<<

NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) © . Coxhead, 2006 Page 1

Natural Processing & Applications Phones and

1 Phonemes If we are to understand how might be generated or recognized by a computer, we need to study some of the underlying linguistic theory. The aim here is to UNDERSTAND the theory rather than memorize it. I’ve tried to reduce and simplify as much as possible without serious inaccuracy. Speech consists of sequences of sounds. The use of an instrument (such as a speech spectro- graph) shows that most of normal speech consists of continuous sounds, both within and across boundaries. Speakers of a language can easily dissect its continuous sounds into words. With more difficulty, they can split words into component sounds, or ‘segments’. However, it is not always clear where to stop splitting. In the word strip, for example, should the sound represented by the letters str be treated as a unit, be split into the two sounds represented by st and r, or be split into the three sounds represented by s, t and r? One approach to isolating component sounds is to look for ‘distinctive unit sounds’ or phonemes.1 For example, three phonemes can be distinguished in the word cat, corresponding to the letters c, a and t (but of course English is notoriously non- phonemic so correspondence of phonemes and letters should not be expected). How do we know that these three are ‘distinctive unit sounds’ or phonemes of the ? NOT from the sounds themselves. A speech spectrograph will not show a neat division of the sound of the word cat into three parts. Rather we know these are phonemes because BOTH of the following are true: • The three are ‘unit’ sounds. A different English word cannot be formed by replacing part of the c sound and part of the a sound by a different sound. The whole of a must be replaced to make a valid English word. Thus the c sound in cat is a ‘unit’ sound because it can be removed entirely to change cat into at, or replaced entirely by a sound to change cat into bat. • The three are ‘distinctive’ sounds. Changing a single phoneme in cat is sufficient to make a word which is recognizably different to a speaker of English. The words bat, kit and cad are each minimally different from the word cat but are recognizably different words to an English speaker. In summary, a phoneme is defined as a ‘distinctive unit sound’ of a language: ‘unit’ because the whole of a phoneme must be substituted to make a different word; ‘distinctive’ because changing a single phoneme can generate a word which is recognizably different to a speaker of the language. Note that ‘phoneme’ is a subjective concept, not an objective one. To test whether a partic- ular sound operates as a phoneme in a language we cannot use instruments such as speech spectrographs. Rather we have to ask a speaker of the language whether removing that sound from a word and substituting another (preferably one already known to be a phoneme of the language) generates a new word (or what could be a new word if the new sound sequence doesn’t exist in the language). Since English spelling in particular is non-phonemic, we need some way of consistently representing phonemes. I will use IPA (International Phonetic ) where appropriate. You are NOT expected to learn these; a table (see Appendix) will be given if and when required. By convention, phonemic representations of sounds are enclosed in slashes. Thus the English words discussed earlier, cat, bat, kit and cad, can be represented phonemically as /kæt/, /bæt/, /kt/ and /kæd/. Comparing ‘minimal pairs’ confirms that /k/, /b/, /æ/, //, /t/ and /d/ are indeed English phonemes; e.g. /æ/ is a phoneme because in the word cat it can be substituted by // to make the word kit. (Note that these six might or might

1 I’ve noticed that a common mistake in reproducing this definition in examinations is to replace distinctive by distinct. Don’t! Distinctive here refers to the ability of a phoneme to make distinguish between words; distinct would just mean that the phonemes were different, which isn’t the same. Page 2 NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) not be phonemes in another language.) It’s important to note what I have NOT said. I have not said that a phoneme corresponds to a specific sound. Indeed it does not. No two individuals pronounce the English /k/ phoneme in exactly the same way – for one thing their vocal tracts are of different shapes. Neither does an individual produce exactly the same sound on different occasions. More importantly, the of a phoneme is affected by its neighbours in a word. For example there is a consistent between the pronunciation of the /k/ phoneme in cat and its pronun- ciation in kit. In normal speech phonemes ‘run together’. One consequence is that because /æ/ is pronounced further back in the throat than //, any preceding /k/ will be as well. A phoneme of a language represents a CLUSTER of similar sounds which a speaker of that language does not regard as distinctively different from one another. I will return to this issue later.

2 Production of Phonemes Remembering that a phoneme represents a cluster of sounds treated in some sense as equivalent by speakers of a given language, some 40-odd phonemes can be distinguished in most of English. Although all the sounds corresponding to a phoneme may not be produced in exactly the same way, for each phoneme we can describe the ‘typical’ way in which it is produced. The sounds corresponding to all English phonemes are powered by lung air being pushed out. A sound is then produced in two ways: • By vibrating the vocal ‘cords’: two muscular folds of skin low down in the throat which can be made to vibrate. The frequency of the vibration can be changed (within limits). • By altering the positions of components of the throat and mouth between the vocal cords and the exit of air. These alterations may merely modify the note produced by the vocal cords (by changing the size of the cavity) or may themselves produce a noise (for example by causing air friction). When lung air passes over the vibrating vocal cords and then passes freely out of the mouth, the sounds are called vowels. Thus vowels can be continued until you run out of breath. The positions of the and tongue alter the size and shape of the resonating cavity to produce different sounds. Vowels can be classified along a number of independent directions, including: • The height of the tongue (i.e. the size of the smallest opening). • The part of the tongue (front to back) causing the smallest opening. • The degree of rounding (open to rounded). Some examples in ‘Standard English English’ (SEE): /i/ is a high, front, unrounded , as in beet /bit/ or neat /nit/.2 // is a low, back, unrounded vowel, as in bar and bath. /u/ is a high, back, rounded vowel, as in spoon. Front Mid Back / / beat / / boot High i u // bit // put / / about, Mid  // bet Bert, sofa // bought 3 Low /æ/ bat // but // pot // bar In English, back vowels (other than the very lowest) are automatically rounded, front and

2 A more precise representation of this phoneme is /i/, where the  shows . I will generally ignore length distinctions; the theory presented here is intended to be the minimum necessary. 3 Although it is traditional to use // for this English phoneme, strictly this is not the correct IPA . NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) Page 3 mid vowels are not, so that a classification needs only two dimensions, as in the table below. Note that ‘Standard English English’ (SEE) pronunciation is intended. In addition to these ‘pure’ vowels, English makes considerable use of : sequences of two vowels ‘run together’ to form a SINGLE phoneme. A may include vowels not normally found alone. Examples of SEE diphthongs are given in the following table. /e/ baby, wait, day /o/ bone, soap, no // ear, cheer 4 /a/ kite, cry /a/ cow, out // air, share // coin, toy // tour In principle, vowels are infinitely variable as the position of the tongue and lips can be varied continuously. (This makes learning to make the correct vowel sounds in a foreign language very difficult.) tend to use different sets of more-or-less distinct vowels. English dialects vary greatly in the vowel phonemes used. In particular, differs considerably from English English. This is relevant because the English software currently available is often based on Standard American English (SAE). The main difference is that in SAE the back vowels //, // and // are usually replaced by /a/, so that the vowels in taught, tot and tart are all pronounced as /a/ – the first sound in the SEE diphthong /a/. (However, // is retained before /r/). The pure vowels /e/ and /o/ may be substituted for the SEE diphthongs /e/ and /o/. Further, most Americans at least partially pronounce r sounds which SEE speakers omit. Thus ear in SAE is closer to /r/ rather than SEE //. Stops By contrast with vowels, some sounds are made by completely stopping and then releasing the flow of air out of the mouth. These sounds are called stops (or ). In SEE there are three stop positions, corresponding to the initial phonemes in pale, tale and kale. The sound is stopped respectively by the lips (bilabial), by the front of the tongue and the ridge behind the top teeth (alveolar), and by the back of the tongue and the soft palate (velar). The phonemes at the start of bale, dale and gale involve exactly the same stop positions as the three above. One significant difference is that in these three phonemes the vocal cords vibrate during the production of the sound. Thus /p/, /t/ and /k/ are voiceless stops; /b/, /d/ and // voiced stops. Nasals If air is allowed to flow out of the nose while being stopped in the mouth, the result is nasal stops or nasals. English has three such phonemes, corresponding to same three stopping positions as ordinary stops. These are the three phonemes at the end of rum //, run // and rung //. Nasals are (almost always) voiced.

4 The diphthongs in this column can also be treated as sequences of two separate phonemes. Page 4 NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07)

Bilabial: Labio- Dental: Alveolar: Palatal: Velar: Glottal: Upper & dental: upper teeth tooth ridge hard palate soft at the lower Upper & tongue & tongue & middle palate & very base lips teeth & tip tip of tongue back of of the lower lip tongue throat

Stop /p/ pale /t/ tale /k/ kale (Voiceless/ Voiced) /b/ bale /d/ dale // gale

Nasal /m/ rum /n/ run // rung (Voiced)

Fricative /f/ fat // heath /s/ sip // mesh // hot (Voiceless/ Voiced) // vat // /z/ zip // measure heathen Affricative /t/ church (Voiceless/ Voiced) /d/ judge

Fricatives If air is not completely stopped from flowing out of the mouth, but made to pass through a narrow passage, a ‘friction’ sound or is produced (i.e. a more-or-less ‘hiss- ing’ sound). In SEE there are four positions where the narrowing can be made. These correspond to the voiceless terminal phonemes in life /f/, breath //, hiss /s/ and mesh //. If the vocal cords vibrate as well, we have the corresponding voiced phonemes in live /v/, breathe //, his /z/ and measure //. The /h/ sound in hot is also a kind of fricative, although no narrowing is involved. Instead air friction is produced low down in the throat. /h/ can be classified as a voiceless glottal frica- tive. Affricatives The phonemes that begin and end church and judge are voiceless and voiced affricatives respectively, composed of a very fast combination of a stop and a fricative; thus = t + sh /t/; compare why choose? with white shoes, both spoken rapidly. The j in judge can be written phonetically as /d/. Note that affricatives are SINGLE phonemes. Stops, nasals, and affricatives can be arranged in the neat table above.5 In these phonemes the tongue partly closes the airway, but not enough to cause a fricative. The phonemes that begin lap /l/ and rap /r/6 are sometimes called liquids. Both are produced in the alveolar/palatal region and are normally voiced. (They differ in whether air passes by the side or over the centre of the tongue.) Two further approximants are the initial phonemes in woo /w/ and you /j/. These are sometimes called glides: the tongue and lips move during the production of the sound. Both are normally voiced. Liquids and glides (especially the latter) have some similarities with vowels. The phonemes described above are NOT an exhaustive set for all dialects of English. For ex- ample, the ‘Cockney’ pronunciation of words like bottle includes a ‘’ //. Remember that a phoneme in the preceding tables really represents a set of closely similar sounds. For example a /t/ can be made with the tip of the tongue in different positions on the tooth ridge (say tea trip quickly and notice the position of your tongue during the /t/ sounds). As noted earlier, different languages use different sets of phonemes. Filled cells in the table above may be missing: for example, French has no dental fricatives, making it difficult for French-speakers to pronounce think or that. (They may produce something like sink zat instead of think that. Why?) Empty cells may be filled. Many languages have dental stops (e.g. Spanish, the Indic languages); Spanish – at least in the spoken around Madrid – has bilabial fricatives; German and some dialects of English (Scottish, Liverpool) have velar

5 Note that this table is simplified. E.g. ‘palatal’ can be subdivided into ‘post-alveolar’, etc. 6 This symbol should really be //; strictly speaking /r/ is the IPA symbol for a Scottish ‘trilled’ r. NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) Page 5 fricatives as in Bach or loch; German has the labial affricative /pf/. Other languages have phonemes which won’t fit into the table. For example, some southern African languages use ‘click’ sounds in speech, similar to the sound sometimes used to tell horses to ‘gee up’; these sounds are not powered by air expelled from the lungs.

3 Phones I pointed out earlier that the sounds corresponding to the t in the English words tea and trip are not in fact quite the same. The position of the tongue is slightly different, which causes a difference in sound detectable by an instrument such as a speech spectrograph. Thus the English phoneme /t/ corresponds to at least two different phones. By convention, phones are written using phonetic symbols enclosed in square . There aren’t any standard IPA symbols for the t phones in tea and trip, so I will use [tv] for [t] before a vowel and [tr] for [t] before [r]. Each of the set of phones which correspond to a single phoneme is called an of that phoneme. Thus [tv] and [tr] are of the English /t/ phoneme. The words Dee and drip show that /d/ similarly has at least two allophones, [dv] and [dr] say. A phone can be defined as a ‘unit sound’ of a language. It is a ‘unit’ sound because the whole of the phone must be substituted to make a different word. [tv] is a unit sound in English, and hence a phone, because the whole of it must be replaced by [dv] to change tea into Dee. [tr] is a phone because it must be replaced entirely by [dr] to change trip into drip. However, [tv] and [tr] are not DISTINCTIVE unit sounds (and hence are not phonemes) because there are no English words in which the only difference is that [tv] is replaced by [tr]. If you try saying tea with the [tr] allophone, you just get a slightly odd pronunciation of tea, not a new word. The problem with the concept of a phone (and hence of an allophone) is that its boundaries are blurred. Although it’s hard for English speakers to hear the difference between [tv] and [tr], it’s fairly clear that a slightly different tongue position is involved. How about the /t/ sounds in tea and tart? A speech spectrograph may show a difference, but here the tongue position differs only very slightly (if at all). Are these different allophones?7 A clearer example of English allophones occurs in the words pin and spin. If you are a native English speaker and you hold your hand in front of your mouth as you say pin and spin you will notice that the p in pin is accompanied by a short burst of air, i.e. is aspirated. The aspirated sound can be written as [p], the unaspirated as [p]. The words bin, spin and pin involve the closely similar English phones [b], [p] and [p]. They differ in the time difference between the release of lip closure and the start of vocal cord vibration (voicing). The somewhat idealized diagram below shows that in [b], voicing begins at lip opening; in [p], voicing begins very soon after lip opening; in [p], voicing is delayed (hence a significant puff of air escapes via the open vocal cords and lips).

[b] [] [n] lips vocal cords

[s] [p] [] [n] lips vocal cords

[p] [] [n] lips vocal cords

7 The more neutral term ‘’ is used by many authors to avoid the need to make essentially theoretical distinctions in contexts where this issue isn’t relevant. Page 6 NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07)

Three important points can be illustrated by these three phones. Firstly, PERCEIVED differ- ences between phones do not depend on actual differences. A sound spectrograph shows clearly the differences between [b] and [p] and between [p] and [p]. Objectively these three phones are easily recognizable. Yet English speakers normally notice only the first differ- ence. Why? The answer is that the phones [p] and [p] are not DISTINCTIVE in English since they are allophones of the same /p/ phoneme. There are no two words whose only difference is that [p] is replaced by [p], so English speakers don’t need to learn to distinguish them. To an English speaker [p] and [p] represent the ‘same sound’, even though they are actually different. Put another way, the discreteness of phonemes is a property of the listener, not the sound. By generating speech sounds artificially, it is possible to vary the time between lip opening and the beginning of vocal cord vibration. It is found that if this time is less than 30 milliseconds, English speakers hear /b/; more than this and they hear /p/. Only very close to the boundary time do they sometimes report hearing /b/, sometimes /p/. (Neural networks offer a good model of how continuous patterns of variation can be converted into discrete decisions.) Secondly, phoneme boundaries vary between languages. Both German and French have distinct /b/ and /p/ phonemes (i.e. in both languages there are words whose only difference is the replacement of one of these phonemes by the other). However, the precise boundary be- tween the allophones of /b/ and those of /p/ is different. In the case of the /b/ phoneme, some German speakers tolerate a longer interval between lip opening and vocal cord vibration beginning than do most French speakers. Thus some German speakers producing what to them is /b/ appear to French speakers to be producing /p/. One illustration of this is that when the French novelist Balzac wanted to show that the speaker was German he wrote, for ex- ample, “Eh pien” instead of “Eh bien.” It cannot be assumed that either vowel or phones are the same in languages that appear to have the same phonemes. This complicates multilingual . Infants presumably learn to make language-specific distinc- tions. Thirdly, phonemes are specific to languages. • In the Indic languages, aspirated and unaspirated stops represent different phonemes (al- though the difference in aspiration is greater than that in the English words pin and spin). In for example, /p/ and /p/ are different phonemes written using different characters (5 and 6 respectively). A Hindi speaker would be expected both to notice the difference between [pn] and [pn] and also to be able to produce both sound sequences. • In English, there is only one /p/ phoneme, but it has two allophones, [p] and [p], used in different positions within words. Thus an English speaker will find it difficult to hear any difference between [pn] and [pn], but will consistently produce [pn] for pin and [spn] for spin. • The phone [p] does not occur in French, i.e. is not an allophone of the French phoneme /p/. Hence native French speakers are likely to produce [pn] for [pn] when speaking English, just as native English speakers are likely to produce [pti] for [pti] (petit) when speaking French. We have the following phoneme:phone relationships in Hindi, English and French. Hindi English French /p/ [p] /p/ [p] /p/ [p] /p/ [p] /p/ [p] As a further example, /l/ and /r/ are different phonemes in English, each having a number of allophones. But in Korean and Japanese there is only one phoneme. Speakers of these lan- guages do not normally notice the difference between sounds which English speakers divide into /l/ and /r/ phonemes. Thus a Japanese or Korean speaker may produce [ri] for the English word gully. The discussion so far implies that the phoneme:phone relationship in a given language is 1:many. In fact it can be even more complex. Consider the prefix in (with its ‘direction’ meaning). The in in input, intake and income is clearly the ‘same’ element, so we would like to represent it by the same phonemes in each case, i.e. /n/. In slow, careful speech the NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) Page 7 corresponding pronunciation is [n]. Many speakers of Scottish English seem to be able to maintain this pronunciation in fast speech. But in fast SEE, these words will normally be pronounced [mpt], [ntek] and [km]. So in this dialect of English we have the relation- ship shown below, with the phone [m], for example, derived from more than one phoneme. phoneme phone /m/ [m] /n/ [n] // [] A question I asked but avoided answering earlier was how many allophones we need to dis- tinguish for each phoneme. Because phones ‘blend’ in continuous speech, there is likely to be some variation in the sound corresponding to a phoneme in every different environment in which it occurs. For example, in keen and card, the /k/ phoneme is clearly affected by the following vowel, being pronounced further forward in the mouth in keen than in card. What about the /k/ phoneme in keen and kin? Do we need a separate allophone of /k/ for every possible following vowel sound? In the context of speech synthesis or recognition, how far we divide up a phoneme into allophones is a purely pragmatic issue. In speech recognition, we may only need to recognise phonemes, ignoring all allophone distinctions. In speech synthesis, a large number of allophones may be needed for good quality, since although comprehensible speech can be synthesised using relatively few allophones, it will not sound very natural. For example, native English speakers will recognize both [pn] and [pn] as the word pin, and both [spn] and [spn] as the word spin, even though they would normally only produce [pn] and [spn]. But they are likely to be aware that there is ‘something not quite right’ about the synthesised speech, although they may not be able to identify it. Currently, the best speech synthesisers use several hundred phones to cover all the different allophones of (American) English phonemes.

4 Phonological Rules To summarize the previous sections, native speakers of a language hear phonemes but speak (allo)phones. An important issue, then, is how the interconversion of phonemes to phones takes place: how is it that native speakers of English hear [p] and [p] as the same but yet produce these phones quite distinctly to a native speaker of an Indic language? An answer which has the great merit of being programmable is that different phonological rules are being used. Phonological rules describe the relationships between phonemes and phones. For example, the following is a possible (but incomplete) rule for English:8 A voiceless stop at the beginning of a word is aspirated when followed by a vowel. In NLP, the important question is whether we can make these rules sufficiently detailed so that they can be programmed. The answer to this question is yes. One general approach is: • First, describe phonemes/phones by sets of features. • Second, write rules which describe changes in features based on the left and right contexts. For example, English can be described using the set {Type, Position, Voicing, Aspiration}. [p] is then {stop, bilabial, voiceless, aspirated}.9 The ‘best’ set of features is a pragmatic question for computer scientists (although of great theoretical interest to linguists). The table in Appendix 1 shows a minimal set (it omits aspiration). As always in this module, note that I have tried to simplify as much possible in order to show the principles without

8 Incomplete because voiceless stops are also usually aspirated at the beginning of stressed . Thus upon is pronounced [=pn], where = marks the boundary. 9 Throughout I’m using the standard convention, derived from Prolog, that an initial capital letter marks a variable, an initial lower-case letter marks a constant. This is a ‘set’ in that elements may be omitted; however it’s usual to keep the same ordering, Page 8 NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) excessive detail. The sets of features normally used by linguists are considerably more complicated than those discussed here. It appears to be an empirical fact about English (but not necessarily other languages) that at most one element before and one after a given phoneme determines which of its allophones is chosen. So the rule “a voiceless stop at the beginning of a word is aspirated when followed by a vowel” can be written as: word-boundary voiceless-stop vowel → word-boundary aspirated-voiceless-stop vowel Using the set makes such rules even more explicit. The rule above can be written as: word-boundary {stop, voiceless} {vowel} → word-boundary {stop, voiceless, aspirated} {vowel} The general format of a rule is: left-context input right-context → left-context output right-context In order to avoid repeating the unchanging left and right contexts, slightly different are often used. One such is: input → output : left-context _ right-context Thus the rule that a voiceless stop at the beginning of a word is aspirated when followed by a vowel can be written as: {stop, voiceless} → {stop, voiceless, aspirated} : word-boundary _ {vowel} In this notation, the information after the ‘:’ gives the context, the information before gives the change required by the rule.10 Either of the left or right contexts can be omitted if irrelevant. Note that I have written the rule as it would be used in the production of speech, i.e. converting phonemes to phones. Phonological rules can, in principle, be used in either direction. In recognition, the rule could be used ‘backwards’, to convert (allo)phones to phonemes.

5 Case Study: Nasal The pronunciation of income (discussed earlier) illustrates an important type of found in many languages. There is a tendency for neighbouring phonemes to influence one another in such a way that their phonetic representations become more similar; this presumably makes the word easier to pronounce.11 A rule which covers income and uncle in SEE is: /n/ when followed by [k] becomes [] In other words, /n/ in the context ‘_ [k]’ becomes []. Or in notation I’ve adopted above: /n/ → [] : _ [k] Replacing the IPA symbols by their feature sets (ignoring aspiration) gives: {nasal, alveolar, voiced} → {nasal, velar, voiced} : _ {stop, velar, voiceless} While it seems clear that it is useful to regard income as /nkm/ rather than /km/, in order to maintain the in component, it is less clear whether words like uncle or ankle should be treated as containing /n/ or //. However if /n/ is chosen, the rule will generate the correct phones, replacing /nk/ by [k]. Whenever we have specified a rule, it is useful to ask whether it can be generalized. If we omit voicing from the rule above, we have: {nasal, alveolar} → {nasal, velar} : _ {stop, velar}

10 A ‘/’ is often used in place of my ‘:’, but can be confused with the ‘/’ used to mark a phoneme. 11 Another general term for this phenomenon is ‘co-articulation’. I’ve restricted my examples to cases where the effect is so strong that an allophone of a different phoneme is involved. NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) Page 9

(In words, an alveolar nasal followed by a becomes a velar nasal, regardless of voicing.) Is the revised rule correct? We need to test it with a voiced velar stop (i.e. //) rather a voiceless velar stop (i.e. /k/). The word anger could be represented phonemically as /æn/, but is pronounced in SEE as [æ], which suggests that the revised rule may indeed be correct. However, the rule as formulated so far does not show what seems to be the underlying reason for the change. This is that the nasal /n/ is being assimilated to the following stop so that it has the same position of articulation. An even more general rule is: A nasal in the context ‘_ stop’ becomes articulated at the same position as the stop. Or in the more formal notation adopted here: {nasal} → {nasal, Position} : _ {stop, Position} Note that I am using the Prolog convention that constants start with lower-case letters, vari- ables with upper-case letters. This rule suggests that anple should not be an English word, since if we input the appropriate phonemes, the output is ample: np = /n/ /p/ = {nasal, alveolar} {stop, bilabial, voiceless} → {nasal, bilabial} {stop, bilabial, voiceless} (by applying the rule above) = [m] [p] = mp Starting from the sequence {nasal} {stop}, the only possible outputs are [mp], [mb], [nt], [nd], [k] and []. Thus the rule correctly predicts the existence of words like ample, amble, antler and handle (all pronounced as spelt), and ankle and angle (where /n/ becomes []). Other assimilation rules are covered in the Exercises. Deciding on the appropriate rules for a given language is (hopefully) a task for a linguist rather than a computer scientist, whose role is to implement the rules in a computer program. However, it is important to understand the nature of phonological rules, and studying some simple examples seems a good way of acquiring and demonstrating this understanding. Page 10 NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07)

Exercises Don’t feel you need to tackle all of these! I suggest as a minimum you try (1) – (5) and a selection of the others. The object is to reinforce your understanding of the concepts: phoneme, allophone and phonological rule. 1. How many phonemes are there in (a) Keith (b) coughs? What are they in the IPA? In each case try to demonstrate the correctness of your answer by finding words differing by only one of the phonemes you have identified.

2. You are NOT expected to know the IPA symbols; the table given in the Appendix will be provided if and when necessary. However it is useful to have some practice in using them. Study the following of a verse of Lewis Carroll’s poem The Walrus and the Carpenter. The transcription corresponds to my ‘careful’ pronunciation. Write down the normal English spelling. If your pronunciation differs from mine, write down an amended transcription in the IPA.  tam hæz km  wlrs sd tu tk v mni z v uz ænd  ænd sil wæks v kæbdz ænd kz ænd wa  si z bl ht ænd w pz hæv wz 3. Consider the prefix in with the meaning ‘not’ followed by an English word beginning with one of the 6 English stops. (For example, in + defensible = indefensible meaning ‘not defensible’.) Make sure that the word after in can occur as a separate word and that the meaning is ‘not’ rather than motion as in input. Do these words show the ‘nasal assimilation’ rule developed above?

4. Aston is usually pronounced [æstn]; Asda [æzd]. Assuming that both words contain the phoneme /s/ (i.e. that the phonemic representations are something like /æstn/ and /æsd/), suggest an appropriate phonological rule to generate the correct pronunciation. Try generalizing your initial rule. Can you find other examples to fit your rule? (In this and following exercises, try to write the rules in both English and feature set notation.) 5. Assume that the standard way of forming the plural of an English noun is to add the phoneme /z/ (NOT /s/) to the end of the word. Consider words ending in one of the 6 English stops. Construct phonological rule(s) to generate the correct pronunciation. For example, the plural of bid is [bdz] as expected but the plural of bit is [bts]. 6. In German, words whose spelling ends with d are pronounced with [t], as are words whose spelling ends with t. Many such words have inflected forms in which e is added with the (approximate) pronunciation []. These are pronounced as spelt. Thus: das Bund ist bunt = [das bnt st bnt] (the bundle is colourful) bunte Bunde = [bnt bnd] (colourful bundles) Write appropriate phonological rule(s).

7. Another kind of phonological rule actually DELETES phonemes from the output. Consider English words whose spelling suggests that they end in a nasal phoneme followed by a stop (e.g. lend). The nasal assimilation rule suggests that the nasal and the stop will have the same position of articulation so that only 6 endings are possible. Is this correct? Write rule(s) to generate the correct pronunciation of all the endings you find. NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) Page 11

8. The table below shows the pronunciation of some verbs and their negatives in one dialect of (spoken rapidly). [anvazo] I lift up [n anvazo] I don’t lift up [trksa] I ran [n trksa] I didn’t run [viazom] I’m in a hurry [ viazom] I’m not in a hurry [ls] you say [ ls] You don’t say [bno] I enter [ bno] I don’t enter [pirazi] it matters [m birazi] it doesn’t matter [trksan] they ran [n drksan] they didn’t run [katalava] I understood [ atalava] I didn’t understand Assume that the negative of a verb is formed by preceding it by the phoneme string /n/. Suggest rule(s) to predict the pronunciation of the negative form of the verb. 9. There is a general tendency in languages to make successive phones ‘more similar’. For example, we have seen that sequences such as /nk/ may be mapped to [k], where the position of articulation of both phones is velar. A different kind of rule INSERTS phones into the output to improve the similarity between successive phones. For example, in the sequence /mk/ we move from a voiced bilabial (nasal) to an voiceless velar (stop). English names with this sequence, such as Tomkin, are often pronounced as though spelt Tompkin. The sequence is then voiced bilabial (nasal) to voiceless bilabial (stop) to voiceless velar (stop), now with only one voicing or position change with each new phone. Another example is Hamton which may yield Hampton. a) Write the appropriate rule(s) for the sequence /m/. b) The alternation of Tomson and Tompson suggests that this rule can be generalized to /m/. Try it. 10. Consider the following information about Spanish in the dialect spoken around Madrid. Spelling Pronunciation Meaning dato [dato] fact, piece of information, datum dardo [daro] dart dolar [dolar] dollar drama [drama] drama duda [dua] doubt madre [re] mother claridad [klaria] brightness, clarity ([a], [o] and [e] are PURE vowels which occur in Spanish and in SAE but not in SEE which only uses them in the diphthongs [a], [o] and [e].) a) Assuming that /d/ is a phoneme in Spanish, and that [d] is the same phone as in English (which the Collins Spanish Pocket says that it is), write rule(s) to generate the correct of the words given above. b) In fact, in the relevant dialect of Spanish, the initial phone in words like dato is a ([d] in IPA), not an as in English. In other words, the tip of the tongue starts on the back of the upper teeth rather than on the tooth ridge. A feature set definition for this phone is {stop, dental, voiced}. Revise your rules from (a) to generate pronunciations using this phone (e.g. duda should give[dua]). c) Suppose the rule(s) from (b) are generalized to ALL voiced stops. Predict the pronunciation of the // phonemes in gafas (=a pair of glasses) and paga (=payment) and the /b/ phonemes (spelt v) in via (=route) and dividir (=to divide). You may end up with phones which do not occur in English. (To check your answer, you need a Spanish speaker from the right area of Spain!) Page 12 NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07)

11. Construct a similar example to those given here, ideally based on a different language or dialect of English. (If you know French, for example, consider the pronunciation of final consonants in phrases such as petit enfant, petit livre or petit oiseau. Speakers of dif- ferent English dialects could attempt to produce phonological rules mapping SEE to their speech, assuming the phonemic representation is stored in SEE. Thus, for example, /b/ → [bæ] in northern English English.) I would be interested to see any such examples. NLPA-Phon1 (4/10/07) Page 13

Appendix IPA SEE Examples ASCII Partial Feature Set [i] heel, me IY {vowel,voiced} [] hit IH {vowel,voiced} [e] SAE bait EY {vowel,voiced} [] met, head EH {vowel,voiced} [æ] hat AE {vowel,voiced} [a] SAE father, pot AA {vowel,voiced} [] about, after, fern AX {vowel,voiced} [] up, fun UX {vowel,voiced} [u] soon UW {vowel,voiced} [] put, foot UH {vowel,voiced} [o] SAE boat OW {vowel,voiced} [] fork, taut AO {vowel,voiced} [] hot OH {vowel,voiced} [] bath, bar AH {vowel,voiced} [e] wait, cake EI {vowel,voiced} [a] kite, buy AY {vowel,voiced} [] coin, toy OY {vowel,voiced} [o] bone, open OU {vowel,voiced} [a] cow, out AW {vowel,voiced} [] ear, sheer IA {vowel,voiced} [] air, share EA {vowel,voiced} [] tour UA {vowel,voiced} [p] spin p {stop,bilabial,voiceless} [bp]] bpooin b {stop,bilabial,voiced}phone}p_h,stop,bilabial,voiceless,aspirated} [t] stop t {stop,alveolar,voiceless} [dt]] dtopog d {stop,alveolar,voiced}phone}t_h,stop,alveolar,voiceless,aspirated} [k] scan k {stop,velar,voiceless} [k]] gcanate g {stop,velar,voiced}phone}k_h,stop,velar,voiceless,aspirated} [m] mat m {nasal,bilabial,voiced} [n] not n {nasal,alveolar,voiced} [] king NG {nasal,velar,voiced} [f] fat f {fricative,labiodental,voiceless} [v] vat v {fricative,labiodental,voiced} [] thumb TH {fricative,dental,voiceless} [] that DH {fricative,dental,voiced} [s] sat s {fricative,alveolar,voiceless} [z] zip z {fricative,alveolar,voiced} [] mesh SH {fricative,palatal,voiceless} [] measure ZH {fricative,palatal,voiced} [h] hot h {fricative,lottal} [t] chair CH {affricative,palatal,voiceless} [d] edge, jam JH {affricative,palatal,voiced} [l] lot l {,voiced} [r] rot r {approximant,voiced} [j] yawn y {approximant,voiced} [w] win w {approximant,voiced} Notes bilabial = both lips labio-dental = upper teeth and lower lip dental = tongue tip and upper teeth alveolar = tongue tip and tooth ridge palatal = tongue and hard palate velar = tongue and soft palate