This article was downloaded by: [Harvard Library] On: 03 June 2014, At: 13:23 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Memory Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pmem20 Episodic and semantic components of autobiographical and imagined future events in post-traumatic disorder Adam D. Browna, Donna Rose Addisb, Tracy A. Romanoc, Charles R. Marmara, Richard A. Bryantd, William Hirste & Daniel L. Schacterf a Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, USA b School of Psychology, The University of Auckland, New Zealand c Center for Complex Systems and Brain Sciences, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL, USA d School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia e Department of Psychology, New School for Social Research, New York, NY, USA f Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA Published online: 25 Jun 2013.

To cite this article: Adam D. Brown, Donna Rose Addis, Tracy A. Romano, Charles R. Marmar, Richard A. Bryant, William Hirst & Daniel L. Schacter (2014) Episodic and semantic components of autobiographical memories and imagined future events in post-traumatic stress disorder, , 22:6, 595-604, DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2013.807842 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.807842

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions Memory, 2014 Vol. 22, No. 6, 595604, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.807842

Episodic and semantic components of autobiographical memories and imagined future events in post-traumatic stress disorder

Adam D. Brown1, Donna Rose Addis2, Tracy A. Romano3, Charles R. Marmar1, Richard A. Bryant4, William Hirst5, and Daniel L. Schacter6

1Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, USA 2School of Psychology, The University of Auckland, New Zealand 3Center for Complex Systems and Brain Sciences, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL, USA 4School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia 5Department of Psychology, New School for Social Research, New York, NY, USA 6Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA

Individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) tend to retrieve autobiographical memories with less episodic specificity, referred to as overgeneralised autobiographical memory. In line with evidence that autobiographical memory overlaps with one’s capacity to imagine the future, recent work has also shown that individuals with PTSD also imagine themselves in the future with less episodic specificity. To date most studies quantify episodic specificity by the presence of a distinct event. However, this method does not distinguish between the numbers of internal (episodic) and external (semantic) details, which can provide additional insights into remembering the past and imagining the future. This study employed the Autobiographical Interview (AI) coding scheme to the autobiographical memory and imagined future event narratives generated by combat veterans with and without PTSD. Responses were coded for the number of internal and external details. Compared to combat veterans without PTSD, those with PTSD generated more external than internal details when recalling past or imagining future events, and fewer internal details were associated with greater symptom severity. The potential mechanisms underlying these bidirectional deficits and clinical implications are discussed. Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014

Keywords: Post-traumatic stress disorder; PTSD; Autobiographical memory; Future thinking; Mental time travel; Memory.

Among the many disorders classified in the of PTSD often focus their on memory as Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental a key factor in the pathogenesis of the disorder Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric (e.g., Brewin, 2007; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, Association, 2000), post-traumatic stress disorder 2000; McNally, 2003; Rubin, Berntsen, & Bohni, (PTSD) is somewhat distinct in that its diagnosis 2008). In addition to studies examining memory- is linked to a precipitating event. Since PTSD related symptoms in PTSD, such as intrusive symptoms often persist long after exposure to the memories, flashbacks, and (for a review stressor, researchers elucidating the mechanisms see McNally, 2003), there has been increasing

Address correspondence to: Adam D. Brown, Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, 50 First Avenue, OBV-Bldg A, # A645, New York, NY 10016, USA. E-mail: [email protected]

# 2013 Taylor & Francis 596 BROWN ET AL.

awareness that PTSD represents, in part, a cesses as imagining and projecting one’s into problem with the retrieval of autobiographical the future (for reviews, see Schacter, Addis, & memories (for a review see Moore & Zoellner, Buckner, 2008; Suddendorf & Corballis, 2007; 2007). When asked to an autobiographical Szpunar, 2010). According to the constructive memory, individuals with PTSD tend to exhibit episodic simulation hypothesis (Schacter & Addis, difficulties in recalling unique memories that took 2007), imagining future episodic events engages a place on a specific time and day, producing what constructive memory system that facilitates the is often referred to as ‘‘overgeneralised’’ auto- flexible recombination of elements from past biographical memories. events in order to project and simulate novel The phenomenon of greater overgenerality in events in to the future. In other words, imagining PTSD has been documented across multiple the future depends on remembering the past. trauma-exposed population including Vietnam From this perspective, individuals with PTSD may veterans (McNally, Lasko, Macklin, & Pitman, report overgeneralised episodic simulations be- 1995; McNally, Litz, Prassas, Shin, & Weathers, cause these future simulations are constructed 1994), cancer survivors (Kangas, Henry, & Bry- around overgeneralised recollections of past epi- ant, 2005), and injured individuals with acute sodes. The close link between overgenerality in stress disorder (Harvey, Bryant, & Dang, 1998). autobiographical memory and future thinking Overgenerality does not appear to be a conse- suggests that those with PTSD face a problem quence of trauma exposure alone (Moore & not only in remembering their past in specific Zoellner, 2007), is found independently of terms, but also in imagining an episodically rich depression (McNally et al., 1995), and does not future, a difficulty that may contribute to the seem to be a marker of overall psychopathology persistence of the disorder. (e.g., Wenzel, Jackson, & Holt, 2002; Wilhelm, Conway and Pleydell-Pearce (2000) propose a McNally, Baer, & Florin, 1997). Overgeneralised hierarchical representation of autobiographical autobiographical memory has been shown to be memory based on the temporal specificity of the an important predictor in the time course of memories, with event-specific knowledge (ESK; PTSD. A prospective study of firefighters identi- e.g., eating dinner in Montmartre while watching fied overgenerality as a risk factor for PTSD, as the sun set over Sacre´ Coeur) lower in the firefighters exhibiting greater overgenerality be- hierarchy than general events that are repeated fore trauma exposure were found to be at greater or ‘‘categorical’’ (e.g., commuting to a particular risk for developing PTSD following trauma ex- job) or extended (e.g., my trip to Paris) in time. posure (Bryant, Sutherland, & Guthrie, 2007). Within this hierarchy, retrieval of autobiogra- Overgenerality in PTSD has also been linked with phical memories typically begins at the general deficits in social problem solving, suggesting a event level, and from there ESKs are accessed role in symptom maintenance (Sutherland & (Haque & Conway, 2001). According to Williams

Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 Bryant, 2008). Furthermore, a decline in over- et al. (2007), ruminative thinking at a categorical generalised memories may be a marker of recov- level, avoidance of the distressing affect asso- ery. For example, the remission of PTSD ciated with ESKs, or limitations of executive symptoms following cognitive behavioural ther- control caused by competing demands of cognitive apy (CBT) appears to correspond with a reduc- resources can truncate retrieval prior to accessing tion in overgeneral memories (Sutherland & ESKs. When these disruptions occur, as is believed Bryant, 2007). to be the case in PTSD, retrieval does not progress More recently, Brown et al. (2013) have shown beyond the level of general events, resulting in that not only autobiographical memories are memories that lack the rich sensory details asso- characterised by overgeneralisation, but so too ciated with ESK (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, are the projections that PTSD individuals make 2000) and comprise primarily conceptual informa- about possible future events. Relatedly, Blix and tion (Williams et al., 2007). The same factors also Brennen (2011) found that PTSD symptom se- appear to truncate episodic simulations, producing verity was positively associated with decreased simulations that share many of the characteristics specificity for autobiographical memories and ima- of the affected autobiographical memories. gined future events among a sample of sexual In this study we explore a new approach to assault victims. These findings accord with growing examining the relationship between the retrieving evidence that retrieving episodic memories involves personal memories of the past and imagining many of the same neural and cognitive pro- future events in individuals with PTSD based on EPISODIC SPECIFICITY IN PTSD 597

the Autobiographical Interview (AI) developed autobiographical memories as specific or general. by Levine and colleagues (Levine, Svoboda, Importantly for the present study, the AI coding Hay, Winocur, & Moskovitch, 2002). A key idea scheme can successfully detect tendencies of integral to the AI is that even if a remembered different populations to generate more or less of event occurs at a specific time and place*meet- one detail type. For instance, younger adults ing the definition of ESK*the representation generate more internal details when generating itself may contain both the rich episodic details past and future events compared with older typical of ESK, as well as more generic informa- adults, whereas older adults generate past and tion such as personal or general . future events with more external details (Addis, Indeed, Conway and Pleydell-Pearce (2000) Wong, & Schacter, 2008; Levine et al., 2002). explicitly acknowledged that a combination of Given that the AI coding scheme provides a ESK with the ‘‘personal’’ semantic knowledge of more nuanced profile of the types of details general events provides a transitory representa- comprising past and future events than has yet tion that is roughly equivalent to the construct of been produced for PTSD, we reasoned that it (Tulving, 1972). Given that would be a useful tool to characterise further the episodic and semantic memory are thought to autobiographical memories and episodic simula- rely on different neural processes (Moscovitch et al., 2005), Levine et al. (2002) sought to distin- tions of those with PTSD. From a clinical per- guish episodic and non-episodic details compris- spective, the analysis may provide therapists with ing a memory by analysing the quality of each a more nuanced view of how those with PTSD detail comprising the verbalised memory. In their imagine their future. To account for their results AI coding scheme, episodic details*referred to showing that older adults generated fewer inter- as internal details (e.g., details regarding the nal details, but more external details for both past central event, including, perceptual, emotional, and future events, Addis et al. (2008) proposed spatial, and temporal details)*are scored using that the pattern found with older adults might categories adapted from the Memory Character- reflect, at least in part, deficits associated with istic Questionnaire (Johnson, Foley, Suengas, & hippocampally dependent relational memory, Raye, 1988). Non-episodic details*referred to as making it difficult for these participants to either external details*include primarily personal and retrieve or link together elements of episodic generic semantic information, as well as repeti- memory during both the memory and the episodic tions and metacognitive statements. Traditionally, simulation tasks. Recent neuroimaging evidence clinical studies investing episodic specificity em- with older adults provides some support for this ploy the coding scheme developed by Williams view (Addis, Roberts, & Schacter, 2011). Similar and colleagues (1996) in which response to a cue difficulties might arise in individuals with PTSD word were give a score of ‘‘3’’ (specific) if the because PTSD is associated with poor perfor- Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 event took place, or could take place, within a mance on assessments sensitive to hippocampal 24-hour time period, included people, and a functions, such as the Verbal Pairs Associate I specific location; a ‘‘2’’ (intermediate) if the (VPA; Golier et al., 2002; Yehuda et al., 2006). response was a repeated event or a past or future Moreover, individuals with PTSD show reduc- event that took place or could take place over a tions in hippocampal volume (e.g., Apfel et al., period of more than 1 day; a ‘‘1’’ (general) if it did 2011; Felmingham et al., 2009; for reviews see not contain specific details or was something other than a memory; and a ‘‘0’’ if the individual did not Geuze, Vermetten, & Bremner, 2005a, 2005b; generatearesponse. Karl et al., 2006). The AI coding scheme differs in several ways The aim of this study was to examine whether from the Williams et al. (1996) approach. The individuals with and without PTSD differ in their Williams approach distinguishes at a fairly broad generation of internal and external details when level between specific events and general events remembering past events and simulating future by determining the temporal specificity of the events. We predicted that a diagnosis with PTSD event, without examining the episodic or semantic will be associated with more external, and fewer nature of the details comprising the memory. In internal, details for both the generation of past contrast, Levine et al.’s coding scheme focuses on and future events, and that the number of observed the qualitative nature of the details that make up external details would be associated with PTSD a recollection rather than on classifying whole symptom severity. 598 BROWN ET AL.

METHOD TABLE 1 Demographic and clinical summary for OEF/OIF veterans

This study is based on a re-analysis of data that Veterans Veterans were collected by Brown et al. (2013). with without PTSD PTSD

Variable M SD M SD t Participants Age 30.25 6.03 31.38 8.69 8.69 Gender% male 92% 82% A total of 28 Operation Iraqi Freedom and Years of education 14.08 1.78 14.94 1.52 1.52 Operation Enduring Freedom combat veterans Months deployed 15.55 11.75 14.38 6.29 6.29 between the ages of 19 and 50 years participated Months since combat 54.50 28.91 45.56 25.52 25.52 in the study. Participants were recruited from the CAPS 70.17 25.15 14.06 9.52 8.22*** Combat exposure 17.25 3.84 10.13 3.69 4.94*** New York metropolitan area (via craigslist.com, BDI-II 13.58 7.08 9.47 6.87 1.71 advertising) and were required to demonstrate COWAT 37.25 9.38 39.94 10.14 0.72 proof of military identification (e.g., DD-214) in order to participate in the study. Individuals were CAPS Clinician Administered PTSD Scale; Combat pre-screened and excluded if they met criteria for Exposure Combat Exposure Scale; BDI II Beck Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI; Hoge et al., 2008)or Depression Inventory Second Edition (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996); COWAT Controlled Oral Word Association had endorsed prior behavioural or psychophar- Task. ** p B.001. *** p B.0001. macological treatment for PTSD. The Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale graphical memory and episodic future thinking (CAPS; Blake et al., 1995), a semi-structured (e.g., Addis et al., 2008). Individuals were pre- interview, was used to determine the presence of sented with 20 cue words and were instructed to PTSD. The CAPS possesses good sensitivity (.84), generate close (past month) or distant (past 520 specificity (.95), and testretest reliability (.90) years) memories and close (next month) and relative to the SCID PTSD diagnosis (Blake et distant (next 520 years) imagined future events. al., 1995). Individuals were assigned to the PTSD Temporal distance (close/distant) was manipu- group if they met DSM-IV-TR PTSD Criteria A1 lated because previous work shows that past and (exposure to an event involving actual or perceived future event phenomenology, such as personal life threatening or serious injury) and A2 (a significance and level of event detail, vary as a subjective response of fear, helplessness, or horror function of time (Addis et al., 2007; D’Argem- during or immediately after the event) and ex- beau & van der Linden, 2004). Previous research hibited at least one re-experiencing symptom, at has used similar time frames; that is, distant least three avoidance symptoms, and at least two temporal distance of 520 years (e.g., Addis hyperarousal symptoms (Blake et al., 1995). Based

Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 et al., 2007). All cue words were neutral nouns on these criteria, 12 individuals did and 16 indivi- selected from Clark and Paivio’s extended norms duals did not meet criteria for PTSD. In addition (Clark & Paivio, 2004). Although autobiographi- participants completed a battery of demographic cal memory studies in clinical disorders often use and clinical self-reports. For the purposes of this positively and negatively valenced word cues, study we have included self-report data concerning neutral words cues were employed because we demographic information, depression, combat ex- were closely adhering to the paradigm used by posure, and executive functioning (see Table 1 for Addis et al. (2007). In addition we were reluctant description of the measures and findings). to explicitly ask this clinical population to recall or imagine negative events (for a more detailed rationale, see Brown et al., 2013). Stimulus material, design, and The conditions were blocked, the 20 words procedure were randomly divided into four lists (one list per temporal condition) of 5 words, and the order of The stimulus material and methodology are based presentation and temporal direction were coun- on a recently modified autobiographical memory terbalanced. Participants were instructed to either task (Crovitz & Schiffman, 1974) and the AI recall or imagine a personal event in as much (Levine et al., 2002), as has previously been detail as possible in response to each cue word. In employed in earlier studies examining autobio- addition to being personally relevant, participants EPISODIC SPECIFICITY IN PTSD 599

were told that each event, past or future, should episodic future thoughts, individuals with PTSD occur within a 24-hour time period, be realistic, generated 48.81 (SD15.96) details and indivi- and for future events, not previously experienced duals without PTSD generated 52.32 (SD10.24), by the participants. Four practice trials were t(26).71, p.49, d.26. completed before beginning each task. In order to assess our claims about internal and Word cues were presented in the centre of an external details, separate repeated-measure AN- Apple desktop computer with a 21.5-inch moni- OVAs were conducted for internal and external tor, with the task (recall past event or imagine AI scores, in which Group (PTSDor no-PTSD) future event) and temporal distance (month or 5 served as the between-participants variable, and 20 years) displayed underneath the cue, as well as Time (Close or Distant) and Task (Memory or a reminder to describe as much detail about the Future) were the within-participant variables (see event as possible. Participants then described the Figure 1). Participants with PTSD produced fewer event in detail into a tape recorder. There were no internal details than individuals without PTSD, time constraints on the verbal description. Re- F(1, 26)10.79, p.003, h.29. Main effects sponses were later transcribed. were also found for Time, F(1, 26)7.73, p.01, h.23, and Task, F(1, 26)20.24, p.001, h .44. The main effect of Time arose because close Scoring events contained more internal details than dis- tant events (Close: M56.70, SD17.62, Dis- For each response the central event was identi- tant: M50.64, SD17.59), t(27)2.83, p.009, fied. Each response was segmented into distinct d.34). As for the main effect of Task, it reflects details and these details were then labelled as that fact that remembered past events contained internal (episodic details related to the central more internal details than imagined future events event, e.g., ‘‘It was dark’’, ‘‘I jumped out of the (Memory: M57.83, SD14.25, Future M way’’, ‘‘He was standing to my left’’) and external 49.52, SD20.33), t(27)3.99, p.000, d.47). details (semantic information: ‘‘Paris is the capital In addition there was an interaction between of France‘‘; repetitions: ‘‘They liked what I did’’, Group and Task, F(1, 26)4.54, p.04, h.15. ‘‘They liked my work’’; information unrelated to Follow-up t-tests revealed that participants with the event: ‘‘That doesn’t matter’’, ‘‘That was PTSD generated more internal details for mem- amazing’’; and information for extended periods ories than future events, (PTSD: M86.89, SD or facts about the person: ‘‘I always hated yams’’ 32.51; No PTSD: M122.70, SD35.25), t(26) ‘‘I worked as an engineer’’). The numbers of 3.29, p.003, d1.05. Furthermore the number internal and external details for each event were of internal details for past and future events were totalled and averaged across all trials in each significantly correlated for both participants with condition, creating total Internal and External PTSD, r.80, p.001 and without PTSD, r.81, Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 scores for each of the four conditions. Two scorers p.001. were blind to the hypotheses of the study and group membership. Based on the scoring of 20 responses, scorer inter-rater reliability using inter- class correlations was high for Internal (a.91) and External (a.88) scores.

RESULTS

In order to obtain a rough estimate of the level of detail of the reported autobiographical mem- ories and episodic future thoughts, we calculated the total number of details (combined internal and external details) contained in the responses. For autobiographical memories, individuals with PTSD generated 55.01 (SD9.84) details, whereas Figure 1. Mean number of internal and external details for individuals without PTSD generated 54.65 (SD autobiographical memories and imagined future events. Data 9.34), t(26).09, p.92, d .04.Intermsof collapsed across temporal distance. 600 BROWN ET AL.

In contrast, a repeated-measures ANOVA for recalls or imagines an event that is temporally external details revealed that PTSD-diagnosed specific, taking place within a 24-hour time individuals generated more external details than period. Although this technique has been a useful individuals without PTSD F(1, 26)11.89, p means of observing this phenomenon in PTSD .002, h.31(PTSD: M119.17, SD22.68; No (Moore & Zoellner, 2007) and other clinical PTSD: M87.53, 24.96), t(26)3.45, p.002, disorders (Williams et al., 2007), this method d1.32. A main effect of Time was observed in does not make a clear distinction between the which distant events contained more external use of internal (episodic) and external (semantic) details than close events, F(1, 26)9.99, p details within responses. This report provides .004, h.28 (Close: M45.69, SD17.42, Dis- evidence that individuals with PTSD were more tant: M55.39, SD14.97), t(27)3.27, p.003, likely to generate personal past and future events d.60). The number of external details for past with a greater proportion of external details, or future events were also significantly correlated whereas individuals without PTSD generated in participants with PTSD, r.74, p.006 and responses with a greater proportion of internal approached significance among participants with- details. Inasmuch as we failed to find any differ- out PTSD, r.71, p.002. ence in the level of depression across these two A series of correlations among individuals with groups, the deficit we observed can probably be PTSD were conducted examining the relation traced to the PTSD itself. between AI scores and PTSD symptom severity, To date the CARFAX model is the most as measured by the Clinician Administered PTSD comprehensive model for conceptualising the Scale (CAPS). Internal details were negatively mechanisms underlying retrieval specificity (Wil- correlated for both past and (r.41, p.03) liams et al., 2007). This model proposes that and future (r.47, p.01) events with PTSD capture and rumination, functional avoidance, symptom severity. Thus individuals with more and executive control deficits independently or severe PTSD symptoms generated fewer internal interdependently underlie the well-established details when describing past and future events. In phenomenon of overgeneralised autobiographical contrast, the generation of external details for memory in a range of clinical disorders. Although past events were positively correlated with total there is considerable empirical support for these scores on the CAPS, r.48, p.01. mechanisms, recent reviews have suggested that Although group differences did not emerge on additional research is needed to more fully under- the word fluency task, given the importance of stand the processes and characteristics linking executive functioning in overgeneralised memory overgeneralised autobiographical memory and (e.g., Williams et al., 2007), a series of correlations psychopathology (Moore & Zoellner, 2007; Sum- were conducted between the FAS and scores on ner, 2012). To that end, the primary aim of this the AI. These correlations were not significant study was to adopt a more fine-grained approach

Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 when conducted for the total sample (Memory to examining the composition of details generated Internal: r.03; Memory External, r.03; Future for past and future events. That is, although there Internal: r .08; Future External, r.13) or now exists a robust body of work showing a when examined separately for PTSD (Memory consistent association between a lack of episodic Internal: r.12; Memory External, r.36; Future specificity for autobiographical memory and ima- Internal: r .10; Future External, r.38) and gined future events in a number of psychological non-PTSD individuals (Memory Internal: r.16; disorders, the majority of research to date has Memory External, r.11; Future Internal: r focused on the inability of patients to generate .14; Future External, r.01). events within a distinct spatiotemporal context, without quantifying the number of episodic and semantic details comprising the events. However, DISCUSSION cognitive models of autobiographical memory suggest that autobiographical events, past and Retrieval deficits are commonly observed in future, include episodic and semantic details PTSD (Moore & Zoellner, 2007), and recent (e.g., Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Levine work has shown that these patterns also occur et al., 2002; Tulving, 1972). Therefore, although for imagined future events (Brown et al., 2013). preliminary, these findings suggest that in addition To date these studies have employed a coding to deficits in retrieving temporally distinct events, method that is based on whether an individual overgeneralised autobiographical memories are EPISODIC SPECIFICITY IN PTSD 601

also characterised by the presence of numerous generality (e.g., Williams et al., 2007) and PTSD external details as well as few internal details. (e.g., Polak, Witteveen, Reitsman, & Olff, 2012). Prior work in amnesic patients has demon- However, we did not observe a difference be- strated that impaired access to episodic details is tween the two groups on executive functioning associated with impaired construction of specific using the word fluency task, nor did scores on the and detailed future simulations (e.g., Tulving, FAS correlate with internal or external scores for 1985; Klein, Loftus, & Kihlstrom, 2002; Hassabis past or future events across the whole sample or & Maguire, 2007; Race, Keane, & Verfaillie, 2011; when examined separately among individuals but see also Squire et al., 2010). In line with these with and without PTSD. findings we found significant correlations be- Future studies using a wider range of neuro- tween the number of internal details generated cognitive tests, including other measures of ex- for past and future events*a pattern previously ecutive functioning, would help to better detect observed in older adults (Addis et al., 2008) and the various neurocognitive processes contributing patients with hippocampal damage (Race et al., to this phenomenon. Furthermore, examining 2011). Another important component process of other aspects of past and future narratives, such simulation is the recombination of memory de- as event quality, may help to link these findings tails into a coherent scenario. It is thought that with the CARFAX model. For example, recent this recombination process relies on hippocam- work has shown that self-report ratings of reliving pally mediated relational processing (Addis & of past and future events were associated with Schacter, 2012). Indeed, positive correlations rumination, a cognitive process highly associated between internal details for past and future events with PTSD (Thomsen, Shneiber, & Olesen 2011). and deficits in performance on the VPA, a We believe that the present findings further hippocampal-sensitive measure of relational clarify how episodic specificity may differ be- memory (Addis et al., 2008). Consistent with tween individuals with and without PTSD. Based these observations a recent fMRI study revealed on these findings, and in the context of the Brown that during the construction of remembered past et al. (2013) study, individuals with PTSD are less events and imagined future events, older adults likely to generate temporally specific events, and showed reduced hippocampal activation com- are less likely to incorporate contextually rich pared with younger adults (Addis et al., 2011). episodic details when generating these events. Deficits in hippocampal function are likely to As noted earlier, this pattern is similar to that impair one’s ability to recombine past episodic previously observed in older adults (Addis et al., details in order to generate future events, and 2008, 2010). In attempting to understand why may be associated with more fragmented simula- older adults produce fewer internal and more tions (Addis, Musicaro, Pan, & Schacter, 2010; external details than younger adults, Gaesser, Hassabis et al., 2007). Although we can only Sacchetti, Addis, and Schacter (2011) conducted

Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 speculate, in light of the present findings and experiments in which older and younger adults other evidence documenting decreased VPA were instructed to describe, one at a time, performance (Golier et al., 2002; Yehuda et al., individual coloured pictures of natural scenes in 2006) and hippocampal volume (Bremner, 2007; as much detail as possible; a different set of Geuze et al., 2005a, 2005b; Schaefer et al., 2006) pictures were then also used to cue simulations of in PTSD, it is possible that alterations in hippo- imagined future events or memories of past campal functions in PTSD are associated with events. The resulting protocols were scored for neuro-cognitive deficits in simulating detailed internal and external details with a version of the future scenarios. Future studies will need to AI similar to the one used in the present study, examine this issue directly by examining whether except that it was further adapted to score performance on relational memory tasks in PTSD internal and external details for the picture is associated with episodic past and future event description task. Two experiments revealed that specificity, and also by using fMRI to examine older adults produced fewer internal and more hippocampal activity in PTSD during episodic external details than younger adults in all three memory and future simulation. It will also be experimental conditions; picture description, important to assess how findings from such memory, and imagination. It was also found that studies bear on the mechanisms proposed by the age-related changes in picture description perfor- CARFAX model. In particular, deficits in execu- mance could not fully account for all the changes tive control have been associated with both over- observed on the memory and imagination tasks. 602 BROWN ET AL.

Nonetheless, the finding that older adults pro- on the role of the . Future studies duced fewer internal and more external details on would benefit from structural and functional the picture description task, which does not neuroimaging data. Second, the sample size is require access to episodic memory, led Gaesser small, and comprises almost entirely males who et al. (2011) to argue that performance on the AI served in combat. Thus we must be cautious in can be affected by non-episodic factors, such as generalising to other populations. The cross- age-related changes in narrative style or inhibi- sectional design does not allow us to demonstrate tory control. In order to assess the possible causality, thus we cannot say that the reduced spe- contribution of non-episodic factors such as cificity is the result of trauma exposure, especially narrative style or inhibitory control to the pattern in light of prospective studies showing func- of results reported here, it will be important for tioning prior to trauma exposure is a risk factor future studies to determine whether PTSD pa- for PTSD. However, this study does suggest that tients also produce fewer internal and increased individuals with PTSD appear to have difficulty external details on a picture description task, and retrieving and imagining episodic details of past whether any such deficits fully account for the and future experiences, which may contribute to deficits in episodic memory and episodic simula- symptom maintenance tion that we have documented. Despite the need for additional studies to Manuscript received 24 January 2013 clarify the basis of the impairments we have Manuscript accepted 20 May 2013 observed, our findings may have implications for First published online 21 June 2013 treatment. Studies suggest that less cohesive and more fragmented narratives may be an important predictor of symptom severity and treatment REFERENCES outcome (Foa, Molnar, & Cashmere, 1995). Although the presence of fragmented narratives Addis, D. R., Musicaro, R., Pan, L., & Schacter, D. L. in PTSD is not strongly supported through (2010). Episodic simulation of past and future events empirical studies (e.g., Porter & Birt, 2001) these in older adults: Evidence from an experimental recombination task. Psychology and Aging, 25, findings suggest that the way in which ‘‘fragmen- 369376. ted’’ was operationalised by Foa and colleagues Addis, D. R. Roberts, R. P, & Schacter, D. L. (2011). (1995) may be similar to the types of content Age-related neural changes in autobiographical coded as external details in the AI. Thus, whether remembering and imagining. Neuropsychologia, 49, 36563669. greater use of external details reflects ‘‘fragmen- Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2012). The hippocam- tation’’ or ‘‘narrative cohesion’’, it may be a pus and imagining the future: Where do we stand? useful indicator of prognosis. Additionally, emer- Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5,115. ging evidence suggests that training programmes Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 in which patients practise retrieving specific Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science, 19,3341. autobiographical memories corresponds with a Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). reduction in depressive symptoms (e.g., Neshat- Remembering the past and imagining the future: Doost et al., 2013; Raes, Williams, & Hermans, Common and distinct neural substrates during event 2009). Such training programmes may also benefit construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45, PTSD patients. The findings from this study 13631377. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic suggest that teaching individuals to generate and statistical manual of mental disorders (revised more internal details may be particularly useful 4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric in this population. Furthermore, future interven- Association. tions might seek to help PTSD patients to Apfel, B. A., Ross, J., Hlavin, J., Meyerhoff, D. J., Metzler, T. J., Marmar, C. R., ... Neylan, T. C. generate future events with greater episodic de- (2011). Hippocampal volume differences in Gulf tail. Recent work by Brown, Dorfman, Marmar, War veterans with current versus lifetime post- and Bryant (2012) found that episodic specificity traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Biological Psy- of future events predicted performance on two chiatry, 69, 541548. indices of a social problem-solving task, which if Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd ed. generalisable to PTSD, might aid in recovery. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. A number of limitations must be acknowledged. Blake, D. D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, First, as alluded to earlier, we can only speculate D. G., Gusman, F. D., Charney, D. S., & Keane, T. M. EPISODIC SPECIFICITY IN PTSD 603

(1995). The development of a clinician-administered Findings in neuropsychiatric disorders. Molecular PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8,7590. Psychiatry, 10, 160184. Blix, I., & Brennen, T. (2011). Mental time travel after Golier, J. A., Yehuda, R., Lupien, S., Harvey, P. D., trauma: The specificity and temporal distribution of Grossman, R., & Elkin, A. (2002). Memory perfor- autobiographical memories and future-directed mance in Holocaust survivors with post-traumatic thoughts. Memory, 19, 956967. stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, Bremner, J. D. (2007). Neuroimaging in post-traumatic 16821688. stress disorder and other stress-related disorders. Haque, S., & Conway, M. A. (2001). Sampling the Neuroimaging Clinics of North America, 17, 5234 process of autobiographical memory construction. 5538. The European Journal of , 13, Brewin, C. R. (2007). Autobiographical memory for 529547. trauma: Update on four controversies. Memory, 15, Harvey, A. G., Bryant, R. A., & Dang, S. T. (1998). 227248. Autobiographical memory in acute stress disorder. Brown, A. D., Dorfman, M. L., Marmar, C. R., & Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, Bryant, R. A. (2012). The impact of perceived 500506. self-efficacy on mental time travel and social pro- Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing blem solving. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cog- 299306. nitive Science, 11, 299306. Brown, A. D., Root, J. C., Romano, T. A., Chang, L. J., Hoge, C. W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J. L., Cox, A., Bryant, R. A., & Hirst, W. (2013). Overgeneralized Engel, C. C., & Castro, C. A. (2008). Mild traumatic autobiographgical memory and future thinking in brain injury in U.S. soldiers returning from Iraq. combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 453463. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., & Raye, Psychiatry, 44, 129134. C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of mem- Bryant, R. A., Sutherland, K. G., & Guthrie, R. M. ories for perceived and imagined autobiographical (2007). Impaired specific autobiographical memory events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Gen- as a risk factor for post-traumatic stress after eral, 117, 371376. trauma. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, Kangas, M., Henry, J. L., & Bryant, R. A. (2005). A 837841. prospective study of autobiographical memory and Clark, J. M., & Paivio, A. (2004). Extensions of the post-traumatic stress disorder following cancer. Paivio, Yuille, and Madigan (1968) norms. Behavior Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 36, 293299. 371383. Karl, A., Schaefer, M., Malta, L. S., Do¨ rfel, D., Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The Rohleder, N., & Werner, A. (2006). A meta-analysis construction of autobiographical memories in the of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD. Neu- self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, roscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 10041031. 261288. Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Crovitz, H. F., & Schiffman, H. (1974). Frequency of Memory and temporal experience: The effects of episodic memories as a function of their age. episodic memory loss on an amnesic patient’s ability Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 517518. to remember the past and imagine the future, Social D’Argembeau, A., & van der Linden, M. (2004). Cognition, 20, 353379. Phenomenal characteristics associated with project- Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 ing oneself back into the past and forward into the Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical future: Influence of valence and temporal distance. memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 844858. retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17, 677689. Felmingham, K., Williams, L. M., Whitford, T. J., McNally, R. J. (2003). Remembering trauma. Cam- Falconer, E., Kemp, A. H., Peduto, A., & Bryant, bridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University R. A. (2009). Duration of post-traumatic stress Press. disorder predicts hippocampal grey matter loss. McNally, R. J., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., & Pitman, NeuroReport, 20, 14021406. R. K. (1995). Autobiographical memory disturbance Foa, E. B., Molnar, C., & Cashman, L. (1995). Change in combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder. in rape narratives during exposure therapy for Post- Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 619630. traumatic Stress Disorder. Journal of Traumatic McNally, R. J., Litz, B. T., Prassas, A., Shin, L. M., & Stress, 8, 675690. Weathers, F. W. (1994). Emotional of Gaesser, B., Sacchetti, D. C., Addis, D. R., & Schacter, autobiographical memory in post-traumatic stress D. L. (2011). Characterising age-related changes in disorder. Cognition& , 8, 351367. remembering the past and imagining the future. Moore, S. A., & Zoellner, L. A. (2007). Overgeneral Psychology and Aging, 26, 8084. autobiographical memory and traumatic events: Geuze, E., Vermetten, E., & Bremner, J.D. (2005a). An evaluative review, Psychological Bulletin, 133, MR-based in vivo hippocampal volumetrics: 1. 419437. Review of methodologies currently employed. Neshat-Doost, H. T., Dalgleish, T., Yule, W., Kalantari, Molecular Psychiatry, 10, 147159. M., Ahmadi, S., Dyregrov, A., & Jobson, L. (2013). Geuze, E., Vermetten, E., & Bremner JD. (2005b). Enhancing autobiographical memory specificity MR-based in vivo hippocampal volumetrics: 2. through cognitive training: An intervention for 604 BROWN ET AL.

depression translated from basic science. Clinical Sumner, J. (2012). The mechanisms underlying over- Psychological Science, 1,8492. general autobiographical memory: An evaluative Polak, A. R., Witteveen, A. B., Reitsma, J. B., & Olff, review of evidence for the CaR-FA-X model. M. (2012). The role of executive function in post- Clinical Psychology Review, 32,3448. traumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. Sutherland, K., & Bryant, R. A. (2007). Rumination Journal of Affective Disorders, 141,1121. and overgeneral autobiographical memory. Beha- Porter, S., & Birt, A. R. (2001). Is traumatic memory viour Research and Therapy, 45, 24072416. special? A comparison of traumatic memory char- Sutherland, K. G., & Bryant, R. A. (2008). Social acteristics with memory for other emotional life problem solving and autobiographical memory in experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, post-traumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research 101117. and Therapy, 46, 154161. Race, E., Keane, M. M., & Verfaellie, M. (2011). Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic future thought: An Medial temporal lobe damage causes deficits in emerging concept. Perspectives on Psychological episodic memory and episodic future thinking not Science, 5, 142162. attributable to deficits in narrative construction. Thomsen, D. K., Schnieber, A., & Olesen, M. H. (2011). Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1026210269. Rumination is associated with the phenomenal Raes, F., Williams, J. M. G., & Hermans, D. (2009). characteristics of autobiographical memories and Reducing cognitive vulnerability to depression: A future scenarios. Memory, 19, 574584. preliminary investigation of memory specificity Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In training(MEST) in patients with depressive sympto- E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of matology. Journal of Behavior Therapy, 40,2438. memory (pp. 381402). New York, NY: Academic Rubin, D. C., Berntsen, D., & Bohni, M. K. (2008). A Press. memory-based model of post-traumatic stress dis- Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Cana- order: Evaluating basic assumptions underlying the dian Psychologist, 25,112. PTSD diagnosis. Psychological Review, 115, 985 Wenzel, A., Jackson, L. C., & Holt, C. S. (2002). Social 1011. phobia and the recall of autobiographical memories. Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive Depression and Anxiety, 15, 186189. neuroscience of constructive memory: Remember- Wilhelm, S., McNally, R. J., Baer, L., & Florin, I. (1997). ing the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Autobiographical memory in obsessive-compulsive Transactions of the Royal Society (B), 362, 773786. disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). 2131. Episodic simulation of future events: Concepts, data, Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., Healy, H., and applications. Annals of the New York Academy Rose, G., & MacLeod, A. K. (1996). The specificity of Sciences, 1124,3960. of autobiographical memory and imageability of the Squire, L. R., van der Horst, A. S., McDuff, S. G. R., future. Memory & Cognition, 24, 116125. Frascino, J. C., Hopkinse, R. O., & Mauldin, K. N. Williams, J. K. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Hermans, (2010). Role of the hippocampus in remembering D., Raes, F., & Watkins, E. (2007). Autobiographical the past and imagining the future. Proceedings of the memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psycho- National Academy of Sciences of the United States of logical Bulletin, 133, 122148. America, 107, 1904419048. Yehuda, R., Tischler, T., Golier, J. A., Grossman, R., Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolu- Brand, S. R., Kaufman, S., & Harvey, P. D. (2006). tion of foresight: What is mental time travel and is it Longitudinal assessment of cognitive performance

Downloaded by [Harvard Library] at 13:23 03 June 2014 unique to humans? The Behavioral Brain Sciences, in Holocaust survivors with and without PTSD. 30, 299313. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 714721.