninthedistributionofformsbetweenreferencetoentitiesintroduceddirectly byclauses(whichareassumedtobeinfocus)andthoseintroducedindirectly(whichare assumedtobeactivated).

Withthatcaveatinmind,thedistributionofpronominalsinthiscorpusshowsa

numberofparticularfeaturesotherthanthoserelatedtothecognitivestatusofreferents.

Themostobviousdistributionalfactissimplytherelativefrequencyofforms.Nullsare

themostfrequent,followedby o, o’ziu , bu/sho ,and shu .Theoverallfrequencyof o

comparedto bu , sho ,and shu isinteresting,anditsrelativefrequencymaybethereason

somegrammaticaldescriptionslabel oasapersonalpronoun,butnot bu , sho and shu .

Thefactthatthecorpusincludesonlyonetokenof shu raisesaproblemfortheanalysis

andsuggeststhatthisformmayevenbedisappearingfromthelexiconofsomespeakers.

Oneinterestingfactrelatedtofrequencyisthatahigherpercentageoftokensof bu occur

inoralasopposedtowrittentexts,eventhoughtheamountoforalversuswrittenmaterial

inthecorpusissmaller.Itcouldbeinterestingtodetermineifthisfrequencyinanyway

correlateswiththeuseofvisualrepresentationsofthereferentthroughpicturesandvideo

insomeoraltexts.Amoreinterestingquestioniswhetherinplannedtextstheform bu

wouldbemorelikelytobereplacedbynullsorbythepronoun o.Answeringthis

questioncouldhelptodeterminewhy bu ismorefrequentinunplannedoraltexts.

Myanalysisshowsseveralinterestingaspectsofthedistributionofpronounsin

relationtoentitytype—thatisbothintermsofanimacyfeaturesandthetypesofentities

introducedbyfullclauses(events,states,facts,etc.)andVPs(activities).First,nullsand

137 bu refertoentitiesthatarebothanimateandinanimate,butforeachform,thereisonly onecaseofreferencetoanentitynotintroducedbyanominal.Inthecaseof bu , moreover,theoneentityofthistypeisanactivityintroducedbyaverbphraseexhibiting ahighdegreeofnominalization(cf.4.3.4).Thereflexiveform o’ziu isevenmore restricted,referringonlytoentitiesintroducedbynominals,whetheranimateor inanimate.Thepronoun oissimilarindistributiontonullsand bu intermsoflackof restrictionsrelatedtoanimacyfeatures.Thereare,however,apparentdifferencesinthe degreeofrestrictivenesswithentitiesnotintroducedbynominals.Whilethefive referentsof onotintroducedbynominalscompriseonlyaslightlyhigherpercentageof thetotalnumberofcodedreferentsthanthelessfrequentform bu (5.6%for o versus3.2

%for bu ),thelinguisticformswhichintroducethereferentsof odonotexhibit

nominalization,whiletheoneverbphrasewhichintroducesareferentof bu exhibitsa

degreeofnominalization.Theform sho isuniqueinthesensethatthemajorityofits

referentsareclausallyintroducedentities,andonlytwoofitsreferentsarehuman.As bothofthehumanreferentsaregroups,itfollowsthat sho isnotusedinreferenceto

individualhumans,thoughitdoesrefertoindividualinanimateentities.

Afinalquestionwhichisdiscussedbecauseithasbeenraisedinanumberof

studiesofdemonstrativesiswhetherornotthedistributionofanyformcanbedescribed

intermsofrestrictiontoareferentpreviouslymentionedasasubjectorareferentnot

mentionedpreviouslyasasubject(KaiserandTrueswell2004,Bosch,KatzandUmbach

2007).Thecorpusanalysisindicatesthatnoformisrestrictedtousewithaprevious

subject,thoughformssuchasnullsand o’ziu haveahighpercentageofreferentsthatare

138 previouslymentionedinsubjectposition.Theform sho ,ontheotherhand,isusedwith referentsthatarenotpreviouslymentionedassubjectsinallbutonecase.

4.7.3. Evidenceofcontextualeffects Whiletheprimaryeffectsofthechoiceofpronominalformdiscussedinmy analysisisthesignalingofwhere/howtosearchforpotentialreferentsintheattention stateandtheeffectsofscalarimplicatures,myinterpretationofthedistributionof pronominalsalsosuggeststhatcertainformsareintentionallyusedbyaspeakertocreate othercontextualeffects.Forexample,thedataindicatethatonlynullsand o’ziu are associatedwithbackwardsanaphoraand,amongovertnonreflexivepronouns,only bu is associatedwithcataphora.Iproposedthatsomecasesofbackwardsanaphoraand cataphoraproducecontextualeffectsrelatedtoimposedsalience,suchas inmedia resor

highlightingtheimportanceoftheinformationinrelationtootherinformation

components.Inothercases,backwardsanaphoraappearstobeassociatedwiththe

orderingofinformationforthepurposeoftextualcoherence.Examplesofcontextual

effectsassociatedwith o’ziu includetheexpressionoflogophoricity,contrast,and

imposedrelativesalience.Theform bu isassociatedwithcontinuedreferencetohighly

salientparticipantsincontextswhichinvolvesometypeofcontrastsuchastopicshiftor

changeoflocation.Finally,certainpronounsappeartobeassociatedwithcertaintypes

ofrelationshipsbetweenunitsofinformation.Namely, bu isusedincontextsinvolving

elaboration,while sho isusedincontextswhichinvolveexemplification.

139 4.7.4. Methodologicalissues Thecorpusanalysisinthisstudyraisesanumberofmethodologicalissuesthat

meritfurtherdiscussion.Fourareasworthyofmentionare1)factorsthatarelikelyto bringareferentintofocusinKumykthatarenotmentionedinthecodingprotocol,2)the

codingofpluralentities,3)theroleofeyegazeandothercontextualfeaturesincoding

forcognitivestatus,and4)variousissuesrelatedtothecodingofclausallyintroduced

entities.

ThecriteriaoftheprotocolusedincodingstatusesontheGivennessHierarchy

areminimalcriteria,andevidencefromthecorpusanalysissuggestsseveraladditional

factorsthatarelikelytobringareferentintofocus.Firstofall,theuseofnullarguments

forreferentswhicharementionedforthefirsttimeintheprevioussentencebutnotasthe

syntacticsubjectorsyntacticfocussupportstheideathatcriteriarelatedtothesyntactic prominenceofmentionintheprevioussentencecanbemorebroadlydefined—anidea

whichhasalreadybeenincorporatedinmorerecentversionsofthecodingprotocol

(Gundel2006).InKumyk,inparticular,thereisevidencethatthefollowingtypesof

syntacticprominencearelikelytobringareferentintofocus:topicalization,mentionasa possessorinapossessiveclause,and,asdiscussedinChapter2,mentionasthenon subjectagentofapsychologicalverb.

Anothercriterionassociatedwithbringingareferentintofocusisparallelism.

Thefactthatnullargumentsareacceptableincaseswherethereareparallelsinargument structureoreventstructuresuggeststhatthedegreeofexpectednessaffectsthecognitive statusofareferent.Inotherwords,themoreexpectedanactivatedreferentisdueto textualpatterns,themorelikelyitistobebroughtintofocus.Inthemoreuniquecasesof 140 parallelismthatIcallscriptrepetition,parallelismcombineswiththementionofseveral differentmembersofthesamereferentclass(e.g.thefirstsuitor,thesecondsuitor,etc.) tocreateadegreeofexpectednessthatevenallowsforanulltobeusedwithareferent thatwouldotherwisebecodedasatmostfamiliar.

Achallengingareaofcodinginthisstudyistheconsistenttreatmentofplural entities.Ononehand,thecodingofapluralentitythatismentionedasapluralnominal isjustlikethecodingofanynominallyintroducedentity—thatis,suchanentityisat leastactivatedandcanbecodedasinfocusifitismentionedinanyofthecontextswhich bringanynominallyintroducedentityintofocus(e.g.subjectorfocusposition,mention intwoconsecutiveclauses,higherleveltopic).Anaspectofanalysisthatisless straightforward(andnotspecificallymentionedinthe2004codingprotocol),however,is theroleofthementionofanindividualmemberofasetindeterminingcognitivestatus ofthepluralentity.Inthisstudy,Icodeapluralentityasatmostactivatedifithasbeen mentionedonceasapluralnominalinapositionthatdoesnotautomaticallybringitinto focus,andthenonememberofthesetismentionedagaininthefollowingclause(s); however,itisequallylikelythatsuchacontextwouldbeenoughtobringthereferent intofocus.

Thecodingofcompositeentitieswhicharenotintroducedaspluralnominals,but areintroducedasdistinctmentionsofindividualentities(orviathesubtractionof membersfromapluralentity,asinonecasediscussedinsection4.4.2,example(81)),on theotherhand,isalsonotspecificallyaddressedinthecodingprotocol.Since compositesaresimilartootherindirectlyintroducedentitiesinthattheyarenot

141 introducedbyindividualnominals,Idonotcodetheseentitiesasinfocus,evenifallthe membersofthepluralsetareinfocusatthetimeofmentionoftheplural.Inanother sense,however,compositesformedfromindividualentitiesintroducedbynominalsare inherentlydifferentthanabstract/higherorderentitiesindirectlyintroducedbyclauses.

Forexample,myanalysisshowsonecaseinwhichanullargument,whichIclaimsignals

‘infocus’,canbeusedforapluralentity(introducedforthefirsttime)whenatleastone memberofthesetisalreadyinfocus.Theseandotherquestionsrelatedtothecognitive statusofcompositepluralentitiesmeritfurthertesting(cf.BrownSchmidtetal.2005for someexperimentalstudiesrelatedtoreferencetocompositeentities).

Anothermethodologicalissuewhichmeritsfurtherresearchistheroleof contextualfactorssuchaseyegazeinbringingareferentintofocus.Whilethecoding protocolspecificallymentionsthateyegazeisasufficientcriterionforactivatingan entity,thecriteriadonotaddressthequestionofwhetherornoteyegazeshouldcountas amentionofareferentinevaluatingthecriterionforbringingareferentintofocus.The roleofcontextualfeaturesinrelationtotheactivated–infocusdistinctioniscertainlyan areaofinteresttofutureresearch.

Anareaofcodingwhichpresentssomeuniquechallengesisthecodingofentities notintroducedbynominals.Theidentificationofhigherorderentitiessuchasevents, states,activities,orsituationsislessclearthantheidentificationofnominallyintroduced entities,andyetthecodingoftheentityasactivatedorinfocususuallyrestsentirelyon thecorrectidentificationoftheentitytype,sincetheGHmodelassumesthateventsand statesaredirectlyintroducedbyfullclauses,whileothertypesofhigherorderentitiesare

142 indirectlyintroducedbyclauses.Moreover,asnotedabove(cf.4.5.2)onemustalso considerthedistinctionbetweenevents(andactivities)introducedbyamatrixclauseand thoseintroducedbyembeddedclauses.Inthelattercases,embeddingcanpreventan eventfrombeingbroughtintofocusbyaclause;however,thenominalizationwhich accompaniessometypesofembeddingcanalsoaffectreferentialchoiceinadifferent way.Also,thecodingprotocoldoesnotclearlystatehowtotreatatimeorplacewhich maybeconsideredtobeintroducedindirectlyviaassociationwithanevent—justasa situationwouldbeintroducedindirectlyinassociationwithanevent.

Afinalissueraisedbythecodingofclausallyintroducedentitiesinthisstudyis thetreatmentofentitiesthatareintroducedbymultipleclauses.WhileIhavelistedthese casesspecificallyinmypresentationofthedata,Ichoosetotreattheminthesameway asentitiesintroducedbyasinglesentence.Ontheotherhand,furtheranalysisisrequired todetermineifsuchreferentsshareanyofthecharacteristicsofpluralorcomposite entities.

143 Chapter 5: Questionnaire Results

5.0. Goals of the questionnaire

Theprimarygoalofthequestionnaireistotesthypothesesaboutformstatus correlationswhicharebasedonresultsofthecorpusstudy.Morespecifically,following fromthefactthatonlyaverysmallnumberofreferentsofnullarguments,reflexives,and theovertpronoun bu arenotinfocus,thefirsthypothesisisthattheseformssignalthat thereferentisinfocus.Ifoneclaimsthataformsignals‘infocus’,itfollowsthatthat formcannotbeusedwithareferentthatisatmostactivated.Thequestionnaire, therefore,teststheclaimthattheuseoftheseformsrequiresareferentinfocusinorderto demonstratethatthisistrulyaconstraintandnotjustalimitationofthedata.

Inadditiontothisfirsthypothesis,thequestionnaireprovidesameanstotestthe predictionsoftheGivennessHierarchyinrelationto oand sho ,theformspredictedto

signal‘activated’.73 AccordingtotheGHmodel,whileformswhichsignalactivation maygiverisetoascalarimplicaturethattheirreferentsarenotinfocus,theyarenot constrainedtousewithentitiesthatareatmostactivated,astheunidirectionalentailment inthehierarchy,thefactthatanythinginfocusisbydefinitionalsoactivated,meansthat anyformthatsignalsactivationcanalsobeusedforaformthatisinfocus.Inother words,aformthatsignalsactivationisunderspecifiedforanyhigherstatus(infocus) ratherthanspecificallyexcludingit.Accordingtotheresultsofthecorpusstudy,itis clearthat oisnotconstrainedtoreferentsthatareatmostactivated,asmorethanhalfof itsreferentsareinfocus.Fortheform sho ,ontheotherhand,onlyaverysmallnumber

144 ofreferentsarecodedasinfocus;thereforeIusethequestionnairetoverifythat sho is notrestrictedtoreferentsthatareatmostactivated.

Whileverifyingtheabsenceofconstraintsoninfocusreferentsfor oand sho ,the questionnairealsoprovidesevidencethattheseformscreatescalarimplicatures(cf. section5.8).Corpusdataonthedistributionofreferentsintermsofcognitivestatus provideslittleinformationtosupportorcontradicttheexistenceofsuchimplicatures, thoughonemightexpectformssignalingactivationtobeassociatedpredominantlywith activatedreferents.InGundel,Hedberg,andZacharski1993,themajorityofpronominal formsthatsignal‘activated’infourofthefivelanguagesinvestigatedhave75100%of theirreferentscodedasatmostactivated(1993:291292). 74 Forexample,94.4%ofthe

referentsoftheEnglishform that and100%ofthereferentsfor this areatmost

activated.ForRussian,77.8%ofthereferentsof eto ‘this’areatmostactivated.Inmy

corpusstudy,thedistributionofreferentsof sho issimilartothatofformsthatsignal

activationintheGundeletal.study,with87.9%ofitscodedreferentshavingthestatus

‘activated’;butthedistributionof odifferssignificantly,asthemajority(71.9%)ofits

codedreferentsareinfocus.Thisraisesthequestionofwhetherboththeseformsare

usedtothesamedegreetoimplicate‘notinfocus’.Thequestionnaireelicitsevidenceof

scalarimplicaturesbasedontheirfunctionaleffect:thefactthatthechoiceofformcan

74 ThisgeneralizationdoesnotfitthedistributionofreferentsoftwooftheJapanese pronouns/demonstrativepronounssignalling‘activated’: kare ‘he’,forwhich100%(4outof4)of referentsareinfocusand kore ‘this’,forwhich50%(1outof2)referentsareinfocus.However,for sore ‘that(medial)’,whichalsosignals‘activated’,100%(1outof1)ofreferentsareatmostactivated(Gundel etal.1993:291).

145 beusedtodisambiguatetheintendedreferentofapronouninacontextwithmultiple activatedreferents.Forexample,ifonereferentisinfocusandtheotheratmost activated,theuseofaformwhichsignalsactivation( oor sho )promptsthehearerto

associateitwiththereferentthatisnotinfocus,eventhoughthereisnoconstraint

againstusingtheformforareferentinfocus.Evidencefromthedatashowthatthis

rankingeffectcanalsobeextendedtocontextsinwhichthespeakerimplicatestheless prominentoftwoentitiesofthesamestatusviatheuseof oor sho .Thiseffectdoesnot

followautomaticallyfromtheGivennessHierarchy,thoughitisnotinconsistentwithit.

Secondarygoalsofthequestionnaire,particularlysentencecontinuationtestsand

fillintheblanksections,are1)toprovidefurtherevidenceofthenaturaluseofpronouns

withreferentsofaparticularcognitivestatus,2)toprovideevidencefororagainst

constraints(orpreferences)relatedtocategoriesofreferentsthatarerelevantbutnot

whollyrelatedtocognitivestatus—specifically,introducedbyasubjectversusnon

subject,humanversusnonhuman(animacyfeatures),introducedbyanominalversus

introducedbyaclause,and,finally,3)totestthegeneralacceptabilityof shu inlightof

theverylimitednumberofcorpususes.

Thefirstsectionofthischapterprovidesgeneralcommentsabouthowthe

questionnairedataisinterpretedinrelationtothecodingguidelinesdevelopedwithinthe

GivennessHierarchymodel.Sections5.25.6providegeneralinformationonreferent

categorycorrelationsforeachformandtheresultsoftestinginrelationtothespecific predictionsoftheGivennessHierarchy.Evidenceforscalarimplicaturesandranking

146 effectsisdiscussedseparatelyin5.8.Thefinalsectionofthischapterdescribesthe conclusionsbasedontheseresults.

5.1. Using the questionnaire to test predictions

Thehypothesisthatnulls,reflexives,and bu areconstrainedtousewithreferents

infocusistestedbylookingattheuseoftheseformswithreferentswhicharelikelytobe

atmostactivated.Iftheuseoftheseformswithatmostactivatedreferentsis

unacceptable,thiswouldprovidestrongevidencethattheseformsareindeedrestrictedto

referentsinfocus.Thetestingofthishypothesisdependsonidentifyingreferentswhich

areactivatedbutnotlikelytobeinfocus,asdeterminedbythecriteriaofthecoding protocol.Strictlyfollowingthesecriteria,thiscouldmeanthatareferentismentionedin one(butnotboth)oftheimmediatelyprecedingtwosentences,butnotmentionedasthe subject,higherleveltopic,orinthesyntacticfocuspositionorthatitis“aproposition, fact,orspeechactassociatedwiththeeventuality(eventorstate)denotedbythe immediatelyprecedingsentence(s)”(Gundel2004).Withintheseparameters,any referentintroducedasanonsubjectargumentoftheprevioussentencewouldbecodedas atleastactivated,butnotinfocus.However,oneoftheunderlyingpremisesofthe codingprotocolisthatthecriteriaaresufficient,butnotnecessary.Inotherwords,the criteriaareintendedtocapturesituationswhichalwaysbringareferentintofocus,yet therearestillsometypesofsituationswhichsometimes,butnotalwaysbringareferent intofocus.Thisfeatureofthecodingprotocolmakestheprocessofdeterminingthata referentisatmostactivatedanextremelydifficultone.

147 AccordingtoGundeletal.1993,onecasewhereareferentcanbecodedasatleast activated,butmayalsobeinfocusinsome,butnotallcontextsisthecaseofareferent mentionedasapragmaticallyprominentnonsubjectargument(280).InEnglish,for example,thereareanumberofcasesinwhichtheunstressedpronoun it ,whichrequiresa referentinfocus,isusedtorefertoanentityintroducedasthedirectobjectofthe previoussentence.Thus,thereferentof anewlaptop in(97)couldbebroughtintofocus, whichissupportedbythefactthatitisreferredtowith it inthesecondsentence.

(97) Paulboughtanewlaptoptoday.Itwasonsale.

Oneshouldnot,however,drawtheconclusionthatalldirectobjectsbringtheirreferents

intofocus,asdirectobjects,unlikesubjects,arenotbroughtintofocussimplybyvirtue

oftheirsyntacticposition.Moreover,thecognitivestatusofreferentsmentionedas

directobjectsmayvarycrosslinguisticallybasedonparameterssuchaswordorder,as

wellaswithinthesamelanguagebasedonfeaturessuchasthenumberofargumentsor

theorderofinformationcomponents. InKumyk,objectsmostoftenoccurinpreverbal position,whichisknownasafavoredpositionforinformationfocusinTurkishandother

SOVlanguages(Đsever2003).Nevertheless,inordertoeliminatesubjectivity,Icodeall

referentsintroducedasdirectobjectsasatmostactivated(inthecorpusstudyandthe

questionnaire),butdiscussthepossibilityofsomeofthesereferentsbeinginfocusinmy

analysisinthischapter.Referentsofothernonsubjectarguments,forexampledative

objectsornominalswhichoccurinprepositionalphrases,areassumedtobeactivatedbut

notinfocuseventhoughthiscouldpossiblyvarydependingonotherfactors,particularly

148 pragmaticfactors,thenumberofargumentsrepresentedinthesentence,orwordorder variationswhichincreasetheprominenceofthedativeobject(Gundeletal.1993). 75

Asstatedintheintroductiontothischapter,thequestionnairealsotestswhether ornotanyoftheformssaidtosignalactivationareinfactsignalingthatthereferentisat mostactivated.SucharestrictionwouldviolatetheGivennessHierarchypredictionthat formsthatsignalaparticularstatuscanalwaysbeusedwithreferentsofahigher

(entailing)status,butitisimportanttotest,inparticularduetothefactthatfew(4outof

33)ofthereferentsfor sho arecodedashigherthanactivated.Inordertoforma conclusionaboutpossiblerestrictions,Iusequestionnairedatatotestwhetherornot sho canbeusedwithareferentthatisdefinitelyinfocusaccordingtothecodingprotocol.

Determiningthatareferentisdefinitelyinfocusismuchmorestraightforwardthan determiningthatareferentisactivatedbutnotinfocus,asanyreferentswhichmeetthe infocuscriteriaofthecodingprotocolareconsideredtobeinfocus.Inthequestionnaire

Iprimarilyuseanentityintroducedasthesubjectofaprevioussentenceortheevent introducedbytheprevioussentenceasaninfocusreferentfortesting.Anyinstances wheretheuseof sho or oisjudgedacceptablewiththesetypesofreferentsinsentence continuationsorfillintheblankportionsareassumedtobeevidencethatthereisno

75 Gundeletal.1993citethefollowingexamplewhere it isusedinreferencetoanentitymentionedasan NPwithinaprepositionalphrasethatisanalyzedasinfocus: “However,thegovernmentofBarbadosislookingforaprojectmanagerforalargewindenergyproject. I’mgoingtoseethemaninchargeofitnextweek.”(280,example11a).

149 constraintthatprohibitsusingtheseformswithentitiesinfocus—inotherwordsthatthis isanimplicatureandnotanecessaryinference.

Finally,inthequestionnaireIusesomedifferentkindsofdatatoprovideevidence ofrankingeffectsandscalarimplicatures.Sentencecontinuationssuggestthatcertain formspreferaparticulartypeofreferent,whilefillintheblankexerciseswithmultiple pronounsprovideindirectevidenceofrankingeffects.Section4ofthequestionnaire, moreover,isdesignedtoprovidedirectevidencethatthechoiceofpronominalform influencesthechoiceofreferentinanambiguouscontext.Thissectiontestssetsof sentencesinwhichthepronouns oand sho alternateandcanpotentiallyrefertomultiple possiblereferentsofdifferentstatuses.Respondentsareaskedtoidentifythereferentof thepronoun,andtheresultsareanalyzedinordertodeterminethestrengthofthe correlationbetweenthechoiceofpronounandthereferenttype.

5.2. Questionnaire data on null arguments

Theprimarygoalofthequestionnaireinrelationtonullargumentsistoconfirm thehypothesisthatthisformsignalsthestatus‘infocus’and,thus,isunacceptablewith referentsthatareatmostactivated.Also,whiletheresultsofthecorpusstudyindicate thattherearenorestrictionsontheuseofnullsintermsofanimacyfeaturesor subjecthood(ofpreviousmention),theydoindicateapossiblerestrictiononreferenceto entitiesnotintroducedbynominals,asonlyoneoutof229tokensinthecorpusrefersto aclausallyintroducedentity.Thequestionnaireisdesignedtodetermineifthisisan absoluterestrictionorsimplyaconsequenceofthefactthatclausallyintroducedentities

150 aretypicallynotsufficientlysalienttobebroughtintofocusorsimplythateventsand statesarenotcommonreferenttypesinthistextcorpus.

5.2.1. Commentsonpotentialcategoryrestrictions Themajorityofreferentsofnullargumentsinthequestionnairearementionedas previoussubjectsand,thus,areacceptedwithoutquestiontobeinfocus.The questionnairedataalsodemonstratesthatnullargumentscanrefertoeventsintroduced bythepreviousclause(alsoinfocus)—acategoryofreferentnotsufficientlyrepresented inthecorpusstudy.Theuseofnullstorefertoeventsdemonstratestheacceptabilityof usebothwithentitiesnotintroducedbynominalsandwithinanimateentities(For referencetonominallyintroducedinanimateentities,seeexamples(101)and(102)in section5.2.2.).Theacceptabilityofnullargumentsinreferencetoeventsandentities introducedassubjectsisdemonstratedinthreetypesofexercisesinthequestionnaire: sentencecontinuations,fillintheblank,andgrammaticalityjudgments. 76 Insentence continuations,nullsareusedonlywithreferentsmentionedasthesubjectoftheprevious sentence,whichisaclearcaseofareferentinfocus.Anexampleofthistypeisshownin

(98).Inthefillintheblanksectionofthequestionnaire,nullargumentsareusedwith bothtypesofinfocusreferents(e.g.previoussubjectsandeventsdenotedbythe precedingsentence)asin(99).Theuseofnullargumentsinreferencetoanevent

76 Thereisnoevidenceforthefollowingtypesofinfocusreferentsofnullargumentsinthequestionnaire: referentsmentionedearlierinthesamesentence,referentsmentionedinthesyntacticfocuspositionofthe precedingclause,higherleveltopicswhicharepartoftheinterpretationoftheprecedingclauseand referentsmentionedinthetwoimmediatelyprecedingclauses(withoutbeingmentionedasthesubjectof thepreviousclause).

151 denotedbytheprevioussentenceisalsoconfirmedbygrammaticalityjudgments,asin

(100),whichisjudgedacceptablebythreeoutoffourrespondents.(Thejudgmentsof eachofthefourrespondentscompletingthissectionarelistedinparenthesesafterthe freetranslation.)

(98) Ibrag’im restorandan shorpa aldy.(Ø) Abraham restaurant. ABL soup take.3S.PST ‘Abrahamorderedsoupintherestaurant.’ ØBitgende, ofitsiantny chak”yrdy. finish.PST .PRT .LOC waiter. ACC invite.3PST ‘Whenhefinished,hecalledthewaiter.’

(99) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg’yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST Øbolma iaramas! happen. INF allow. NEG ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.That’snotpossible!(Lit.‘Forthatto happenisnotallowed.’ (2respondentsuseanullargument)

(100) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST Ø Song muallimstudentni k”olun aldy. 77 after teacher student. GEN hand.3POSS .ACC take.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.After(that)theteachershookthe student’shand.’ (1,1,3,1)

77 Iproposethatthereisanullinthissentencebecauseoftheparallelwiththesentenceswithovert pronounsthatstartwith“ Ondansong …”.Itisalsopossiblethattherenonullargumentinthiscase.

152 5.2.2. Useofnullswithreferentscodedasactivated Thequestionnaireprovidesevidencerelatedtotheuseofnullargumentswithtwo categoriesofreferentsthatareatleastactivatedbutnotnecessarilyinfocus:entities mentionedasdirectobjectsorthosementionedasdativeobjectsoftheprevioussentence.

Nullreferencestoentitiesintroducedbynonsubjectsdonotoccurinsentence continuationsandareinfrequentinthefillintheblankportionofthequestionnaire,with atotaloffivereferencestoanentityintroducedbyadirectobjectandonereferencetoa anentityintroducedbyadativeobject,allbutoneofwhicharebythesamerespondent

(orange).Thegrammaticalityjudgmentportionofthequestionnaireindicatesthattheuse ofnullswithentitiesintroducedbydirectobjectsisacceptable,butthattheuseofnulls withentitiesintroducedbydativeobjectsisunacceptable.InthissectionIdiscussthe evidencefornullreferencestoentitiesintroducedbydirectobjectsandthoseintroduced bydativeobjectsseparately.

Examples(101)and(102)illustratetwotestsentencesinvolvingreferenceto entitiesmentionedasdirectobjectsinthefillintheblanksection.(Threeofthefivenull referencestodirectobjectscomefromthesepairs.)

(101) Ibrag’im restorandan shorpa aldy. Abraham restaurant. ABL soup take.3S.PST O Ø dag”y da alarmy edi eken? 3.2 again also take.3FUT .INTER AUX .3 PST COP ‘Abrahamgotsoupattherestaurant.Wouldhegetitagain?’ (1respondentusesanullargument)

(102) A. Ibrag’im restorandan shorpa aldy. Abraham restaurant. ABL soup take.3S.PST 153 B.Øtatyvlumu edi? sweet. INTER be.3PST ‘Abrahamgotsoupattherestaurant.Wasitgood?’ (2respondentsuseanullargument)

Inexample(101),oneoutoffiverespondentsusesanullargumenttofillintheblank assumedtorefertothesoup,whileothersusetheovertpronoun oor sho .Inresponseto thisexample,onemightclaimthattheuseofthenullargumentinreferencetothesoupis acceptableduetotheparallelisminargumentstructure,whichcontributestothesalience ofthereferent(cf.Chapter4,section4.1.3),asthetwosentencesbothmentionAbraham insubjectpositionandthesoupinobjectposition.Intheresponseshownin(102), however,twooutoffiverespondentsfillintheblankwithanullargument(whilethe othersuse oor sho ),yetthereisnoparallelismhere,asthesoupismentionedasthe objectofonesentenceandasthesubjectofastativeverbinthenextsentence.

Athirdsentencepairinwhichtheorangerespondentusesanulltofillinablank representingthedirectobjectoftheprevioussentenceisshownin(103).Thisexampleis presentedherebecausethefirstsentenceinthepairisparalleltothesentenceusedinthe grammaticalityjudgmentportion.

(103) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. O Ø chalt okhuma teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST 3.2 quickly read. INF

taryg”yn aĭtdy. necessary.3POSS say.3 PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.(S)hesaid((s)he)neededtoread(it) quickly.’ (1respondentusesanullargument)

154 Ifoneassumesthatnullreferencestoentitiesintroducedasdirectobjectsare acceptable,thenegativegrammaticalityjudgmentrepresentedinsentence(104)provides anunexpectedcontrasttotheacceptabilityofthenullargumentusedin(103)ifoneonly considerstheargumentpositionofpreviousmention.

(104) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. *Ø Tangcholpanedi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST Tangcholpan be.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.(It)wasTangcholpan.’ (3,4) Althoughthesoupin(101)and(102)andthebookin(103)and(104)arementionedin

thesamesyntacticpositionintheprevioussentence,theuseofthenullargumentin(104)

isjudgedunacceptablebyallrespondents(notethatthisexampleonlyoccursintwo

questionnaires).However,Iproposethatthisdifferenceisnotrelatedtothecognitive

statusofthereferentortheargumentpositionofthepreviousmentionbuttothe

differenceinthephonologicalfocusstructurebetweentheidentificationalsentencein

(104),whereanovertargumentisrequired,andtheinterrogativeanddeclarative

sentencesin(101)(103),wherethephonologicalfocusstructuredoesnotrequirean

overtpronoun. 78 Thegrammaticalityjudgmentofunacceptabilityin(105),whichis structuredsimilarlyto(104),providessupportingdataforthisclaim.

78 Thegreatestsimilarityisbetweensentences(104)and(102),whichdifferstructurallyonlyintheaddition oftheinterrogativesuffix–mu ,onthepredicateadjective tatyvlu ‘delicious’.Aseconddifferencebetween thetwosentencesisthat(102)isstructuredasaninterchangebetweentwospeakersAandB,while(104)is structuredasacontinuingutterancebythesamespeaker;however,thisdoesnotseemtobetherelevant factorinrelationtotheacceptabilityofthenullargument.

155 (105) Bolatatyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST *Ø Tiunegiunboldu . yesterday happen.3pst ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.(It)wasyesterday.’ Ø(3,3) [sho (2,1,2,1); bu (2,3,2,3);o(2,4(onerespondentdoesnotuseofornon humans)]

Althoughthenullargumentin(105)referstoanevent,whichcanbeassumedtobein

focus,andsuchreferencesareshowntobeacceptableinexample(99),bothrespondents judgethisexampletobeunacceptable.Iproposethattheunacceptabilityofnullsinboth

(104)and(105)isrelatedtothefactthattheverb bolmak ‘tobe’or‘tohappen’isa

copularverbthatrequirestwoelementsthatcanbearphonologicalfocus(stress),while

(102)isacceptablebecausetheinterrogativemorphemeservesasa“host”for phonologicalfocus.Takentogetherwiththecorpusresults,theevidencein(101)(105)

suggeststhatnullreferencetoapreviousdirectobjectcanbeacceptableandnot

dependentonparallelism,aslongasthefocusstructureofthesentenceinwhichthenull

occursdoesnotrequireanovertform.Thisdatasupportsthehypothesisthatmentionin

directobjectpositionbringsareferentintofocus,butfurthertestingofnullreferenceto

directobjectsisneeded.

Incontrasttotheprobableacceptabilityofsomenullargumentsreferringto previousdirectobjects,thequestionnaireresultsindicatethatnullreferencetothedative

objectofaprecedingsentenceisunacceptable.Firstofall,inthesectionon

grammaticalityjudgments,intwocaseswherethedativeobjectisassumedtobe

156 activated,(106)and(107),theuseofthenullargumentisjudgedunacceptablebyall participants.

(106) *Muallimstudentge kitapberdi. Øsavboldedi. teacherstudent. DAT bookgive.3PST thank.yousay.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.(S)hesaidthankyou.’ (4,4,4,3) (107) *Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. Song muallim teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST then teacher

Øekzameni bulank”utlady. exam.3 POSS withcongratulate.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.Thentheteachercongratulated(him/her) ontheexam.’ (3,3,3,4) Inspiteofwhatseemslikeaclearcaseofjudgmentsofunacceptabilityfornull referencestodativeobjects,thereisonefillintheblankexampleinthequestionnaire wheretheorangerespondentusesanullforapreviousdativeobject.In(108)anull argumentisusedtofillinablankassociatedwiththedativeobjectoftheprevious sentence, Patimatg”a ‘toPatimat’.

(108) MuallimPatimatg”a kitapberdi. Sho birinchisavg’atbolg”an song, teacherPatimat. DAT bookgive.3PST 3.3 first gift be. PST .PRT since Ø(Patimat) onu (book) anasyna gërsetme ciuedi. 3.2. ACC mother.3POSS .DAT show. INF want.3PST ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook.Sinceitwas(her)firstgift,shewantedto showittohermother.’

Myanalysisofthiscaseisthattheoccurrenceofsavg’at ‘gift’inthefirstclauseofthe

secondsentenceincludesanimplicitmentionofPatimat,therecipient,thusthereferent

157 wouldsatisfyacriterionforinfocusstatusatthetimeoftheoccurrenceofthenull argument,asthereferenthasbeenmentionedonceineachoftheprevioustwoclauses.

Assumingthisanalysisof(108),thedataconsistentlysupportstheunacceptabilityofnull referencestoentitiesintroducedasdativeobjects.Oneareaforfurtherstudywouldbeto testtheacceptabilityofnullreferencetoentitiespreviouslymentionedasdativeobjects incaseswherethereisparallelisminargumentstructure,asthecorpusdataindicatesthat parallelismmayplayaroleinbringinganentityintofocusincaseswheresyntactic positionaloneisinsufficient.

Inadditiontolookingatreferencetonominallyintroducedactivatedentities,one sentenceinthegrammaticalityjudgmentportionofthequestionnaire,shownin(109),is structuredtotestnullreferencetoaclausallyintroducedentityconsideredtobeatmost activated.

(109) Bolatatyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’ Ø barynda tamashaetdi. everyonesurprisedo.3PST ‘PROsurprisedeveryone.’ Ø(2,3,2,3) [o(1,3,1,1); bu (2,4,2,2); sho (1,3,2,3); shu (1,3,2,3)]

WhileIintendedforthisexampletocreateacontextinwhichthenullargumentwouldbe understoodtorefertothefactthatBolatfelloffhishorse,responsesfromasimilarly structuredexampleinanothersectionofthequestionnaire,shownin(110),indicatethat thereferentin(109)ismorelikelytobeunderstoodasanentityintroducedasanominal

158 argumentoftheprevioussentence—thatis,Bolat.In(110),allrespondentsidentifythe referentoftheformtheyusetofillintheblankasanentityintroducedbyanominalin theprevioussentence(thatis,thebookortheteacher),ratherthanafact.Byanalogy,I concludethattheacceptabilityjudgmentsfor(109)cannotbeinterpretedasevidencefor theacceptabilityoftheuseofparticularformsinreferencetoafact.

(110) Muallimstudentgekitapberdi. teacherstudent. DAT bookgive.3PST ___ barynda tamashaetdi. everyone surprisedo.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.___surprisedeveryone.’ o’ziu (teacher); ol (teacher); o(teacher); bu (book)

Basedonthecombinedresultsdescribedabove,onepossibleconclusionisthat, sincenullscanrefertononsubjectarguments(includingbothdirectandindirectobjects) whicharecodedasatmostactivatedbasedontheprotocolalone,nullsarenotrestricted toreferentsinfocus—aconclusionthatwoulddisconfirmthehypothesisbasedonthe resultsofthecorpusstudy.Ontheotherhand,thedelineationbetweentheacceptability oftheuseofnullswithdirectobjectsversustheunacceptabilityofusewithindirect objectsbyallbuttheorangerespondentprovidesevidencethatmentionincertainless prominentargumentpositionsmaynotnecessarilybesufficienttobringareferentinto focus.Thisdatasuggeststhatthereisadistinctioninthesalienceofdirectobjects mentionedinpreverbalpositionandindirectobjects(eitheringeneraloratleastthose notmentionedinpreverbalposition);thereforeIassumethatmentionasadirectobject inpreverbalpositionbringsanentityintofocusinmostcases(especially,butnotonly,

159 incaseswherethereisparallelismintheargumentstructure),whilementioninothernon subjectargumentpositionsdoesnot.Withinthisinterpretationofthedata,theresults confirmthehypothesisfromthecorpusstudythatnullargumentsrequireareferentthatis infocus.Furthermore,thedatasuggeststhatthereisnorestrictionontheuseofnull argumentsinreferringtoclausallyintroducedentitiesthatareinfocus—thatis,events— basedonthehypothesisthatsomenegativejudgmentsofreferencestoeventsarerelated tofocusstructureratherthantothecognitivestatusofthereferent.

5.3. Reflexives

Asmostofthecorpusdataonthereflexiveo’ziu involvesreferencetoentities mentionedpreviouslywithinthesamesentence,thequestionnaireisusedtoprovidedata onreflexiveswhichrefertoentitiesintheprevioussentenceandpossibleconstraintson theseuses.Theintentistoverifythatreflexivescanreferatleasttothesubjectofthe previoussentenceandtojudgetheacceptabilityoftheirusewithreferentsofother argumentsoftheprevioussentencewhichmaybeatmostactivated—thatis,direct objectsordativeobjects.Thedataonreflexivescomesfromfillintheblankexercises, andgrammaticalityjudgmentsinthequestionnaire.Noothercategoryrestrictionsare testedinthequestionnaire,asthecorpusstudyalreadyindicatestheacceptabilityofthe useof o’ziu withinanimateentities(althoughIassumethatthereflexivecannotbeused withclausallyintroducedentities).

Inthefillintheblanksection, o’ziu isusedinninedifferentsentencepairs(by onerespondenteachtime,exceptforexample(111),wheretworespondentsusethis form).Forsevenoftheninesentences, o’ziu isusedinreferencetoaprevioussubject,as

160 in(111).Oneuseof o’ziu isinreferencetoapreviousdirectobject,asshownin(112);

however,followuptestingofthisexampleshowsthatthisuseisatbestmarginal.Inthe

otherexceptionalcaseillustratedin(113)o’ziu isusedtorefertoapreviousdative

object,butfurthertestingofthisexampleshowsittobeunacceptable,aswell.

(111) Ibrag’im restorandan shorpa aldy. Abrahamrestaurant. ABL soup take.3S.PST Adatly g’alda o pilavnu o’ziune/o’ziu aldy. customarycase. LOC 3.2 pilaf. ACC self.3S.DAT /self.3S take.3S.PST ‘Abrahamgotsoupfromtherestaurant.Usuallyhegotpilafforhimself./Usually he(himself)gotthatpilaf.’ (2respondents)

(112) ?Ibrag’imrestorandan shorpa aldy. O’ziu tatyvlumu edi? Abrahamrestaurant. ABL soup take.3S.PST self.3 Ssweet. INTER be.3PST ‘Abrahamgotsoupattherestaurant.Wasitgood?’ (1respondent)

(113) ?Muallim studentge kitap berdi. Ol o’ziune chalt teacher student. DAT book give.3PST 3.2 self. 3S.DAT quickly okhumataryg”yn aĭtdy. read. INF necessary.3POSS say.3PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.(S)hetold(him/her)(s)heneededtoreadit quickly.’ (1respondent)

Inthegrammaticalityjudgments o’ziu isjudgedtobeunacceptablewhenitsreferentis

mentionedasthedativeobjectoftheprevioussentence,assumedtobeatmostactivated.

(Notethatthesesentencepairsareidenticaltothosein(106)and(107),exceptforthe

alternationbetweenthenullargumentandthereflexive.)Neitherofthesesentences

involvesparallelargumentstructure.

161 (114) *Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. O’ziu savbol dedi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST self.3 Sthank.yousay.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.(S)hesaidthankyou.’ (3,4,3,4) (115) *Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. Song muallim o’ziunu teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST later teacher self.3 S.ACC ekzameni bulan k”utlady. exam with congratulate.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.Thentheteachercongratulated(him/her) onhis/herexam.’ (4,4)

Insummary,theevidencesuggeststhatreferencetopreviousnonsubjectsis

unacceptable;however,furthertestingoftheuseofreflexivesinreferencetoanimate

entitiesmentionedasdirectobjectsisneeded. 79

5.4. Questionnaire data on bu

Theprimaryissuethequestionnairedataaddressesinrelationto bu iswhetheror notitsuserequiresareferentthatisinfocusasconcludedonthebasisofthecorpusdata.

Followingtheanalysisofnullsandreflexives,particularattentionisgiventowhetheror not bu isacceptablewhenusedwithentitiesintroducedbyargumentsoftheprevious sentencewhichareneithersubjectsnordirectobjects.Whilethequestionnaireis

79 WhileseparateworkonlongdistancereflexivizationinKumyk(Humnick2007)indicatesthatitis unlikelythatareflexivecanbecoreferentwithanonsubjectargumentofaprevioussentence, furtherwork isrequiredtotesttheacceptabilityofreferencetoanonsubjectargumentthatisclearlyinfocusbecauseit hasbeenmentionedintwoormoresentences(criterion5ofthecodingprotocol).Ifsuchusesofthe reflexiveareacceptable,itwouldclearlyindicatethattherestrictionisrelatedonlytocognitivestatusand nottosyntacticposition.Otherwise,Iassumethattheapparentcognitivestatusrestrictionisasecondary effectofageneralrestrictiononnonsubjectantecedentsoftheprevioussentence.

162 primarilystructuredtoprovideevidenceofcognitivestatusconstraints,thereisalsodata onthequestionofwhetherornot bu isacceptablewhenusedinreferencetoeventsor othertypesofclausallyintroducedentities.Thisquestionisintendedtofollowupthe corpusanalysis,where bu isdocumentedasbeingusedonlywithclausallyintroduced entitieswithadegreeofnominalization.Thecorpusanalysisalreadyprovidesstrong evidencethattherearenorestrictionson bu relatedtogrammaticalroleoranimacy features;therefore,datarelatedtothesefeaturesisnotspecificallydiscussedinthis section,thoughtheacceptableuseof bu withnonsubjectsandinanimateentitiesis evidentfromthediscussionofitsusewithactivatedentities.

5.4.1. Useof bu withreferentscodedasactivated Sentencecontinuationsandfillintheblankexercisesprovidethemostnatural evidenceofpossibleacceptableusesofthepronoun bu withatmostactivatedentities.

Therearetwosentencecontinuationsforwhichtherespondentisaskedtowritea sentenceusing bu .Thefirstoftheseislistedasexample(116).Whilethreeoutoffive respondentsuse bu forthereferentof Ibrahim ,thesubjectoftheprevioussentenceand, hence,areferentwhichisinfocus,oneresponse,illustratedin(a),usesthisformin referencetothesoup,anentitywhichismentionedasthedirectobjectoftheprevious sentenceandiscodedasactivated,whileanotherresponse(b)uses bu inreferencetothe restaurant,whichismentionedasalocativeargumentandnotlikelytobeinfocus.For thesentencecontinuationshownin(117),threerespondentsuse bu torefertothestudent, anentitymentionedasthedativeobjectoftheprevioussentence,andoneofthesecases isillustratedin(a).Onerespondentuses bu torefertothebook,mentionedasthedirect

163 objectoftheprevioussentence,asillustratedin(b).Thoughthelattercaseiscodedas activated,itcouldeasilyrepresentareferentinfocus,asdescribedpreviouslyinsection

5.1.

(116) Ibrag’imrestorandashorpaashady.(bu) Abrahamrestaurant. LOC soupeat.3PST ‘Abrahamatesoupintherestaurant.’ a. Bu tuzluedi. 3.1saltybe.3PST ‘Itwassalty.’ b. Mundag”y ashlar aĭrycha tatyvlu edi. 80 3.1. LOC .MOD food. PL especiallydelicious be.3PST ‘Thefoodherewasespeciallydelicious.’

(117) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi.(bu) teacherstudent. DAT book give.3 ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ a. Bu shossag’at okhumabashlady. 3.1 immediatelyread. INF begin.3PST ‘(S)hestartedtoreadimmediately.’ b. Bunu betleri këp edi. 81 3.1. GEN page. PL .3POSS many be.3PST ‘Ithadalotofpages.’

Thefactthat bu canbeusedinreferencetothedirectobjectoftheprevioussentencein freesentencecontinuationsisparticularlystrongevidencethatthistypeofuseis acceptable.Asdiscussedinrelationtonullsandreflexives,however,itislikelythat

80 Thisistherespondentforwhomthedataongrammaticalityjudgmentshadtobedisregarded. 81 Bunu isunderstoodasanalternationof munu ,thegenitiveform.

164 referentsintroducedasdirectobjectsshouldatleastsometimesbeanalyzedasinfocus, inspiteofthefactthattheyareonlycodedasactivated(cf.5.2.2and5.3).Theuseof bu withalocativeargumentandadativeobjectinsentencecontinuationsissignificant,as thereislesslikelihoodthatmentionintheseargumentpositionscanbringanentityinto focus.

Inthefillintheblanksectionofthequestionnaire,elevenoutoftwentyone tokensof bu occurwithreferentsoriginallycodedasactivated. 82 Sixoftheserefertoan

entitymentionedasadirectobjectintheprevioussentenceandfivetoanentity

mentionedasadativeobject(eitherofamatrixclauseorofanembeddedclause).A

comparisonoftheseuseswithresponsesfromthegrammaticalityjudgmentportion

confirmstheacceptabilityof bu withbothofthesetwocategories.

Thesentences,shownin(118)(121),allofwhicharecontinuationsofa

similarlystructuredsentence(eitherfillintheblank,grammaticalityjudgment,orboth),

indicatetheacceptabilityof bu inreferencetoanentitymentionedasadirectobject—in

eachcase,thebook.

(118) MuallimPatimatg”akitap berdi. teacherPatimat. DAT book give.3PST ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook.’ Bu birinchisavg”atbolg”an song,ol shonu anasyna 3.1first gift be.3PST .PRT since 3.2 3.3. ACC mother.3POSS .DAT

82 Itisinterestingthat15outof21usesof bu inthefillintheblanksectionarebythesame(blue) respondent.Thoughnotalloftheactivatedcasesarefromthisrespondent,amajorityofthemare(others arefromblackrespondent);however,grammaticalityjudgmentsforsimilarsentencepairsusing bu donot suggestthatacceptablejudgmentsarelimitedtooneortworespondents.

165 gërsetme siuedi. show. INF want.3PST ‘Sinceit washerfirstgift,she wantedtoshowit tohermother.’ (2respondents) (119) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. Student munu chalt okhudu. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST student 3.1. ACC quickly read.3PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.Thestudentreaditquickly.’ (1respondent)

(120) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. Student,munu alyp, teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST student3.1. ACC take. GER u’ĭge getdi. home. DAT go.3PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook,Thestudent,takingit,wenthome.’ (2,2)

(121) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. Bu “Tangcholpan” edi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST 3.1 Tangcholpan be.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.ItwasTangcholpan.’ (2respondents) Grammaticalityjudgmentsofthesamesentence:(3,1,2,3) [Ø(3,4); o(2,4); shu (2,3;3,4); sho (1,1,2,1)]

Inthefillintheblankresponse,respondentsusebu torefertothebooktwotimesin

(118)andoncein(119).Inasimilarlystructuredsentencepairinthegrammaticality judgmentportion,shownin(120),bothrespondentsjudge bu tobeacceptable.(Thispair

wasgivenonlyto2respondents.)Inexample(121)inthegrammaticalityjudgment portion,however,theresponsetothissentencepairwasinconsistent,withtwo

respondents(blueandred)judgingthesentencetobeunacceptableandtwo,acceptable.

Itisinterestingthatoneofrespondentwhojudgesthesentencetobeunacceptable(blue) 166 isthesameasoneofthosewhoused bu forthesamepairinthefillintheblanksection.

Theother(red)judgesboth oand bu tobe“3”,whichseemstoindicatethatredprefers sho inreferencetoaninanimateentity(seedataon sho )ratherthananytypeofrestriction basedoncognitivestatus.(Notethatthesamesentencepair(cf.example(104))isjudged

tobeunacceptable(by2outof2respondents)whenanullargumentisusedinthesame positionas bu ,butIproposethatthisrestrictionisrelatedtoparticularsentencetypesand phonologicalfocus.)

Sentences(122)(129)illustratetheuseof bu withreferentsmentionedasthe

dativeobjectoftheprevioussentence.In(122)andinthefirstusesof bu in(123)and

(124) thisformisusedtorefertothestudent/Patimatbyonerespondentforeachpairin

thefillintheblankportionofthequestionnaire.

(122) Muallim studentge kitap berdi. Shunu bu chalt teacher student. DAT book give.3PST 3.4. ACC 3.1 quickly okhuma taryg”yn aĭtdy. read. INF necessary.3POSS say.3PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.((S)he)told(him/her)(s)he(bu )neededto readit(shunu )quickly.’ (1respondent)

(123) MuallimPatimatg”akitap berdi. Bu shonu këp ushatdy. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST 3.1 3.4. ACC much be.pleased.3PST Song , bu u’ĭgegetdi. later3.1home. DAT go.3PST ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook.Shewasverypleasedwithit.Thenshewent home.’ (1respondent)

167 (124) MuallimPatimatg”a jkitap berdi. teacherPatimat. DAT book give.3PST O birinchi savg”atbolg”an song,bu j munu anasyna 3.2 first gift be.3PST .PRT since 3.1 3.1. ACC mother.3POSS .DAT gërsetme siuedi. show. INF want.3PST ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook.Sinceitwasherfirstgift,shewantedtoshow ittohermother.’ (1respondent)

Insimilarexamplesinthegrammaticalityjudgmentportiontheuseof bu inreferenceto

thestudent/Patimatisjudgedtobeacceptablebyallrespondentsfor(125)andallbutone

respondentfor(126),whiletheuseofnullargumentsorreflexivesineachcaseisjudged

unacceptablebyallrespondents.Thisprovidesstrongevidencethattheuseof bu isless

restrictedthanthatofnullsandreflexives.

(125) Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. Bu savboldedi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST thank.yousay.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.(S)hesaidthankyou.’ (2,2,2,2) [shu (4,4,2,4); o’ziu (3,4,3,4); Ø(4,4,4,3); o (2,2); sho (3,4,2,4) cf.example(106)] (126) Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. Song muallim teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST then teacher

munu ekzameni bulan k”utlady. 3.1. ACC exam.3 POSS with congratulate.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.Thentheteachercongratulatedhim/her ontheexam.’ (1,2,2,3) [o’z (4,4); Ø(3,3,3,4);o (2,1); sho (3,4,3,4); shu (3,4;3,4);cf.example(107)]

168 Aninterestingaspecttothedatasetrepresentedin(122)(126)isthatthedativeobjectis human,whiletheaccusativeobjectisinanimate.LaterinthischapterIshowthat animacyrankingcanalsoplayaroleinthechoiceofpronoun;thereforeinthesecases, theacceptabilityoftheuseof bu withthedativeobjectmayalsobeexplainedbythefact thatthehumanreferentoutrankstheinanimatereferentintermsofsalience.

Similarresultsoccurforasecondsetofsentencesinwhichapronounreferstoa nonhumanentitymentionedasthedativeobjectofthesubordinateclauseinthe precedingsentence,whichmustbeconsideredatmostactivatedaccordingtothecoding criteria.In(127)and(128)bu or munu (theaccusativeof bu) are usedtofillinablankin referencetothehorsebyonerespondentforeachexample. Notethattheacceptabilityof bu in(127)doesnotdependonparallelargumentstructure,asin(122)(126).

(127) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg’yldy. Bolathorse.3POSS .DAT ride. GERgo.PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST

Sho saialy, o munu satma khyĭal etdi. 3.3 because3.2 3.1. ACC sell. INF decisionmake.3 PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Becauseofthat,hedecidedtosellit.’

(128) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST G’ali bug”ar dag”yminip barmazhak”man dep aĭtdy. now 3.1. DAT again ride.GER go. NEG .FUT .1S COMP say.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Now(he)saidthat(he)won’trideit anymore.’

Inthegrammaticalityjudgmentportion,threeoutoffourrespondentsjudge(129)tobe

acceptable,confirmingtheresultsfor(127)and(128).Theonerespondent(red)who

169 judgesthisexampletobeunacceptabledoessoforallovertpronounstestedexcept sho ; thusIconcludethattheissueofacceptabilityinthisonespecificcaseisrelatedtothe respondent’spreferencefor sho withnonhumanreferentsratherthancognitivestatus restrictionsontheotherforms. 83

(129) Bolat atynaminip barag”anda ĭyg’yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST

Bolat Ø(Bolat) dag”ybug”ar minmezhegin aĭtdy. Bolat more 3.1. DAT ride.3FUT .3POSS say.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Bolatsaidhewon’trideitanymore.’ (2,2,2,3) [o(1,2,2,4); sho (1,2,1,2)]

Sincetheacceptableusesof bu torefertothehorsedonotinvolvereferencetothe

highestrankingparticipantintermsofanimacy(Bolat,thehuman,isrankedhigher),the

datainthissectionshowsthattheacceptabilityofreferencetodativeobjectsisnot

conditionalontheaddedsalienceresultingfromreferencetoahuman.

Tosummarizethissection,thetestingof bu withentitieswhicharenotcodedas

infocusbythecriteriaofthecodingprotocolshowsthatthispronounisacceptablewith

entitiesintroducedintheprevioussentenceasdirectobjects.Inadditiontothis,a

numberofexamplesinthequestionnaireindicateacceptabilitywithentitiesintroduced byotherargumentsoftheprevioussentence,includingdativeobjectsandlocative

argumentsofbothmatrixandembeddedclauses.Oneresultofthegrammaticality

83 Thisrespondentisnotentirelyconsistentanduses bu fornonhuman/inanimatereferentsinsomecases yetalsoindicatesaconstraintagainstusing ofornonhumans.

170 judgmentsisthefindingthat bu isjudgedtobeacceptableincaseswhereneithernull argumentsnorreflexivesarejudgedtobeacceptable—morespecifically,usewith previousargumentsotherthansubjectsordirectobjects—showingthatnullsand reflexivesaremorerestrictedthan bu .Myconclusionisthattheuseof bu isnot constrainedtoreferentsinfocus,inspiteofthefactthatitisrarelyusedwithreferentsnot infocusinthecorpus;anditisthusanalyzedassignalingactivation.

5.4.2. Useof bu withclausalentities Duetotheabsenceinthecorpusstudyofusesof bu withentitiesdenotedbya precedingsentence,thequestionnaireteststheuseof bu torefertoeventsdenotedbythe precedingsentencetodeterminewhetherthisisindeedaconstraint.Thedatarelatedto

thisissue,however,provestobeinconclusive.Forthefillintheblanksentencepairsin

(130),responses(a)and(b)(bytwodifferentrespondents)indicatethat bu isacceptably

usedwithaclausallyintroducedentity(event)ineachcase.

(130) Bolatatyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’ a. Bu bolmaiaramas! 3.1happen. INF allow. NEG .GER ‘That’snotpossible!’(Lit.‘Forthattohappenisnotallowed.)(blue) b. Bu nechikbolmabola? 3.1howhappen. INF happen. PR ‘Howcoulditbe/happen?’(red)

Ontheotherhand,theuseof bu torefertoaneventinexample(131)fromthe

grammaticalityjudgmentsectionisjudgedunacceptablebyallbutonerespondent,while

171 sho isjudgedacceptablebyallrespondentsand oandnullarejudgedacceptablebyall butonerespondent.Likewise,inexample(132),tworespondentsjudgedtheuseof bu in

referencetotheeventintroducedbytheprevioussentencetobeunacceptable. 84

(131) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST

?Mundan songmuallimstudentni k”olun aldy. 3.1. ABL afterteacher student. GEN hand.3POSS .ACC take.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.Afterthattheteachershookthestudent’s hand.’ (2,4;3,4) [sho (1,1;2,1); shu (3,3;2,2); Ø(1,1;3,1);o(1,1,2,4(thisrespondentdoesnotuse ofornonhuman))] (132) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg’yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST

?Bu tiunegjun boldu. 3.1 yesterday happen.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.(It)happenedyesterday.’ (2,3,2,3) [sho (2,1,2,1); Ø(3,3); o(2,4(thisrespondentdoesnotuse ofornonhuman))] Notethat(131)and(132)aretheonlytypeofexamplesinthegrammaticalityjudgment sectioninwhich bu wasviewedaslessthanacceptablebythenonredrespondents.Red, however,issomewhatrestrictiveintheuseof bu and owithnonhumanreferents,and

84 Thereissomequestionastowhetherthereferentof bu inexample(131)actuallyreferstoaneventor, morespecifically,tothetimeoftheevent.Ifthereferentisthetime,itmaybeconsideredtobeintroduced indirectlyinassociationwiththeevent,whichwouldaffectitscognitivestatusand,thus,thechoiceof referringform.

172 thismayalsobethefactoraffectingblack’sresponsein(132).(Otherrespondents,such asblue,use bu withnonhumanreferents–cf.(127).)

Intheanalysisofthesefacts,onemustalsoconsiderthatthereisasignificant differencebetweenthesentencepairsshownin(130)andthosein(131)and(132):the formerpairsrepresentanexchangebetweentwospeakersinwhichthesecondspeaker givesanemphaticresponse,whilethelatterpairsrepresentcontinuationsbyonespeaker, withnoemphaticfeature.Themixedreactiontothesecondsetmightindicatethatthe useof bu inacontextwithouttheemphaticfeatureismarginalbasedonpragmatic

featuresotherthancognitivestatus.Anotherconsiderationisthatthesentencesin(131)

and(132)mayalsobenonreferentialusesof bu ratherthanactualreferencestoevents,

whichcanbecommoninconventionalizedexclamations.

Onelimitationofthequestionnaireisthatitdoesnottesttheacceptabilityof bu

whenusedwithaclausallyintroducedentityfromtheprevioussentencethatis not broughtintofocus—thatis,afact,proposition,situationorspeechact.Asdescribedin relationto(109)and(110)insection5.2.2,thisisduetoanunexpectedambiguityinthe pairstargetedfortestingthistypeofreferent.Furthertestingofthistypeofreferent wouldprovidefurtherinformationonboththelimitsofacceptabilityoftheuseof bu with clausallyintroducedentitiesandconfirmationthat bu isacceptableinusewithalltypes ofentitieswhichareconsideredtobeatmostactivated.Ifonecandemonstratethat bu canbeusedacceptablyinreferencetoaspeechactoraclausallyintroducedentitythatis indirectlyintroducedbythepreviousclause(suchasasituationoraproposition),thenit clearlycannotberestrictedtoreferentsinfocus.

173 5.5. Questionnaire data on o

Sincethecorpusanalysisshowssignificantnumbersoftokensof owithbothat

mostactivatedandinfocusreferents,thequestionnairedoesnotdirectlytestrestrictions

relatedtoeithercategoryofcognitivestatus;however,thedataabout ofromthe

questionnairedoesprovidesomeinterestinginformationandalsoservesasacontrolfor

someoftheothertestsforconstraints.Thefactthat oisusedinreferencetobothin

focusentitiesandatmostactivatedentitiesinthequestionnairecorroboratesfindings basedonthecorpusanalysisthatthisformonlysignalsactivationandconfirmsthatthis formdoesnotconventionallysignal‘notinfocus’(whichwouldhavebeeninconsistent withtheGHmodel),whilestillallowingforthepossibilitythatitimplicates‘notin focus’.Apossiblysignificantobservationfromthequestionnaireisthatonerespondent appearstohaveaconstraintagainstusing ofornonhuman/inanimateentitiesthatisnot exhibitedbytheotherrespondents.

In(133)and(134),thetwotestsentenceswhichrequirecontinuationsthatuseo, thepronounisusedtorefertoahumanreferentwhichisinfocusandismentionedasthe subjectoftheprevioussentence(eitherBolat(133)orAbraham(134))inallexcepttwo cases.Inthelattertwocases,(134), oisusedtorefertothesoup,thedirectobjectofthe previoussentence.

(133) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg’yldy.(o) Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’ (Inallcontinuationsofthissentence, oreferstoBolat.)

174 (134) Ibrag’im restorandan shorpa aldy. Abraham restaurant. ABL soup take.3S.PST ‘Abrahamorderedsoupintherestaurant.’ (Inthreesentencecontinuations oreferstoAbraham;intwo, oreferstothesoup.)

Inthefillintheblanksectionofthequestionnaire,mostrespondentsuse oto refertobothhumanandnonhuman/inanimateentities.Theredrespondent,however, onlyuses oforhumanentities.Similarly,theredrespondentjudges otobeunacceptable whenreferringtoanonhumanentity,asin(135)(137),whiletheremaining respondentsjudgetheseusestobeacceptable.

(135) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg’yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST

Bolat Ø dag”yog”ar minmezhegin aĭtdy. Bolat more 3.1. DAT ride.3 FUT .3 POSS say.3 PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Bolatsaidhewon’trideitanymore.’ o(1,2,2,4)

(136) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. O “Tangcholpan” edi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST Tangcholpan be.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.ItwasTangcholpan.’ o(2,4)

(137) Muallimstudentgekitapberdi. teacherstudent. DAT bookgive.3PST

Ondan songmuallimstudentni k”olun aldy. 3.2. ABL after teacher student.GEN hand.3 POSS .ACC take.3 PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.Afterthattheteachershookthestudent’s hand.’ o(1,1,2,4)

175 Theredrespondent’sresponsesareconsistentacrossallsectionsofthequestionnairewith aconstraintagainstusing owithnonhumanreferents.Insection4,inparticularfortwo responsesforwhich ocouldonlyrefertoanonhumanentity,thisrespondentwrites“Ø” whenaskedtoidentifythereferent.Evidencefromtheotherrespondents,however,does notsupportsuchaconstraint.

Exceptfortherespondentwhoappearstohaveaconstraintagainstusing owith nonhumanreferents,allexcepttwosentencepairscontaining oarejudgedtobe acceptable.Inoneexceptionalcaseshownin(138),whichonlyappearsintwo questionnaires,onerespondentjudgesallovertpronounstobeacceptable,whiletheother judgesallovertpronounstobeunacceptable;thus,onemayconcludethatsomeaspectof thesentencepairotherthanthepronounchoiceisunacceptable.Asimilarconclusion maybedrawnabout(139),whereoneoutoffourrespondentsjudge oandallotherforms tobeunacceptable,whiletheremainingrespondentsjudge otobeacceptable.

(138) Murato’ziuniu mashini bulan tiukenge barmatok”tashdy. Muratself.3 S.GEN car.3POSS with store. DAT go. INF decide.3PST

Song o onu dëgerchegiboshalg”anny bildi. later 3.2 3.2. ACC tire.3POSS empty.PST .PRT .ACC know.3 PST ‘Muratdecidedtogotothestorewithhis(own)car.Thenhe(o)foundoutthat his/itstirewasflat.’ o (2,3) [sho (2,3); bu (1,3)] (139) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’

176 O barynda tamashaetdi. 3.2 everyone surprisedo.3PST ‘It/hesurprisedeveryone.’ o(1,3,1,1) Ø(2,3,2,3); bu (2,4,2,2); sho (1,3,2,3); shu (1,3,2,3)

5.6. Questionnaire data on sho

Sincethenumberofcorpusreferentsfor sho thatarecodedasinfocusissmall,

oneofthegoalsofthequestionnaireistotestwhetherornottheuseof sho isrestrictedto

referentsthatarenotinfocus.Thequestionnaireprovidesfurtherdatathat sho occurs

mostfrequentlywithatmostactivatedentities,butitalsospecificallyteststhe

acceptabilityof sho withtwotypesofinfocusreferentswhichdonotoccurwith sho in

thecorpusstudy:eventsandsubjectsoftheprevioussentence.Intheanalysisofthe

questionnairedata,Ialsoconsiderwhetherornottwomorespecificconstraintsmight

applyto sho :aconstraintagainstindividualhumanreferentsoraconstraintagainst

referencetoprevioussubjects.

Thequestionnairedatasupportsthegeneralizationthat sho prefers(ormost

commonlyrefersto)entitiesthatarenotinfocus—morespecifically,nonhumanentities

thatarenotthesubjectoftheprevioussentence.Sentencecontinuationsprovidethemost

naturaldataonthetypesofentitieswhichcanbereferredtowith sho .Thethree

sentencesusedtoelicitsentencecontinuationswith sho aregivenin(140)(142),with fouroftheresponsesentencesillustratedin(142)(a)–(d).85

85 Oneresponseinthissectionwasnotconsideredbecauseitused sho asadeterminerratherthana pronoun.

177 (140) Muallimstudentgekitapberdi. (sho) teacherstudent. DAT bookgive.3PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ (4responsesuse sho torefertothebook;1responseuses sho torefertothegiving ofthebook) (141) Ibrag’im restoranda shorpaashady. (sho) Abraham restaurant. LOC soup eat.3PST ‘Abrahamatesoupintherestaurant.’ (3responsesuse sho torefertotherestaurant;2use sho torefertothesoup) (142) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy.(sho) Bolathorse.3POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’ (a) Shonu aiag”y avurtdu. 3.3. GEN leg.3POSS be.injured.3PST ‘Hislegwasinjured.’ (b) Bolat shog”ar dag’y minmezhek. Bolat 3.3. DAT more ride.3 FUT ‘Bolatwillnotridehimanymore.’ (c) Bolat sholaĭ ĭyg”ylag”any birinchigezik. Bolat 3.3. MOD fall. PST .PRT .3POSS first time ‘Itwasthefirsttimehehadfallenlikethat.’ (d) Ol shog”ar k”amuchu iamanurg”an edi. 3.2 3.3. DAT whip bad hit. PST .PRT AUX .3 PST ‘Hewhippedhimhard.’

In(140)and(141),thecontinuationsuse sho torefertoentitiesthatareinanimateandnot mentionedasthesubjectoftheprevioussentence:thebook(4times)orthegivingofthe book(once)in(140)andtherestaurant(3times)orthesoup(twice)in(141).Ineachof thesecases,thereisnocodingcriterionbywhichthereferentof sho isautomatically codedasinfocus,thoughitispossible,asexplainedabove,thatatleasttheentities mentionedasdirectobjectsmaybeinfocus.Themajorityofresponsestosentence 178 (142),showasimilarpreferencefornonhumanentitiesthatarenotcodedasinfocus,as shownin(b)–(d),wherethereferentsarethehorse(2responses)ortheactofridinga horse(1response).86 Thecontinuationin(a),however,uses sho torefertoBolat,a

humanentitythatismentionedasthesubjectoftheprevioussentenceandthusmeetsa

sufficientcriterionofareferentinfocus.Theresponsesfromthissectionaloneindicate

that sho referstoanonhumanentitythatisnotinfocusmuchmorefrequentlythantoa

humanentitythatisinfocus,butthatthisisnotanabsoluterestriction.

Additionalevidencefortheabsenceofaconstraintagainstentitiesinfocusisthe

useof sho witheventsintroducedbyaprevioussentence.Inthequestionnairedata,the

acceptabilityof sho inreferencetoeventsisillustratedbothbyfillintheblankcases

(wheretheblankisassociatedwithaneventreferent)andgrammaticalityjudgmentsof

sentencesinwhich sho referstoaneventmentionedbytheprevioussentence.Infillin

theblankresponses,likethoseillustratedin(143), sho isnotonlyacceptable,butisused

moreoftenthananyotherpronominalforminreferencetotheeventdenotedbythe previoussentence. 87 Ingrammaticalityjudgments,everyuseof sho inreferencetoan

eventisratedasacceptablebyallparticipants,asillustratedin(144)and(145).

86 Thisactivityiscodedas‘activated’ratherthan‘infocus’duetothefactthatthisactivityisintroduced, notbythematrixverbphrase, ĭyg”yldy ‘fell’butbythesubordinateclauseverbphrase, atynaminip barag”anda‘whenriding(his)horse’. 87 Thoughthereferentof sho in(143)and(144)isinterpretedhereasaneventwhichisintroduceddirectly bythepreviousclauseand,thus,infocus,inlaterstagesoftheresearchaquestionhasarisenastowhether thesearereferencesstrictlytothetimeoftheeventandthatthetimeisnotnecessarilyintroduceddirectly bythepreviousclause.However,evenwithoutthesetwoexamples,thereisstilltheevidencefrom(145), whichisaclearreferencetoaneventandillustratestheacceptabilityof sho withthistypeofreferent.

179 (143) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST

shondan songmuallimstudentni k”olun aldy. PRO. ABL after teacher student.GEN hand.3 POSS .ACC take.3 PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.Afterthat,theteachershookthestudent’s hand.’ shondan (4times); ondan (1time) (144) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST Shondan songmuallimstudentni k”olun aldy. 3.3. ABL after teacher student.GEN hand.3 POSS .ACC take.3 PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.After__theteachershookthe student’shand.’ [correspondstothefillintheblankexamplein(143).] sho isgood(1,1,2,1) (145) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolathorse.3POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST

Sho tiunegiun boldu. 3.3 yesterday hapen.3 PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Ithappenedyesterday.’ sho (2,1,2,1)

Thoughtheuseof sho inreferencetoeventsisenoughtoconfirmthat sho canbe usedwithentitiesinfocus,anotherpossibilitywhichshouldbeexplorediswhetherornot theuseof sho forsomespeakersismorespecificallylimitedtoentitieswhicharenot introducedassubjectsintheprevioussentence.Asmentionedinthebeginningofthis section,oneresponseinthesentencecontinuationportionofthequestionnairealready suggeststhelackofasubjectconstraint(greenrespondent).Intheanalysisof questionnairedatafromothersections,theabsenceof sho inreferencetoprevious subjectsinthefillintheblanksectionandtheresultsofexample(146)inthe

180 grammaticalityjudgmentportionsuggestthatusewithprevioussubjectsisnotacceptable forallspeakers.

(146) Mariamanasyna alma berdi. Mariammother.3POSS .DAT apple give.3 PST ‘Mariamgaveanappleto(her)mother.’ O shog”ar raziligin bildirdi. 3.2 3.3. DAT pleasure.3POSS .ACC know. CAUS .3PST ‘She(o)expressedherpleasureto/ather.’ (2,3)

Inthisexample,where sho mostlikelyreferstoMariam,onerespondentjudgesthe acceptabilityas“2”,whiletheothergivesa“3”. 88 (Notethattherespondentwhojudges thisexampleas“2”isnotthesamerespondentwhogivesasentencecontinuationwith sho referringtoaprevioussubject.)Furtheranalysis,howeversuggeststhatthese judgmentsaredifficulttodistinguishinthisdatasetfromanapparentconstraintfor some,butnotall,speakersagainstusing sho withhumanreferents. 89 (Notethatblueis oneoftworespondentswhodoesn’tuse sho withhumans.)

Section4ofthequestionnaireprovideslessdirectevidencethat sho isacceptable,

atleastforsomespeakers,whenusedinreferencetoprevioussubjects.Inanumberof

examplesinthissectioninwhichthepronoun sho isusedandmultipleinterpretationsare

88 Thiswasanalternativeexamplethatdidnotappearongreen’sandred’squestionnaires,andtheorange respondent’sanswersonthegrammaticalityjudgmentsweredisqualified.Thereispossiblysome unexpectedabiguitywhichwouldallowforthereferenttobeunderstoodastheappleorthegivingofthe apple. 89 Unfortunatelythequestionnairedoesnotprovideanydirectdataontheacceptabilityof sho inreference toanonhumansubjectofthepreviousclause.Section4hasindirectdatathatreferencetopreviousnon humansubjectsisacceptable.

181 possible,respondentsselectareferentwhichismentionedasthesubjectoftheprevious sentence.Example(147)illustratesoneofthesecaseswheretworespondentsidentify thereferentof sho astheteacher.

(147) Muallimstudentgekitapberdi. teacherstudent. DAT bookgive.3PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ Sho barynda tamashaetdi. 3.2 everyone surprisedo.3PST ‘It/(s)hesurprisedeveryone.’ (2respondentsidentifytheteacherasthereferentofsho ;2respondentsidentify thebookorthegivingofthebookasthereferentof sho )

Forthistypeofdata,however,identifyingthereferentofapronounisnotnecessarilythe equivalenttoajudgmentofacceptabilityoranindicationthatthepronounwouldbeused inthesamewayinnaturalspeech.

Unexpectedly,thequestionnairedataprovidesevidencethatsome,butnotall, speakersuse sho onlywithnonhumanreferents.Twodifferentrespondents(redand blue),use sho onlyfornonhumans,regardlessofcognitivestatusorthegrammaticalrole ofthepreviousmention.Theserespondentsdonotuse sho torefertohumansinany continuationsorfillintheblanksentencesandjudgeallexampleswhichuse sho in referencetoahumanindividualasunacceptable. 90 Thisisillustratedforboth respondentsby(148)and(149),wherethereferentisactivated,andforonerespondentin

90 Sincethecorpusdataincludestokensof sho thatrefertohumangroupsbutnottohumanindividualsand thegrammaticalityjudmentsdonottesttheacceptabilityof sho withhumangroups,thereisenough evidencetosuggestaconstraintagainsthumanindividuals,butnotenoughtosuggestaconstraintagainst groupsofhumans.

182 (146),wherethereferentisinfocus.Theunacceptabilityof sho withtheactivated,non subjectreferentsuggeststhatonlythehumanparameteraffectstheacceptabilityof sho

forthesetworespondents. 91

(148) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. Song muallimshonu teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST then teacher 3.3. ACC ekzameni bulan k”utlady. exam.3 POSS with congratulate.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.Thentheteachercongratulatedhim/her on(his/her)exam.’ sho (3,4,3,4) [oand bu arejudgedtobeacceptable:cf.(126)bu (1,2,2,3);o (2,1)] Ontheotherhand,asshowninexample(149)fromthegrammaticalityjudgment portionofthequestionnaire,onerespondent(green)judgestheuseof sho withahuman

individualtobeacceptable(inthiscase,thereferentismentionedasthedativeobjectof

theprevioussentence).Thesamerespondentalsouses sho inreferencetoahumaninthe

continuationtestshownpreviouslyin(142)(a).

(149) Muallimstudentgekitapberdi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’

91 Itispossiblethatjudgmentsofunacceptabilityarerelatedtotherankingofparticipants.Inotherwords, ifthereisahumanreferentintheprevioussentence,and sho specializesinindicatingthelowerrankedof twoormoreentities,assubsequentsectionsofthisstudyclaim,thenthejudgmentofunacceptabilitycould relatetothefactthat sho shouldnotbeusedwithahigherrankingentities(intermsofcognitivestatus and/oranimacy)ratherthanageneralconstraintagainstusewithhumans.

183 Sho savbol dedi. 3.3 thank.yousay.3PST (2,3,4,4)

Thedatafromthegrammaticalityjudgmentin(149)couldbeconsideredsomewhat

questionable,astheotherthreerespondentsjudgethisuseof sho heretobeunacceptable.

Inadditiontothiscase,however,therearetwodifferentanswersfromtwoother

respondentsthatindicatethatusing sho torefertoindividualhumanreferentsispossible.

Inthefillintheblankresponsein(150),wheretherespondentsarealsoaskedtoidentify

thereferentofthepronoun,onerespondent(orange)uses sho inreferencetoPatimat

(objectoftheprevioussentence).

(150) MuallimPatimatg”a kitap berdi. teacher Patamat. DAT book give Sho (Patimat)Ø(kitap) këp ushatdy. Song, ol u’ĭge getdi. 3.3 muchplease.3PST later 3.2 home. DAT go.3PST ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook.Shewasverypleased(withit).Latershewent home.’ Inadditiontotheevidencefrom(150),in(146)anotherrespondent(black)judges sho to beacceptableinreferencetoMariam.Notethatblackisinconsistentandjudges sho as

unacceptablein(149)and(148),wherethereferentishumanandactivated,suggesting

noconsistentparameter. 92 Basedonthesethreeexamplesfromthreedifferent respondents,thereisnoevidenceofaconstraintagainsthumanreferentsamongall

92 Iftheblackrespondentunderstands sho torefertoareferentotherthanMariamin(146),theresponses wouldbemoreconsistent..

184 speakers.Iassumethatthisisapreferencewhichisnotfullygrammaticizedasa constraintorperhapsvariesbyageorregion.Theunacceptabilityofcertainexamples where sho isusedforahumanmayalsodependsolelyonthenumberandtypesof activatedentitiesandrankingtendenciesdiscussedinthenextsection(5.8).

Tosummarizethissection,thequestionnairedataindicatethat,whilethereisa strongtendencyagainsttheuseof sho forreferentsthatareinfocus,especiallywhen thesearehuman,thiscannotbeconsideredanactualconstraint,butitisconsistentwith thepresenceofastrongimplicaturethatthereferentisnotinfocus.

5.7. Questionnaire data on shu

Thequestionnairedatashowninsentences(151)and(152)providesevidencethat thepronoun shu isacceptableforatleastsomespeakers.Inthesentencecontinuation portion,allrespondentsuse shu inacontinuationofthesentenceillustratedin(151)to refertothebook(onerepresentativeresponseisprovided).

(151) Muallimstudentgekitap berdi.(shu) teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST

Student shundan tapmuruvlaretezhek. student3.4. ABL assignment make.3FUT ‘Thestudentwilldoanassignmentfromit.’

Inthefillintheblanksection shu isusedtwotimesbythesame(blue)respondent.One ofthesecasesisillustratedin(152).

(152) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’ 185 Shu nechikbolmabola? 3.4 how happen. INF happen. PR ‘Howcouldit be/happen?’(blue) Inthegrammaticalityjudgmentportion,however,theuseof shu receivesa significantnumberofunacceptablejudgments.Onerespondent(black)judgesall exampleswith shu tobe“3”or“4”—thatis‘possible,butpeopledon’tusuallysayitthat way’,or‘noKumykpersonwouldsayitthatway’.Theuseof shu inreferencetohuman

entitiesisjudgedunacceptableinallbutonecase,whichisshowninthegreenresponse

toexample(153).93 Notethat,inthisexample,thejudgmentsfor shu areverysimilarto thoseof sho .

(153) Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. Shu savbol dedi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST 3.4 thank.yousay.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.(S)hesaidthankyou.’ shu (4,4,2,4) [o’ziu (3,4,3,4); bu(2,2,2,2); Ø(4,4,4,3); o(2,2); sho (3,4,2,4)]

Theuseof shu inreferencetononhumanentitiesissomewhatmoreacceptable,

thoughthemajorityofresponses(8outof14)stilljudgethesereferencestobe

unacceptable.Twoexamplesofjudgmentsof shu inreferencetoaninanimateentityare

illustratedin(154)and(155),wherethegrammaticalityjudgmentsare2334and332

2,respectively.

93 Thoughresponsesfromtheorangerespondentarenotincludedinthediscussionofacceptability judgmentsinthissectionbecauseonlyonesentencewasjudgedunacceptable,itisworthyofnotethatthe oneunacceptablesentencewasthiscaseoftheuseof shu inreferencetothestudentin(153).

186 (154) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. Shu “Tangcholpan” edi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST 3.4 Tangcholpan be.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.ItwasTangcholpan.’ shu (2,3,3,4) [bu (3,1,2,3); Ø(3,4); o(2,4); sho (1,1,2,1)]

(155) Muallimstudentge kitap berdi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST

Shundan songmuallimstudentni k”olun aldy. 3.4.ABL afterteacher student. GEN hand.3POSS .ACC take.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.After__theteachershookthestudent’s hand.’ shu (3,3,2,2) [sho (1,1,2,1);Ø(1,1,3,1); o(1,1,2,4); bu(2,4,3,4)]

Incomparingthesetwoexamples,itishardtodetermineanycriteriafordegreeof

acceptabilitybasedonthecontext,asthebluerespondentjudges shu tobeacceptablein

(154)butnotin(155),whiletheredandgreenrespondentsjudge shu tobeacceptablein

(155)butnotin(154).(Theblackrespondentjudges shu tobeunacceptableinallcases.)

Itisalsointerestingthat sho isconsistentlyacceptableinalluseswithinanimateentities,

while shu isnot.Giventhevariabilityofresults,thereisnotenoughevidencetopredict

whentheuseof shu isacceptable;nevertheless,thereisenoughevidencetosuggestthat

thereisnorestrictiontoentitiesinfocus,and,thus,onecanclaimthat shu ,totheextent

thatitisusedatall,signalsthatitsreferentsareactivated.

5.8. Evidence for implicatures

Thissectiondiscussesevidencefromthequestionnairethattheuseofforms hypothesizedtosignal‘activated’onthebasisofthecorpusstudy,thatis, o, sho ,and shu , sometimesgiverisetotheimplicature‘notinfocus’.Inadditiontothis,basedonthe

187 questionnaireresultsdescribedinsection5.4,therevisedpredictionforthepronoun bu is thatitsignals‘activated’,ratherthan‘infocus’;thereforethissectionincludesa discussionofanyevidencethat bu alsoimplicates‘notinfocus’.Sinceimplicatures typicallyariseonlywhentheinformationsignaledbyastrongerformisrelevant,they generallyarisewithpronominalformswhentheyfunctiontodifferentiatebetweentwoor morepossiblereferentsofthesameminimalstatus(e.g.bothatleastactivated)by implicatingthatoneofthemisnotinfocus.Thisphenomenonisparticularlynoticeable inutterancesinwhichtwoormoreentitiesarereferredtowithpronouns.Forexample, inacontextwhereanullargument and sho areusedtorefertodifferententities,oneof whichisinfocusandoneofwhichis(atmost)activated,thenullargumentnecessarily referstothereferentinfocus(sincenullsrequireareferentinfocus),while sho isusedto refertothereferentthatisatmostactivated,eventhough,inprinciple,itcouldreferto eitherone.Eveninacontextinwhichthespeakerreferstoonlyoneofmultiplepossible activatedentities,theuseofanyoftheformswhichonlysignalactivation,thatis, bu , o, sho ,or shu ,canimplicate‘notinfocus’. 94

Evidencebothfrompreviousstudies(Mulkern2003)andfromthecurrent analysisshowsthattheimplicature‘notinfocus’issometimesextendedtodifferentiate betweentworeferentsofthesamemaximalstatus(e.g.bothinfocusorbothatmost

activated)byimplicatingthatoneislesssalient(thatis,lessinfocusorlessactivated)

thantheother.Inacontextwithmultiplepronominalreferences,ifthisrankingeffectis

94 Note,however,thatimplicaturesdonotnecessarilyariseincontextswhichrequireanovertformforan independentreason,suchasphonologicalfocusorotherfocuscontexts(cf.example(104)).

188 extendedinordertodifferentiatebetweentworeferentsinfocusonewouldexpect,for example,thatanullargument(oranyformsignalinginfocus)couldbeusedwiththe referentofgreatersalienceand sho (oranyformsignalingactivated)couldbeusedwith

thereferentoflessersalience.Inthecaseofreferencetotwoentitiesthatareatmost

activated, sho oranyotherovertpronouncouldbeusedtorefertothelesssalientone,but

apronounsignalinginfocuscannotbeusedwithanentitythatisatmostactivated;thus,

anotherovertpronounwouldbeused.Ifallovertpronounscanimplicate‘notinfocus’

(orlesssalient),thenthereisnologicalbasisfordifferentiatingtherelativesalienceof

twoentitiesthatarebothatmostactivated.Iproposethat,forthisreason,oneofthefour

overtpronounsinKumyk( sho )hasbecomespecializedincommunicatingthe

implicature‘notinfocus’and,byextension,designatingthelesssalientoftwoentities.

Boththecorpusstudyandquestionnairedatasupporttheideathattheuseof sho hasa

strongcorrelationwithentitiesthatareoflowerrelativecognitivestatusorless prominent.Inparticular,section4ofthequestionnairedemonstratesthat sho ismuch morelikelythanotoimplicate‘notinfocus’. 95

Finally,thereanalysisof bu raisesthequestionofthedegreetowhichthisformis

usedtoimplicate‘notinfocus’.Followingtheargumentationaboveaboutthe

relationshipbetweenimplicaturesandtherelativesalienceofreferents,thisanalysis

looksattheuseof bu comparedtootherpronominalformsincontextswithmultiple

95 Thegoalofthissectionofthequestionnairewasprimarilytoexplorethedifferenceindistribution between oand sho ,twoformshypothesizedtosignal‘activated’.Thequestionnairedidnottest sho in relationto bu inthesameway,as bu wasoriginallyhypothesizedtosignal‘infocus’basedonthecorpus results. Shu wasnottestedinthiswayduetotheoverallrarityoftheuseofthisform.

189 possiblereferents.Theresultsshowthat,whiletheuseof sho oftenimplicatestheless

saliententity, bu appearstohaveaspecializedfunctionofindicatingthemostsalientof

twoormoreentitieswiththesameminimalcognitivestatus.Thiswouldaccountforthe

factthat bu almostexclusivelyreferstoentitiesinfocusinthecorpus,eventhough,asthe

questionnairedataindicates,itisnotabsolutelyrestrictedtoentitiesinfocus,andforthe

factthat bu israrelyassociatedwiththeimplicature,‘notinfocus’.

5.8.1. Overtpronouns,topicshift,andimplicatures Inanalyzingtheuseof bu , o, sho and shu tocreatescalarimplicatures,itis importanttoexplorefactsaboutthedistributionandinterpretationoftheseformswithin simpleandcomplexclausestructures.Whilethesefactsaregenerallyconsideredtobe syntactic,theymay,infact,bederivedfromtheimplicaturescreatedbytheuseofovert formsversusnullarguments.

InKumyk,nonreflexiveovertpronounsarealwaysinterpretedasnotcoreferent withasubjectwithinthesameclause,asshowninthesecondsentenceof(156).96

(156) Bolat io’ziuniu i uchun kitap aldy. Bolatself.3POSS .GEN forbookbuy.3S.PST ‘Bolatboughtabookforhimself.’

96 Oisusedasarepresentativeofanovertpronoun,asitissyntacticallyinterchangeablewith bu , sho ,or shu .Notethatanonreflexivepronouncanbeusedinreferencetoanominalwithinthesamenoun phrase—suchas Katjavaonuk”ardashlar ‘Katyandherfriends’.

190 Leĭla j og”ar i/*j da kitapaldy. Leyla3.2. DAT EMPH book buy.3. PST ‘Leyla iboughtabookforhim/*herself,too.’

Thesecondsentenceof(156),showsthatthepronoun og”ar(dative form of o ),cannot beinterpretedascoreferentwith Leĭla ,thesubjectoftheclauseinwhichitoccurs,but canbeinterpretedascoreferentwithBolat,theentitymentionedasthesubjectofthe previoussentence.Inthesametypeofcontext,areflexivecanbecoreferentialwiththe subject,asshowninthefirstsentenceof(156),where o’ziuniu iscoreferentwiththe subject, Bolat .

Anotherrestrictiononcoreferentialityisthatanovertnonreflexivesubject pronouninoneclauseisalwaysinterpretedasnotcoreferentwiththesubjectofa conjoiningclauseofthesamesentence,asillustratedin(157).

(157) [Naima i getgende,] o*i/j ĭylady. Naima go. PST .PRT .LOC 3.2 cry.3. PST ‘WhenNaimaleft,he/she(notNaima)cried.’

In(157),theovertpronoun omustbeinterpretedasnotcoreferentwith Naima ,the subjectoftheprecedingnonfiniteclause.Inthesamecontext,eitheranullargumentor areflexivecouldbeusedtoexpressacoreferentsubject,asillustratedin(158).97

97 Interestingly,theuseofnullarguments,ontheotherhand,isdifficulttodescribeintermsof coreferentialityrestrictions.Asshownin(i),anullargumentinoneclauseofacomplexclause constructiondoesnotnecessarilyhavetobeinterpretedascoreferentwiththesubjectoftheotherclause. Whetherthenulloccursinthematrixclause,asin(a),orintheprecedingnonfiniteclause,asin(b),itcan beinterpretedasreferringeithertoBolatortoNaima.

191 (158) [Naima i getgende,] Øi / o’ziu iĭylady. Naima go. PST .PRT .LOC 3S cry.3. PST ‘WhenNaimaleft,she(Naima)cried.’

Therestrictionsoncoreferentialityincomplexclauseconstructionsdonot, however,applytocaseswhereanonsubjectofoneclauseiscoreferentwithasubjectof anadjoiningclause,asillustratedin(159),wherethepronoun onu canbeinterpretedas

referringtoNaimaorsomeoneotherthanBolat,butnottoBolat.

(159) [Naima i getgende,] Bolat j onu i/*j arysyndanĭylady. Naima go. PST .PRT .LOC Bolat3S aftercry.3. PST ‘WhenNaimaleft,Bolatcriedafterher.’

Astheseexamplesshow,withintheunitoftheclause/sentence, oisunderstoodas notcoreferentwiththenearestsubject—whetherwithinthesameclause,asin(156),or withinanadjoiningclauseasin(157).98 Sincethementionofareferentinsubject positionisassociatedwithgreaterrelativesalience,theuseoftheovertpronouncanbe saidtoimplicate‘notthemostsalientreferent’.In(159),however,thepronoun onu does

(i) Bolat iNaimany jkëpsiuedi. BolatNaima. ACC muchlove.3PST

a.[Naima i getgende, ] Øi/j ĭy lady. Naima PST .PRT .LOC cry.3. PST “WhenNaimaleft,(Naima)/(Bolat)cried.”

b.[ Øi/j getgende, ] Naima ĭy lady. PST .PRT .LOC Naima cry.3. PST “WhenNaimaleft,(Naima)/(Bolat)cried.” 98 Notethatanonreflexivepronouncanbeusedinreferencetoanominalwithinthesamenounphrase— suchas Lindaandherfriends .

192 notimplicate‘notinfocus’,sinceitcanrefertoNaima,areferentwhichisatleastin focus.

Whileovertpronounshavecertaincoreferentialityrestrictionswithinsentences, theiruseisnotconstrainedinthesamewayinintersententialcontexts.Forexample,in

(160)(repeatedfromChapter1),theuseofanovertpronouninthesecondsentencecan refereithertothereferentofthedativeobject erine (Salimat’shusband)ortothereferent

ofthesubjectoftheprevioussentence(Salimat). 99

(160) Salimat ierine j ashete. Song, oi/j savutsaba zhuĭe. Salimathusband.3POSS .DAT foodmake. PR later3.2 S dishes wash. PR ‘Salimatfixesherhusbandameal.Laters(he)washesthedishes.

Basedonexampleslike(160),thepronoun oisnotautomaticallyinterpretedasnot coreferentwiththesubjectoftheprevioussentence.Corpusdata,likewise,provide evidenceofanumberofovertsubjectpronounsthathavereferentsthatarementionedas subjectsoftheprevioussentence. 100 (While‘notthesubject’isbydefinitionasubsetof

‘notinfocus’,theGivennessHierarchycategory‘notinfocus’isbroaderandisnot

solelydefinedonthebasisofsyntacticcriteria.)Suchdatais,however,consistentwith

theideathatovertpronounssometimesimplicatethattheirreferentsarenotinatthe

centerofattention(infocus)and,hence,nottheprevioustopic.

99 Itisalsopossibleherethatthepronounreferstoanentitynotmentionedintheprevioussentence. 100 Itshouldbenotedthatovertpronounsareusedmuchmoreoftenthannullsinanonsubjectpositionin referencetoanentitywhichismentionedasthesubjectoftheprevioussentence,asin(156),whereog”ar referstoBolat.

193 WithintheGHmodel,theapparentassociationinanumberoflanguages, includingKumyk,betweenovertpronounsandtopicshiftisoneofthenatural consequencesoftheimplicature‘notinfocus’whichcanbeattributedtoanyformwhich signals‘activated’aspartofitsconventionalmeaning.Inastudyofpronominalsubjects inTurkish,Enç(1986)alsosuggeststhatthesignalingoftopicshiftisnotthemostbasic functionofovertor‘semanticallyredundant’pronouns,butthattheindicationofcontrast istheprimaryfunctionofovertpronounsandtopicshiftisasubcategoryofthisfunction

(204206). 101 Ençexplorestheideathattheeffectoftheuseofanovertpronounwhena nullargumentwouldbemoreefficient(“parsimonious”)conveys“extrapragmatic information”whichcanbederivedbasedonGrice’sMaximofQuantity,butrejectsit, presumablybecauseshedoesnothaveanoverallframeworkwithinwhichitcanbe situated.ViewedwithintheGHmodel,theideathatovertformsversusnullscanbe associatedwiththescalarimplicature‘notinfocus’basedontheMaximofQuantity followsasaconsequenceoftheunidirectionalentailmentofcognitivestatuseswithinthe theoreticalmodel.Incaseswheretheimplicatureisblocked,theuseofalowerformon thehierarchy(inthiscase,anovertform)maystillbesaidtoconveyaddedcontextual effects(seealsoMulkern2003)suchascontrastorunexpectedness.

101 Ençcallsthesspronounssemanticallyredundantsincetheirreferentisrecoverablefrompersonand numbermarkingontheverb.

194 5.8.2. Implicatures,frequency,andspecialization Whiletheimplicature‘notinfocus’maybeattributedtoanyformsignaling activated,myanalysisofthedistributionoftheseformssuggeststhatthisimplicatureis moreoftenassociatedwithoneparticularpronoun–thatistheform sho –thananyofthe otherformssignaling‘activated’.While sho israrelyusedforinfocusreferents, oand

bu areoftenusedinsuchcontexts,includingcontextswheretherearecompeting

referents. 102 Thepronoun oisfrequentlyusedinthecorpusstudyandinvariouspartsof

thequestionnaire(includingsentencecontinuations)inreferencetoaninfocusentity

includingthoseintroducedbythesubjectofaprevioussentence(seesection5.5),aswell

asotherreferentsthatareclearlyinfocus.Theform sho ,ontheotherhand,refersonly oncetothesubjectofaprevioussentenceinthecorpusandrarelyreferstoareferent categorizedasinfocusbasedonanyothercriteria.Thequestionnairedatalikewise suggeststhattheuseofthisformwithreferentsmentionedpreviouslyassubjectsis limitedorevenunacceptablewithsomespeakers(cf.section5.6).Thefactthat sho consistentlyprefersentitiesthatarementionedasnonsubjectargumentsoftheprevious sentenceandarenotinfocusisstrongevidencethatthisformfrequentlygivesrisetothe implicature‘notinfocus’,evensuggestingthattheassociationbetween sho andthis

implicaturehasbecomeconventionalizedforsomespeakers.

102 TheassumptionbasedontheinteractionoftheGHmodelwithpragmaticprinciplesisthat,scalar implicaturesaremostlikelyincaseswithcompetingreferents;henceitisunusualforaformwhichsignals ‘activated’tonotcreatetheimplicature‘notinfocus’inacontextwithmultiplereferentswiththesame minimalstatus.

195 IuseSection4ofthequestionnairetoverifyrespondents’intuitionsaboutwhatthe useof sho (incontrastto o)implicatesaboutthereferentincontextswithmultiple possiblereferentsofdifferentstatuses.Infourpairsofsentencesconstructedtoallowfor

referenceambiguity,ifaformcreatesanimplicature‘notinfocus’,thenrespondents

shouldconsistentlypickouttheentitywhichisnotinfocusasthereferentofthatform.

Foreachsentencepairtherearetwoversionswhichdifferonlyinwhether oor sho is

usedinthesecondsentence,andrespondentsareaskedtoidentifythereferentofthe pronoun.(Althoughtheseareshownhereinsummaryform,inthequestionnairethe

versionsusing oversus sho wereinterspersedinrandomorderratherthanshownsideby

side.)Inexamples(161)(164)eachpairincludesatleastonepossiblereferentcodedas

infocusandatleastonethatisatmostactivated.Someoftheseexamplesalsoinclude

referentswhichhavethesamecognitivestatusbutdifferinanimacyfeatures.Inexample

(165),thetwopossiblereferentshavethesamecognitivestatus,butoneisintroducedby

aclausewhiletheotherisintroducedbyanominal.

Differentexamplesdifferinthedegreetowhichthereisacorrelationbetweenthe

useof sho andtheidentificationofthereferentasoneoflowercognitivestatus.In(161),

forexample,thepronouninthesecondsentencecanrefereithertoBolat,mentionedas

thesubjectoftheprevioussentenceandinfocus,ortothehorse,mentionedasthedative

objectofasubordinateclauseoftheprevioussentenceandconsideredtobeatmost

activated.

(161) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST

196 Og”ar/Shog”ar bolg”an zat boldumu? PRO. DAT happen.3PST .PRT thing happen.3PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.DidanythinghappentoPRO?’

When oisused,allfiverespondentsidentifythereferentasBolat,butwhenthepronoun sho isused,threerespondentsidentifythereferentasBolat,andtwo,asthehorse.The

factthatthreerespondentsidentifythereferentof sho asareferentinfocusshowthatthis

formisnotrestrictedtoreferentsthatarenotinfocus—inotherwords,that‘notinfocus’

isanimplicature,notpartoftheconventionalmeaningoftheform.Thefactthatthe

implicaturedoesnotariseinthesecasesmaybeduetoastrongnaturaltendencyforthe

readertoexpectthesecondsentencetobeaboutBolat,thesubjectoftheprevious

sentenceandthemostlikelyobjectofconcerninmostpeople’sreadingofthesentence.

Inlightofthetendencyfortopiccontinuity,thetworesponseswhichidentifythereferent

of sho asthehorsearesignificant,showingthestrongassociationbetweenthisformand

theimplicature‘notinfocus’(eventhoughtheydonotrepresentthemajorityresponseto

thistestsentence).

Theresponsestoexample(162)showonlyaslightlystrongercorrelationbetween

theuseof sho andtheentitywithlowercognitivestatus(activated).

(162) Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ O/Sho barynda tamashaetdi. 3.2/3.3 everyone surprisedo.3PST ‘It/(s)hesurprisedeveryone.’(Alternately,‘(S)hewassurprisedbyeverything.’)

197 In(162)fourrespondentsidentifythereferentofoastheteacher,whileonerespondent

(red)identifiesthereferentasthestudent(seealternatefreetranslation).Onlytwo respondentsidentifythereferentof sho astheteacher,withtheremainingthree identifyingthereferentasthebook(twice)orthegivingofthebook.

In(163),wherethepronouncanrefereithertotheprevioussubject, Ansar (in focus),orthesituationinvolvingthegivingtheapple(activated),theuseof oversus sho correlatesstronglywiththecognitivestatusofthereferent.

(163) Ansaranasyna alma berdi. O/Sho rag”muluedi. Ansarmother.3POSS .DAT apple give.3PST PROs weet be.3PST ‘Ansargavehismotheranapple.PROwassweet.’

AllfiverespondentsidentifyAnsar,aninfocusreferent,asthereferentof o,andfourout

offiveidentifythereferentof sho astheactivatedentity—thegivingtheapple(thefifth

respondentidentifiesthereferentof sho asAnsar).

Example(164),whichisbasedonanexcerptfromthetextcorpus,isabitmore complicatedinthesensethatthecontextcontainsfourpossibleentitiesforreferenceat thetimeofutteranceofthefinalclause:1)thewoman(infocus),2)thestorekeeper(in focus),3)theaccordion(infocus),and4)themoney(activated).

(164) Song k”atyngishi tiukenge gire. Tiukenchi bulan sëĭleĭ; later woman store. DAT come.in. PR storekeeper with speak. PR

og”ar arg”anyn sata. Ø(accordian)Satyp, 3.2. DAT accordian.3POSS .ACC sell. PR sell.GER

198 ak”chany da alyp, og”ar/shog”ar k”aramady. money. ACC also take. GER PRO look.at.3PST ‘Laterthewomangoesintothestore.(She)talkswiththestorekeeper;(she)sells himtheaccordion.Havingsoldit,takingthemoney,shedidnotlookatit(organ ormoney)/him.’

Intestingthepronounalternationinthefinalsentence,Iassumethatthereferent

willbeidentifiedasoneofthreepreviouslymentionedentities:thestorekeeper,the

accordion,orthemoney. 103 Whenthepronoun oisusedinthefinalsentence,allfour respondentsidentifythereferentasthestorekeeper,theonlyhumanreferentthatisin focus.Whenthepronoun sho isused,theresponsesaredividedbetweenthestorekeeper

(once),themoney(once)andtheaccordion(twice).104 Whilethereisonlyone respondentwhoidentifiesthereferentof sho asanactivatedentity,itisinterestingthat,

ofthetwopossiblereferentsinfocus,tworespondentsselectaninanimateentityrather

thanthehumanentity,suggestingthattheuseof sho communicatesalowerranking

entityintermsofanimacyorcognitivestatusforatleastthreeofthefourrespondents.

Thequestionnaireincludesoneadditionalsentencepairinwhichthereferentof

theform oor sho couldbeambiguousbutinwhichthecontextisbiasedtowardstwo possiblereferentsofidenticalcognitivestatus(bothinfocus).In(165)onepossible

referentof olany/sholany (pluralaccusativeforms)isthefood,whichisintroducedasa

nominal(insubjectposition)intheprevioussentenceandtheotherisasituation

introducedbythepreviousclause–theruiningofthefood.

103 Note:Inoneinterpretation,whichisnotconsideredintheresults,therespondentassumesthatthe storekeeperwassellingtheaccordianandthestorekeeperisalsothesubjectofthelastclause. 104 Theonerespondentwhoselectedthemoneyputtheaccordianasasecondchoiceinparentheses.

199 (165) U’ĭune girip k”arasa: onda këpulluk”almag”albar. home. DAT go.in. GER look. PR .3 COND 3.2.LOC muchbigmessexist. PR G’artiurliuashlar tashlang”an, buzulg”an. eachvariousfood. PL be.thrown.down.3PST be.ruined.3PST Olany/sholany o’ziuniuanasy etgen. PRO self. GEN mother.3 POSS do.3PST ‘Comingintothehomeif(he/she)looks,thereisabigmessthere.Variousfoods arethrowndown,ruined.His/herownotherdid/madethem/thesethings.’ Fourrespondentsidentifythereferentsofboth oand sho asthefood,whileone

respondent(red)identifiesthereferentofbothformsastheruiningofthefood.These

responsesindicatenodifferenceinreferenceidentificationduetothealternationof oand sho ,eventhoughthetwopossiblereferentsarementionedamongtheresponses.These

responsessuggestthatthealternationinformisnotassociatedwithimplicaturebecause

thepossiblereferentsarenotrankedinrelationtooneanother.

Thoughtheresultsaresomewhatvariable,thereisenoughdatatoconfirmthat sho oftenimplicatesareferentwithlowercognitivestatusinrelationtoanotherpossible

referent.Atthesametime,thedatadescribedinthissectionindicatesthat oisnot

consistentlyusedtoimplicatethatareferentisnotinfocus.Infact,givenachoice betweenareferentinfocusandareferentthatisatmostactivated, oismorelikelyto

refertothereferentinfocus.

Thoughthepronoun bu isnottestedinthesamewayas oand sho (primarilydue

tomyhypothesisbasedonthecorpusstudythat bu signals‘infocus’)thereisinteresting

datainthequestionnairerelatedtosentenceswithmultiplepronounswhichshowsthat

bu ,like o,ismorelikelytorefertoanentityinfocus,givenafocusactivatedreferent

200 pair.Datafromsection3ofthequestionnaireshowsthat,insentencesinwhichmore thanoneovertpronounisused, bu consistentlyreferstoanentitythatishigherranking

thantheentityreferredtoby oor sho ,suggestingthatbu hasaparticularassociationwith

referentsofhigherrelativeprominence.Outof10sentenceswithmultipleblanks,there

are8sentencepairswhichcontainatleastonereferentinfocusandonethatiscodedas

atmostactivated,onesentencepairwherebothreferentsareinfocus,andonesentence pairwherebothareatmostactivated. 105 Theresponsestothese10testsentencescontain

11instancesoftheuseof bu ,and,foreachinstance,Ilookateachpossiblepairingofthe

referentof bu withanotherreferent.Inotherwords,inasentencewiththreeblanksin

which bu isusedonce,thereferentof bu canbepairedwithtwodifferentreferents.

Thereare16suchpairingsof bu ,andthedataonthesepairingsissummarizedinTable7, wherethefirstcolumnindicatestheexamplenumberinthequestionnaire,thesecond columngivesthepairofformsthatoneormorerespondentsusedtofillintheblanks, thethirdandfourthindicatethecognitivestatusandanimacyfeaturesofthereferentof theform,andthelastcolumnsummarizesthetypeofrankingbasedoncognitivestatus or,wherecognitivestatusisequal,basedonanimacyfeatures.

105 Aspreviouslydescribed,allentitiesmentionedassubjectsinthecontrolsentencearecodedasinfocus andnonsubjectsmentionedinthecontrolsentencearecodedasatmostactivated.

201 Example#in Pronouns Cognitive Animacy Ranking questionnaire Status DifferentMaximumCognitiveStatus 30 bu FOC Human bu>o onu ACT Human FOC>ACT 32 bu FOC Human bu>o o ACT Human FOC>ACT 34b(blue) bu 2 FOC inanimate bu>o o ACT inanimate FOC>ACT 34b bu ACT Inanimate *o>bu (2red+black) sho FOC Inanimate FOC>ACT 37 bu FOC Human bu>sho sho ACT Inanimate FOC>ACT (alsohum> inanim) 39a bu FOC Human bu>o o ACT animate(horse) FOC>ACT (alsohum>anim) 38a bu ACT animate(horse) *o>bu o FOC Human FOC>ACT (alsohum>anim) SameMaximumCognitiveStatus 31 bu ACT Human bu>shu shu ACT Inanimate hum>inanim 33 bu ACT Human bu>sho sho ACT Inanimate hum>inanim 34a(blue) bu 1 ACT Human bu>o o ACT Inanimate hum>inanim * 34a bu ACT Inanimate o>bu (2red+black) o ACT Human hum>inanim 38b bu ACT animate(horse) bu>sho sho ACT inanimate(fact) anim>inanim 39b bu FOC Human bu>sho sho FOC Inanimate(event) 106 hum>inanim 41 bu FOC Human bu>sho sho FOC inanimate hum>inanim Table7: Bu andrankedentities

106 Thisentitycouldalternatelybeinterpretedasasituation,whichwouldchangethepairtothecategory ‘differentmaximumcognitivestatus’butwouldsupportthehypothesizedranking.

202 In8oftheformpairsfromthedata,thereferentof bu hasthesamemaximum cognitivestatusastheotherreferentbutdifferentanimacyfeatures,whilein8ofthese pairsthereferentof bu hasadifferentmaximumcognitivestatus.Ofthelattercases bu

referstotheentityinfocus5times,whilein3cases(twoofwhicharedifferentresponses

tothesameexample)bu referstothelowerrankingoractivatedentity.Inoneofthe pairs(example38)where bu referstoanentitythatislowerrankingincognitivestatus,

theentityishigherrankingintermsofanimacy.

Inthe8pairswherereferentshavethesamecognitivestatusbutcanberankedin

termsofanimacyfeatures(human>animate>inanimate), bu referstothehigherranking

entityin6cases(5ofwhicharehumanvs.animate/inanimateandoneofwhichis

animatevs.inanimate).Thetwocaseswherethisrankingdoesnotholdarethesame

responses(example34,redandblack)whichresultedintwoofthecaseswhichdonot

followcognitivestatusranking,suggestingthatthereispossiblysomeotherfactor

associatedwiththeuseof bu inthisexample.Tosummarizethisanalysis,themajorityof

usesof bu insentenceswithmultiplepronominalformscorrelatewithahigherranking

referentintermsofcognitivestatuswherethemaximumcognitivestatusisdifferent,or

intermsofahierarchyofanimacywheremaximumcognitivestatusisequal.

Theproposalthattheuseof bu promptsthehearertoselectthehigherranked

amongmultiplepossiblereferentsissomewhatoutsidethestructureoftheGivenness

Hierarchymodel.TheGHmodelprovidesfortheindicationofthehigherranking

referentonlyviatheuseofformsrestrictedtoahigherstatus.Inotherwords,if bu is

restrictedtousewithformsinfocus,thenitmakessensethatitisusedwiththehigher

203 rankedoftwoforms,oneofwhichisinfocusandoneofwhichisactivated.Ontheother hand,if,asthequestionnairedataindicates, bu isnotrestrictedtoreferentsinfocus,then thepreferenceof bu forhigherrankingentitiescanonlybedescribedintermsofthe implicaturecreatedbytheuseofadifferentform(suchasthestrongcorrelationof sho withtheimplicature‘notinfocus’).Sincemostoftheusesof bu arepairedwith orather

than sho inmydata,Iwouldneedtoclaimthattheuseofoinasentencewhere bu isalso usedimplicatesthatthereferentisnotinfocusoristhelowerrankedoftwoentitiesof thesamestatus,whiletheuseof oinconjunctionwith sho doesnotimplicatethesame thing(sincepreviousanalysisindicates sho istheformmostlikelytobeassociatedwith theimplicature‘notinfocus’).Analternateandmorereasonablepossibilityisthat bu hasaspecializedfunctionasanindicatorofthehigherrankingoftwoentitieswhichare atmostactivated(whichisnotcontradictorytotheGHmodel)andthatthisfunctionis sometimesgeneralizedtoincludeindicationofactualcognitivestatusranking(orperhaps incaseswherenullsorreflexivescannotbeusedforsomereason).

Tosummarizethissection,Iclaimthattheovertpronouns bu , o, sho ,and shu can all,tosomeextent,implicate‘notinfocus’attheintersententiallevel.Thisimplicature, however,ismostoftenassociatedwiththeuseof sho .Theform bu appearstohavea specializedfunctionofindicatingthehigherrankedentityamongentitieswiththesame minimalstatus,whichleaves oastheneutralorunmarkedform.

5.8.3. Rankingdata Asaconclusiontothischapter,and,inparticular,asafollowuptodiscussionof scalarimplicatures,thissectionprovidesamorecompletediscussionoftheranking

204 effectscreatedbytheuseofdifferentformsofreferringexpressions.InnaturalKumyk discourse,itisrarefortwoidenticalpronounformstobeusedtorefertotwodifferent entitiesinthesameclauseorsentence. 107 While,inmanycases,apronounisusedfor oneentityandafullnounphrasefortheother,therearealsoanumberofinstanceswhere twodifferentpronounformsareusedfortwodifferententities,anditisoftenthecase thattheformschosenprovidesomecluewhichallowsthespeakertodisambiguate amongpotentialreferentsofthepronoun.Assumingthatnullsandreflexivessignalthat areferentisinfocus,that bu , o, sho ,and shu signalthatthereferentisatleastactivated andtovaryingdegreesimplicate‘notinfocus’,andthat sho specializesinindicatingthe lowerrankedofasetofentitieswhile bu specializesinindicatingthehigherrankedofa setofentities,Ihypothesizethatspeakersuseacombinationofthejuxtapositionofforms associatedwithdifferentstatuses,implicatures,andspecializationsinordertocreate rankingeffects.Morespecifically,ifonereferentisinfocusandtheotheratmost activatedoriftworeferentsareinfocus,arankingeffectiscreatedbyusingnullsor reflexives(basedoncognitivestatusrestrictions)torefertothehighestranked—thatis thereferentwiththehighercognitivestatusorthemoreprominentoftworeferentsin focus,while o, sho ,and bu areusedforthelowerrankedreferent(intermsofcognitive status)orthelessprominentoftheentitiesinfocus.Therankingeffectcouldalsobe

107 InEnglish,itismorecommontousetwodifferentpronounsinthesamesentence,perhapsduetothe factthatdifferentpronominalformsallowthehearertodisambiguatereferentsbasedongenderoranimacy features,asinthefollowingsentence: PaultoldBarbaratobringhercatinsidebecausehewantedhertofeedit.

205 createdbyusing sho torefertothelessprominententityinjuxtapositionwithanother formthatsignals‘activated’—thatis o,shu ,or bu—becauseofthespecializedfunctionof sho inimplicating‘notinfocus’.Assumingthat bu alsospecializesinindicatingthe moreprominentoftwopossiblereferents,thenthejuxtapositionof bu withanotherform thatsignals‘activated’wouldalsocreatearankingeffect.Iftworeferentsareatmost activated,therankingeffectcanonlybecreatedbyjuxtaposingoneofthetwo specializedforms, sho or bu, withanotherformthatsignals‘activated’,sincetheinfocus formscannotbeusedinreferencetoentitieswhosemaximumstatusisactivated.Figure

6summarizesthepairswhichcancreaterankingeffectsbasedontheseprinciples.Note thatpairswhichusebothaformrestrictedtoinfocusreferentsand sho ,whichspecializes forlowerrankedreferents,aresomewhatredundant.Also,thepairswhichconsistofa formthatsignalsinfocusand bu aresomewhatanomalous,and,thus,notexpectedor attestedinthedata,sinceonewouldbesignalingthemorerestrictedcognitivestatusand theotherisassociatedwiththehigherrankingentity—inotherwords,bothformswould bepointingtothesameentity.

206 RankedPair BasisforRanking Øbu Cognitivestatusrestriction+ specializationforhigherranking o’ziu–bu referent Øo Cognitivestatusrestriction o’ziuo Øsho Cognitivestatusrestriction+ specializationof sho forlowest o’ziusho rankingreferent osho specializationof sho forlowest rankingreferent bu–sho bu–o specializationof bu forhigher rankingreferent Figure6 : Rankedpairs

Totesttheserankingclaims,IanalyzetenfillintheblankexercisesinSection3 ofthequestionnairewithmorethanonepronoun(blank)109 .(Aportionofthisanalysisis discussedpreviouslyinthediscussionof bu insection5.8.2.)Thequestionnaireresults supporttheideathatKumykspeakersinfrequentlyusetwoidenticalpronounstoreferto differententitiesinthesamesentence.Forthesixcaseswithtwoblanks,outofthe24 responsesexamined,onlytwouseidenticalpronounsfordifferentreferents.Likewise, outofthe16responsesforthefourcaseswiththreeblanks,onlythreeuseidentical pronounsfordifferentreferents. 110 Inalltenresponsesthereare37caseswhere2

109 InthisportionofthequestionnaireIconsideritcrucialthattherespondentsidentifythereferentofthe pronoun;thereforeIeliminateonesetofdatawheretherespondentdoesnotdothis. 110 Thereare2additionalcasesinwhichthesameformisusedfortwodifferentmentionsofthe same referent.

207 differentformsareusedforapairofreferentswithdifferentmaximumcognitivestatuses

(includingthe2caseswherethesamereferentismentionedtwiceandhasadifferent cognitivestatusatthetimeofthesecondmention)and23caseswhere2differentforms areusedwithreferentpairswiththesamemaximumcognitivestatus(allofwhichhave differencesinanimacyfeatures).Ofthe37pairsofformswithreferentsrankedfor cognitivestatus,26(70.3%)correlatewiththepossiblerankingslistedinFigure6,with themostcommonformpairbeing o–sho (13instances,or35.1%).Ofthe23pairsof

referentswiththesamemaximumstatus,19(82.6%)correlatewiththefollowing

animacyranking:human>animate>inanimate.Inthelattercase,the o–sho pairisalso

themostfrequent(11instances,or47.8%).Interestingly,thereare5unexpectedusesof

nullargumentswithentitiesassumedtobeatmostactivated(3frompairswhereone

referentisinfocusandoneatmostactivatedand2frompairswherebothreferentsare

activated).

Theresultsfromsectionthreeofthequestionnaireprovideevidenceforranking

effectsderivedfromthesignalingofcognitivestatus,implicaturesandspecializations(as

describedabove).Theanalysisofpairsofreferentswiththesamemaximumcognitive

statusindicatesthatanimacyfeaturesalsoplayaroleinrankingandthattherankings

whichderivefromformstatuscorrelationsandtheirrelatedimplicaturescanbeextended

toothertypesofcategorieswhichinvolvesometypeofrelativeprominence–inthiscase

animacycategories.Itisalsopossiblethat,ratherthanbeinganextensionofcognitive

statusrankings,rankingsthatcorrelatewithanimacyfeaturesactuallyindicatethat

208 animacycontributestosalienceand,therefore,cognitivestatus(i.e.thelikelihoodthat somethingwillbeinfocus).

Todemonstratethesegeneralizations,Idiscussthefollowingtypesofexamples: thoseinwhichtworeferentshavedifferentmaximumstatusesbuthaveidenticalanimacy features,andthoseinwhichpairsofreferentshavedifferentmaximumcognitivestatuses anddifferentanimacyfeatures.Finally,Idiscusspairsofreferentsthathavethesame minimumandmaximumcognitivestatus,buthavedifferentanimacyfeatures.

Example(166)illustratestheuseoffourdifferentformpairsinreferencetotwo entitiesthatarebothhumanbuthavedifferentcognitivestatuses.

(166) Muallimstudentgekitap berdi. Song ___ ___ teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST then

ekzameni bulan k”utlady. exam.3 POSS with congratulate.3PST ‘Theteachergaveabooktothestudent.Then(s)hecongratulatedhim/heronthe exam.’[numberedas#30inquestionnaire] Responses teacher( FOC ) Student( ACT ) bu onu o’ziu onu Ø onu ol onu

Allfourrespondentsselecttheteacherasthereferentassociatedwiththefirstblankand thestudentasthereferentassociatedwiththesecondblank.Basedonthecodingofthese referentsaccordingtotheGivennessHierarchycriteria,theteacherisinfocusandthe studentislikelytobeatmostactivated.Intwooutoffourresponses,respondentsusea

209 pronounfromthefocusset(oneeachofnull,reflexive)torefertotheteacherandthe pronoun onu (accusativecaseof o)torefertothestudent.Onerespondentuses bu juxtaposedwith o,whichisalsooneofthepredictedrankingsbasedontheproposed

specializationoftheactivatedform bu .Thefourthrespondentuses o/onu forboth

references.Theresponsesinthisexamplesupporttheproposedrankingeffects.

Example(167)alsoillustratestwomentions(associatedwiththefirstandthird blanks)ofreferentswithdifferentmaximumcognitivestatusesbutidenticalanimacy

features.Thisexampleisnoteworthyinthesensethatitillustratesarankingeffect betweenthefirstandsecondpronominalmentionofthesamereferent.

(167) MuallimPatimatg”a kitap berdi. __ __ këp ushatdy. teacherPatimat.DAT book give.3PST verypleased Song , __ u’ĭge getdi. later home. DAT go.3PST ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook.Shewasverypleasedwithit.Thenshewent home.’[numberedexample#33inquestionnaire] Responses Patimat 1(ACT ) (book) Patimat 2(FOC ) bu (shonu ) bu sho (Ø) ol o (shonu ) Ø ol (shonu ) ol Theresponsestothisexampleareasfollows:2respondentsusethesameform,and2use rankedforms.Thefactthatrankedpairsofformsareusedinreferencetothesameentity isstrongevidencethattherankingsderivefromcognitivestatusdifferences,sincethe cognitivestatusoftheentitywouldbehigheratthetimeofthesecondmention.Itisalso

210 interesting(andsomewhatunexpected)that,amongalltheresponseswheretwo referencestohumansarejuxtaposed, sho isusedonlyonceinreferencetoahuman entity.Inmostcasesinvolvingtwohumanreferentswithdifferentcognitivestatuses, ois

usedforthelowerrankingentity.

Example(168)alsocontains2mentionsofthesamereferent—inthiscasethe bookassociatedwiththefirstandthirdblanks.Asinthepreviousexample,thebookis

consideredtobeatmostactivatedatthetimeofmentionassociatedwiththefirstblank

andinfocusatthetimeofmentionassociatedwiththesecondblank.

(168) MuallimPatimatg”akitap berdi. teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST ___ birinchisavg”atbolg”an song,______ anasyna first gift be.3PST .PRT since mother.3POSS .DAT

gërsetmesiuedi. show. INF want.3PST ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook.Sinceit washerfirstprize,she wantedtoshow it tohermother.’[correspondsto#34inquestionnaire] Responses book 1(ACT ) (Patimat)( ACT ) book 2(FOC ) o (bu) munu sho (Ø) onu bu (o) shonu bu (ol) shonu Inthepairsofformsassociatedwiththesementionsthereissomeevidenceforranking andsomecounterevidence.Theresponsesofonerespondentsupporttheexpected rankingpairs sho(referentatmostactivated)–o(referentinfocus),andtheresponseof

211 anothersuggests o(referentatmostactivated)–bu(referentinfocus)ranking;however,

tworespondentsuse bu forthefirstpronominalmentionofthebookand sho forthe

second.Ifoneacceptsthehypothesisthathumannesscontributestoprominence,thenthe

unexpecteduseof sho inthesecondmentionofthebookcouldbeexplainedbythefact

thatitcontrastswithapronominalreferencetoahumaninthesameclause. 111

Therearethreecasesinthequestionnaireinwhichthecognitivestatusranking

coincideswithananimacyranking(human>nonhuman).Eachofthesecasesisbased

onanexamplewiththesamefirstsentence,shownin(169)(a),sothereferentrankingis

identical:Bolat,thehumanreferent,isinfocus;thehorse,anonhumananimateentityis

activated;andthefactthatBolatfellfromthehorseisalsoactivated.Whetherthe

comparisonisbetweenBolatandthehorseorBolatandthefactthathefell,ineachcase

Bolatisareferentthatisrankedhigherintermsofbothcognitivestatusandanimacy.

OutoftwelvepossiblepairingsofBolatandthehorse(4ofwhicharedisplayedin(169)

(b),eightuserankedformpairingswhichcorrespondtothecognitivestatus/animacy

ranking,threeusethesameform( o)forbothreferents,andonlyoneusesaformpairing

whichcontradictsthecognitivestatus/animacyranking( o–bu ).WhenBolatispaired

withthefactthatBolatfell,fouroutoffourresponseshaverankedpairsofformswhich

correspondtothecognitivestatusandanimacyrankings.

(169) a.Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg’yldy. Bolathorse.3POSS .DAT ride. GER go.PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST

111 Alternately,theuseof bu inthefirstclauseofthesecondsentencecouldpossiblybeattributedtoother pragmaticfactors,suchasthefocusstructureoftheequativeclause.

212 b.___saialy, ______satma khyial etdi. because sell. INF decisionmake.3 PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Becauseofthat ,he decidedtosellit .’ [correspondsto#38inquestionnaire] Responses fact–falling(ACT ) Bolat( FOC ) horse( ACT ) sho o munu sho Ø onu sho o onu sho ol shonu

Example(170)involvestwononhumanreferentswhicharerankedintermsof cognitivestatusandalsodifferintermsofanimacy:oneisanevent(inanimate),whichis infocus,andtheotherisahorse(animate),whichisactivated. 112

(170) Bolat atyna minip barag”anda ĭyg”yldy. Bolat horse.3 POSS .DAT ride. GER go. PR .PRT .LOC fall.3PST

___song___dag”y___ minip barmag”an. afteragain ride. GER go. NEG .3 PST ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Afterthathewouldn’trideitanymore.’ [correspondsto#39inquestionnaire]

112 Therespondents’identificationofthereferentinthisexampleallowsforsomeambiguitybetweenan event(infocus)andasituation,whichwouldbeconsideredatmostactivated.Ihavetreatedthisasa referencetoanevent,butifthereferenceis,infact,asituationthatisintroducedindirectlybythemention ofanevent,thenthisexamplewouldnotbeacounterexampletomyrankinghypotheses,asthiswouldbe arankingoftwoentitieswiththesamemaximumstatusand,thus,animacyrankingwouldbeexpectedto playtheprimaryrolehere.

213 responses event( FOC ) horse( ACT ) sho o Ø sho sho o sho sho

Iftheonlyfeatureconsiderediscognitivestatus,twooftheseresponseswouldbea counterexampletotherankingprediction,as sho isusedinreferencetoanevent(in focus),andahigherrankingformisusedinreferencetothehorse(atmostactivated).

Theseresponses,however,couldbeexplainedintermsofrankingifoneproposesthatthe animacyrankingtakesprecedenceoverthecognitivestatusranking,inwhichcasethe animatehorsewouldberankedhigherthantheinanimateevent.

Intheexampleillustratedpreviouslyas(168)thesecondandthirdblanks

(associatedwithPatimatandthebook,respectively)representapairingofmentionsof tworeferentsthatarerankedonewayintermsofcognitivestatusandrankedtheopposite wayintermsofanimacy.Inthiscase,thebook,aninanimateentityinfocus,ispaired withahumanentityassumedtobeatmostactivated—thatis,Patimat.Whileone responseusesthesameform( bu )forbothentities,theremainingthreeformpairssupport theanimacyrankingratherthanthecognitivestatusranking,suggestingthatanimacy rankingscanbemoreimportantthancognitivestatusrankingsinsomecases.

Thecorrelationbetweenrankedpairsofformsandanimacyrankingsisalso supportedbytheanalysisofpronominalpairsrepresentingentitiesofthesamemaximum status(bothinfocusorbothatmostactivated).Inmostoftheexamplesinthiscategory ahumanreferentcontrastswithaninanimatereferentintroducedbyanominal,as 214 illustratedin(171).(SeealsothebookincontrastwithPatimatinexamples(167)and

(168),aswellasthe shu –bu contrastin(122).)

(171) MuallimPatimatg”akitap berdi. U’ĭge gelgen song___ teacherstudent. DAT book give.3PST home.DAT come.PST .PRT after g’ak”ynda anasyna aĭtdy. 113 about mother.3POSS .DAT say.3PST ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook.After(s)hecamehome,((s)he)toldhis/her motheraboutPRO.’ Responses Patimat( ACT ) book( ACT ) ol shonu o shonu ol shonu

Inonecaseahumanreferentcontrastswithaninanimatereferentintroduceddirectlybya clause(theeventofBolatfallingoffhishorse.Example(169)illustratesacasewherean animateentity(horse)contrastswithaninanimateentityintroducedindirectlybyaclause

(thefactthatBolatfelloffthehorse).

Aspreviouslystated,ofthe23responsesthatuserankedpairsofforms,allbut4 supporttherankingofentitiesaccordingtoanimacy.Ofthe4exceptions,2involvea nullargumentinreferencetotheentityrankedlowerintermsofanimacy(cf.(167)and

(170))and2casesinvolvetheuseof bu inreferencetothebookin(168),(discussed previously).Oneinterestingobservationisthat,ofthe19pairswhichcorrelatewith animacyranking,17use sho forthelowerrankingentityincontrastwithanotherform.

113 Thebluerespondentfillsinthesecondblankwiththeform munu andidentifiestheteacherasits referent.

215 Eventhough sho isnotrestrictedtousewithinanimateentitiesforallspeakers,thisfact

couldexplainwhy,forcertainspeakers,thecontrastbetween sho and omightbecome

conventionalizedasanonhuman–humancontrast(cf.section5.6).

Insummary,thedatadiscussedinthissectionsupporttheuseofrankedform pairsbasedonformstatuscorrelations,implicatures,andthespecializationof bu and sho .Therankingsoftheentitiestowhichtheseformsreferaredemonstratedtocorrelate, notonlywithcognitivestatusdifferences,butalsowithdifferencesinanimacyfeatures.

Asamatteroffact,thecorrelationbetweenformpairsandanimacyrankingsforentities withthesamemaximumcognitivestatusismoreconsistentthanthecorrelationbetween formpairsandcognitivestatusrankings,thoughthefactthatthedatasetfortheformer categoryismorelimitedthanthelattercategoryalsoplaysaroleinthestatistical outcome.Onecannot,however,drawtheconclusionthatanimacyrankingsaretheonly orprimaryfactorinvolved,asthereareclearexampleswhererankedformpairings correlatewithreferentswhichsharethesameanimacyfeaturesbuthavedifferent cognitivestatuses.Furthermore,animacyrankingscanbeinterpretedasaffectingthe inherentprominenceofanentity,whilecognitivestatuscanpotentiallybeinterpretedasa resultofbothinherentprominenceandcontextualprominence.Theroleofanimacy featuresinrelationtocognitivestatusisanimportantareaforfuturestudyinKumykand inotherlanguageswhereanimacydistinctionsarenotgrammaticizedasdifferent pronounsets.

Aninterestingobservationinthisportionofthequestionnaireanalysisisthe frequencywithwhich osho pairsoccurinthedata:24(or40.0%)outofthetotalof60

216 rankedformpairs.Withtheadditionofnull–sho and bu –sho pairs,thereare33(or55

%ofthetotal)rankedformpairingsinvolvingtheuseoftheform sho .Inallcasesexcept

onewhere sho isusedwithreferentsofdifferentcognitivestatus,itreferstothereferent oflowerstatus.Intheoneexceptionalcase,animacyfeaturesseemtoplayastronger factor,as sho representsanevent,while bu and orepresentanimateentities(human, horse,see(170)).Insituationswithreferentsofthesamestatus,whetherbotharein focusorbothareatmostactivated,inallbutonecase sho representstheentitywhichis lowerontheanimacyhierarchy(human,animatenonhuman,inanimate).Thisdata stronglysupportsthehypothesisthat sho isusedtoimplicatetheloweroftworanked entities.

Whilethequestionnaireprovidesevidencefortheseimplicaturesprimarilyin othercontextswithmultiplepronominalreferences,Iassumethattheseimplicaturesare notdependentonwhetherornotmorethanonepronounisused.The“specializationsor strongimplicatureassociatedwith sho ,evenincaseswheretherearenotnecessarilytwo pronounsused,isconfirmedbyevidencefromsentencecontinuations.Kaiserand

Trueswell2004suggestthatcontinuationtestsprovideevidencethatthechoiceof proform(fromamongthesetofproformspossibleinthelanguage)allowsthehearerto identifythereferentofthatformfromamongasetofmultiplepossiblereferents.In otherwords,whenarespondentisgivenacontextsentenceandaskedtowritea continuationusingaparticularproform,thechoiceofformisassumedtoinfluencethe topicofthecontinuingtext.ThistypeofdataisparticularlyinterestinginKumyk,where thereisnomorphologicaldistinctionbetweenanimateandinanimateentities.The

217 detailedresultsofthecontinuationtestsinKumyk,whicharediscussedinsection5.6(cf. examples(140)(142),showthatcontinuationswiththepronoun sho overwhelmingly

selectthereferentofthepronounasaninanimatereferentthatisnotlikelytobeinfocus

(morespecifically,nottheprevioussubject). 114

5.9. Conclusions

Theevidencefromthequestionnaireconfirmsthehypothesisbasedonthecorpus

studythatatleasttwopronominalformsinKumykarerestrictedtoreferentsinfocus:

reflexivesandnulls.Asfortheform bu ,whilethecorpusstudysuggeststhisformis

restrictedtoreferentsinfocus,thequestionnairedatadisconfirmthishypothesis.

Nevertheless,thedataindicatethat bu ismorerestrictedinusethantheother

demonstrativeformsandexhibitsapreferenceforreferencetothemoreprominentina

setofentities.

Thequestionnairedataalsoclearlydemonstratethevaryingdegreestowhichthe

overtdemonstrativepronounsgiverisetotheimplicature‘notinfocus’.Theanalysis

showsthat sho hasastrongassociationwiththisimplicatureincomparisonto bu and o.

Moreover,inspiteofthestrongassociationwiththeimplicature‘notinfocus’,evidence

fromthequestionnairedemonstratesthattherearenoabsoluteconstraintsonitsusewith

higherstatusforms,supportingthepredictionsoftheGivennessHierarchymodel.

114 Unexpectedly,inthesentencecontinuationportionofthequestionnaire,theresultsfor bu aresomewhat variableorshowaweakercorrelationwiththemostprominentreferentifonejudgesmainlybythe syntacticprominenceofpreviousmention.

218 Likethecorpusstudy,thequestionnairedatacontainveryfewmentionsofthe pronoun shu .Speakerjudgmentsindicatethatitsuseissomewhatunacceptable(for some,butnotall,speakers).Inthefewcasesavailableforanalysis,theuseofthisform ischaracterizedinaverysimilarwaytothatof sho .Theevidencebothfromfrequency andfromspeakerjudgmentssuggeststhat,asinsomeotherTurkiclanguages,the distinctionbetween sho and shu maysoonbelost.

Oneofthemostimportantcontributionsofthequestionnaireisthatitprovides evidenceofthepsychologicalrealityofscalarimplicaturescreatedbythedifferencesin cognitivestatusrestrictions,aswellasthespecializationof bu forhigherrankingentities

(whichcannotnecessarilybederivedfromcognitivestatusdifferencesbasedonthe

questionnaireevidence).Thediscussionbothoftheuseofrankedformsandthe

frequencywithwhichsentencecontinuationsforpronounssignaling‘activated’picka

lowerrankingreferentprovidesevidenceofrankingeffectsinreferentdisambiguation.

WhilepreviousresearchwithintheGHmodelfocusesontheroleofprevious

mentionandfeaturesrelatedtothespeechcontextinrelationtothecognitivestatusofan

entity,datafromthequestionnairesuggestthataninherentfeatureofanentity—thatis,

animacycharacteristics—mayalsoplayaroleincognitivestatus(oratleastrelative prominence)andchoiceofform.UnlikeEnglish,wherethegenderandanimacy

categorieseachhavetheirownpairsofpronounsrepresenting‘activated’and‘infocus’,

inKumyk,whereanimacyisnotgrammaticallymarkedinpronounform,onesetof pronounsmustbeusedtorepresentbothcognitivestatusrankingsandpotentialrankings

derivedfromdifferencesinanimacyfeatures.Inmyanalysisofrankedpairs,the

219 correlationwithanimacyrankingsisevenstrongerthanthecorrelationwithcognitive status,whichsuggeststhatanimacyfeaturesmayplayaroleinthecognitivestatusofan entitythatisnotreflectedinthecodingcriteriaoftheGHmodel.

Therelationshipbetweenanimacyfeaturesandtheuseofdifferentcategoriesof referringformsisaddressedtosomeextentbyDahlandFraurud1996andFraurud1996.

Whilebothstudiesaddresstheideathathumanreferentsaremorelikelytobereferredto withpronounsthannonhumanreferents,DahlandFraurudrelatethispropensitytothe factthathumansaremorelikelytobetopicsthannonhumans,aswellasthefactthat narrativepointofviewrepresentsahumanperspective(1996:5962).Fraurud1996takes theideaastepfurtherbygivingsomestatisticalevidencefortheclaimthat,giventhe samedegreeoftopicalityorcognitivestatus,humansaremorelikelythannonhumansto bereferredtowithpronouns.Specifically,Fraurud’sdatasuggeststhathumansaremore likelythannonhumanstobereferredtowithpronounswhenareferentismentionedin thetextearlierthanthesameorprecedingsentence(1996:6667). 115 Asubsequentstudy byPratSalaandBranigan(2000)providesfurtherevidenceoftheindependentinfluence ofsocalled inherentsalience ,whichisaresultofanimacyorotherontologicalfeatures, and derivedsalience orcontextualgivenness—thoughtthelatterisconsideredtobethe strongerofthetwo(169,177).Dahl2008justifiestherelativecognitiveinfluenceof differentontologicalcategoriesintermsofadevelopmentalprocesswhichbeginswhena

115 Thiscouldberelatedtothedegreeoftopicalityinlargerunitsoftext,aswiththecriteronfromthe codingfor‘infocus’status,whichreferstotheroleofanentityasa‘higherleveltopic’thatcanbepartof theinterpretationofasentenceevenwhennotovertlymentioned(Gundel2004).

220 memberofaspeciesfirstbecomesawarethatthereareotherbeingslikeitselfinthe worldandeventuallydevelopsintoatheoryofmind.InDahl’swords,“theselfisthe modelforotheranimateindividuals,whichareintheirturnmodelsforinanimateobjects whenunderstoodasindividual‘things’”(149). 116

Myanalysis,likewise,assumesthatbothontologicalfeaturesandcontextually

derivedfeaturescontributetothecognitivestatusofanentity,whichIsupportwitha

comparisonofreferencetoentitieswiththesamecontextualsalience(basedprimarilyon

thesyntacticstatusofpreviousmention)butdifferentanimacyfeatures.Moreover,as

withPratSalaandBranigan’sstudy,theevidenceprimarilyshowsthat,incaseswhere

rankingintermsofanimacyfeaturesparallelsrankingintermsofcontextualsalience,the

correlationswithrankedpairsofformsaremoreconsistentthanincasesinwhich

animacyrankingsareinthereverseorderfromrankingsassociatedwiththesyntactic prominenceofpreviousmention.However,furtherstudyinthisareaisneeded—in particularexperimentaldatawhichhasbeenspecificallystructuredtoestablishthe

independentaffectsofontologicalfeaturesversustheformofpreviousmentioninthe

context.

Oneofthelimitationsoftheanalysisofthequestionnairedataisthatanalyzing

constraintsontheuseofformsbasedprimarilyonthecategoryoftheargumentposition

ofpreviousmentionisnotverysatisfactoryandyieldsinconclusiveresultsintestingthe

116 NotethatFraurudaddressesthehumannonhumandistinctionintermsofanontologicaldifference betweenindividuatedentitiesand“instantiationsoftypes”(1996:72).Insuchcase,onewouldexpectthe form awoman torepresentanentitythatisaninstantiation,while WarandPeace representsan individuatedentity.

221 hypothesisthatcertainformsareconstrainedtoreferentsinfocus.Aspreviously discussed,thecriteriaofthecodingprotocolareconsideredtobeminimumcriteria;thus, forexample,evenifareferentismentionedinanonsubjectpositionintheprevious sentence,itisnoteasytodeterminethatareferentisatmostactivated.SinceIcannot determinedefinitelythatareferentmentionedinaparticularnonsubjectargument positionintheprecedingsentenceisatmostactivated,Ilookatwherenaturaldistinctions betweentheuseofformsfall.Forexample,thefactthatnullsareacceptablewith referentsmentionedpreviouslyassubjectsanddirectobjectswhilenotacceptablewith referentsmentionedpreviouslyasdativeobjectsleadsmetooperateunderthe assumptionthatdativeobjectsareatmostactivated.Inlightofthislimitationofthe methodology,Ibelievethat,whileIamabletoreachsoundconclusionsabouttherelative degreeofrestrictiveness,itispossiblethatthelinewhichseparatestheformsthatsignal

‘infocus’andthosethatsignal‘activated’isnotentirelyaccurate.Suchastatementis notintendedtochallengetheoverallvalidityoftheGivennessHierarchymodel,but merelytoindicatethatthereareaspectsofthecodingprocesswhichneedfurther considerationandwhichhavealreadyundergonesomerevisionsincethe2004version usedinmyresearch.

222

Chapter 6: Implications of the Analysis

Anintegratedanalysisoftheresultsfromthecorpusstudyandquestionnaire demonstratesboththerestrictionsandthepreferencesofeachpronominalformin

Kumyk.Section6.0summarizestheresultsintermsoftheformstatuscorrelationsofthe

GHmodelandthepredictionsaboutscalarimplicatureswhichfollowfromthe unidirectionalentailmentofthehierarchy.Insection6.1,Idiscusstheadequacyofthe theoreticalmodelandmethodologyindemonstratingthepsychologicalrealityofform statuscorrelationsandtherankingeffectsderivedfromthem.Section6.2summarizes theresultsintermsofageneralizedhierarchyofdegreeofrestrictiveness,whichtakes intoconsiderationmultiplefactorsdemonstratedtorelatetothecognitivestatusof referents—boththosewhichderivefromcontextualuseandthoseinherenttotheentity.

Insection6.3Idiscusstherankingeffectsassociatedwithcontrastsinpronominalforms, andsection6.4relateshowtherankingeffectsassociatedwithparticularformscontribute toimposedsalience.Finally,section6.5addressesthesignificanceoftheresearchtoa numberofissuesofwiderinterest,aswellassuggestingseveralareasofpossiblefuture research.

6.0. Form-status correlations and predictions of the GH model

Thecombinedanalysisofthecorpusstudyandquestionnairedatasupportsthe followingformstatuscorrelationsaspartofthelexicalmeaningofpronominals:nulland o’ziu signalthatthereferentisinfocus,whiletheovertdemonstrativepronouns bu , o,

223 sho ,and shu signalthatthereferentisactivated.Figure7providesasummaryof proposedformstatuscorrelations.

FOC ACT Ø,o'ziu, bu,o,sho, shu

Figure7 : Final FormStatusCorrelations

Themostdifficultformintheanalysisis bu ,whichappearsfromthecorpusdata toberestrictedtoreferentsinfocusandoftenindicatesthehigheroftwoormoreranked entities,but,inthequestionnairedata,doesnotexhibitthesamelevelofrestrictionas nullsandreflexives.Ontheotherhand, bu patternslikenullsinbeingrestrictivetoa certaindegreeintheareaofreferencetoclausallyintroducedentities,unlike o, sho,and shu ,whichappeartobeunrestrictedinthisarea.Therestrictioninrelationtoclausally introducedentities,however,couldbepartiallyexplainedbythefactthat bu isusually associatedwithhighrelativeprominenceand,giventhesamecognitivestatus,entities introducedbyclausesaretypicallyoflowerrelativeprominencethanentitiesintroduced bynominals.Intheend,theproposalthat bu signalsthestatus‘activated’ismore consistentwiththedata,althoughitsassociationwithentitiesofrelativelygreater prominencecannotbederiveddirectlyfromthiscategorization.

Inadditiontotheformstatuscorrelations,thereisevidenceoffurther specializationinthedistributionofdemonstratives.Thoughinprinciple,any

224 demonstrativeformcangiverisetotheimplicature‘notinfocus’,thepronoun sho hasa strongassociationwiththisimplicature.Basedonboththeanalysisofreferentsof sho in thecorpusandthereferentpreferencesofthisformdemonstratedinthequestionnaire,I claimthatthisformspecializesinindicatingtheloweroftwoormorerankedentities.

Suchaspecializationiseasilyderivedfromthescalarimplicaturesinherentinthe unidirectionalentailmentoftheGHmodel.Finally,eventhoughthemajorityofreferents of sho areatmostactivated,thetestingofthisformdemonstratesthat,aspredictedbythe

GHmodel,itcanalsobeusedwithentitiesthatareinfocus.

Incontrastto sho ,theform oisoftenusedwithoutgivingrisetotheimplicature

‘notinfocus’andistheleastspecializedofthedemonstratives.Thispronounappearsto besomewhatneutraland,infact,occursthreetimesasfrequentlyas sho or bu .

Thepronoun bu ,thoughnotrestrictedtoentitiesinfocus,mostoftenreferstothe moreprominentoftwoormoreentitiesthatareatleastactivatedandcanbeconsidered tospecializeinindicatinghigherrelativeranking.Thisspecializationcannot,however, bederiveddirectlyfromthestructureoftheGHmodel.Iassumethat,givenmultiple possiblepronounsthatsignalthestatus‘activated’,itisanaturallinguisticprocessfor formstobecomespecializedforparticularfunctions. 118 Inparticular,giventhefactthat pronounsthatarerestrictedtoreferentsinfocuscannotbeusedtoindicatethemore

118 Theoretically,myclaimissimilartothatofGundel,Hedberg,andZacharski1993,whoclaimthatthe Englishpronoun this specializesinindicatingspeakeractivation,notjustactivationingeneral.

225 prominentoftwoentitiesthatareatmostactivated,itseemsnaturalthatoneofthe pronounsthatsignalactivationwouldbecomespecializedinsignalinghigherrelative prominence.Also,inKumyk,giventhefactthatpronounreferentscannotbe disambiguatedonthebasisofanimacycategories,itseemsevenmorelikelythatthere wouldbeamoredetailedwaytodisambiguatereferentsonthebasisofrelativecognitive statusrankings—inadditiontothecategoricaldistinctionsbetweenthesetofforms whichsignal‘infocus’andthesetthatsignals‘activated’.

6.1. Adequacy of the GH model in this analysis

OneoftheprominentfeaturesoftheGHmodelisthatitaddressesdifferencesin theuseofreferringformsonthebasisofcognitivestatus,whilestillallowingforother possibledistinctionstobeencodedbydifferencesinform—forexample,gender, animacy,orentityclass/nounclass.ForKumyk,wherepronominalshavenocategorical differentiationbasedongenderoranimacy,theevidencefromthisstudysuggeststhat choiceofpronominalisbasedalmostentirelyonrelativecognitiveprominence;thusthe

GHmodelprovidesanexcellentmeansofaccountingfordistributionaldifferencesin pronominalform.

InChapter2,IarguethattheGHmodelispreferabletoothermodelsthatexplain thedistributionofformssolelyonthebasisofsyntacticcriteriaorlineardistancebecause italsotakesintoconsiderationotherfactorsthatcancontributetothecognitivestatusof anentity,suchaswhetherornotitisahigherleveltopic,whetheritisdirectlyor indirectlyintroduced,aswellasnonlinguisticfactorssuchasgestureandeyegaze.

Evidencefromthecorpusstudysuggestsafewadditionalcontextualcriteriathat

226 contributetocognitivestatus,suchastherepetitionofparallelargumentstructures.One ofthelimitationsoftheGHmodelisthatitdoesnotspecificallyaddresstheroleof featuresthatareinherenttoanentitytype,inparticular,whetherornotanimacy characteristicscontributetothecognitivestatusofanentity.

Oneofthemethodologicalchallengesofanalyzingthedistributionofpronouns withintheGHmodelisthedifficultyindeterminingboththeminimumandmaximum statusofthereferentaformonthebasisofthecurrentcodingcriteriaalone.More specifically,99.8%ofpronounusesoccurwithentitiesthatareatleastactivated,with theonlypossibledistinctionbeingbetweenentitiesthatareatmostactivatedandthose thatareinfocus.Methodologically,itisverydifficulttodetermineinaconsistentway whatthemaximumstatusofareferentis,asthecriteriaforcodingareferentas‘infocus’ areintendedtobesufficient,notnecessary.Thedifferencesbetweenthecodingcriteria forthestatuses‘activated’and‘infocus’suggestthatreferentsmaypossiblybeatmost activatediftheyarementionedinthepreprevioussentence,butnotmentionedinthe previoussentence. 119 However,inmycorpus,only3outof410codedpronountokens

119Basedonthecodingcriteriaalone(2004version),referentsthatarepossiblyatmostactivated(referents satisfyaconditionforthestatus‘activated’butdonotmeetthecriteriafor‘infocus’)includethe following: 1.Mentionedintheprecedingsentence,butnotmentionedovertlypreviouslyinthesame sentence,notahigherleveltopicthatispartoftheinterpretationofapreviousclauseinthesame sentence,notmentionedasasubjectinanyclauseoftheprecedingsentenceorasahigherlevel topicthatispartoftheinterpretationofthepreviousmatrixclauseinshort,anonsubject argumentoftheprevioussentencethatisnotahigherleveltopic. 2.Mentionedinthepreprecedingsentence,butnotmentionedovertlyintheprevioussentenceor previouslyinthesamesentenceandnotahigherleveltopicthatispartoftheinterpretationofthe previousclauseorsentence.

227 wereinreferencetoentitiesnotmentionedintheprevioussentenceandnotcodedas‘in focus’bysomeothercriteria(suchashigherleveltopicormyadditionofscript repetition).Sincealmostallreferentsofpronounsarementionedintheprevious sentence,theremainingpossibledistinctionbetweenthosethatareatmostactivatedand thosethatareinfocusliesindifferencesofsyntacticpositionorthedifferencebetween anevent,whichisdirectlyintroducedbyaclause,andfacts,situations,propositions,and speechacts,whicharemostoftenintroducedindirectlybyaclause.Forthisreason, muchofmyquestionnairefocusesonthedifferentlevelsofacceptabilityofeachform withreferentsmentionedinvariousargumentpositions.TheKumykdatasuggestthata pointofdifferenceinacceptabilityforcertainformsversusothers(forexample,nulls versusovertpronouns)occurswithentitiesmentionedinargumentpositionsotherthan thatofsubjectordirectobject.WhileImakethisdistinctiononthebasisofempirical dataalone,italsocorrelateswithclaimsaboutthesyntacticprominenceofparticular argumentsinSOVlanguages. 120 Whileitischallengingtodeterminethelinebetween formsrequiringareferentinfocusandthosewhichonlyrequireanactivatedreferent,the factthatacceptabilityjudgmentsindicatedifferentdegreesofrestrictivenessatallgives evidencethatthereare(atleast)twodifferentcognitivestatusesonthehierarchythatmay beencodedbypronouns..

120 Note,however,thatIcannotnecessarilygeneralizethisclaimtootherlanguages,particularlythosewith otherwordordercategories.

228 Anotherchallengeinmyanalysisisthat,inprinciple,oneshouldalsobeableto distinguishbetweenformsthatrequireentitiesinfocusandthosethatrequireactivation ontheassumptionthatformswhichrequirereferentsinfocuscanrefertoamuch narrowerrangeofentitiesintroducedbyaclausethanthosethatonlyrequireactivation.

Morespecifically,aformwhichrequiresareferentinfocuscanonlyrefertoaneventor stateintroducedbyasentence,whileformswhichrequireonlyactivationcanalsoreferto entitiesintroducedindirectly,suchasfacts,propositions,orspeechacts.Thedifficultyin myanalysisisthatbothnullsandtheovertpronoun bu ,areveryrestrictedinusewithall

clausallyintroducedentities.Inthecaseof bu ,ifthisformisnotrestrictedtoreferentsin

focus,onewouldhaveexpectedthatitcouldrefertoentitiesintroducedindirectlybya

clause;however,thefactthatitisgenerallyrestrictedtoentitiesintroducedbyanominal

orbyaclausewithsomedegreeofnominalizationmeansthatitisnotpossibletotestthis

differentiation.

Oneofthecontributionsofthisanalysisisfurtherexplorationofanumberof criteriaindeterminingthecognitivestatusofanentity.Morespecifically,thisstudy discussesseveralpotentialcategoriesofsyntacticprominenceinadditiontosubjecthood thatbringareferentintofocus:objectsinSOVlanguages,topicalizedelements,andthe possessorinapossessiveclause.Myanalysisalsoraisesthequestionofwhetherhuman

animacyfeaturesandparallelisminargumentstructurecanbeincorporatedintothe

sufficientcriteriafor‘infocus’status.Intermsofthetechnicalapplicationofthecoding protocol,thisstudyaddressessomeoftheissuesthatariseinthecodingofpluralentities

anddeterminingtheroleofeyegazeinbringinganentityintofocus.

229 Finally,anothercontributionofmystudyisthatitaddressesthemethodological aspectofwhatconstitutesevidenceofscalarimplicatures.Theuseofsentence continuations,exploringthecorrelationbetweenreferenceidentificationandtheuseof alternatingforms,andtheexplorationofrankedpairsallprovideevidencethatthe implicature,‘notinfocus’playsaroleinreferentidentification.

6.2. Degrees of restrictiveness

Theintegratedanalysisofthecorpusandquestionnairedataprovidesevidencefor gradientdegreesofrestrictivenessthatbothsupportdistinctcognitivestatuscategories

‘infocus’and‘activated’andprovideevidenceoffinerlevelsofdifferenceintheuseof forms.Theideaofgradientdistinctionsisconsistentwiththeevidencethatthechoiceof pronominaloftenindicatesrelativecognitiveprominence,whichIcall“rankingeffects”, evenforreferentsofthesamecognitivestatussubcategory—thatis,thosethatsharethe sameminimumandmaximumstatus. 121

InthissectionIsummarizethedistributionofpronounsinrelationtomultiple

featuresdemonstratedtobeassociatedwiththedifferencebetweenanactivatedversusan

infocusreferent(eitherfromthecodingprotocoloftheGHmodelorfrommyown

research):argumentpositionandlineardistanceofpreviousmention,directmention

(introducedbyanominaloreventintroducedbyaclause)versusindirectmention(non

121 Notethatwhile sho could,inageneralsense,beconsideredtobemorerestrictiveduetoitspreference forreferentsthatarenotinfocusorlessprominentintermsofrelativesalienceoranimacyfeatures,Iam definingrestrictivenessinthesenseoftheGHmodel—thatisthedegreetowhichaformisrestrictedin representingentitiesofthehighestlevelofprominenceorhigherrelativeprominence.

230 evententitytypeintroducedbyaclause),andinherentfeaturessuchasthedistinction betweenhuman,animate,andinanimateentities.Notethat,whileanimacyfeaturesare notspecificallydiscussedinthecodingprotocol,myanalysissuggeststhatanimacy featuresplayaroleatleastinrelationtotherelativeprominenceofentities,andperhaps eventheircognitivestatus.Basedonthesefeatures,theformscanberankedasfollows accordingtodescendingdegreeofrestrictiveness:o’ziu , null , bu , o,and sho /shu .122

Figure8providesavisualsummaryoftherankingsandthemostnotabledistinctionsof restrictiveness.Figures9and10illustratedifferentdegreesofrestrictivenessrelated specificallytonominallyintroducedentitiesandclausallyintroducedentities, respectively.

Theform o’ziu isthemostrestrictedofthesixpronominalforms,andallofits referentsinthedataareclearlyinfocus.While,unlikereflexiveanaphorsinsome languages,referenceisnotrestrictedtosubjectsofthelocalclause,acrosssentence boundaries o’ziu isrestrictedtoreferencetoentitiesmentionedassubjectsoftheprevious sentence.Thoughaveryhighpercentageofusesofthisforminthecorpusdataarein referencetohumanentities,thereisnoevidencethat o’ziu isrestrictedtousewith animateorhumanentities.Theuseofthisform,however,isrestrictedtoentities introducedbynominals.Inbrief,reflexivesarenotonlyrestrictedtoreferencetoentities infocus,butalsohaveadditionalrestrictionsrelatedtotheargumentpositionofprevious mentionandmentionbyanominal.

122 Duetothelimitedevidencerelatedto shu andthefactthatitmostlyparallelstheinformationabout sho , Iwillnotdiscussthisformseparatelyhere.

231 Nullpronounsarealsorestrictedtousewithreferentsinfocus.Morespecifically, themajorityofnullsrefertoentitiespreviouslymentionedassubjects(eitherina precedingclauseofthesamesentenceortheprevioussentence).Unlikethereflexive, however,nullscanbeusedwithreferentsmentionedasnonsubjectargumentsinthe previoussentence,particularlywhenthereferenceoccursinthesameargumentposition inbothsentences(parallelism).Ontheotherhand,thereisevidencethatnullsare restrictedtoreferencetoentitiesmentionedeitherassubjects(assumedtobeinfocus)or asdirectobjects(alsolikelytobeinfocus),asmostnullreferencestoentitiesmentioned asindirectobjects(whicharemuchlesslikelytobeinfocus)arejudgedunacceptable.

Thoughthereisonlyoneexampleinthecorpusofnullreferencetoanentityinfocusnot introducedbyanominal—inthiscase,anevent—theanalysisofthequestionnairedata providesevidencethatsomenullreferencetoeventsareacceptable,whileothersmaybe judgedunacceptableforindependentreasons.Namely,anumberofexamplesinthedata suggestthatnullpronounsarenotacceptableincertaincontextswherephonological stressisrequired.Notethatsuchadistributionaldifferencebetweenstressedand unstressedpronounswouldbeexpectedinanylanguage.

Bu exhibitsnoclearrestrictionsintheareaoftheargumentpositionofprevious mention;however,thereappearstobeadegreeofrestrictioninreferencetoclausally introducedentities.Inthecaseoftheonecorpustokenforwhich bu referstoanactivity, theclausewhichintroducestheeventisnominalized.Inthequestionnaire,the acceptabilityoftheuseof bu withclausallyintroducedentitiesappearstobelimitedto certaintypesofemphaticexpressionswhichmaybeconventionalizedexclamations.In

232 termsofrelativeprominence, bu mostoftenindicatesthemoreprominentoftwoormore entities.

Theform oshowsnooutstandingpreferenceforprevioussubjectsorentitiesin focus,eventhoughmoreofitsreferentsareinfocusthanactivated.Thereareno documentedrestrictionsonusewithentitiesnotintroducedbynominals,and ocanbe

usedwitheitherentityinarankedsetofentitiesreferredtowithpronominals.Finally, o islesslikelythan sho togiverisetotheimplicature‘notinfocus’,afactorwhich contributestotheimpressionthatthisformisneutral.

Theform sho (andpossibly shu ,aswell)demonstratesastrongpreferencefor entitiesthatareatmostactivatedandthosenotmentionedasprevioussubjects. Sho is theformmostoftenusedwithentitiesintroducedbyclauses,andshowsnorestrictionsin thatarea.Thispronounoccursmorefrequentlywithnonhumanandanimateentities thanhumanentities,andthehumanentitiestowhichitrefersinthecorpusareprimarily pluralentities.Intermsofrelativeprominence,sho usuallyindicatesalowerranking entityinaset.Overall,thisformistheleastlikelytorefertoanentitythathasany degreeofprominencerelatedtoinherentfeaturessuchasanimacyorcontextualfeatures suchastheargumentpositionofpreviousmention.

Finally,thereisaninterestingcorrelationbetweenthedescriptionofgradient restrictivenessderivedfrombothquestionnaireandcorpusdataandthepercentageof referentsofagivenformthatareinfocusinthecorpusstudy.Basedongradient restrictiveness,therankingofpronominalsisasfollows: o’ziu>Ø>bu>o>sho .As

showninFigure11,themostrestrictiveform, o’ziu has100%ofitsreferentsinfocus,

233 whiletheform sho hasonly18.7%ofitsreferentsinfocus.Exceptfornullsand bu , whichhavevirtuallythesamepercentageofreferentsinfocus(thoughtechnically bu has moreby0.4%),thedescendingpercentageofreferentsinfocusrepresentsthedescending degreeofrestrictivenessdescribedinthissection.

234 o’ziu Requiresareferentinfocus Intersententialreferencerestrictedtoprevioussubjects Notusedwithclausallyintroducedreferents

Ø Requiresareferentinfocus Majorityofreferentsmentionedasprevioussubjects Usewithinfocusentitiesnotintroducedbynominalsmostlikely acceptable Referencetoentitiesmentionedasdirectobjects,butnotdative objectsokay Someentitiesnotautomaticallycodedasinfocusokayif parallelisminvolved Notacceptableinphonologically markedcontexts

bu Majorityofreferentsprevioussubjects Referencetoentitiesmentionedasdirectobjectsanddative objectsokay,regardlessofparallelism Limitedusewithfocusentitiesnotintroducedbynominals Usuallyindicateshigherrankedinasetofentitiesreferredtowith pronominals

o Nooutstandingpreferenceforprevioussubjects Majorityusewithfocusentities Norestrictionwithentitiesnotintroducedbynominals Canbeusedwitheitherentityinarankedsetofentitiesreferredto withpronominals

sho(shu) Strongpreferenceforentitiesnotmentionedasprevioussubjects Strongpreferenceforentitiesatmostactivated Norestrictiononentitiesnotintroducedbynominals Usuallyindicateslowestentityinarankedsetofentitiesreferred towithpronominals Morefrequentlyoccurswithnonhuman/inanimateentities

Figure8 : DegreeofRestrictiveness

235

Requiresareferentinfocus Requiresanactivatedreferent o'ziu Ø bu o sho/shu

Referencetonominallyintroducedentities

Intersentential Prefers Prefers Refersto Prefersnon reference previous previous entitiesat humanentities restrictedto subjects; subjects; least atmost previous likely direct& activated; activated; subjects restrictedto dativeobjects noother preferslower subjector okay;prefers restrictions rankedentity directobject higherranked entity

Figure9 : Referencetonominallyintroducedentities

Ø

o'ziu o sho/shu bu

Referencetoentitiesintroducedbyaclause

Notused Limiteduse Unrestrictedwith Unrestrictedwith with withevents; directandindirect directandindirect clausally possible referents(lower referents(higher introduced restriction frequency frequency referents onentities comparedto sho ) comparedto o) indirectly introduced byaclause

Figure10 :Referencetoentitiesintroducedbyaclause

236 o’zi u Ø, bu o sho shu* 100%96% 72% 12% 0%

Figure11 : Percentofcorpusreferentsinfocus (cf.Table6) *Theplacementof shu isnotreallysignificant,sinceitrepresentsthecodingofonlyoneform.

6.3. Cognitive status and ranking effects

Animportantquestioninthisstudyiswhetherornotthereisadistinction betweencognitivestatusandrelativesalience(ranking).Whilesometheories,suchas

CenteringTheory,focusonlyonrelativesaliencebasedonfactorssuchasargument positionoranimacy,theGHmodelfocusesonhowthesesamefactorsallcontributeto allowareferenttoattainaparticularcognitivestatus.Thedistinctcategoriesofcognitive statusprovideanexplanationforwhycertainformsareunacceptablewithreferentsofa certainmaximumstatus,yetstillallowfordifferentdegreesofrelativesaliencefor referentsofthesamecognitivestatus(Mulkern2003:23).Accordingly,itisplausible that,eventhoughtheuseofformssignalingdifferentlevelsofcognitivestatuscanbe usedtocreatearankingeffect,therearestillotherwaystoindicatefinerdegreesof relativesalience.Anotherpossibleconclusionisthatthecategoriesof‘infocus’and

‘activated’areprototypecategorieswithfuzzyboundaries(ratherthandiscrete categories)mappedontogradientdistinctionsofcognitiveprominence.

InKumyk,Iproposethatthedistinctionbetweenthe(inherent)cognitivestatuses

‘infocus’and‘activated’isprimarilyencodedbytheuseofanullversusanovert

237 pronoun,whilethedifferentdemonstrativesprimarilyencodefinerdegreesofrelative salience.Iwouldfurthersuggestthatinmostlanguages,thecontrastbetweenasetof formswithlowerphonologicalprominenceandonewithgreaterphonological prominence(e.g.nullsversusovertformsorunstressedpronounsversusstressed pronouns)isusedtosignaldifferentlevelsofinherentsalience.Thisfitswiththe

suggestionofGivón(1983)andothersthatunstressedpronounsandnulls(ellipsis—or

generally,phonologicallyreducedform)areassociatedwiththehighestlevelofnatural

cognitiveprominenceor“givenness”,aswellasahighdegreeof“expectedness”or predictability.

IfIclaimthefocusactivateddistinctioninKumykisencodedprimarilybythe

nullovertcontrast,whatcanIthenclaimaboutthereflexive,sincethedataanalysisalso

indicatesthatthisformcanonlybeusedwithreferentsinfocus?Inalllanguages,even

languagessuchasKumyk,whichhasarelativelyunrestricteduseofnulls(purelyin

termsofsyntax),therearestillreasonsanullformcannotbeusedtorefertoahighly prominententity.InKumyk,forexample,anullargumentinarelativeclauseisalways

understoodascoreferentwiththeheadoftherelativeclause,andanyotherargumentof

therelativeclausemustbeexpressedasanovertform,evenhighlysaliententities.In

additiontothis,nullargumentsarenotacceptableincontextswhichrequirephonological

focus.InKumykthesecontextsincludeequativeclauses,expressionsofcontrastive

focus,andotheremphaticallymarkedexpressions(with de ,forexample).Insomeof

thesecontexts,thereflexiveseemstoactasaspecializedovertformwhichsignalsthe

addresseetoblocktheimplicature‘notinfocus’andsearchforthereferentamongthe

238 entitiesatthecenterofattention.Iclaim,moreover,thatthecontrastbetween o’ziu and nulls(unlikethefourdifferentformsthatsignalthestatus‘activated’)isnotusedto indicatetherelativesalienceoftwoentitiesinfocus.Thisisalogicalconsequenceofthe factthat,sincethereisnostatushigherthan‘infocus’,thereisnowayanimplicatureof lowerrankingcanbederived,and,asexpected,thereisnoevidenceforsuchafunctional contrastinthedata.(Notethat,thoughthecontrastbetweennullandreflexiveisnotused forrelativesalience,thecontrastbetween o’ziu andanydemonstrativecanbeusedto indicaterelativesalience.)

Basedonmyanalysis,Iclaimthattheprimaryfunctionofthefourdemonstratives inKumykistoencodetherelativesalienceofentitiesthathavethesameminimum cognitivestatus—thatis,entitiesthatareatleastactivated,butnotnecessarilyatmost activated.Aspreviouslystated,theuseofanyformfromthiscategorypotentiallygives risetotheimplicature‘notinfocus’basedontheunidirectionalentailmentoftheGH model,yettheform sho seemstohaveaspecializedassociationwiththisimplicature.

Theform bu ,ontheotherhandseemstohaveaspecializedfunctionofindicatingthe moresalientofoneormoreentitieswiththesameminimumcognitivestatus.Though thisassociationdoesnotdirectlyderivefromthestructureoftheGHmodel,itisnot surprisingthatthereisaneedforsomemeansofindicatingthemoresalientoftwo entitiesthatarebothatmostactivated,afunctionwhich,bydefinition,cannotbefulfilled byaformwhichrequiresanentityinfocus—thatis,anullorreflexive.

Thepsychologicalrealityofboththerankingeffectresultingfromfocusactivated distinctionsandthefinerdistinctionsofthedemonstrativecontrastareadequately

239 supportedbymydataanalysis.Ipredictthatpairsofrankedformscanbeusedto indicatethesalienceofoneentityinrelationtoanother,andananalysisofsentenceswith multiplepronounsshowsacorrelationwiththesalienceranking(ofentities)whichis derivedfromeithercognitivestatusdifferencesoranimacydifferences.Sentence continuationscomparingthefourdemonstrativeformsprovidefurtherevidencethat contrastsinform(rankedpairs)guideanaphoraresolution.Whenaskedtoreada sentenceandwriteasecondsentenceusingaparticulardemonstrativeform,respondents tendtousethepronoun bu torefertotheentityassumedtobethehighestrankingentity

andconsistentlyused sho torefertoalowerrankingentity.Onepointofinterestisthat

theconsistentpreferencefor sho referringtolowerrankedentitiesseemsmuchmore consistentthancorrelationsbetween bu andhigherrankingentities(invarioustypesof questionnairedata).Themostlikelyexplanationforthisphenomenonisthat bu isaform whichcanbeusedtoimposesalienceonanentity(Mulkern2003).

Abroaderquestionrelatedtotheclaimthatthedemonstrativesencoderelative salienceistherelationshipbetweenrelativesalienceandthecategorizationof demonstrativeuseprimarilyintermsofdistancefeatures.AsIexplaininChapter2, followingtheGHmodel,Iassumethatthecognitivestatusofanentityisamorebasic featurethanspatialdistance,thoughthetwoareperhapsrelated.Whileanumberof recentstudiescharacterizetheuseofdemonstrativesintermsofattentionstates(Enfield

2003,interalia),itisinterestingtolookatpossiblecorrelationsbetweentraditional descriptionsoftheKumykdemonstrativesandthespecializationsIclaiminthisstudy.

Thecorrelationbetweentheform bu ,whichisdescribedasaproximaldemonstrative,

240 anditsspecializationinindicatingthehigherrankingofoneormoreentitiesisrather iconic.Ontheotherhand,theform sho ,whichspecializesinindicatingthelower rankingentityinaset,isdescribedasamedialdemonstrative,whichislessiconic,asone mighthaveexpectedthedistaldemonstrativehere.Itisinteresting,however,thatin

Kumyk,asinanumberoflanguages,thedistaldemonstrative oseemstofunctionasthe

default/neutralpersonalpronoun,whereas bu and sho aremuchlessfrequentandmore specialized.

6.4. Inherent versus imposed salience

FollowingMulkern2003andLevinsohn2008thisstudyassumesadistinction betweenthenaturalcognitiveprominenceofanentity,whichisrelatedtoinherent featuresofentitytypeandcontextualfeatures(lineardistanceofpreviousmention, argumentposition,eyegaze,introductionbyanominalorclause,etc.)andsalience whichcanbeattributedtoanentityviathelinguisticform.Mulkern(2003)describes salienceattributedviaalinguisticformasimposedsalience thatreflects“howthespeaker intendsthehearertorank[orrerank]discourseentitiesrelativetooneanother”(25).

Levinsohn,ontheotherhand,describesthistypeofsalienceasthematicprominence,or theattentiondrawntoanalreadyestablishedtopic,withtheassumptionthat“normally” attentionisdrawntonewinformation(2008:61).

Whathappensifaspeakerwantstoimposesalienceonanentitybythechoiceof linguisticform—here,choiceofreferringexpression?Imposedsalienceisimpossible withnullsandgenerallynotlikelytoworkwithphonologicallyreducedforms;henceitis associatedwithstressedforms.Functionally,imposedsalienceisassociatedwithboth

241 reranking,suchastopicshiftorothertypesofunexpectednessor“heightenedemotion”

(Mulkern2003:25,Levinsohn,2008:62)).

Whichtypesofformscanbeusedtoimposesalience?InKumyk,nullsand reflexivesbothsignal‘infocus’.Nulls,however,bydefinition,cannotimposesalience inanyform.Reflexivesarerestrictedtoentitieswhicharealreadyhighlysalient(most likelythehighestrankingentity)andtheuseoftheseformstoimposesalienceisnot usuallyassociatedwithrerankingorimposedranking,butwithanaddedcontextual effect.ARelevancetheoreticinterpretationofthisphenomenonmightbethattheuseof anovertformwithanentitythatisalreadyinfocus/highlysalientisunexpectedand hardertoprocessthananullargument,buthasagreaterpotentialforcontextualeffect

(SperberandWilson1986/95).Thisisbasedontheideathat,giventhebasiccontrast betweennullsandovertforms(primarilynonreflexiveforms),theuseofnullarguments isrestrictedtothesetofpossiblereferentsinfocus,whichmakesreferentidentification easierthanformostovertforms,whichsignalonlyactivationand,thuscanbeassociated withawidersetofpossiblereferents.Inotherwords,theuseofareflexive communicatessomethingtotheeffectof,“eventhoughI’musinganovertform(instead ofanull),whichusuallymeansalesssalientreferent,searchforahighlysalient referent.”Specificcontextualeffectsoftenmentionedintheliteratureonreflexives includelogophoricity,indicationofpointofviewshift(e.g.shiftfromnarratorasthe deicticcentertotheprotagonistasdeicticcenter),andcontrastivefocus.

WhileMulkernsuggeststhataspeakercanimposesalienceonanentityby“using anexpressionthatsignalsahighercognitivestatusthanareferentactuallyhasforthe

242 addresseeataparticularpointinthediscourse”(2003:27),Isuggestthatthespeakercan alsoimposesalienceonanentitybyusingaformthatsignals‘activated’—more specifically,thespecializedform bu .Thepronoun bu istheformnaturallyassociated

withimposedsalience,sinceitspecializesinindicatingthemoreprominententitywithin

aset.Thepronoun o,ontheotherhand,isbothmorefrequentandmoreneutral(which

followsideasoftypologicalmarkedness),while sho canbesaidtoimposelowerrelative

salience.

Evidencethat bu isassociatedwithimposedsalienceincludestheassociation between bu andreferencetoahighlysaliententitythatinvolvessometypeof

unexpectedness,particularlythecasesinwhich bu isassociatedwithtopicshiftand elaborationinthecorpusstudy(c.f.section4.3.5).Topicshiftinvolvesunexpectedness becausesubjectcontinuityisexpected;thus,asCenteringTheorypredicts,topicshift requiresmoreprocessingeffort(Walker,JoshiandPrince1998).Elaborationisan unexpectedaddingofinformationaboutatopicratherthancontinuingthechronological narrative,whichlikelyalsorequiresmoreprocessingeffort. 123 Particularlyincasesof topicshift,theuseof bu ,givenitsspecializationinsignalingthehighestrankingreferent

(which,inmostcaseswouldbetheprevioustopic),wouldeasetheprocessingeffortby signalingthehearertosearchforthemostsalientreferentofthepreviousutterance.Note

123 Mulkern2003alsopredictsthatalowerform(e.g.activatedformforinfocusreferent)canbeusedfor elaboration,whichis,tosomeextentexemplifiedinKumykbytheuseofdemonstrativeslike oininter sententialelaboration.Iproposethattheform bu ismorespecializedforthefunctionofelaborationwithin thesamenounphrase,while oandotherdemonstrativesareassociatedwithintersententialelaboration.

243 that,especiallyinthecaseoftopicshift,theuseofareflexivewouldnotcreatethesame effectbecauseitislikelytobeassumedtobecoreferentwiththesubjectofthecurrent sentenceratherthanwiththesubjectoftheprevioussentence.

Inothertypesofcases,theuseof bu ,especiallyincontrasttoanotherform,can

say,“payattention,thisentityisextrasalientforareasonprimarilyassociatedwith

speakeremotion.”Inonetext,theuseof bu isassociatedwiththeintroductionofthe

VIPparticipant(alsothe1 st personnarrator)intothenarrative.Cataphoricreferenceis anothercasewherethespeakersubjectivelyimposessalienceonapieceofnew information.Thefactthat bu istheonlydemonstrativeassociatedwithcataphoric referenceisfurtherevidenceofitsfunctioninimposingsalience.Thecontextualeffect ofimposedsaliencethatisassociatedwithspeakeremotionistocommunicatesomething aboutthespeaker’spointofview,thusformssuchas bu caneasilybeperceivedaspoint ofviewmarkers.

6.5. Questions of wider interest and areas for further study

Thedatapresentedinthisstudyofferinsightintoanumberofquestionsofwider interesttolinguistsandraiseanumberofquestionsthatarerelevantbothtotheoriesof referenceandtootherareasoflinguistics.

ThedescriptionofKumykpronominalsinthisstudyprovidesbasicinformation aboutthesyntacticdistributionofovertpronouns,nullsandreflexives.Ofparticular interestinthisdescriptionisthedistributionalevidenceinthecorpusanalysisand speakerjudgmentsthatsupportsthecategorizationofthereflexiveasapronominalinthe bindingtheorysense.Thoughanumberoflanguageshavelongdistanceusesof

244 reflexiveanaphors,fewlanguageshavereflexiveformsforwhichthedistributionis unrestrictedenoughforthoseformstobeclassifiedaspronominals(cf.Coleetal.2001,

Kornfilt2001),thustheKumykdatashouldbeofinteresttothisbodyofresearch.

Thecategorizationofnullreferenceinthisstudyalsocomplementsvarious descriptionsofnullanaphorainTurkiclanguages(ErguvanliTaylan1986,Turan1995, interalia).Thecorpusanalysisofnullargumentsandtheresultinghypothesisthatthese formsrequireareferentinfocussupportstheideaofpragmaticordiscourserestrictions onnullanaphoraandshouldbeofparticularinteresttothoseworkingwithinthesyntax pragmaticsinterface.Moreover,theevidencefortheroleofparallelismanddegreeof expectednessinzeroanaphorainKumykrelatestotheoriesofellipsis,notonlynominal ellipsis,butalsotoVPellipsis,whereparallelismiscitedasafactorthatcontributesto thefelicityofthisphenomenon.

Animportantquestionofinteresttoanumberofsubdisciplinesoflinguistics— particularlytypology—isthedegreetowhichanimacyfeaturesaffecttheinherent salienceofanentity.Croft(2003)claimsthatanimacyfeaturesareaparameterina numberoftypologicalpatterns.Forexample,differencesinanimacyfeatures(plus definitenessfeatures)cancorrelatewithovertversusimplicitobject/patientcasemarking aswellasdifferentgrammaticaltreatmentsofnounincorporationandindexation(167

168).Otherexamplesincludethefactthatnominalswithhumanreferentscanhavea grammaticizedformofnumbermarkingnotfoundamongthosewithinanimatereferents

(129).Morespecifictotheoriesofreference,Fraurud1996andPrat–SalaandBranigan

2000provideevidencethatinherentsaliencebasedonanimacyfeaturesinfluences

245 referentialchoice.Thekeytotheseclaimsisdevelopingamethodthatallowsthe investigatortodifferentiatebetweentheroleofsaliencederivedfromcontextualfeatures andsaliencederivedfromontologicalfeatures.

Whileanimacyfeatureslikelycontributetocognitivestatuscrosslinguistically, thedegreeandmannerinwhichanimacyfeaturesaffectpronominalchoiceobviously differsinalanguageinwhichanimacycategoriesaregrammaticallymarked(e.g.

English)versuslanguageslikeKumykinwhichanimacycategoriesarenot grammaticallymarked.Since,inthelattercase,onecancomparethecorrelationbetween pronominalchoiceandcognitivestatustothecorrelationbetweenpronominalchoiceand animacyfeatures,furtherstudiesoflanguagesofthistypewouldservetoelucidatethe roleofanimacyinrelationtocognitivestatus.

Arelatedquestioniswhetherornotevents,states,activities,facts,propositions, speechactsarenaturallymoreorlesssalientthanothertypesofinanimateentities.If inherentfeaturesofanentitysuchasanimacycontributetocognitivestatus,itisnot unreasonabletosupposethatothertypesofinherentfeaturescouldcontributetosalience, aswell.WhiletheGHmodelmakesadistinctionbetweenentitiesthataredirectlyversus indirectlyintroducedbyclauses,themodeldoesnotmakeadistinctioninsalience betweenthetypesofentitiesdirectlyintroducedbyclauses(namely,eventsandstates) andentitiesdirectlyintroducedbynominals.Nevertheless,certainaspectsofthisstudy, suchastherestrictivenessinformssuchas bu inreferencetoclausallyintroduced entities,suggestthattheremaybeadistinction,raisingquestionsabouthowentityclass affectsinherentsalience.

246 Finally,oneofthemoreinterestingtypologicalquestionsthisstudyindirectly addressesistherelationshipbetweencognitivestatusdistinctionsandtraditional categorizationsofdemonstrativesintermsofdistancefeatures.Thisstudydoesnottake thepositionthatcognitivestatusdistinctionsarederivedfromwhatsomewouldperceive asmorebasicdistinctionsintermsofdistancefeatures.Infact,theresultsofthestudy showthatitisthesocalledmedialdemonstrativesho inKumyk,notthedistal

demonstrative othatismostcloselyassociatedwiththeimplicature‘notinfocus’andthe

indicationoflowerrelativesalience.Somerecentstudieshavesuggestedclassifications

ofdemonstrativesbasedonjointattention(Enfield2003,interalia).Forexample,

ÖzyürekandKita2001(ascitedinEnfield2003)classifytheTurkishdemonstrative u, previouslydescribedasthemedialdemonstrative,asaformwhichindicatesareferent

thattheaddresseeisnotattendingto.ThisclassificationofTurkish uwouldfitbetter withthecurrentstudy’sevidenceabouttheKumyk“medial”demonstrativethanan explanationthatproposesthatdistancefeaturesaredirectlymappedontothediscourse useofdemonstrativesTakingintoconsiderationthefactthatdifferentlanguageshave differentnumbersofdemonstrativeswithvariationsrelatedtothelinguisticencodingof animacy,number,gender,orotherfeatures,furthercrosslinguisticcomparisonof discoursefeaturesofdemonstrativesandthecognitivestatusoftheirreferentsshould provideinterestingdataonwhatisbasictotheseforms.

247 References Abdulaeva,A.Z.1973.Slozhnopodchinennoepredlozhenievsovremennom Kumykskomiazyke.Dissertationinfulfillmentofthedegree“Kandidat FilologicheskieNauk”.Baku. Abdurakmanova,PasikhatDzhalilovna.2005.Osnovnyevoprosyistoricheskoĭ morfologiiKumykskogoiazyka:Morfemika,imennyeIglagol’nyekategorii. Disserationinfulfillmentofthedegreeof“DoktoraFilologichesieNauki.” Makhachkala. Ariel,Mira.1988.Referringandaccessibility.JournalofLinguistics 24:6587. Ariel,Mira.1990. Accessingnounphraseantecedents. London:Routledge. Asher,Nicholas.1993. Referencetoabstractobjectsindiscourse. Dordrecht:Kluwer AcademicPublishers . Baker,C.L.1995.Contrast,discourseprominence,andintensification,withspecial referencetolocallyfreereflexivesinBritishEnglish. Language 71:63101. Bosch,Peter,GrahamKatzandCarlaUmbach.2007.TheNonSubjectBiasofGerman DemonstrativePronouns.In Anaphorsintext:Cognitive,formal andappliedapproachestoanaphoricreference ,ed.MonikaSchwarzFriesel, ManfredConsten,andMareileKnees,145–164.Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Branco,Antonio,TonyMcEneryandRuslanMitkov,eds.2005. Anaphoraprocessing: Linguistic,cognitiveandcomputationalmodelling .Amsterdam:John Benjamins. BrownSchmidt,Sarah,DonnaK.Byron,andMichaelK.Tanenhaus.2005.Beyond salience:Interpretationofpersonalanddemonstrativepronouns. Journalof MemoryandLanguage 53(2):292313. Chafe,Wallace.1976.Givenness,contrastiveness,definiteness,subjects,topics,and pointofview.In Subjectandtopic ,ed.C.Li,2555.NewYork:Academic Press. Chomsky,Noam.1980.Onbinding. LinguisticInquiry 11:146. 1982. Someconceptsandconsequencesofthetheoryofgovernmentandbinding. Cambridge,Mass:TheMITPress. 248 1986. Barriers .Cambridge,Mass:TheMITPress. 1995. Theminimalistprogram .Cambridge,Mass:TheMITPress. Clinton,Paul.1997.TheGrammaroftheKumyklanguage.Unpublishedmanuscript. Cole,Peter,GabriellaHermon,andC.T.JamesHuang.2001. Syntaxandsemantics volume33:Longdistancereflexives .SanDiego:AcademicPress. Cornish,Francis.1999. Anaphora,discourse,andunderstanding:Evidencefrom EnglishandFrench .Oxford:ClarendonPress. Croft,William.2003. Typologyanduniversals .Cambridge:CambridgeUniversity Press. Dahl,Östen.2008.Animacyandegophoricity:Grammar,ontologyandphylogeny. Lingua 118:141–150. Dahl,ÖstenandKariFraurud.1996.Animacyingrammaranddiscourse.In Reference andReferentAccessibility ,ed.T.FretheimandJ.K.Gundel,4764.Amsterdam: JohnBenjamins. Diessel,Holger.1999. Demonstratives:Form,function,andgrammaticalization: Typologicalstudiesinlanguage42 .Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Diessel,Holger.2006.Demonstratives,jointattention,andtheemergenceofgrammar. CognitiveLinguistics 17:463489. Dik,S.C.1997. Thetheoryoffunctionalgrammar,I:Thestructureoftheclause . Berlin:MoutondeGruyter. Dixon,R.M.W.2003.Demonstratives:Acrosslinguistictypology. Studiesin Language 27(1):61112. Djanmavov.Iu.D.1967.DeeprichastiiavKumykskomliteraturnomiazyke .. Dmitriev,NikolaĭKonstantinovich.1940. Grammatikakumyksogojazyka .Moscow: Izdatel’stvoAkademiinaukSSSR. Enç,Murvet.1986.TopicswitchingandpronominalsubjectsinTurkish.In Studiesin Turkishlinguistics (Typologicalstudiesinlanguage,volume8 ),ed.DanIsaac SlobinandKarlZimmer,195–208.Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins.

249 ErguvanlıTaylan,E.1986.Pronominalversuszerorepresentationofanaphorain Turkish.In StudiesinTurkishLinguistics ,ed.D.I.SlobinandK.Zimmer,209 231.Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Erkü,FerideandJeanetteK.Gundel1987.Thepragmaticsofindirectanaphors.In The PragmaticPerspective ,ed.JefVerschuerenandMarcellaBertuccelliPapi,533 545.Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Enfield,Nick.2003.DemonstrativesinSpaceandInteraction. Language 79(1):82117. Fretheim,ThorsteinandJeanetteGundel,eds.1996. Referenceandreferent accessibility .Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Gadzhiakhmedov,N.E.1987. GrammaticheskiekategoriiglagolavKumykskomiazyke . Makhachkala. Gadzhiakhmedov,N.E.1998.Slovoizmenitel'nyekategoriiimeniiglagolav kumykskomiazyke.“Avtoreferat”oftheDissertationinfulfillmentofthedegree of“DoktoraFilologicheskieNauki.”Makhachkala. Givón,T.,ed.1983. Topiccontinuityindiscourse:Aquantitativecrosslanguagestudy . Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Gordon,RaymondG.,Jr.(ed.),2005. Ethnologue:Languagesoftheworld,fifteenth edition .Dallas,Tex.:SILInternational.Onlineversion: http://www.ethnologue.com/ .(Accessed111006.) Grice,H.P.1975.LogicandConversation.In SyntaxandSemantics3:SpeechActs, ed. P.ColeandJ.L.Morgan,4158.NewYork:AcademicPress. Grosz,BarbaraJ.,AravindK.Joshi,andScottWeinstein.1983.Providingaunified accountofdefinitenounphrasesindiscourse. Proceedingsforthe21stAnnual MeetingoftheAssociationforComputationalLinguistics,Cambridge,Mass :44 50. Grosz,Barbara,AravindJoshi,andScottWeinstein.1995.Centering:Aframeworkfor modellingthelocalcoherenceofdiscourse. ComputationalLinguistic s21:203 225. Grüning,André,andAndrejKibrik.2005Modellingreferentialchoiceindiscourse:A cognitivecalculativeapproachandaneuralnetworkapproach.In Anaphora processing:Linguistic,cognitiveandcomputationalmodelling ,ed.A.Branco,T. McEnery,andR.Mitkov,163198.Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins.

250 Gundel,Jeanette,MamadouBassene,BryanGordon,LindaHumnick,andAmel Khalfaoui.Referenceandcognitivestatus:Resultsfromcorpusstudiesof Eegimaa,Kumyk,Ojibwe,andTunisianArabic.Paperpresentedatthe10 th InternationalPragmaticsConference,Göteborg,Sweden,July813,2007. Gundel,Jeanette,NancyHedberg,andRonZacharski.1993.Cognitivestatusandthe formofreferringexpressionsindiscourse. Language 69:274307. Gundel,Jeanette,NancyHedberg,andRonZacharski.2005.PronounswithoutNP antecedents:Howdoweknowwhenapronounisreferential?In Anaphora processing:Linguistic,cognitiveandcomputationalmodelling ,ed.A.Branco,T. McEnery,andR.Mitkov,351364.Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Gundel,JeanetteK.andAnneMulkern.1998.Quantityimplicaturesinreference understanding. PragmaticsandCognition 6:2145. Gundel,J.K.,K.Borthen,andT.Fretheim.1999.Theroleofcontextinpronominal referencetohigherorderentitiesinEnglishandNorwegian.In Modelingand UsingContext.ProceedingsfromtheSecondInternationalInterdisciplinary Conference,CONTEXT’99(LectureNotesinArtificialIntelligence 1688),ed.P. Bouquetetal.,475478Berlin:SpringerVerlag. Gundel,JeanetteK.,MichaelHegarty,andKajaBorthen.2003.Cognitivestatus, informationstructure,andpronominalreferencetoclausallyintroducedentities. JournalofLogic,LanguageandInformation 12:281299. Gundel,Jeanette.2003.Informationstructureandreferentialgivenness/newness:How muchbelongsinthegrammar? Proceedingsofthe10thInternationalConference onHeadDrivenPhraseStructureGrammar,MichiganStateUniversity,July18 20,2003 (invitedtalk),ed.Müller,Stefan,122142.CSLIPublications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/.(Accessed10262006.) Gundel,Jeanette. 124 2004.CodingprotocolforstatusesontheGivennessHierarchy. Ms.UniversityofMinnesota.

124 Thisisarevisionofthe1993versionofthecodingprotocoltowhichNancyHedbergandRonZacharski contributed.AnnMulkern,TonyaCustis,BonnieSwierzbin,alsocontributedtothe2004revision.

251 Gundel,Jeanette. 125 2006.CodingprotocolforstatusesontheGivennessHierarchy. Ms.UniversityofMinnesota. Gundel,JeanetteK.,DimitriosNtelitheos,andMelindaKowalsky.2007.Children’suse ofreferringexpressions:Someimplicationsfortheoryofmind.In ZASPapersin Linguistics,Nr.48.SpecialIssueonIntersententialPronominalReferencein ChildandAdultLanguage ,ed.D.BittnerandN.Gagarina,122.Berlin. Halliday,M.A.K.,andR.Hasan.1976. CohesioninEnglish .London:Longman. Hankamer,JorgeandIvanSag.1976.Deepandsurfaceanaphora. LinguisticInquiry 7:(3):391426. Haspelmath,Martin.1995.TheConverbasacrosslinguisticallyvalidcategory.In Converbsincrosslinguisticperspective ,ed.M.HaspelmathandandE.König,1 56.Berlin:MoutondeGruyter. Hedberg,Nancy,JeanetteK.Gundel,andRonZacharski.2007.Directlyandindirectly anaphoricdemonstrativeandpersonalpronounsinnewspaperarticles. ProceedingsofDAARC2007 (theSixthDiscourseAnaphoraandAnaphora ResolutionColloquium,Lagos,Portugal.March2930,2007):3136. Hegarty,Michael.2003.Semantictypesofabstractentities. Lingua 113:891927. Hegarty,Michael,JeanetteK.Gundel,andKajaBorthen.2001.Informationstructureand theaccessibilityofclausallyintroducedreferents. TheoreticalLinguistics 27:163 186. Huang,Yan.2000. Anaphora:Acrosslinguisticapproach .Oxford:Oxford UniversityPress. Humnick,Linda.2002.SomecategoriesofparticipantreferenceinKumykNarrative. In CommentsondiscoursestructuresintenTurkiclanguages ,ed.JohnM.Clifton andDeborahA.Clifton,105133.Dallas:SILInternational.

125 Additionalcontributorstothe2006versionofthecodingprotocolincludeAmelKhalfoui,Linda Humnick,BryanGordon,MamadouBassene,andShanaWatters.

252 Humnick,Linda.2005.Pronominalreferencesandtheencodingofcognitivestatusin Kumyk.In ChicagoLinguisticSociety41:TheMainSession,ed.Rodney Edwards,PatrickMidtlyng,ColinSprague,andKjerstiStensrud,149163. Chicago:TheChicagoLinguisticSociety. Humnick,Linda.2008.NullargumentsinKumykadverbialclauses.In Subordination andcoordinationinNorthAsianlanguages:Currentissuesinlinguistictheory, Vol.300 ,ed.EdwardJ.Vajda,4762.Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Humnick,Linda.2007.PronominalreflexivesinKumyk.Paperpresentedatthe ConferenceontheLanguagesoftheCaucasus,December79,2007,Leipzig, Germany. Đsever,Selçuk.2003.InformationstructureinTurkish:Thewordorder–prosody interface. Lingua 113:1025–1053. Kadyradzhiev,K.S.1999.Istoricheskaiamorfologiiaimenivkumykskomiazyke. “Avtoreferat”oftheDissertationinfulfillmentofthedegreeof“Doktora FilologicheskieNauki.”Makhachkala. Kaiser,ElsiandJohnTrueswell.2004.ThereferentialpropertiesofDutchpronounsand demonstratives:Issalienceenough?In ProceedingsoftheConference“sub8– SinnundDedeutung” (ArbeitspapierNr.177,FBSprachwissenschaft),ed.Cécile Meier,MatthiasWeisgerber,137149.UniversitätKonstanz,Germany. Kaiser,Elsi.2005.Differentformshavedifferentreferentialproperties:Implicationsfor thenotionof'salience'.In AnaphoraProcessing:linguistic,cognitiveand computationalmodelling ,ed.A.Branco,T.McEnery,andR.Mitkov,261282. Philadelphia/Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Khangishiev,Zh.M.1985.PrichastiebKumykskomIazyke.Makhachkala. Khangishiev,ZhangishiM..1995. K”umyk”til:morphologiia .Makhachkala.  Kibrik,Andrej.1999.Referenceandworkingmemory:Cognitiveinferencesfrom discourseobservations.In Discoursestudiesincognitivelinguistics:Selected papersfromtheFifthInternationalCognitiveLinguisticsConference,Amsterdam, July1997 ,ed.KarenVanHoek,AndrejA.KibrikandLeoNoordman,2952. Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Kornfilt,Jaklin.2001.LocalandlongdistancereflexivesinTurkish.In Syntaxand semanticsvolume33:Longdistancereflexives ,ed.P.Cole,G.Hermon,and C.T.J.Huang,197225.SanDiego:AcademicPress.

253 Kuno,Susumo.1987. Functitonalsyntax:anaphora,discourseandempathy .Chicago: UniversityofChicagoPress. Küntay,A.C.andA.Özyürek.2002.Jointattentionandthedevelopmentoftheuseof demonstrativesinTurkish. BostonUniversityConferenceonLanguage Development 26:336347.Somerville:CascadillaPress. Lambrecht,Knud.1994. Informationstructuresandsentenceform .Cambridge: CambridgeUniversityPress. Levinsohn,Stephen.2008.Selfinstructionmaterialsonnarrativediscourseanalysis. OnlineURLathttps://mail.jaars.org/~bt/narr.zip.(ProducedfortheSummer InstituteofLinguisticsattheUniversityofNorthDakota,copyright2008bySIL International.) Levinson,StephenC.2004.Deixis.In HandbookofPragmatics ,ed.G.WardandL. Horn,97121.Oxford:Blackwell. Lewis,G.L.1967. Turkishgrammar .Oxford:OxfordUniversityPress. Lyons,John.1977. Semantics:Volume2 .Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress. Mulkern,Ann.2003.Cognitivestatus,discoursesalience,andinformationstructure: EvidencefromIrishandOromo.Ph.D.Dissertation,UniversityofMinnesota. Muratchaeva,Farida.2001.Deĭktichieskiesredstvavsintaksicheskoĭorganizatsii predlozheniiakumykskogoiazyk.PaperpresentedatLENCA1:International symposiumondeicticsystemsandquantificationinlanguagesspokenin andNorthandCentralAsia,,May2125,2001,UdmurtStateUniversity, Izhevsk,UdmurtRepublic.Abstractsed.ValeryKel'makov,PirkkoSuihkonen, PjotrVoroncov,BernardComrie,andMariaBeznosova. http://www.ling.helsinki.fi/uhlcs/symposiumizhevsk/abstraktitrtf.pdf. (Accessed1072006.) Ol’mesov,N.Kh.1994. Morfonologiiakumykskogoiazyka. Makhachkala: DagestanskiiGosuldarstvenoyUniversitet. Özyürek,AsliandSotaroKita.2001.Interactingwithdemonstratives:Encodingof jointattentionasasemanticcontrastinTurkishandJapanesedemonstrative systems.Ms.IstabulandNijmegen:KoçUniversityandMaxPlanckInstitutefor Psycholinguistics.(CitedbyNickEnfield,2003in“DemonstrativesinSpaceand Interaction” Language 79(1):82117.)

254 Pica,Pierre.1987.Onthenatureofthereflexivizationcycle. ProceedingsofNELS 17(2):483499. Poesio,MassimoandNissim,Malvina.2001.SalienceandpossessiveNPs:Theeffects ofanimacyandpronominalization.ProceedingsofAMLAP (PosterSession), Saarbruecken,September. PratSala,M.andH.P.Branigan.2000.Discourseconstraintsonsyntacticprocessingin languageproduction:AcrosslinguisticstudyinEnglishandSpanish. Journalof MemoryandLanguage 42:168182. Prince,E.F.1981.Towardataxonomyofgivennewinformation.In Radical pragmatics ,ed.P.Cole,223255.NewYork:AcademicPress. Schütze,CarsonT.1996. Theempiricalbaseoflinguistics:Grammaticalityjudgments andlinguisticmethodology .Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress. Silverstein,M.1976.Hierarchyoffeaturesandergativity.In Grammaticalcategoriesin Australianlanguages ,ed.Dixon,R.M.W.,112–171.AustralianInstituteof AboriginalStudies,Canberra. Sperber,DanandDeirdreWilson.1986/1995. Relevance:Communicationand cognition .London:Blackwell. Sperber,DanandDeirdreWilson.2004.Relevancetheory.In HandbookofPragmatics , ed.G.WardandL.Horn,607632.Oxford:Blackwell. Sultanmuradov,A.M.1997.Sposobyvyrazheniiaaspektualnykhznacheniĭv Kumykskomiazyke.Dissertationinfulfillmentofthedegree“Kandidat FilologicheskieNauk”. Turan,U.D.1995,NullversusovertsubjectsinTurkishdiscourse:Acenteringanalysis. Ph.D.dissertation,UniversityofPennsylvania. Turan,Ümit.1998.RankingforwardlookingcentersinTurkish:Universaland languagespecificproperties.In Centeringtheoryindiscourse ,ed.M.Walker,A. Joshi,andE.Prince,139160.Oxford:ClarendonPress. Vendler,Z.,1967. Linguisticsinphilosophy .CornellUniversityPress,Ithaca. Walker,Marilyn,AravindJoshi,andEllenPrince.1998.Centeringinnaturally occurringdiscourse:Anoverview.In Centeringtheoryindiscourse ,ed.M. Walker,A.Joshi,andE.Prince,128.Oxford:ClarendonPress.

255 Watters,ShanaandJeanetteGundel.2007.Anempiricalinvestigationoftherelation betweencoreferenceandquotations:Canapronounthatislocatedinquotations finditsreferent?.In Anaphora,Analysis,AlgorithmsandApplications , 6th DiscourseAnaphoraandAnaphorResolutionColloquium,DAARC2007,Lagos (Algarve),Portugal,March2930,2007 ( LectureNotesinArtificialIntelligence 4410)ed.AntoniorBranco,94106. ZribiHertz,A.1995.Emphaticorreflexive?OntheendophoriccharacterofFrench luimêmeandsimilarcomplexpronouns. JournalofLinguistics 31:333374.

256 Appendix 1: Geographical Distribution of the Languages of Dagestan Gordon,RaymondG.,Jr.(ed.),2005.Ethnologue:LanguagesoftheWorld,Fifteenthedition.Dallas,Tex.:SILInternational. Onlineversion:http://www.ethnologue.com/ .Accessed111006.ReprintedwithpermissionfromSIL,International.

257 Appendix 2: Text corpus

Text Title/ID Mode/ Source #pages Genre (glossed) 1 IAmGuilty,Marian, Written Abukov,Kamal.1973.Men 5 Introduction,Excerpt Narrative gunag’lyman,Mar’ian.In Siuĭsede fromChapter1 siuĭmesede .Makhachkala: (IDMarian) Dagestanskoeknizhnoe izdatel’stvo. 2 Theimportanceof Written Elicited 8 MTLiterature Expository (IDMotherTongue) 3 Hibat(Slander)and Written Akaev,Abusupiĭan.Reprintedin 7 Ekiiuzliuliuk Hortatory 1993. Paĭkhammarnyëlbulan . (Hypocrisy) Makhachkala:Dag’ystankitap (IDCharacter) basmakhanasy. 4 Khalk”ybyznyaburu Written G’amzatov,Ag’mat. 9 artsyn,o’rbolsyn. Hortatory Khalk”ybyznyaburuartsyn,o’r (IDEditorial) bolsyn. Yoldash. April20,2001:8. 5 Aihalay’sSlippers Written AĭhalaĭnyMachiĭleri. 9 (IDAihalay) Narrative Tangcholpan [Kumyklanguage journal] 6 Birth Oral Elicited 6 (IDBirth) Expository 7 Obituary Written Hasayev,Amet.Yoldash . 7 (IDObit) Narrative/ September7,2004:8.[newspaper] expository 8 TheProphet’s Written Pajxammarnyk”yzlary 12 Daughters Narrative (1993. Tangcholpan 4:1819.No (IDProphet) authorlisted.)[Kumyklanguage journal] 9 Oralnarrativesof Oral Elicited 9 video Narrative [IDOral] 10 Agriculturetext Written D.Zhavathanov.Ihshlamegen 15 (IDAgr) Expository Tishnemes.Yoldash .Aug.31, 2004:3.[newspaper] 11 Oralnarrativeof Oral Elicited 8 wordlessbook Narrative (IDOral) 12 Oralnarrativeof Oral Elicited 4 publicationevent Narrative (IDNT) (historical)

258 Appendix 3: Sample Text

TextTitle: TheProphet’sDaughters TextCode: Prophet Language: Kumyk PrimaryCoder: LindaHumnick Date: 25September2006 CodingGuidelines: 7/04 Thistext,entitled“Pajxammarnyk”yzlary”wasoriginallyprintedin Tangcholpan ,a Kumykliterarypublicationandisreproducedherebypermissionfromthepublisher. (Tangcholpan 1993,volume4:1819,noauthorlisted) WhilethistextprimarilyusesthetransliterationguidelinessuggestedbyALALC1997, thefollowingvariationsapply:“j”isusedinteadof“ĭ”torepresenttheCyrillicsymbol “ҕ”,“ja”isusedinsteadof“ia”torepresentthesymbol,“ р”and“jo”isusedinsteadof “ë”torepresent“ë”. Thefollowingabbreviationsareusedinglossing:ABL =Ablative, ACC =Accusative, COND =Conditional,CON =Conjunction,COP =Copula,DAT =Dative, DET =Determiner, EMPH =Emphatic, FUT =Future, GEN =Genitive, GER =Gerund/Converb, INF =Infinitive/VerbalNoun, LOC =Locative, MOD =Modifier, NOM =Nominalizer, PASS =Passive, PL =Plural, POSS =Possessive, PR =Present, PRT =Participal, PST =Past, RFLX =Reflexive(verbal), S=Singular, SPEC =Speculative.3.1,3.2,3.3and3.4indicatethe fourdistinctthirdpersonpronouns, bu , o, sho ,and shu ,respectively. Eachformcodedinthistextisenclosedinsquarebracketswithacorresponding superscriptnumber,e.g.[ birk”yzy ]x.Thecodinginformationappearsinthe correspondingfootnote,wherethefirstcolumnrepresentsthereferringform,thesecond columnidentifiesthereferent,andthethirdcolumnprovidesthecodinginformation. Thefollowingabbreviationsareusedincoding: FOC =infocus, ACT =activated, FAM =familiar, UNQ =uniquelyidentifiable, REF =referential, TYP =typeidentifiable,and UNDET =codingundetermined.Numbersfollowingthestatusindicatethefirstcriterionof thecodingprotocolwhichapplies(sometimesmorethanonecriterionisrelevant).

259 #Prophet.0001 Бирпайхаммарныбиркъызы,бирэшеги,бирмишиги,бирдеитиболгъан. [[Bir pajkhammarny] 1 bir k"yzy]2 [bir esheki]3 one prophet.GEN onegirl.3.POSSonedonkey.3.POSS [bir mishiki]4 [bir de iti]5 bolg"an. one cat.3.POSS oneEMPHdog.3.POSSbe.PST 'Aprophethadadaughter,adonkey,acatandadog.'

#Prophet.0002 Къызныэргебермеярайгъанвакътисигелип,бугъаргелечигеле. [[[K"yzny] 6 [erge]7 berme jarajg"an girl.ACC husband.DAT give.INFpossible.PST.PRT vak"tisi]8 gelip [bug"ar]9 time.3.POSS]come.GER3.1.DAT [gelechi]10 gele. one.who.requests.engagement come.PR 'Whenitcomestimeforthedaughtertobegiveninmarriage, apersonrequestingengagementcomestoher.'

#Prophet.0003 Къызныатасыдаразиболуп,къызынбережекбола. [[K"yzny] 11 atasy]12 da razi [girl.GEN]father.3.POSS]andagreed

1Birpajkhammarny; prophet; REF1 2Birpajkhammarnybirk"yzy daughter; UNQ1 3biresheki donkey; UNQ1 4birmishiki cat; UNQ1 5birdeiti dog; UNQ1 6K"yzny daughter; ACT1 7erge husband; TYP1 8K"yznyergebermejarajg"anvak"tisi; time; UNQ1 9bug"ar daughter; FOC1 10 gelechi suitor1(individual); REF1 11 K"yzny ; daughter; FOC5 12 K"yznyatasy; prophet; ACT1

260

bolup, [Ø] 13 [k"yzyn] 14 berezhek bola. Be.GER% girl.3.POSSgive.FUTbe.PR 'Thegirl'sfather,agreeing,willgivethegirl.'

#Prophet.0004 Экинчигюнбашгъаерденгелечигеле,огъардабережекбола. [ekinchi giun]15 [bashg"a erden]16 [gelechi]17 secondday otherplace.ABL one.who.requests.engagement gele, Come.PR [Ø] 18 [og"ar]19 da [Ø] 20 berezhek bola. % 3.2S.DATEMPH% give.FUTbe.PR 'Theseconddayapersonrequestingengagementcomesfrom anotherplace;[he]willgive[her]tohim,also.'

#Prophet.0005 Уьчюнчюгюнбирдагъысыгеле,огъардасёзбере,дёртюнчюгюнбирдагъысы геле,огъардакъызынбережекболупйибере. [U'chiunchiu giun]21 [Ø] 22 [birdag"ysy] 23 gele, Third day % one.more.3S.POSScome.PR

13 Ø ; prophet FOC1/2 14 k"yzyn; girl; FOC2/1 15 ekinchigun; secondday UNQ1 16 bashg"ajerden; anotherplace TYP1 17 gelechi; suitor2(individual) REF1 18 Ø; prophet; FOC4 19 og"ar suitor2 FOC1 20 Ø; daughter; FOC4/5 21 U'chiunchiugiun; thirdday; UNQ1 22 Ø; suitor(type) FOC5 23 birdag"ysy; suitor3(individual) REF1

261 [Ø] 24 [og"ar]25 da [sjoz]26 bere, % 3.2S.DATEMPHword give.PR [djortiunchiu giun] 27 [Ø] 28 [birdag"ysy]29 gele, fourthday % one.more.3S.POSScome.PR [Ø]30 [og"ar ]31 da [[Ø]32 k"yzyn] 33 berezhek bolup jibere. % 3.2S.DATEMPH% girl.POSS give.FUTbecome.GERsend.PR 'Thethirddayonemore[personrequestingengagement]comes; [he]promiseshim,also;thefourthdayonemore[personrequesting engagement]comes,[he]sendsthegirltobegiventohimalso.' #Prophet.0006 Гечеятгъанда,оьзюэтгентерсишгеоьзюгьёкюнюп,тобагъатюшетуруп бирдагъынамазкъыла. [Geche]34 [Ø] 35 jatg"anda [[o'ziu]36 etgen night% lie.down.PST.PRT.LOC self.3.POSSdo.PST.PRT ters ishge]37 [o'ziu]38 g'jokiuniup wrongwork.DATself.3.POSS regret.GER [tobag"a]39 [Ø] 40 tiushe turup repentance.DAT% descend.PRstay.GER

24 Ø; prophet; FOC4 25 og"ar; suitor3; FOC1 26 sjoz; word; TYP1 27 djortiunchiugiun; fourthday; UNQ1 28 Ø; suitor(type) FOC5 29 birdag"ysy; suitor4(individual) REF1 30 Ø; prophet; FOC4 31 og"ar; suitor4; FOC1 32 Ø; prophet; FOC2 33 k"yzyn; girl; FOC4 34 Geche; night; UNQ1 35 Ø; prophet; FOC1 36 o'ziu; prophet; FOC1 37 tersishge; badthing FAM 38 o'ziu; prophet; FOC1 39 tobag"a; repentance; TYP1 40 Ø; prophet; FOC1

262 [Ø] 41 [birdag"y namaz]42 k"yla. %one.moreprayerdo.PR '[That]nightwhen[he]laydown,heregretsthewrongthingthathe haddone,[he]fallsintorepentance,[and]praysonemoreprayer.' #Prophet.0007 БувакътидеЖабраилмалайикгелип, Аллагьутаъаласагъаэтгенишингегьёкюнме,бергенсёзюнгдетабуларсан депайтды, дей. [Bu vak"tide]43 [Zhabrail malajik]44 gelip, 3.1.DETtime.LOC Gabrielangelcome.GER [Allag'uta"ala]45 [sag"a]46 [[ [Ø] 47 etgen ishinge]48 Most.High.God 2S.DAT % do.PST.PRTwork.2S.DAT [Ø] 49 g'jokiunme, [[Ø] 50 bergen sjoziungde]51 % regret.NEG.PR% givePST.PRTword.2S.POSS.LOC [Ø] 52 tabularsan]53 dep ajtdy, % keep.FUT.2S]COMP say.3S.PST - [Ø] 54 dej. % say.PR 'AtthistimetheangelGabrielcomesand(he)says,"TheMostHighGod saidnottoregretthethingyouhavedone,thatyouwillkeepyour word."'

41 Ø; prophet; FOC1 42 birdag"ynamaz; prayer; TYP1 43 Buvak"tide ; time; ACT3 44 Zhabrailmalajik; angelGabriel; FAM2 45 Allag'uta"ala; God; NADirectspeech 46 sag"a; prophet; NADirectspeech 47 Ø; prophet; NADirectspeech 48 etgenishinge; NADirectspeech 49 Ø; prophet; NADirectspeech 50 Ø; prophet; NADirectspeech 51 Bergensjozjungde; NADirectspeech 52 Ø; prophet; NADirectspeech 53 etgenishinge g'jokiunme,bergensjoziungdetabularsan; NADirectspeech 54 Ø; AngelGabriel; FOC1

263 #Prophet.0008 Пайхаммарданамазынкъылып,юрегиндепарахатэтипята. [Pajkhammar]55 da [namazyn] 56 k"ylyp prophet EMPHprayer.3.POSS do.GER [Ø] 57 [iurekin]58 de [parakhat]59 etip [Ø] 60 jata. % heart.3.POSSEMPHpeace do.GER% lie.down.PR 'Theprophetfinishestheprayer,makeshisheartpeaceful,andgoesto bed.'

#Prophet.0009 Эртентургъандатилегенлергелипкъызныэлтелер. [Erten]61 [Ø]62 turg"anda [tilegenler]63 gelip morning % get.up.PST.PRT.LOC request.PST.PRT.PLcome.GER [Ø] 64 [k"yzny]65 elteler. % girl.ACC]lead.PR.PL 'Inthemorning,when[he]getsup,therequesterscomeandtakeaway thegirl/daughter.'

#Prophet.0010 Къызгетип,гечесиндепайхаммарныуьюндеоьзюнюкъызындандаисбайы къызбола. [K"yz]66 getip [gechesinde]67 girl leave.GER night.3S.POSS.LOC

55 Pajkhammar; prophet FOC4 56 namazyn; prayer ACT1 57 Ø ; prophet; FOC1 58 yurekin ; heart(prophet’s) UNQ1 59 parakhat peace; TYP1 60 Ø ; prophet; FOC1 61 Erten ; morning; TYP1; 62 Ø ; prophet; FOC1 63 tilegenler; suitor1+friends/relatives; FAM1 64 Ø; suitor1+friends/relatives; FOC1 65 k"yzny ; girl; FAM1 66 K"yz; girl; ACT1 67 gechesinde; evening/night UNQ1

264 [[pajkhammarny] 68 u'iunde]69 [[o'ziuniu]70 prophet.GEN at.3POSS.home self.3POSS.ACC.3.POSS k"yzyndan] 71 da [isbajy k"yz] 72 bola. girl.3.POSS.ABLand slendergirl be.PR 'Afterthegirl/daughterleaves,thatevening,intheprohet'shome, besideshisowndaughterthereappearsaslendergirl.'

#Prophet.0011 Оуьйдегимишигиболгъан. [O] 73 - [ [Ø]74 [u'jdegi] 75 mishiki]76 bolg"an. 3.2S% home.LOC.MOD cat.3.POSS be.PST 'Shehadbeen[his]housecat.'

#Prophet.0012 Эртенгелипгелешегенлеронудаэлте. [Erten]77 [Ø]78 gelip [geleshegenler]79 morning % come.GERask.for.in.marriage.PR.PRT.PL] [onu]80 da elte. 3.2S.ACCEMPHlead.PR 'Cominginthemorning,theonesasking[forher]inmarriage leadher[away],too.'

68 pajkhammarny; prophet; ACT1 69 pajkhammarnyu'iunde ; prophet’shome; UNQ1 70 o'ziuniu; prophet; FOC2 71 o'zuniuk"yzyndan; girl; FOC1/2 72 isbajyk"yz; slendergirl(formerlycat);REF1(possiblyFAM) 73 O; girl(cat); FOC1 74 Ø; prophet; FOC5 75 u'jdegi house TYP1; 76 u'jdegimishiki ; cat; FAM1(possiblyFOC) 77 Erten ; morning; TYP1 78 Ø; suitor2(+friends/relatives);UNDET 79 geleshegenler; suitor2(+friends/relatives);FOC1 80 onu; catdaughter; FOC1

265 #Prophet.0013 Уьчюнчюгечеитикъызбола,эртениндегелипокъызныдаалыпгете. [U'chiunchiu geche] 81 [ [Ø]82 iti] 83 [k"yz] 84 bola, thirdnight % dog.3.POSS girl become.PR [erteninde]85 [Ø]86 gelip [o k"yzny] 87 da morning.3.POSS.LOC % come.GER3.2.DETgirl.ACC EMPH [Ø]88 alyp gete. % take.GERleave.PR 'Thethirdnight[his]dogbecomesagirl,inthemorning[they] comeandtakethatgirl,too.'

#Prophet.0014 Араданбирэкигюнгетипбиргечеэшегикъызбола. Aradan [bir - eki giun] 89 getip [bir geche]90 Interval.ABL onetwoday pass.GER one night [[ Ø]91 [esheki]] 92 [k"yz] 93 bola. % donkey.3.POSS girl become.PR 'Afteracoupleofdayspass,onenight[his]donkeybecomesagirl.'

#Prophet.0015 Эртениндеонудаэлтелер.

81 U'chiunchiugeche; thirdnight; UNQ1 82 Ø; prophet; ACT1 83 iti; dog; FAM1 84 k"yz; doggirl TYP1 85 erteninde ; morning; UNQ1 86 Ø; suitor3(+friends/relatives);ACT1 87 ok"yzny; girl(previouslydog) FOC1 88 Ø ; suitor3; FOC1 89 birekigjun 12days; TYP1 90 birgeche; onenight; TYP1 ACTnullwillgohere(ref=prophet) 91 Ø; prophet; ACT1 92 esheki; donkey; FAM1 93 k"yz; girl; TYP1

266 [Erteninde]94 [onu]95 da [Ø] 96 elteler. Morning.3.POSS.LOC 3.2S.ACCEMPH% lead.PR.PL 'Inthemorning,[they]takeher,too.'

#Prophet.0016 Арадан45айгетиппайхаммаркъызларын,гиевлерингёрмегете. Aradan [4-5 ai]97 getip [pajkhammar]98 interval.ABL45 month pass.GERprophet [[ Ø]99 [k"yzlaryn]] 100 [[ Ø]101 [gijevlerin] 102 % girl.PL.3.POSS.ACC % son.in.law.PL.3.POSS.ACC gjerme gete. see.INF1 go.PR 'Aperiodof45monthshavingpassed,theprophetgoestoseehis daughtersandsonsinlaw.'

#Prophet.0017 Башлапэшегинебара. Bashlap [[ Ø]103 [eshekine]] 104 [Ø] 105 bara. Begin.GER% donkey.3S.DAT % go.PR '(He)beginsbygoingtothedonkey.'

94 Erteninde; morning; UNQ1 95 onu donkey; FOC1 96 Ø; suitor4(+friends/relatives)ACT1 97 45ai ; 45months; TYP1 98 pajkhammar; prophet; FAM1 99 Ø; prophet; FOC2 100 k"yzlaryn; daughters; FAM1 101 Ø; prophet; FOC2 102 gijevlerin ; sonsinlaw; FAM(coreferentw/suitors) 103 Ø; prophet; FOC1 104 eshekine; daughter/donkey; ACT1 105 Ø; prophet; FOC1

267 #Prophet.0018 Ахшамашапичип,онданмунданлакъырбашлаппайхаммаргиевюнеяшав гьалдансорай. [Akhsham]106 [Ø] 107 ashap - ichip ondan - mundan evening % eat.GER–drink.GERfrom.therefrom.here [lak"yr] 108 [Ø] 109 bashlap [pajkhammar] 110 [[ Ø]111 [gijeviune]] 112 conversation %begin.GERprophet %son.in.law.3.POSS.DAT [yashav g'aldan]113 soraj. lifesituation.ABL ask.PR ‘Intheevening,havingeatenandtakendrink,beginningthe conversationaboutthisandthat,theprophetaskshissoninlawabout [his]lifesituation.'

#Prophet.0019 Гиевю:Барыныдаэбитабулар,текбирзатяманбиргьайванны уьйретегензаманболду. [[Ø]114 [Gijeviu]] 115 -- “[Baryny_da]116 ebi tabular %son.in.law.3.POSSeverything okaykeeps.FUT2 tek [bir zat]117 jaman - [[bir g'ajvanni] 118 [Ø]119 u'jretegen but onething bad one animal.GEN% teach.PR.PRT

106 Axsham; evening; TYP1 107 Ø; prophet; FOC1 108 lak"yr; conversation; TYP1 109 Ø; prophet; FOC2 110 pajkhammar; prophet; FOC1 111 Ø; prophet; FOC2 112 gijeviune; soninlaw; FOC4 113 yashavg'aldan; lifesituation; UNQ1 114 Ø; prophet; FOC1 115 Gijeviu; soninlaw; ACT1 116 Baryny_da; NADirectspeech 117 birzat; NADirectspeech 118 bir g'ajvanni; NADirectspeech 119 Ø ; NADirectspeech

268 zaman]120 boldu.” time be.3S.PST 'Soninlaw:“Everything’sokay,butonethingisbad–enoughtime haspassedtoteachananimal.”'

#Prophet.0020

Бугъарайтгъандабир,айтмайкъойгъанда;къуллукъданишденбек парахат,деген. “[Bug"ar]121 ajtg"an da bir, ajtmay k"ojg"an 3.1.DAT say.PST.PRTEMPHonesay.NEG.PR leave.PST da; [k"ulluk"dan] 122 - [ishden]123 bek parakhat” - degen. EMPHservice.ABL work.ABLverypeacefullysay.PST '”Whetheryousaysomethingtoherornot,it’sthesame;verypeaceful inattitudetowardswork.”'

#Prophet.0021 Эшекишнибажармайэкендеппайхаммарныэсинегеле. [[Eshek] 124 [ishni] 125 bazharmaj eken] 126 dep [[pajkhammarny] 127 donkey work.ACCknow.how.NEG.PRCOP COMPprophet.GEN esine]128 gele. memory.3.POSS.DATcome.PR '”Adonkeywillnotknowhowtowork,”theprophetthought.'

#Prophet.0022

Гечебулардакъонакъдаболуп,эртенитинебара.

120 birg'ajvanniu'jretegenzaman; ; NADirectspeech 121 Bug"ar; NADirectspeech 122 k"ulluk"dan; NADirectspeech 123 ishden; NADirectspeech 124 Eshek; donkey(type); TYP1; 125 ishni; work; UNQ1(theworkofadonkey) 126 Eshekishnibazharmayeken; thoughtoftheprophet; ACT3 127 pajkhammarny; prophet; ACT/FOC(addresseeindialogue) 128 pajkhammarnyesine; prophet’smind; UNQ1

269 Geche [Ø] 129 [bularda] 130 [k"onak"] 131 da bolup erten night% 3.1.PL.LOC guest thusbe.GERmorning [[Ø] 132 [itine]] 133 [Ø] 134 bara. % dog.3.POSS.DAT% go.PR 'Atnightheisaguestattheirplace;inthemorninghegoestothe dog.' #Prophet.0023 Шундадашолайгечболгъунчаяшавгьалдансорашып,гиевюдебугъаркант эте:“Бирзатайтмаамалёкъ,итйимикгьапгьазир,сенбирни айтсанг,сагъаоннуайтажакъ.” [Shunda] 135 da [sholaj]136 gech bolg”uncha 3.4.LOC EMPH3.3.ADV late become.before.GER [[yashav] 137 g'aldan]138 [Ø]139 [Ø] 140 sorashyp lifesituation.ABL % % greet.GER

[[Ø] 141 [gijeviu]] 142 de [bug"ar] 143 [kant] 144 ete: % son.in.law.3.POSSEMPH 3.1.DAT complaint do.PR “[Bir zat] 145 ajtma amal jok" [it]146 yimik one thing say.INF1 situationexist.NEG dog like

129 Ø; prophet; FOC1(experiencerofpsych.verb) 130 bularda; son.in.law+daughter; FOC4 131 k"onak"; guest; TYP1 132 Ø; prophet; FOC2 133 itine; doggirl; FAM1 134 Ø; prophet; FOC2 135 Shunda; dog’shome; ACT1 136 sholaj; NA:intensifier 137 yashav; life; TYP1; 138 yashavg'aldan; lifesituation; UNQ1 139 Ø; prophet; FOC1 140 Ø; soninlaw FOC2/4(via shunda =dog’shome) 141 Ø; prophet; FOC2 142 gijeviu; soninlaw; FOC2 143 bug"ar; prophet; FOC1 144 kant; complaint; TYP1 145 Birzat; NA:DIALOGUE 146 it; NA:DIALOGUE

270 g'ap-g'azir [sen]147 [birni]148 ajtsang [sag"a]149 [onnu]150 ready2S one.ACC] say.2S.COND2S.DAT ten.ACC [Ø] 151 ajtazhak." % say.FUT1 'Atthatplace,thus(asbefore),beforeitgotlate,(he)asksabout thesituationoflife,andhissoninlawanswershim:“It’snot possibletosayasingleword.Ifyousayoneword,she’s]readylikea dogandwillsaytenwords.”' #Prophet.0024 Авзунжыйсабагьанасыёкъэди,дей. [Avuzun]152 djyjsa bag’anasi [mouth.3.POSS] hold.3.COND reason .3.POSS jok" edi - [Ø] 153 dej. exist.NEG3.PAST.AUX– % say.PR '”If(she)hadheldhertonguetherewouldhavebeennoreason,”(he) says.' #Prophet.0025 Итгьапламаймыдадепатасыныэсинегеле. [It]154 g’aplamajmy da dep dog barkNEGINTEREMPHCOMP [[ [Ø] 155 [atasyny]] 156 esine]157 gele. % father.3.POSS.GENmemory.3.POSS.DATcome.PR 'Itcametoherfather’smind,“don’tdogsbark?”'(formofindirect speech)

147 sen; NA:DIALOGUE 148 birni; NA:DIALOGUE 149 sag"a; NA:DIALOGUE 150 onnu; NA:DIALOGUE 151 Ø; daughter(previouslydog)NA:DIALOGUE 152 Avuzun; NA:DIALOGUE 153 Ø; soninlaw; FOC1 154 It; dog(generic); TYP1 155 Ø; soninlaw; FOC1 156 atasyny; prophet; FOC4 157 atasynyesine; father’smind; FOC(coreferentialwithprophet)

271 #Prophet.0026 Булардагечедекъалмай,мишигинебара. [Bularda]158 [geche]159 de [Ø] 160 k"almaj [[Ø]161 [mishikine]]162 3.1.PL.LOCnight EMPH % stay.NEG.PR%cat.3.POSS.DAT [Ø] 163 bara. % go.PR ‘Notstayingwiththemforthenight,(he)wenttothecat.’

#Prophet.0027 Хошбешэтипсорашыпашгъаолтуралар. Hosh-besh [Ø] 164 etip sorashyp [ashg"a]165 [Ø] 166 olturalar. Greeting%do.GER greet.GER food.DAT % sit.3PL.PR ‘Greetingeachother,(they)sitdowntoeat.'

#Prophet.0028 Ашапбитгенде,буяшныкъатыныбарыпкараватдаятыпюхлай. [Ø] 167 Ashap bitgende [[bu yashni]168 % eat.GERfinish.PST.PRT.LOC3.1child.GEN] katiny]169 baryp [karavatda]170 [Ø] 171 jatyp jukhlaj. wife.3.POSSgo.GERbed.LOC % lie.down.GERsleep.PR 'When[they]finishedeating,thischild'swifegoesandliesdownto sleepinthebed.'

158 Bularda; dogdaughter+soninlaw;FOC5 159 geche; night UNQ 160 Ø; prophet; FOC1(experiencerofpsych.verb) 161 Ø; prophet; FOC2 162 Mishikine; cat; FAM1 163 Ø; prophet; FOC1/2 164 Ø; prophet+cat+soninlaw ACT 165 ashg"a; food TYP1 166 Ø; prophet+cat+soninlaw; FOC1/2; 167 Ø; prophet+cat+soninlaw; FOC1; 168 buyashni; soninlaw(w/cat); ACT(mentionedinplural) 169 buyashnikatiny; wife/cat; ACT(mentionedinplural) 170 karavatda; bed; TYP1 171 Ø; cat/daughter; FOC1

272

#Prophet.0029 Пайхаммаргиевюнесорай:Яшавгьалнечикдир?деп. [Pajkhammar]172 [ [Ø]173 [gijeviune]] 174 soraj: - [yashav g'al]175 prophet % son.in.law.3.POSS.DATask.PR–[lifestate nechikdir? - dep. how.COPsay.GER 'Theprophetaskshissoninlaw,"How'sthestateoflife?"' #Prophet.0030 Гиевюайта:Мунасенгёрегенкюйде,ишкъуллукъбуланбираварасыда ёкъ. [Gijeviu]176 ajta: - Muna [[sen] son.in.law.3.POSSsay.PR behold2S gjorgen kuyde ]177 [ish - k"ulluk"]178 see.PST.PRTway.LOC work–service bulan [Ø] 179 [bir avarasy] 180 da jok". with% oneconcern.3.POSS EMPHexist.NEG 'Thesoninlawsays,"Asyousee,shehasnoconcernforthework.”’

#Prophet.0031 Ашапбитсе,тепсинидежыймай,барыпятыпкъала. [Ø] 181 Ashap bitse [tepsini]182 de % eat.GERfinish.3.CONDtray.ACC EMPH

172 Pajkhammar; prophet; ACT1 173 Ø; prophet; FOC2 174 Gijeviune; soninlaw; FOC5 175 yashavg'al; lifecondition UNQ(yourlifecondition) 176 Gijeviu; soninlaw; FOC5; 177 sengjorgenkuyde; NADirectspeech 178 ishk"ulluk"; NADirectspeech 179 Ø; NADirectspeech 180 Biravarasy; NADirectspeech 181 Ø; NADirectspeech 182 tepsini; NADirectspeech

273 [Ø] 183 zhyjmaj baryp jatyp k”ala. % clean.up.NEG.PR go.GERlie.down.GER stayPR '”If[she]finisheseating,[she]doesnotcleanupthedishes;[she] goesandliesdown.”'

#Prophet.0032 Юхубусадагъызаттарыкътюгюл,дей. [Iukhu]184 busa [dag”y zat]185 taryk” tugul - sleep howevermorething necessary not [Ø] 186 dej. % say.PR '”Otherthansleep,[she]doesnotneedanything,"[he]says.'

#Prophet.0033 Гечебулардадакъалып,пайхаммароьзюнюкъызынабара. Geche [Ø]187 [bularda]188 da k"alyp [pajkhammar]189 night% 3.1.PL.LOCEMPHstay.GERprophet [[o'ziuniu]190 k"yzyna]191 bara. self.3POSS.GEN girl.3.POSS.DAT go.PR '[After]stayingwiththematnight,theprophetgoestohisown daughter.'

#Prophet.0034 Атасынариденгёргендекъызыда,гиевюдеалдынабарыпхошбешэтип, атасынгётерипуьйгегийирелер.

183 Ø; wife; NADirectspeech 184 Iukhu; NADirectspeech 185 dag”yzat; NADirectspeech 186 Ø; soninlaw; FOC1 187 Ø; prophet; UNDET(backwardsanaphora) 188 bularda; soninlaw FOC1(pluralisidiomatic) 189 pajkhammar; prophet; FOC1 190 o'ziuniu; prophet; FOC2; 191 o'ziuniuk"yzyna; girl(owndaughter); FAM1;

274 [[Ø]192 Atasyn]193 ariden [Ø] 194 gjorgende father.3.POSS.ACCfurther.ABL% see.PST.PRT.LOC [[Ø]195 k"yzy ]196 da [[Ø]197 [gijeviu]]198 de % girl.3.POSSand % son.in.law.3.POSSand [Ø] 199 aldyna baryp [Ø] 200 [khosh-besh] 201 etip [Ø] 202 % before.DATgo.GER% greeting do.GER % [[Ø]203 atasyn]204 gjoterip [Ø] 205 [u'jge ]206 gijireler. % father.3.POSS.ACCbring.up.GER% home.DAT bring.in.3PL.PR 'Seeingtheirfatherfromfaraway,hisdaughterandsoninlawgo beforehimandgreethim,bringingtheirfatherupandbringing(him) intotheirhome.'

#Prophet.0035 Ашгьазирболгунчаюрекавуртмасдепгиев,къатынгъакъычыра:Ариги уьйденбирхарбузалыпгелчидеп. [Ash] 207 g’azir bolguncha [iurek]208 avurtmas foodready be.before.GER heart cause.to.be.heavy.NEG dep [gijev ]209 [k”atyng"a ]210 k"ychyra: COMPson.in.law wife.DAT cry.out.PR

192 Ø; daughter/husband; UNDET(backwardsanaphora) 193 Atasyn; prophet; FOC1 194 Ø; daughter/husband; FOC2(viaPOSSsuffix) 195 Ø; prophet; FOC2 196 k"yzy; girl; FOC2 197 Ø; prophet; FOC2 198 gijeviu; soninlaw; FOC2 199 Ø; prophet; FOC2 200 Ø; husband/wife; FOC1/2 201 khoshbesh; greeting; TYP 202 Ø; husband/wife; FOC1/2 203 Ø; husband/wife; FOC1/2 204 atasyn; prophet; FOC5 205 Ø; husband/wife FOC1/2 206 u'jge; home UNQ 207 Ash; food; TYP 208 iurek; heart TYP 209 gijev; soninlaw ACT(partofpluralsubject) 210 k”atyng"a; wife; ACT(partofpluralsubject)

275 - [Arigi u'jden ]211 [bir kharbuz ]212 [Ø] 213 alyp further.MODhome.ABLonewatermelon % take.GER gel chi dep. comeEMPHsay.GER 'Sothattheheartwon'tbeheavywhilewaitingforthefoodtobe ready,thesoninlawshoutstohiswife,saying,"Bringawatermelon fromthebackroom."'

#Prophet.0036 Аригиуьйдеашэтегенкъатыныбирхарбузалыпгеле. [[Arigi u'jde]214 [ash]215 etegen [[Ø]216 k”atyny]] 217 Further.MODhome.LOC food do.PR.PRT % wife.3.POSS] [bir kharbuz ]218 alyp gele. onewatermelontake.GERcome.PR 'Thewife,whowasmakingfoodinthebackroom,bringsone watermelon.'

#Prophet.0037 Эрихарбузгъакъарап:Бучубечиген,башгъасынгелтирдей. [[Ø]219 Eri]220 [kharbuzg"a]221 k"arap: - “[Bu]222 % husband.3.POSS watermelon.DATlook.GER–3.1

211 Arigiu'jden; NADirectspeech 212 birkharbuz; NADirectspeech 213 Ø; wife NADirectspeech 214 Arigiu'jde; backroom; FAM1 215 ash ; food; ACT1; 216 Ø; husband; FOC1 217 Arigiu'jdeashetegenk”atyny; wife; ACT1 218 birkharbuz ; watermelon; ACT1(viadirectspeech) 219 Ø; wife; FOC5 220 Eri; husband/soninlaw; FOC1; 221 kharbuzg"a; watermelon1; FOC5 222 Bu; watermelon1; NADirectspeech

276 chu bechigen [bashg"asyn]223 [Ø] 224 geltir,” - [Ø] 225 dej. EMPHwither.PST.PRTother.3.POSS.ACC%bring%say.PR 'Thehusband,lookingatthewatermelon,says,"Thisoneiswithered, bringanotherone."'

#Prophet.0038 Катыныохарбузнуэлтип,башгъасыналыпгеле. [[Ø]226 K”atyny] 227 [o kharbuznu]228 eltip % wife.3.POSS 3.2.DET watermelon.ACCbring.GER [[Ø] 229 bashg"asyn ]230 [Ø] 231 alyp gele. % other.3.POSS.ACC% take.GERcome.PR 'Thewifetakesawaythatwatermelonandbringsanotherone.'

#Prophet.0039 Эридаъыдахарбузгъакъарап:Некъалынкъабукълусунгелтирдинг, адамшавлусуёкъму,дей. [[Ø] 232 Eri] 233 [dag"y da kharbuzg"a]234 k"arap: % husband.3.POSS anotheryetwatermelon.DATlook.GER - “Ne [k"alyn [ [Ø] 235 k”abuk”lusun]] 236 [Ø]237 whatthick % rind.3.POSS.ACC %

223 bashg"asyn; anotherone(watermelon2);NADirectspeech 224 Ø; wife; NADirectSpeech 225 Ø; husband; FOC1/2 226 Ø; husband; FOC1 227 K”atyny; girl/daughter/wife; FOC5 228 okharbuznu; watermelon1; FOC5(directspeech+narrative) 229 Ø; watermelon(type) FOC4 230 bashg"asyn; anotherone(watermelon2);ACT1(directspeech) 231 Ø; wife; FOC1/2 232 Ø; wife; FOC1 233 Eri; husband; FOC5 234 dag"ydakharbuzg"a; watermelon2; FOC5 235 Ø; NADirectspeech 236 k"alynk”abuk”lusun; NADirectspeech 237 Ø; NADirectspeech

277 geltirding, [[Ø] 238 adamshavlusu] 239 jok"mu, - [Ø] 240 dej. bring.2S.PST % decent.3.POSSexist.NEG.INTER % say.PR 'Thehusband,againlookingatthewatermelon,says,"Whatathick skinnedoneyoubrought,don'tyouhaveadecentone?"'

#Prophet.0040 Къатындаъыдаохарбузнуэлтип,бирбашгъасынгелтире. [K”atyn]241 dag"y da [o kharbuznu ]242 wife againand3.2.DET watermelon.ACC eltip [bir [ [Ø]243 bashg"asyn]]244 [Ø] 245 geltire. bring.GERone% other.3.POSS.ACC % bring.PR 'Thewifeagaintakesthatwatermelonawayandbringsanotherone.'

#Prophet.0041 Эрихарбузнуалып: [[Ø] 246 Eri] 247 [kharbuznu] 248 alyp: % Husband.3.POSS watermelon.ACCtake.GER

'Thehusband,takingthewatermelon...'

#Prophet.0042 Мунашулайбизиняшавубузда,арадагъытатывлукъда. - Muna [shulaj]249 [bizin yashavubuz]250 da behold3.4.ADV 1PL.GENlife.1PL.POSS EMPH

238 Ø; NADirectspeech 239 adamshavlusu; watermelon3; NADirectspeech 240 Ø; husband; FOC1/2 241 K”atyn; wife; FOC4(viadirectspeech) 242 okharbuznu; watermelon2; FOC5 243 Ø; watermelon(type) FOC4 244 birbashg"asyn; watermelon3; ACT3(viadirectspeech) 245 Ø; wife; FOC2 246 Ø; wife; FOC1 247 Eri; husband; FOC5 248 kharbuznu; watermelon3; FOC5 249 shulaj; NAdirectspeech 250 bizinyashavubuz; NAdirectspeech

278 [aradag"y tatyvluk”da]251 . interval.even.soagreement.LOC '"Behold,ourlife,ourmutualagreement(relationship),islike this.”'

#Prophet.0043 Бизинуьйдебиргинебирхарбузубузбарэди. [Bizin u'jde] 252 [birgine bir kharbuzubuz] 253 bar edi. 1PL.GENhome.LOCone.MOD.whatonewatermelon.1PL.POSS exist3.PAST.AUX '"Inourhousetherewasonlyonewatermelon.”'

#Prophet.0044 Мен...... нечекеренкъайтардым,ояманбирисибечигендеп,будаариги уьйгегетип,къайтыпшохарбузнуалыпгелеэди. [Men]254 [neche keren]255 [Ø] 256 k"ajtardym [o]257 1S how.manyinstance % cause.to.return.1S.PST3.2S jaman [[Ø] 258 [birisi]] 259 bechigen dep [bu ]260 da bad% one.of wither.PST.PRTsay.GER 3.1 EMPH [arigi u'jge ]261 getip kajtyp further.MODhome.DAT leave.GERreturn.GER

251 aradag"ytatyvluk”da; NAdirectspeech 252 Bizinu'jde NAdirectspeech 253 birginebirkharbuzubuz; NAdirectspeech 254 Men; NAdirectspeech 255 nechekeren; NAdirectspeech 256 Ø; wife; NAdirectspeech 257 o; watermelon; NAdirectspeech 258 Ø; watermelon(type) FOC4 259 birisi; one(watermelon) NAdirectspeech 260 bu; wife; NAdirectspeech 261 arigiu'jge; NAdirectspeech

279 [sho kharbuznu]262 [Ø] 263 alyp gele edi. 3.3 watermelon.ACC % take.GER come.PR 3.PAST.AUX '"HowevermanytimesIwouldhavesent(her),sayingthatoneisbad, oneiswithered,shewouldhavegonetothebackroomandbroughtthis watermelon."'

#Prophet.0045 Дагъыхарбузёкьдегензатныайтмады. [[[Dag"y kharbuz]264 jok"] 265 degen zatni]266 morewatermelonexist.NEGsay.PST.PRTthing.ACC [Ø] 267 ajtmady. %say.NEG.3.PST '"(She)didnotsaythattherearenomorewatermelons."'

#Prophet.0046 Мунашулайтатывлутурабыз,деген. Muna [shulaj] 268 tatyvlu [Ø] 269 turabyz behold3.4.ADV delicious% stay.PR.1PL - [Ø] 270 degen. % say.PST '"Lookhowwestayinsuchasweetstate,"(he)said.'

#Prophet.0047 Гечебулардакъонакъдаболуп,пайхаммарэртенуьюнекъайта.

262 shokharbuznu; NAdirectspeech 263 Ø; wife; NAdirectspeech 264 Dag"ykharbuz; NAdirectspeech 265 Dag"ykharbuzjok"; NAdirectspeech 266 Dag"ykharbuzjok"degenzatni; NAdirectspeech 267 Ø; wife; NAdirectspeech 268 shulaj; NAdirectspeech 269 Ø; wife+husband; NA;directspeech 270 Ø; husband; FOC1(clauseb/fquotedspeech)

280 Geche [Ø] 271 [bularda]272 [k"onak" ]273 da bolup night% 3.1.PL.LOCguest EMPHbe.GER [pajkhammar ]274 erten [ [Ø] 275 [u'iune ]] 276 kajta. prophet morning % home.DATreturn.PR 'Theprophetstaysattheirplaceforthenight,and,inthemorning, returnstohishome.'

271 Ø; prophet; UNDET(backwardsanaphora) 272 Bularda; husband(+wife); FOC1 273 k"onak"; guest; TYP1; 274 pajkhammar; prophet; FOC1/2 275 Ø; prophet; FOC2 276 u'iune; prophet’shome; FAM1

281 Appendix 4: Questionnaire Thequestionnairewasadministeredinwrittenformto5respondents. 1Thedirectionsforeach sectionarewritteninRussian. 2Allothermaterial(includingsamplesentences)isinKumyk. Theresponsesarecodedaccordingtocolors,andtheorderpresentedhereisasfollows:blue, orange,black,red,green. I. ДлякаждогопредложенияA,напишитепредложениеБ,которыйявляется продолжением.ВпредложенииБ,используйтеслово,котороеотмеченовкруглых скобках.Форма(падеж)этогословоможетизмениться,взависимостиот предложения.Есливозможно,избегитепредложений,вкоторыхэтослово сопровождаетсясуществительным,например,‘Студентогъар савболдеди.’,ани ‘Студентомуаллимге савболдеди.’ ForeachsentenceA,writeasentenceforBwhichwouldbeacontinuationofA.Inthe sentenceinB,usethewordinparentheses.Theform(case)ofthiswordcanchangeaccordingto thesentencecontext.Ifpossible,avoidsentencesinwhichthiswordisfollowedbyanoun,for example‘Thestudentthankedhim .’Andnot‘Thestudentthankedthat teacher.(Thoughin Englishthesesentencesaretranslatedwithtwodifferentforms, him and that, inKumyk,theform oisusedinbothsentences,sincethisformcanbeeitherapronounoradeterminer.) Например: Forexample А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ Б.(о) Response:Осавболдеди. ‘(S)hesaidthankyou.’ Или…(Or…)

1ThisresearchiscarriedoutunderNSFgrant#0519890toJeanetteGundelandreceivedIRBexemption underthesamegrant. 2ThechoiceofRussianforthedirectionsisrelatedsolelytotheprocessofmydevelopingthe questionnairewithlimitedaccesstoKumykspeakers.

282 А.Марияманасынаалмаберди. ‘Mariamgavehermotheranapple.’ Б.(о) Response:Анасыогъар разилигинбилдирди. ‘Hermotherexpressedherappreciationtoher.’ Therespondentsgivecompletesentences.Here,Ilistthereferentofthetargetpronounforeach respondent. 1. А.Ибрагьимресторандашорпаашады. ‘Abrahamatesoupintherestaurant.’ Б.(бу)

Responses:Ibragim;restaurant;Ibragim;Ibragim;soup

2. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’ Б.(о) Responses:Bolat;Bolat;Bolat;Bolat;Bolat

3. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ Б.(шо) Responses:book;book;book;book;bookorgivingofbook

4. А.Ибрагьимресторандашорпаашады. ‘Abrahamatesoupintherestaurant.’ Б.(о) Responses:Ibragim;Ibragim;Ibragim;Ibragim;soup

5. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ Б.(шу) Responses:student;book;book;book;book

283 6. А.Ибрагьимресторандашорпаашады. ‘Abrahamatesoupintherestaurant.’ Б.(шо) Responses:restaurant;soup;restaurant;restaurant;soup

7. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. ‘Bolatfellwhileriding(his)horse.’ Б.(шо) Responses:horse;horse;DEM;thus;(ambiguousbetweenBolatorthehorse) 8. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ Б.(бу)

Responses:student;incorrectresponse;student(presentational);student;book

В(9)и(10),думайте,каквызакончилибыпредложениеснеявнымподлежащим вместоместоимения.МыиспользуемсимволØ,чтобыпредставитьнеявный подлежащийилиобъект. In(9)and(10)thinkhowyouwouldfinishthesentencewithanimplicitforminsteadofa pronoun.Weusethesymbol“Ø”tostandforanimplicitsubjectorobject. Например:

А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.’ Б.Ø….. Response:Сонг,гетди. Later((s)he)left. 9.А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.’ Б.Ø….. Responses: Bolat;incorrectform;Bolat;Bolat;Bolat

284 10.А.Ибрагьимресторандашорпаашады. ‘Abrahamatesoupintherestaurant.’ Б.Ø….. Responses: Ibragim;Ibragim;Ibragim;Ibragim;Ibragim II:Прочитайтекаждуюпарупредложений.ЗаполнитесьвбланкевопредложенииБс одномместоимениемвсоответствующемпадеже.Еслиестьнескольковозможных вариантов,выбираютодногоизних.Еслинеявляетсясоответствующее использоватьместоимение,заполнятьсявтипесловаилислов,которые соответствовалибыконтексту. Readeachpairofsentences.Fillintheblankinthesecondsentencewithapronouninthe appropriatecase.Ifthereareseveralpossibilities,chooseoneofthem.Ifitisnotappropriateto useapronoun,fillintheblankwiththetypeofwordorwordswhichwouldbeappropriateforthe context. Местоимении:бу, о, шо, шу, оьзю

11. Например: А.Муалимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Студент…огъар..савболдеди. Forexample: ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. Thestudenttoldhim/her thankyou. ‘ 12. А.Муалимстудентгекитапберди. Б.……..„Тангчолпан”эди.

‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. ____wasTangcholpan.’ Responses:bu;sho;bu;sho;ol

13. А.Муалимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонгмуаллим….....экзаменибуланкъутлады. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. Latertheteachercongratulated___ontheexam.’ Responses: onu;onu;onu;onu;onu

285 14. А.Муалимстудентгекитапберди. Б.…….дансонгмуаллимстудентникъолуналды. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. After___theteachershookthestudent’shand.’ Responses:shondan;shondan;shondan;shondan;ondan 15. А.Муалимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Студент…....чалтохуду. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. Thestudentread___quickly.’ Responses:munu;shonu;shonu;shonu;onu

16. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Хали……..барамагъанындепайтды. ‘Bolatfellwhileridinghishorse. Now(he)saidthat(he)willnotride___.’(Now(he)saidthat__willnotride(it).) Responses: G’aliog”ar dag’yminipbarma’ak”mandepajtdy;o’ziu; G’alibug”ar ;dag’yminipbarma’ak”mandepajtdy;shog”ar dag’yminip barma’ak”mandepajtdy;noanswer 3 17. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.…….къолусынды. ‘Bolatfellwhileridinghishorse. ___broke(his)arm.’ Responses:munu;onu;onu;onu;onu 18. А. БиркъышгюндеБолатрестаурандануьйгеиссишорпаалмасюеди. Б.Текуьйгегелгенсонг…..субукъэди.

3Someunnaturalnessintheoriginalconstructionofthetestsentencewascorrectedin3versions.

286 ‘OnewinterdayBolatwantedtobringhotsouphomefromarestaurant. Butaftercominghome___wascold.’ Responses:o;sho;sho;ol;o 19. А.Муратоьзюнюмашинибулантюкенгебарматокъташды. Б.Сонго…..дёгерчегибошалгъанныбилди. ‘Muratdecidedtogotothestoreinhisowncar. Laterhefoundoutthat___’stirewasflat.’ Responses: shonu;song onu djegerchegi…;o’ziuniu;shonu;song onu djegerchegi… 20. А.Мариямонуанасынаалмаберди. Б.О…..бектизивтатывубардеди. ‘Mariamgaveanappletohermother. Shesaidthat___wasverydelicious.’(oneresponse:(She)saidthat__wasvery delicious.) Responses:shonu;shonu;shonu;shonu; Onu bektizivtatyvubardedi. Вследующихпарахпредложения,предложениеАпредставляетутверждениеодним человеком,ипредложениеБпредставляетвопросиливосклицаниеотразличного человека.ЗаполнитесьвбланкевопредложенииБсместоимением,если приспособлено.Есликажетсялучшеиметьнеявнуюформу(Ø),отметьтеэто. Inthefollowingpairsofsentences,thesentencein(A)representsanutterancebyonespeaker, andthesentenceinBrepresentsaquestionorexclaimation/statementbyanotherspeaker.Fillin theblankinsentence(B)withapronoun,ifappropriate.Ifitseemsbettertouseanullform(Ø), notethat. Местоимении: Бу, О, Шо, Шу, Оьзю, Ø 21. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.……..гёрдюнгмю? ‘Bolatfellwhileridinghishorse. Didyousee___?’ Responses:shonu;Ø;shonu;shonu;onu

287 22. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.……..нечикболду? ‘Bolatfellwhileridinghishorse. Howdid___happen?’ Responses:o;sho;sho;sho;o 23. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Мен……..инанмайман. ‘Bolatfellwhileridinghishorse. Idon’tbelieve___.’ Responses:shog”ar;shog”ar;shog”ar;shog”ar;shog”ar 24. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.……..болмаярамас! ‘Bolatfellwhileridinghishorse. ___isnotpossible!’ Responses:bu;Ø;Ø;sho;olaĭ 25. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.……..нечикболмабола? ‘Bolatfellwhileridinghishorse. Howdid___happen?’ Responses:shu;olaĭ;sho;bu;o 26. А.Ибрагьимресторанданшорпаалды. Б.……татывлумуэди? ‘Abrahamboughtsoupfromtherestaurant. Was___good?’ Responses:o’ziu;Ø;Ø;sho;o

27. А.Ибрагьимресторанданшорпаалды. Б.Адатлыгьалдаопилавну…..ала.

‘Abrahamboughtsoupfromtherestaurant. Heusuallybuyssoup___.’ Responses:o’ziune;Ø;shondan;shondan;o’ziu 288 28. А.Ибрагьимресторанданшорпаалды. Б.О…..дагъыдаалармыэдиэкен?

‘Abrahamboughtsoupfromtherestaurant. Willhebuy___again?’ Responses:onu;Ø; shonu;shondan;onu III.Следующиепредложениямогутиметьодноилимногократныеместоимения. Заполнитесьвкаждомбланкеместоимением(всоответствующемпадеже)илипустой формой(Ø)иукажите,ккомуиличтообращаетсякаждаяформа. III.Thefollowingsentencesmayhaveoneormorepronouns.Fillineachblankwithapronoun intheappropriatecaseoranullform(Ø)andindicatetowhomorwhattheformrefers.

(Belowthepresentationoftheresponses,IincludethecodingofreferentsIuseinmy analysis.Theresponsestoonequestionnairearenotincludedinthissummarybecause therepsondentfailedtoidentifythereferent.) Местоимении:Бу, О, Шо, Шу, Оьзю, Ø 29.А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.…….……..кёпушатды. 12 1=… 2=… ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. ___waspleasedwith___.’ 12 Responses: 1 2 O(student) shonu(book) O(student) Ø(book) O(teacher) shonu(book) Ol(student) shonu(book) (teacher=FOC;student=ACT;book=ACT)

289 30.А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонг……..……..экзаменибуланкъутлады. 12 1=… 2=… ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. Later___congratulated____with(his/her)exam.’ 1 2 Responses: 1 2 Bu(teacher) onu(student) O’ziu(teacher) onu(student) Ø(teacher) onu(student) Ol(teacher) onu(student) (teacher=FOC;student=ACT) 31.А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.……..……..чалтохуматарыгъынайтды. 12 1=… 2=… ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. ___saidtoread___quickly.’ 1 2 Responses:

1 2 shunu(book) bu(student) o(teacher) Ø(book) O(teacher) shonu(book)

ol(teacher) o’ziune(student) (teacher=FOC;student=ACT;book=ACT) 32.А.МуаллимПатиматгъакитапберди. Б.…….уьйгегелгенсонг…….гьакъындаанасынаайтды. 12 1=… 2=… 290 ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook. After___camehome,(she)told(her)motherabout___.’ 1 2 Responses: 1 2 O(Patimat) munu(teacher) Ol(Patimat) shonu(book) O(Patimat) shonu(book) Ol(Patimat) shonu(book) (teacher=FOC;Patimat=ACT;book=ACT) 33.А.МуаллимПатиматгъакитапберди. Б.…….……..кёпушатды.Сонг,…….уьйгегетди. 123 1=… 2=… 3=… ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook. ___waspleasedwith___.Later,___wenthome.’ 12 3 Responses: 1 2 3 Bu(Patimat) shonu(book) bu(Patimat) Sho(Patimat) Ø(book) ol(Patimat) O(Patimat) shonu(book) Ø(Patimat) Ol(Patimat) shonu(book) ol(Patimat) (Patimat1=1ACT;book=ACT;Patimat 3=FOC) 34.А.МуаллимПатиматгъакитапберди. Б.…….биринчисавгьатболгъансонг,…..………...анасынагёрсетмесюеди. 123 1=… 2=… 3=… ‘TheteachergavePatimatabook. Since___was(her)firstprize,___wantedtoshow___tohermother. 123

291 Responses: 1 2 3 O(book) bu(Patimat) munu(book) Sho(book) Ø(Patimat) onu(book) Bu(book) o(Patimat) shonu(book) Bu(book) ol(Patimat) shonu(book) (book 1=ACT;Patimat=ACT;book 3=FOC) 35.А.Муалимстудентгекитапберди. Б.……..барындатамашаэтди. 1 1=… ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook. ___surprisedeveryone.’ 1 Responses: 1 O’ziu(teacher) Ol(teacher) O(teacher) Bu(book) (teacher=FOC;book=ACT) 36.А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.……..болгъанзатболдуму? 1 1=… ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. Didanythinghappento___?’ 1 Responses: 1 og”ar(Bolat) og”ar(Bolat) og”ar(Bolat) o’ziune(Bolat) (Bolat=FOC) 292 37.А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Биржумагетип,…….…….сатды. 12 1=… 2=… ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. Afteraweek___sold___.’ 1 2 Responses: 1 2 bu(Bolat) shonu(horse) Ø(Bolat) shonu(horse) o(Bolat) onu(horse) ol(Bolat) shonu(horse) (Bolat=FOC;horse=ACT) 38. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.……..саялы,……..……..сатмахыялетди. 123 1=… 2=… 3=… ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. Becauseof___,___sold___.’ 123 Responses: 1 2 3 Sho(falling) o(Bolat) munu(horse) Sho(falling) Ø(Bolat) onu(horse) Sho(falling) o(Bolat) onu(horse)

Sho(falling) ol(Bolat) shonu(horse) (falling(fact)=ACT;Bolat=FOC;horse=ACT) 39.А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.……..сонг…….дагъы……..минипбармагъан. 123 1=… 2=… 3=… 293 ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. After______didnotride___anymore.’ 123 Responses: 1 2 3 Shondan(falling) bu(Bolat) og”ar(horse) Ø o(Bolat) shog”ar(horse) Shondan(falling) o(Bolat) og”ar(horse) Shondan(falling) ol(Bolat) shog”ar(horse) (falling(event)=FOC;Bolat=FOC;horse=ACT) 40.А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.…….къолунсындырды. 1 1=… ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. ___brokehisarm/hand.’ 1 Responses: 1 O’ziu(Bolat) Ol(Bolat) O(Bolat) Ol(Bolat) (Bolat=FOC) 41.А.Агьматгъаргюнсюттюкенгебара.Бугюндесюталмакъучунбарды. Б.Уьйгегелгенсонг,къатыны……..………негеалгъанынсорады.Уьйде экишишабарынайтды.12 1=… 2=… ‘Ahmatgoestothemilkstoreeveryday.(He)wenttodaytobuymilk,aswell.When (he)camehome,(his)wifeasked___whyhebought___.Shesaidthatthereweretwo bottlesathome.’ 12

294 Responses: 1 2 bug”ar(Amat) shonu(milk) Ø(Amat) shonu(milk) og”ar(Amat) shonu(milk) og”ar(Amat) shonu(milk) (Amat=FOC;milk=FOC) IV.Следующиепредложенияимеютодноилимногократныеместоимения.Укажите, ккомуиличтообращаетсяподчёркнутаяформа. IV.Thefollowingsentenceshaveoneormorepronouns.Indicatetowhomorwhatthe underlinedformrefers. 42.Сонгкъатынгишитюкенгегире.Тюкенчибулансёйлей;огъараргъанын сата.Сатып,акъчаныдаалып,огъар къарамады. 1 ‘Laterthewomangoesintothestore.(She)talkswiththestorekeeper;(she)sellshimthe accordian.Havingsoldit,takingthemoney,shedidnotlookatit/him.’ 1=… Responses:storeperson,storeperson,storeperson,storeperson,woman

43.А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Шогъар болгъанзатболдуму? 1 ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Didanythinghappentohim/it?’ 1=…….. Responses:horse,Bolat,Bolat,horse,Bolat 44.Уьюнегирипкъараса:ондакёпуллукъалмагъалбар.Гьартюрлюашлар ташлангъан,бузулгъан.Оланы оьзюнюанасыэтген. 1 ‘Comingintothehomeif(he/she)looks,thereisabigmessthere.Variousfoodsare throwndown,ruined.His/herownotherdid/madethem/thesethings.’ 1=……. Responses:food,food,food,theruiningofthefood–themess,food

295 45.А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Огъар болгъанзатболдуму? 1 ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse.Didanythinghappentohim/it?’

1=…….. Responses:Bolat,Bolat,Bolat,Bolat,Bolat 46.Муалимстудентгекитапберди.Обарындатамашаэтди. 1 ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.(S)he/that/itsurprisedeveryone.’’ 1=…….. Responses:teacher,teacher,teacher,student,teacher 47. Ансаранасынаалмаберди.Шо рагьмулуэди. 1 ‘Ansargavehismotheranapple.That/hewassweet.’ 1=…….. Responses:givinghismothertheapple, ish (‘work’)/givingtheapple,givingtheapple, givingtheapple,Ansar 48.Сонгкъатынгишитюкенгегире.Тюкенчибулансёйлей;огъараргъанынсата.Сатып, акъчаныдаалып,шогъар къарамады. 1 ‘Laterthewomangoesintothestore.(She)talkswiththestorekeeper;(she)sellshimthe accordian.Havingsoldit,takingthemoney,shedidnotlookatit/him.’ 1=… Responses:organ/accordian,storeperson,money;organ,storeperson 49.Уьюнегирипкъараса:ондакёпуллукъалмагъалбар.Гьартюрлюашлар ташлангъан,бузулгъан.Шоланы оьзюнюанасыэтген. 1 ‘Comingintothehomeif(he/she)looks,thereisabigmessthere.Variousfoodsare throwndown,ruined.His/herownotherdid/madethem/thesethings.’ 1=…….

Responses:food,food,food,theruiningofthefood–themess,food

296 50.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди.Шо барындатамашаэтди. 1 ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook.(S)he/that/itsurprisedeveryone.’ 1=…….. Responses:book,teacher,givingthebook,book,teacher 51.Ансаранасынаалмаберди.Орагьмулуэди. 1 ‘Ansargavehismotheranapple.That/hewassweet.’ 1=…….. Responses:Ansar,Ansar,Ansar,Ansar,Ø(notsurewhatthismeans),Ansar

V.Оценитеследующиепредложениядлятого,какхорошоонизвучат.Используйте следующиймасштаб: 1великолепнозвучит 2хорошозвука 3возможный,нообычнонеговорилбытак 4никакихКумыковнесказалибытак Ratethefollowingsentencesaccordingtohowwelltheysound.Usethefollowingscale: 1 Soundsgreat 2 Soundsgood 3 Possible,butpeopledon’tusuallysayitthatway 4 NoKumykpersonwouldsayitthatway

(Thereareanumberofsetsofsentencepairswhichareidenticalexceptfortheformofpronoun usedinthesecondsentence.ThelistofsentencesinKumykareshownhereforreference,butthe summaryofresponsesisgivenwiththeEnglishtranslationswhichfollow.) VersionA: 1. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Бу„Тангчолпан”эди. 1 2 3 4 2. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Шусавболдеди. 1 2 3 4 3. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Онукъолусынды. 1 2 3 4

297 4. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Шондансонгмуаллимстудентникъолуналды. 1 2 3 4 5. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Барындатамашаэтди. 1 2 3 4 6. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонгмуаллимоьзюнюэкзаменибуланкъутлады. 1 2 3 4 7. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Болатдагъыбугъарминмежегинайтды. 1 2 3 4 8. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Оьзюсавболдеди. 1 2 3 4

9. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Будагъыогъарминмежегинайтды. 1 2 3 4 10. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.„Тангчолпан”эди. 1 2 3 4 11. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Шотюнегюнболду. 1 2 3 4 12. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонгмуаллиммунуэкзаменибуланкъутлады. 1 2 3 4 13. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Къолусынды. 1 2 3 4 14. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Бусавболдеди. 1 2 3 4 15. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Шундансонгмуаллимстудентникъолуналды. 1 2 3 4 16. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Бубарындатамашаэтди. 1 2 3 4 17. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонгмуаллимэкзаменибуланкъутлады. 1 2 3 4 18. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Савболдеди. 1 2 3 4

298 19. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Болатдагъыогъарминмежегинайтды. 1 2 3 4 20. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.О„Тангчолпан”эди. 1 2 3 4 21. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонгмуаллимонуэкзаменибуланкъутлады. 1 2 3 4 22. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Обарындатамашаэтди. 1 2 3 4 23. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонгмуаллимстудентникъолуналды. 1 2 3 4 24. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Бутюнегюнболду. 1 2 3 4 25. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Шу„Тангчолпан”эди. 1 2 3 4 26. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Шобарындатамашаэтди. 1 2 3 4 27. А.Муллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонгмуаллимшонуэкзаменибуланкъутлады. 1 2 3 4 28. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Осавболдеди. 1 2 3 4 29. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Ондансонгмуаллимстудентникъолуналды. 1 2 3 4 30. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Шубарындатамашаэтди. 1 2 3 4 31. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Тюнегюнболду. 1 2 3 4 32. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Оьзюнюкъолусынды. 1 2 3 4 33. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Шо„Тангчолпан”эди. 1 2 3 4

299 34. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Одагъыогъарминмежегинайтды. 1 2 3 4 35. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Сонгмуаллимшунуэкзаменибуланкъутлады. 1 2 3 4 36. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди Б.Мундансонгмуаллимстудентникъолуналды. 1 2 3 4 37. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Одагъышогъарминмежегинайтды. 1 2 3 4 38. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Отюнегюнболду. 1 2 3 4 39. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Шосавболдеди. 1 2 3 4 40. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Болатдагъышогъарминмежегинайтды. 1 2 3 4 41. А.Болататынаминипбарагъандайыгъылды. Б.Отюнегюнболду. 1 2 3 4 SentencesfromVersionBthatdonotappearinA: 42. А.Муаллимстудентгекитапберди. Б.Студент,онуалып,уьйгегетди. 12 3 4

43. А.Муратоьзюнюмашинибулантюкенгебарматокъташды. Б.Сонгошонудёгерчегибошалгъанныбилди. 12 3 4 44. А.Марияманасынаалмаберди. Б.Обугъарразилигинбилдирди. 12 3 4

300 CombinedresultsforVersionsAandB (Bresultsdon’tshoworangeresponses(),whichwereeliminatedbecauseallbutoneresponse wasjudgedacceptable.) 1. Theteachergaveabooktothestudent. __wasTangcholpan.(cf.1,10,20,25,33) Responses: bu(3,,1;3,2) Ø(4,3).[onlyversionA] o(4,2).[onlyversionA] shu(2,,3;4,3) sho(1,,1;1,2)

2. Theteachergaveabooktothestudent. __saidthankyou.(cf.2,8,14,18,28,39) Responses: shu(4,,4;4,2) o’z(3,,4;4,3) bu(2,,2;2,2) Ø(4,,4;3,4) o(2,2)[onlyversionA] sho(3,,4;4,2) 3. Theteachergaveabooktothestudent. __Thentheteachercongratulated(him)on(his)exam.(cf.6,12,17,21,27,35) Responses: o’z(4,4) bu(1,,2;3,2) Ø(3,,3;4,3) o(1,2)[onlyversionA] sho(3,,4;4,3) shu(3,,4;4,3) 4. Theteachergaveabooktothestudent. After__theteachershookthestudent’shand.(cf.4,15,23,29,36) Responses: sho(1,,1;1,2) shu(3,,3;2,2) Ø(1,,1;1,3) o(1,,1;4,2) bu(2,,4;4,3) 301 5. Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. __handbroke.(cf..3,13,32) Responses: onu(1,,1;1,2) Ø(2,,3;3,2) o’z(1,,3;4,2) 6. Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. Bolat saidhe(Ø)wouldn’tride__anymore.(cf.7,19,40) Responses: bu(2,,2;3,2) o(1,,2;4,2) sho(1,,2;1,2) 6. Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. He (bu)saidhe(Ø)wouldn’tride__anymore.(cf.9) Responses: o(1,,2;3,2) 6. Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. He (o)saidhe(Ø)wouldn’tride__anymore.(cf.34,37) Responses: o(4,2)[onlyversionA] sho(1,,1;1,2) 7. Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. __wasyesterday.(cf.11,24,31,38) Responses: sho(2,,1;1,2) bu(2,,3;3,2) Ø(3,3)[onlyversionA] o(4,2)[onlyversionA] 8. ‘Bolatfelloffwhileridinghishorse. __amazedeveryone.’(cf.5,16,22,26,30)

302 Responses: Ø(2,,3;2,3) bu(2,,2;2,4) o(1,,1;1,3) sho(2,,3;1,3) shu(2,,3;1,3) 9. ‘Theteachergavethestudentabook Thestudent,taking__wenthome.’ (cf.42) Responses: o(1,,1)[onlyversionB] sho(1,,2)[onlyversionB] bu(2,,2)[onlyversionB] shu(2,,4)[onlyversionB] 10. ‘Muratdecidedtogotothestorewithhis(own)car. Thenhe(o)foundoutthat__’stirewasflat.’(cf.43) Responses: sho(2,,3)[onlyversionB] o(2,,3)[onlyversionB] bu(1,,3)[onlyversionB] 11. Mariamgaveanappleto(her)mother. Shemadeknownherpleasure(at/to___).(cf.44) Responses: sho(3,,2)[onlyversionB] o(2,,2)[onlyversionB] bu(1,,2)[onlyversionB] Датаанкетногоопроса: Dateofquestionnaire Возраст: Age Месторождения: Placeofbirth Местажительства: Placeofresidence Red–28yrs.born/residesinBuinaskregion Blue19yrs.born/residesinMakhachkala Orange20yrs.born/residesinBabayurtregion Black–19yrs.born/residesinMakhachkala Green–64yrs.born/residesinBuinaskregion

303