Weak Semantic Context Helps Phonetic Learning in a Model of Infant Language Acquisition

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Weak Semantic Context Helps Phonetic Learning in a Model of Infant Language Acquisition Weak semantic context helps phonetic learning in a model of infant language acquisition Stella Frank Naomi H. Feldman Sharon Goldwater [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] ILCC, School of Informatics Department of Linguistics ILCC, School of Informatics University of Edinburgh University of Maryland University of Edinburgh Edinburgh, EH8 9AB, UK College Park, MD, 20742, USA Edinburgh, EH8 9AB, UK Abstract Feldman et al., 2013a; McMurray et al., 2009; Val- labha et al., 2007). Learning phonetic categories is one of the Models without any semantic information are first steps to learning a language, yet is hard likely to underestimate infants’ ability to learn pho- to do using only distributional phonetic in- netic categories. Infants learn language in the wild, formation. Semantics could potentially be and quickly attune to the fact that words have (pos- useful, since words with different mean- sibly unknown) meanings. The extent of infants’ ings have distinct phonetics, but it is un- semantic knowledge is not yet known, but existing clear how many word meanings are known evidence shows that six-month-olds can associate to infants learning phonetic categories. We some words with their referents (Bergelson and show that attending to a weaker source of Swingley, 2012; Tincoff and Jusczyk, 1999, 2012), semantics, in the form of a distribution over leverage non-acoustic contexts such as objects or ar- topics in the current context, can lead to ticulations to distinguish similar sounds (Teinonen improvements in phonetic category learn- et al., 2008; Yeung and Werker, 2009), and map ing. In our model, an extension of a pre- meaning (in the form of objects or images) to new vious model of joint word-form and pho- word-forms in some laboratory settings (Friedrich netic category inference, the probability of and Friederici, 2011; Gogate and Bahrick, 2001; word-forms is topic-dependent, enabling Shukla et al., 2011). These findings indicate that the model to find significantly better pho- young infants are sensitive to co-occurrences be- netic vowel categories and word-forms than tween linguistic stimuli and at least some aspects a model with no semantic knowledge. of the world. In this paper we explore the potential contribu- 1 Introduction tion of semantic information to phonetic learning Infants begin learning the phonetic categories of by formalizing a model in which learners attend to their native language in their first year (Kuhl et al., the word-level context in which phones appear (as 1992; Polka and Werker, 1994; Werker and Tees, in the lexical-phonetic learning model of Feldman 1984). In theory, semantic information could offer et al., 2013a) and also to the situations in which a valuable cue for phoneme induction1 by helping word-forms are used. The modeled situations con- infants distinguish between minimal pairs, as lin- sist of combinations of categories of salient ac- guists do (Trubetzkoy, 1939). However, due to a tivities or objects, similar to the activity contexts widespread assumption that infants do not know the explored by Roy et al. (2012), e.g.,‘getting dressed’ meanings of many words at the age when they are or ‘eating breakfast’. We assume that child learn- learning phonetic categories (see Swingley, 2009 ers are able to infer a representation of the situ- for a review), most recent models of early phonetic ational context from their non-linguistic environ- category acquisition have explored the phonetic ment. However, in our simulations we approximate learning problem in the absence of semantic infor- the environmental information by running a topic mation (de Boer and Kuhl, 2003; Dillon et al., 2013; model (Blei et al., 2003) over a corpus of child- directed speech to infer a topic distribution for each 1The models in this paper do not distinguish between pho- situation. These topic distributions are then used as netic and phonemic categories, since they do not capture input to our model to represent situational contexts. phonological processes (and there are also none present in our synthetic data). We thus use the terms interchangeably. The situational information in our model is simi- 1073 Proceedings of the 52nd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, pages 1073–1083, Baltimore, Maryland, USA, June 23-25 2014. c 2014 Association for Computational Linguistics lar to that assumed by theories of cross-situational word learning (Frank et al., 2009; Smith and Yu, 200 2008; Yu and Smith, 2007), but our model does not 400 iy uw require learners to map individual words to their ref- ih oo ei er oa erents. Even in the absence of word-meaning map- 600 ae pings, situational information is potentially useful eh uh aw because similar-sounding words uttered in similar F1 800 situations are more likely to be tokens of the same ah lexeme (containing the same phones) than similar- 1000 sounding words uttered in different situations. In simulations of vowel learning, inspired by 1200 Vallabha et al. (2007) and Feldman et al. (2013a), 3500 3000 2500 2000 1500 1000 500 F2 we show a clear improvement over previous mod- els in both phonetic and lexical (word-form) cate- Figure 1: The English vowel space (generated from gorization when situational context is used as an Hillenbrand et al. (1995), see Section 6.2), plotted additional source of information. This improve- using the first two formants. ment is especially noticeable when the word-level context is providing less information, arguably the more realistic setting. These results demonstrate improved phonetic learning. that relying on situational co-occurrence can im- Our own Topic-Lexical-Distributional (TLD) prove phonetic learning, even if learners do not yet model extends the LD model to include an addi- know the meanings of individual words. tional type of context: the situations in which words appear. To motivate this extension and clarify the 2 Background and overview of models differences between the models, we now provide a high-level overview of both models; details are Infants attend to distributional characteristics of given in Sections 3 and 4. their input (Maye et al., 2002, 2008), leading to the hypothesis that phonetic categories could be acquired on the basis of bottom-up distributional 2.1 Overview of LD model learning alone (de Boer and Kuhl, 2003; Vallabha Both the LD and TLD models are computational- et al., 2007; McMurray et al., 2009). However, this level models of phonetic (specifically, vowel) cat- would require sound categories to be well sepa- egorization where phones (vowels) are presented rated, which often is not the case—for example, to the model in the context of words.2 The task is see Figure 1, which shows the English vowel space to infer a set of phonetic categories and a set of that is the focus of this paper. lexical items on the basis of the data observed for Recent work has investigated whether infants each word token xi. In the original LD model, the could overcome such distributional ambiguity by observations for token xi are its frame fi, which incorporating top-down information, in particular, consists of a list of consonants and slots for vowels, the fact that phones appear within words. At six and the list of vowel tokens wi. (The TLD model months, infants begin to recognize word-forms includes additional observations, described below.) such as their name and other frequently occurring A single vowel token, wij, is a two dimensional words (Mandel et al., 1995; Jusczyk and Hohne, vector representing the first two formants (peaks 1997), without necessarily linking a meaning to in the frequency spectrum, ordered from lowest to these forms. This “protolexicon” can help differen- highest). For example, a token of the word kitty tiate phonetic categories by adding word contexts would have the frame fi = k t , containing two in which certain sound categories appear (Swingley, consonant phones, /k/ and /t/, with two vowel phone 2009; Feldman et al., 2013b). To explore this idea slots in between, and two vowel formant vectors, further, Feldman et al. (2013a) implemented the Lexical-Distributional (LD) model, which jointly 2For a related model that also tackles the word segmenta- learns a set of phonetic vowel categories and a set tion problem, see Elsner et al. (2013). In a model of phono- logical learning, Fourtassi and Dupoux (submitted) show that of word-forms containing those categories. Simula- semantic context information similar to that used here remains tions showed that the use of lexical context greatly useful despite segmentation errors. 1074 3 wi0 = [464, 2294] and wi1 = [412, 2760]. 2.2 Overview of TLD model Given the data, the model must assign each To demonstrate the benefit of situational informa- vowel token to a vowel category, wij = c. Both tion, we develop the Topic-Lexical-Distributional the LD and the TLD models do this using inter- (TLD) model, which extends the LD model by as- mediate lexemes, `, which contain vowel category suming that words appear in situations analogous assignments, v`j = c, as well as a frame f`. If a to documents in a topic model. Each situation h word token is assigned to a lexeme, xi = `, the is associated with a mixture of topics θh, which is vowels within the word are assigned to that lex- assumed to be observed. Thus, for the ith token in 4 eme’s vowel categories, wij = v = c. The word `j situation h, denoted xhi, the observed data will be and lexeme frames must match, fi = f . ` its frame fhi, vowels whi, and topic vector θh. Lexical information helps with phonetic catego- From an acquisition perspective, the observed rization because it can disambiguate highly over- topic distribution represents the child’s knowledge lapping categories, such as the ae and eh categories of the context of the interaction: she can distin- in Figure 1.
Recommended publications
  • Grapheme-To-Lexeme Feedback in the Spelling System: Evidence from a Dysgraphic Patient
    COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2006, 23 (2), 278–307 Grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the spelling system: Evidence from a dysgraphic patient Michael McCloskey Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, USA Paul Macaruso Community College of Rhode Island, Warwick, and Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, USA Brenda Rapp Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, USA This article presents an argument for grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the cognitive spelling system, based on the impaired spelling performance of dysgraphic patient CM. The argument relates two features of CM’s spelling. First, letters from prior spelling responses intrude into sub- sequent responses at rates far greater than expected by chance. This letter persistence effect arises at a level of abstract grapheme representations, and apparently results from abnormal persistence of activation. Second, CM makes many formal lexical errors (e.g., carpet ! compute). Analyses revealed that a large proportion of these errors are “true” lexical errors originating in lexical selec- tion, rather than “chance” lexical errors that happen by chance to take the form of words. Additional analyses demonstrated that CM’s true lexical errors exhibit the letter persistence effect. We argue that this finding can be understood only within a functional architecture in which activation from the grapheme level feeds back to the lexeme level, thereby influencing lexical selection. INTRODUCTION a brain-damaged patient with an acquired spelling deficit, arguing from his error pattern that Like other forms of language processing, written the cognitive system for written word produc- word production implicates multiple levels of tion includes feedback connections from gra- representation, including semantic, orthographic pheme representations to orthographic lexeme lexeme, grapheme, and allograph levels.
    [Show full text]
  • ON SOME CATEGORIES for DESCRIBING the SEMOLEXEMIC STRUCTURE by Yoshihiko Ikegami
    ON SOME CATEGORIES FOR DESCRIBING THE SEMOLEXEMIC STRUCTURE by Yoshihiko Ikegami 1. A lexeme is the minimum unit that carries meaning. Thus a lexeme can be a "word" as well as an affix (i.e., something smaller than a word) or an idiom (i.e,, something larger than a word). 2. A sememe is a unit of meaning that can be realized as a single lexeme. It is defined as a structure constituted by those features having distinctive functions (i.e., serving to distinguish the sememe in question from other semernes that contrast with it).' A question that arises at this point is whether or not one lexeme always corresponds to just one serneme and no more. Three theoretical positions are foreseeable: (I) one which holds that one lexeme always corresponds to just one sememe and no more, (2) one which holds that one lexeme corresponds to an indefinitely large number of sememes, and (3) one which holds that one lexeme corresponds to a certain limited number of sememes. These three positions wiIl be referred to as (1) the "Grundbedeutung" theory, (2) the "use" theory, and (3) the "polysemy" theory, respectively. The Grundbedeutung theory, however attractive in itself, is to be rejected as unrealistic. Suppose a preliminary analysis has revealed that a lexeme seems to be used sometimes in an "abstract" sense and sometimes in a "concrete" sense. In order to posit a Grundbedeutung under such circumstances, it is to be assumed that there is a still higher level at which "abstract" and "concrete" are neutralized-this is certainly a theoretical possibility, but it seems highly unlikely and unrealistic from a psychological point of view.
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • Unit 2 Structures Handout.Pdf
    2. The definition of a language as a structure of structures 2.1. Phonetics and phonology Relevance for studying language in its natural or primary medium: oral sounds rather than written symbols. Phonic medium: the range of sounds produced by the speech organs insofar as the play a role in language Speech sounds: Individual sounds within that range Phonetics is the study of the phonic medium: The study of the production, transmission, and reception of human sound-making used in speech. e.g. classification of sounds as voiced vs voiceless: /b/ vs /p/ Phonology is the study of the phonic medium not in itself but in relation with language. e.g. application of voice to the explanation of differences within the system of language: housen vs housev usen vs usev 2.1.1. Phonetics It is usually divided into three branches which study the phonic medium from three points of view: Articulatory phonetics: speech sounds according to the way in which they are produced by the speech organs. Acoustic phonetics: speech sounds according to the physical properties of their sound-waves. Auditory phonetics: speech sounds according to their perception and identification. Articulatory phonetics has the longest tradition, and its progress in the 19th century contributed a standardize and internationally accepted system of phonetic transcription: the origins of the International Phonetic Alphabet used today and relying on sound symbols and diacritics. It studies production in relation with the vocal tract, i.e., organs such as: lungs trachea or windpipe, containing: larynx vocal folds glottis pharyngeal cavity nose mouth, containing fixed organs: teeth and teeth ridge hard palate pharyngeal wall mobile organs: lips tongue soft palate jaw According to their function and participation, sounds may take several features: Voice: voiced vs voiceless sounds, according to the participation of the vocal folds e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • From Phoneme to Morpheme Author(S): Zellig S
    Linguistic Society of America From Phoneme to Morpheme Author(s): Zellig S. Harris Source: Language, Vol. 31, No. 2 (Apr. - Jun., 1955), pp. 190-222 Published by: Linguistic Society of America Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/411036 Accessed: 09/02/2009 08:03 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=lsa. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Linguistic Society of America is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Language. http://www.jstor.org FROM PHONEME TO MORPHEME ZELLIG S. HARRIS University of Pennsylvania 0.1.
    [Show full text]
  • Phones and Phonemes
    NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) © P. Coxhead, 2006 Page 1 Natural Language Processing & Applications Phones and Phonemes 1 Phonemes If we are to understand how speech might be generated or recognized by a computer, we need to study some of the underlying linguistic theory. The aim here is to UNDERSTAND the theory rather than memorize it. I’ve tried to reduce and simplify as much as possible without serious inaccuracy. Speech consists of sequences of sounds. The use of an instrument (such as a speech spectro- graph) shows that most of normal speech consists of continuous sounds, both within words and across word boundaries. Speakers of a language can easily dissect its continuous sounds into words. With more difficulty, they can split words into component sounds, or ‘segments’. However, it is not always clear where to stop splitting. In the word strip, for example, should the sound represented by the letters str be treated as a unit, be split into the two sounds represented by st and r, or be split into the three sounds represented by s, t and r? One approach to isolating component sounds is to look for ‘distinctive unit sounds’ or phonemes.1 For example, three phonemes can be distinguished in the word cat, corresponding to the letters c, a and t (but of course English spelling is notoriously non- phonemic so correspondence of phonemes and letters should not be expected). How do we know that these three are ‘distinctive unit sounds’ or phonemes of the English language? NOT from the sounds themselves. A speech spectrograph will not show a neat division of the sound of the word cat into three parts.
    [Show full text]
  • Grapheme-To-Phoneme Models for (Almost) Any Language
    Grapheme-to-Phoneme Models for (Almost) Any Language Aliya Deri and Kevin Knight Information Sciences Institute Department of Computer Science University of Southern California {aderi, knight}@isi.edu Abstract lang word pronunciation eng anybody e̞ n iː b ɒ d iː Grapheme-to-phoneme (g2p) models are pol żołądka z̻owon̪t̪ka rarely available in low-resource languages, ben শ嗍 s̪ ɔ k t̪ ɔ as the creation of training and evaluation ʁ a l o m o t חלומות heb data is expensive and time-consuming. We use Wiktionary to obtain more than 650k Table 1: Examples of English, Polish, Bengali, word-pronunciation pairs in more than 500 and Hebrew pronunciation dictionary entries, with languages. We then develop phoneme and pronunciations represented with the International language distance metrics based on phono- Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). logical and linguistic knowledge; apply- ing those, we adapt g2p models for high- word eng deu nld resource languages to create models for gift ɡ ɪ f tʰ ɡ ɪ f t ɣ ɪ f t related low-resource languages. We pro- class kʰ l æ s k l aː s k l ɑ s vide results for models for 229 adapted lan- send s e̞ n d z ɛ n t s ɛ n t guages. Table 2: Example pronunciations of English words 1 Introduction using English, German, and Dutch g2p models. Grapheme-to-phoneme (g2p) models convert words into pronunciations, and are ubiquitous in For most of the world’s more than 7,100 lan- speech- and text-processing systems. Due to the guages (Lewis et al., 2009), no data exists and the diversity of scripts, phoneme inventories, phono- many technologies enabled by g2p models are in- tactic constraints, and spelling conventions among accessible.
    [Show full text]
  • The 44* Phonemes Following Is a List of the 44 Phonemes Along with The
    The 44* Phonemes Following is a list of the 44 phonemes along with the letters of groups of letters that represent those sounds. Phoneme Graphemes** Examples (speech sound) (letters or groups of letters representing the most common spellings for the individual phonemes) Consonant Sounds: 1. /b/ b, bb big, rubber 2. /d/ d, dd, ed dog, add, filled 3. /f/ f, ph fish, phone 4. /g/ g, gg go, egg 5. /h/ h hot 6. /j/ j, g, ge, dge jet, cage, barge, judge 7. /k/ c, k, ck, ch, cc, que cat, kitten, duck, school, occur, antique, cheque 8. /l/ l, ll leg, bell 9. /m/ m, mm, mb mad, hammer, lamb 10. /n/ n, nn, kn, gn no, dinner, knee, gnome 11. /p/ p, pp pie, apple 12. /r/ r, rr, wr run, marry, write 13. /s/ s, se, ss, c, ce, sc sun, mouse, dress, city, ice, science 14. /t/ t, tt, ed top, letter, stopped 15. /v/ v, ve vet, give 16. /w/ w wet, win, swim 17 /y/ y, i yes, onion 18. /z/ z, zz, ze, s, se, x zip, fizz, sneeze, laser, is, was, please, Xerox, xylophone Source: Orchestrating Success in Reading by Dawn Reithaug (2002) Phoneme Graphemes** Examples (speech sound) (letters or groups of letters representing the most common spellings for the individual phonemes) Consonant Digraphs: 19. /th/ th thumb, thin, thing (not voiced) 20. /th/ th this, feather, then (voiced) 21. /ng/ ng, n sing, monkey, sink 22. /sh/ sh, ss, ch, ti, ci ship, mission, chef, motion, special 23. /ch/ ch, tch chip, match 24.
    [Show full text]
  • Summary of Cruse
    English D Lexical Semantics Course Mitthögskolan HT 2002 Morgan Lundberg Notes on Meaning in Language by D. Alan Cruse Meaning in Language by D.A. Cruse Chapter 5 Introduction to lexical semantics The nature of word meaning [Recap: Phoneme: minimal meaning distinguishing element: /d/, /f/, /u:/. Morpheme: minimal meaning bearing element: dis-, zebra, -tion] The (prototypical) word – ‘a minimal permutable (‘utbytbart’) element’ · Can be moved about in a sentence (or rather, position altered): Am I mad?/I am mad. · Can’t be - broken up by other elements [exception: ‘infixes’ – unusual in Eng.] - have its parts reordered: needlessly – *lylessneed Internal structure: root morpheme [+ derivational and/or inflectional morphemes (affixes)]. Grammatical words are said to have no root morpheme: on, a, or, the Note on compounds : polar bear, bluebird, sea horse – look like two morphemes, but are (semantically) best considered one unit – one root morpheme. Ex: Polar bear – ?This bear is polar. Bluebird – ?a/*any bird which is blue Sea horse - *a horse (that lives/swims/grazes) in/by the sea Cf. ordinary nominal modification: red house – the house is red Prosody is indicative (except in very long words): ‘sea ,lion (compound) vs. ‘big ‘house (common noun phrase). What is a word in lexical semantics? · Word form – phonologically (and/or graphically) distinct “shape”: bank, mole · Lexeme – phonologically and semantically distinct, autonomous symbolic unit (i.e., it can stand alone because it contains a root morpheme): bank1, bank 2, mole1, swimming, etc. - Inflected forms based on the same root count as the same lexeme: swims, swimming, swam, swum. · In lexical semantics, a word is the same as a lexeme (=lexical unit).
    [Show full text]
  • Phonemes Visit the Dyslexia Reading Well
    For More Information on Phonemes Visit the Dyslexia Reading Well. www.dyslexia-reading-well.com The 44 Sounds (Phonemes) of English A phoneme is a speech sound. It’s the smallest unit of sound that distinguishes one word from another. Since sounds cannot be written, we use letters to represent or stand for the sounds. A grapheme is the written representation (a letter or cluster of letters) of one sound. It is generally agreed that there are approximately 44 sounds in English, with some variation dependent on accent and articulation. The 44 English phonemes are represented by the 26 letters of the alphabet individually and in combination. Phonics instruction involves teaching the relationship between sounds and the letters used to represent them. There are hundreds of spelling alternatives that can be used to represent the 44 English phonemes. Only the most common sound / letter relationships need to be taught explicitly. The 44 English sounds can be divided into two major categories – consonants and vowels. A consonant sound is one in which the air flow is cut off, either partially or completely, when the sound is produced. In contrast, a vowel sound is one in which the air flow is unobstructed when the sound is made. The vowel sounds are the music, or movement, of our language. The 44 phonemes represented below are in line with the International Phonetic Alphabet. Consonants Sound Common Spelling alternatives spelling /b/ b bb ball ribbon /d/ d dd ed dog add filled /f/ f ff ph gh lf ft fan cliff phone laugh calf often /g/ g gg gh gu
    [Show full text]
  • Phonemic Awareness: Phoneme Segmentation
    Phonemic Awareness: Phoneme Segmentation College- and Career-Ready Standard Addressed: Demonstrate understanding of spoken words, syllables, and sounds (phonemes). Blend and segment onsets and rimes of single-syllable spoken words Objective: Students will learn to segment words into individual phonemes (sounds). Materials OPTIONAL: Manipulatives, such as blocks, magnetic letters, Elkonin boxes (see page 4), practice word lists (see supplemental materials). Suggested Schedule and Group Size Schedule: Daily, no more than five to ten minutes per session Recommended group size: Individual or small group (up to five students) Note: The following script is intended as a model. Start with words with two or three phonemes. Adjust the difficulty of the words and increase independent practice opportunities as students become more proficient. Activity Intervention Principle Sample Script and Procedures Use explicit instruction, Today, we are going to practice saying the sounds we hear in including modeling and joint words. practice opportunities. Listen and watch. I’ll say the sounds in the word “pat.” (Put up one finger for each sound, or if using Elkonin boxes, point to each box or move an object into each box as you say the sounds. /p/ /aaaa/ /t/) How many fingers (boxes) did I put up (touch)? (Students should say “three.”) Right, that means “pat” has three sounds. Now let’s try it together. Say the sounds in “pat” with me. (Put up one finger for each sound, or, if using Elkonin boxes, point to each box or move an object into each box as you say the sounds. Watch to make sure the student(s) follow along with you: /p/ /aaaa/ /t/.) Adapted with permission from Phonemic awareness instructional routine: Segmenting, Kindergarten level.
    [Show full text]
  • 10. Semic Analysis Summary
    Louis Hébert, Tools for Text and Image Analysis: An Introduction to Applied Semiotics 10. SEMIC ANALYSIS SUMMARY Semic analysis was developed within the field of semantics (the study of meaning in linguistic units). This chapter presents Rastier's interpretive semantics, and semic analysis as formulated therein. Semic analysis is performed on a semiotic act – a text, for example – by identifying the semes, that is, the elements of meaning (parts of the signified), defining clusters of semes (isotopies and molecules) and determining the relations between the clusters (relations of presupposition, comparison, etc., between the isotopies). The repetition of a seme creates an isotopy. For example, in "There was a fine ship, carved from solid gold / With azure reaching masts, on seas unknown" (Émile Nelligan, "The Golden Ship"), the words "ship", "masts" and "seas" all contain the seme /navigation/ (as well as others). The repetition of this seme creates the isotopy /navigation/. A semic molecule is a cluster of at least two semes appearing together more than once in a single semantic unit (a single word, for instance). For instance, in the poem just quoted, there is a semic molecule formed by the semes /precious/ + /dispersion/. It appears in the "streaming hair" ("cheveux épars") of the beautiful Goddess of Love (Venus) who was "spread-eagled" ("s'étalait") at the ship's prow, and also in the sinking of the ship's treasure, which the sailors "disputed amongst themselves" ("entre eux ont disputés"). 1. THEORY Semic analysis is performed on a semiotic act – such as a text – by identifying the semes (the elements of meaning), finding clusters of semes (isotopies and molecules) and determining the relations between the clusters (relations of presupposition, comparison, etc., between the isotopies).
    [Show full text]