Temporally-Specific Retrograde Amnesia in Two Cases of Discrete

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Temporally-Specific Retrograde Amnesia in Two Cases of Discrete HIPPOCAMPUS 9:247–254 (1999) Temporally-Specific Retrograde Amnesia in Two Cases of Discrete Bilateral Hippocampal Pathology Narinder Kapur1,2* and David J. Brooks3 1Wessex Neurological Centre, Southampton General Hospital, Southampton, England 2Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, England 3MRC Cyclotron Unit, Hammersmith Hospital, London, England ABSTRACT: The role of the hippocampus in retrograde amnesia re- tion.’’ As Squire and Alvarez (1995, p 173) have pointed mains controversial and poorly understood. Two cases are reported of out, data relating to the severity and pattern of retro- discrete bilateral hippocampal damage, one of which was a rare case of grade amnesia have implications for theories of memory limbic encephalitis secondary to the human herpes virus 6. Detailed memory testing showed marked anterograde memory impairment, but consolidation. They concluded that ‘‘the facts of tempo- only mild, temporally-limited retrograde amnesia that covered a period of rally-graded retrograde amnesia, and virtually all ac- several years in both autobiographical and factual knowledge domains. counts of this phenomenon that involve the concept of The absence of extensive retrograde amnesia in these two cases points to a memory consolidation, suggest that memory storage and time-limited role for the hippocampus in the retrieval of retrograde memories, and suggests that entorhinal, perirhinal, parahippocampal, or retrieval come eventually to be supported by neocortex neocortical areas of the temporal lobe may be more critical than the as the result of gradual changes in connectivity within hippocampus proper for long-term retrograde memory functioning. Our neocortex.’’ findings offer general support to theories of memory consolidation that Human lesion studies have offered some clues as to propose a gradual transfer of memory from hippocampal to neocortical the role of the hippocampus in retrograde memory, dependency. Hippocampus 1999; 9:247–254. ௠ 1999 Wiley-Liss, Inc. although many of the published studies suffer from some KEY WORDS: human herpes 6 virus; limbic encephalitis; remote limitations. Investigations relating to the patient HM memory have variously reported a mild retrograde amnesia of 1–2 years to a retrograde amnesia of up to 13 years. As has The role of the hippocampus and related structures in retrograde been pointed out elsewhere (e.g., Ogden and Corkin, memory functioning has only recently become the focus of research in 1991; Kapur et al., 1994a), there are some difficulties in cognitive neuroscience. The status of retrograde amnesia in the human interpreting HM’s retrograde memory performance. In amnesic syndrome and its neural basis is more uncertain and controversial addition, his lesion is not restricted to the hippocampus than the status of anterograde amnesia. As noted by Hodges (1995, p 101) (Corkin et al., 1997), and so his case study has a limited in a recent review, ‘‘Much has been learnt, but uncertainty remains in many amount to offer with regard to our understanding of the areas.’’ While the hippocampus has always been strongly implicated in role of the hippocampus in retrograde memory function- memory formation, there has been a greater emphasis on neocortical or ing. hippocampal-neocortical interaction in the case of retrieval of stored Schnider et al. (1994) reported dense anterograde and memories (Eichenbaum, 1997). In his classic paper, Squire (1992, p retrograde amnesia in their patient who suffered bilateral 222–223) concluded on the basis of evidence up to that point, ‘‘The facts of infarction of the hippocampi, with extension of the retrograde amnesia can be summarized as follows: 1. When damage is infarct on the left side to more lateral structures, limited to the CA1 region of human hippocampus, retrograde amnesia is including the parahippocampal gyri and neocortex of limited to a period of a year or two at the most. 2. In patients with more the posterior temporal/anterior occipital lobe. Although complete damage to the hippocampal formation, retrograde amnesia can be formal retrograde memory test data were not reported, extensive and temporally graded across a decade or more, with sparing of the memory loss for pre-illness information appeared to very old memories. 3. Hippocampal damage causes retrograde amnesia for equally affect autobiographical and semantic memories, both factual information and autobiographical, event-specific informa- and to extend back to the patient’s childhood with little in the way of a temporal gradient. The patient also had a significant naming deficit, and there may have been Grant sponsor: Wellcome Trust; Grant number: 045045. some contribution of this deficit to part of his retrograde *Correspondence to: Professor N. Kapur, Wessex Neurological Centre, Southampton General Hospital, Southampton, SO16 6YD England. amnesia. Lee et al. (1992) reported a 4-month period of E-mail: [email protected] autobiographical retrograde amnesia in their patient Accepted for publication 3 March 1999 who suffered an amnesic attack associated with a tempo- ௠ 1999 WILEY-LISS, INC. 248 KAPUR AND BROOKS ral lobe seizure. The anterograde memory impairment in this subtle histological changes in other areas or remote hypoactivation patient was more variable than in a classical amnesic syndrome. It effects of the hippocampal infarcts, particularly with regard to is of note that an MR scan carried out several days after the adjacent cortical areas and the mammillo-thalamic system. A apparent resolution of the retrograde amnesia showed bilateral more recent PET study of this patient (Kapur et al., 1999) showed hippocampal lesions, with no visible extension into more lateral additional pathology in the right thalamus, and so the role of the structures. hippocampal damage in this patient’s retrograde amnesia remains In a later study, Schnider et al. (1995) found that their patient, uncertain. a 55-year-old farmer with bilateral hippocampal lesions who was In light of the above studies, it is clear that further evidence different from the case reported by Schnider et al. (1994), was needs to be gathered from well-documented cases of discrete unaware of personal events over the previous 10–15 years. He hippocampal lesions in order to clarify the role of the hippocam- recognized the photographs of cattle that he owned more than 15 pus in retrograde memory function. Although such patients are years ago, but not more recent ones. He was also reported to have rare, we were able to study two such cases whose hippocampal a temporally-graded retrograde amnesia for politicians who had lesions could be verified using advanced brain imaging proce- become prominent over the previous 10–15 years. The lesions dures. One of these cases was an instance of paraneoplastic appeared to be restricted to swelling of both hippocampi, though encephalitis induced by the human herpes virus 6 in a patient who it is difficult to be certain that there was no swelling of adjacent was immune-compromised due to a bone marrow transplant hippocampal formation structures. The aetiology of the lesion is procedure (Drobyski et al., 1994). uncertain, though the primary medical condition of the patient was systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE). The relatively sudden onset of the amnesia suggests a vascular aetiology, though the authors themselves suggested a pathology related to an autoim- CASE 1 mune process. Reported cerebral complications of SLE tend to be more diffuse than selective hippocampal pathology of the type BE (dob March 24, 1946) is a right-handed man who worked reported here, and there remains the possibility of additional as a college lecturer. His past medical history was unremarkable lesions in this case that were not detected on MRI. A further note except for mild symptoms in the early 1980s related to a possible of caution is that the patient was a farmer, and it is not clear the diagnosis of rheumatoid arthritis. These symptoms resolved with a extent to which he was interested in public events of the type that change in dietary regime, and he only took medication for a short were used as part of the retrograde amnesia assessment; no formal period of time. He developed a viral encephalitis in May 1991, test scores or control data were reported in this respect. which was accompanied by acute memory loss for recent events, Reed and Squire (1998) found extensive episodic and factual an odd sense of smell, and episodes of de´ja` vu. In the days retrograde amnesia following hippocampal pathology only when following admission, he had two grand mal seizures. There were that pathology was accompanied by lesions in additional temporal no positive findings to suggest a herpes simplex virus, though lobe neocortex and not when it was confined to the hippocampus antiviral treatment was given. A CT scan on the day of admission alone. These patients with extensive retrograde amnesia had was reported as normal, as was a routine MRI scan carried out 11 lesions associated with herpes simplex encephalitis. Mattioli et al. days after admission. EEG investigations around this time showed (1996) reported FDG (fluorodeoxyglucose) PET scan data in a bilateral temporal lobe abnormalities. case of focal retrograde amnesia, and found evidence of bilateral In May 1992, a further MRI scan was carried out using a hippocampal and anterior cingulate glucose metabolic values. The specially designed protocol (described in Kapur et al., 1994b) that resolution of their scan did not permit the more detailed gave detailed views of memory-related brain structures. The scans anatomical fractionation of the lesion. were independently reviewed by two neuroradiologists, who were In one of the few post-mortem studies of amnesic patients blind to the neuropsychological test data. The scans were (Rempel-Clower et al., 1996, p 5252), the authors concluded that interpreted as showing discrete bilateral lesions (Fig. 1) that were ‘‘temporally-graded retrograde amnesia covering 15 years or more restricted to the hippocampus (due to some degree of artefact on can occur after relatively circumscribed damage to the hippocam- axial slices, only coronal images are shown here).
Recommended publications
  • Lesions of Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cortex That Spare the Amygdala and Hippocampal Formation Produce Severe Memory Impairment
    The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1989, 9(12): 4355-4370 Lesions of Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cortex That Spare the Amygdala and Hippocampal Formation Produce Severe Memory Impairment Stuart Zola-Morgan,’ Larry Ft. Squire,’ David G. Amaral,2 and Wendy A. Suzuki2J Veterans Administration Medical Center, San Diego, California, 92161, and Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, The Salk Institute, San Diego, California 92136, and 3Group in Neurosciences, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093 In monkeys, bilateral damage to the medial temporal region Moss, 1984). (In this notation, H refers to the hippocampus, A produces severe memory impairment. This lesion, which in- to the amygdala, and the plus superscript (+) to the cortical cludes the hippocampal formation, amygdala, and adjacent tissue adjacent to each structure.) This lesion appears to con- cortex, including the parahippocampal gyrus (the H+A+ le- stitute an animal model of medial temporal lobe amnesia like sion), appears to constitute an animal model of human me- that exhibited by the well-studied patient H.M. (Scoville and dial temporal lobe amnesia. Reexamination of histological Milner, 1957). material from previously studied monkeys with H+A+ lesions The H+A+ lesion produces greater memory impairment than indicated that the perirhinal cortex had also sustained sig- a lesion limited to the hippocampal formation and parahip- nificant damage. Furthermore, recent neuroanatomical stud- pocampal cortex-the H+ lesion (Mishkin, 1978; Mahut et al., ies show that the perirhinal cortex and the closely associated 1982; Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1985, 1986; Zola-Morgan et al., parahippocampal cortex provide the major source of cortical 1989a).
    [Show full text]
  • Anterograde and Retrograde Amnesia in Rats with Large Hippocampal Lesions
    HIPPOCAMPUS 11:18–26 (2001) Anterograde and Retrograde Amnesia in Rats With Large Hippocampal Lesions Gordon Winocur,1–4* Robert M. McDonald,3 and Morris Moscovitch1,5 1Department of Psychology, Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 2Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, Ontario, Canada 3Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 4Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 5Department of Psychology, Erindale College, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada ABSTRACT: A test of socially acquired food preferences was used to study durable representation, available for future recall (Mil- the effects of large lesions to the hippocampal formation (HPC) on antero- ner, 1966; Squire, 1992). Two related predictions follow grade and retrograde memory in rats. In the anterograde test, rats with HPC from this position. The first pertains to anterograde lesions normally acquired the food preference but showed a faster rate of forgetting than control groups. When the food preference was acquired memory and asserts that hippocampal damage will result preoperatively, HPC groups exhibited a temporally graded retrograde amne- in impaired memory for events after long delays (long- sia in which memory was impaired when the preference was acquired within term memory, LTM), but not after relatively short delays 2 days of surgery but not at longer delays. The results support the traditional (short-term memory, STM). The rationale is that, in theory that the HPC contributes to the consolidation of newly acquired information into a durable memory trace that is represented in other brain STM, information is held and recalled for a brief period areas.
    [Show full text]
  • The Parahippocampal Region and Object Identification
    The Parahippocampal Region and Object Identification E.A. MURRAY,a T.J. BUSSEY, R.R. HAMPTON, AND L.M. SAKSIDA Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892, USA ABSTRACT: The hippocampus has long been thought to be critical for memory, including memory for objects. However, recent neuropsychological studies in nonhuman primates have indicated that other regions within the medial tem- poral lobe, specifically, structures in the parahippocampal region, are prima- rily responsible for object recognition and object identification. This article reviews the behavioral effects of removal of structures within the parahippo- campal region in monkeys, and cites relevant work in rodents as well. It is ar- gued that the perirhinal cortex, in particular, contributes to object identification in at least two ways: (i) by serving as the final stage in the ventral visual cortical pathway that represents stimulus features, and (ii) by operating as part of a network for associating together sensory inputs within and across sensory modalities. INTRODUCTION This article reviews the contributions of the parahippocampal region to learning and memory in nonhuman primates, with special emphasis on its role in object iden- tification: the knowledge that a particular object or class of objects is one and the same across the different instances in which it is experienced. In macaque monkeys, the parahippocampal region consists of three main cortical fields, located on the ven- tromedial aspect of the temporal lobe: entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, and para- hippocampal cortex (see FIG. 1). Currently, much more information is available regarding the contributions of the perirhinal cortex to learning and memory than for either the entorhinal cortex or parahippocampal cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroimage
    NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroimage. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 January 01. Published in final edited form as: Neuroimage. 2013 January 1; 64C: 32–42. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.071. Predicting the Location of Human Perirhinal Cortex, Brodmann's area 35, from MRI $watermark-text $watermark-text $watermark-text Jean C. Augustinacka,#, Kristen E. Hubera, Allison A. Stevensa, Michelle Roya, Matthew P. Froschb, André J.W. van der Kouwea, Lawrence L. Walda, Koen Van Leemputa,f, Ann McKeec, Bruce Fischla,d,e, and The Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative* aAthinoula A Martinos Center, Dept. of Radiology, MGH, 149 13th Street, Charlestown MA 02129 USA bC.S. Kubik Laboratory for Neuropathology, Pathology Service, MGH, 55 Fruit St., Boston MA 02115 USA cDepartment of Pathology, Boston University School of Medicine, Bedford Veterans Administration Medical Center, MA 01730 USA dMIT Computer Science and AI Lab, Cambridge MA 02139 USA eMIT Division of Health Sciences and Technology, Cambridge MA 02139 USA fDepartment of Informatics and Mathematical Modeling, Technical University of Denmark, Copenhagen, Denmark Abstract The perirhinal cortex (Brodmann's area 35) is a multimodal area that is important for normal memory function. Specifically, perirhinal cortex is involved in detection of novel objects and manifests neurofibrillary tangles in Alzheimer's disease very early in disease progression. We scanned ex vivo brain hemispheres at standard resolution (1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm) to construct pial/white matter surfaces in FreeSurfer and scanned again at high resolution (120 μm × 120 μm × 120 μm) to determine cortical architectural boundaries. After labeling perirhinal area 35 in the high resolution images, we mapped the high resolution labels to the surface models to localize area 35 in fourteen cases.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects on Visual Recognition of Combined and Separate Ablations of the Entorhinal and Perirhinal Cortex in Rhesus Monkeys
    The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1993, 13(12): 5418-5432 Effects on Visual Recognition of Combined and Separate Ablations of the Entorhinal and Perirhinal Cortex in Rhesus Monkeys M. Meunier,” J. Bachevalier,b M. Mishkin, and E. A. Murray Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892 Performance on visual delayed nonmatching-to-sample was amygdalectomy or hippocampectomy. Monkeys with the amyg- assessed in rhesus monkeys with combined and separate dala plus rhinal cortex ablations (A+Rh) were found to be just ablations of the perirhinal and entorhinal cortex, as well as as severely impaired as those with the completeremoval, where- in unoperated controls. Combined (i.e., rhinal cortex) lesions as monkeys with the hippocampus plus rhinal cortex ablations yielded a striking impairment on this task, one almost as (H+Rh) were impaired far less,and, indeed, no more than those severe as that seen after combined amygdalohippocampal with hippocampectomy alone. It was therefore concluded that removals that included some of this subjacent cortex (Mish- a rhinal cortex lesionwas insufficient by itself to produce a severe kin, 1978; Murray and Mishkin, 1984). Ablations of the peri- memory impairment, and that its effect when combined with rhinal cortex alone produced a deficit nearly as severe as amygdalectomy was due to disconnection of the hippocampus that found after rhinal cortex lesions, whereas ablations of from visual input, thereby resulting in a functional A+H lesion. the entorhinal cortex
    [Show full text]
  • Borders and Cytoarchitecture of the Perirhinal and Postrhinal Cortices in the Rat
    THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 437:17–41 (2001) Borders and Cytoarchitecture of the Perirhinal and Postrhinal Cortices in the Rat REBECCA D. BURWELL* Department of Psychology, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island 02912 ABSTRACT Cytoarchitectonic and histochemical analyses were carried out for perirhinal areas 35 and 36 and the postrhinal cortex, providing the first detailed cytoarchitectonic study of these regions in the rat brain. The rostral perirhinal border with insular cortex is at the extreme caudal limit of the claustrum, consistent with classical definitions of insular cortex dating back to Rose ([1928] J. Psychol. Neurol. 37:467–624). The border between the perirhinal and postrhinal cortices is at the caudal limit of the angular bundle, as previously proposed by Burwell et al. ([1995] Hip- pocampus 5:390–408). The ventral borders with entorhinal cortex are consistent with the Insausti et al. ([1997] Hippocampus 7:146–183) description of that region and the Dolorfo and Amaral ([1998] J. Comp. Neurol. 398:25–48) connectional findings. Regarding the remaining borders, both the perirhinal and postrhinal cortices encroach upon temporal cortical regions as defined by others (e.g., Zilles [1990] The cerebral cortex of the rat, p 77–112; Paxinos and Watson [1998] The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates). Based on cytoarchitectonic and histochemical criteria, perirhinal areas 35 and 36 and the postrhinal cortex were further subdivided. Area 36 was parceled into three subregions, areas 36d, 36v, and 36p. Area 35 was parceled into two cytoarchitectonically distinctive subregions, areas 35d and 35v. The postrhinal cortex was di- vided into two subregions, areas PORd and PORv.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perirhinal, Entorhinal, and Parahippocampal Cortices and 1 9 Hippocampus: an Overview of Functional Anatomy and Protocol for Their Segmentation in MR Images
    The Perirhinal, Entorhinal, and Parahippocampal Cortices and 1 9 Hippocampus: An Overview of Functional Anatomy and Protocol for Their Segmentation in MR Images Sasa L. Kivisaari, Alphonse Probst, and Kirsten I. Taylor Abbreviations fi Fimbria gA Gyrus ambiens A Anterior gS Gyrus of Schwalbe Ab Angular bundle (PHg white matter) HB Hippocampal body aCf Anterior calcarine fi ssure Hf Hippocampal fi ssure al Alveus HH Hippocampal head Am Amygdala Hs Hippocampal sulcus bG Band of Giacomini HT Hippocampal tail cf Crus of the fornix I Inferior Cs Collateral sulcus ILg Intralimbic gyrus di Hippocampal digitations Is Isthmus ERc Entorhinal cortex ITg Inferotemporal gyrus Fg Fusiform gyrus L Laterial Lg Lingual gyrus li-gm Limen insulae gray matter S. L. Kivisaari , Ph.D. (*) li-wm Limen insulae white matter Department of Geriatrics , M Medial Memory Clinic, University Hospital Basel , Schanzenstrasse 55, CH-4031 , Basel , Switzerland Mb Mammillary body e-mail: [email protected] MTL Medial temporal lobe A. Probst , M.D. OTs Occipitotemporal sulcus Department of Geriatrics , P Posterior Memory Clinic, University Hospital Basel , Pu Pulvinar Schanzenstrasse 55, CH-4031 , Basel , Switzerland PHc Parahippocampal cortex Department of Neuropathology , PHg Parahippocampal gyrus University Hospital Basel , PRc Perirhinal cortex Schönbeinstrasse 40, CH-4031 , Basel , Switzerland e-mail: [email protected] qgc Quadrigeminal cistern Rs Rhinal sulcus K. I. Taylor , Ph.D. Department of Geriatrics , S Superior Memory Clinic, University Hospital Basel , SAs Semiannular sulcus Schanzenstrasse 55, CH-4031 , Basel , Switzerland SLg Semilunar gyrus Department of Experimental Psychology , Sp Splenium Centre for Speech Language and the Brain, su Subiculum University of Cambridge , TLV Temporal horn of lateral ventricle Downing Street, Cambridge , CB2 3EB , UK e-mail: [email protected] TP Temporal pole S.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perirhinal Cortex
    NE37CH03-Suzuki ARI 26 May 2014 6:59 The Perirhinal Cortex Wendy A. Suzuki1 and Yuji Naya2 1Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003; email: [email protected] 2Department of Psychology, Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences and PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2014. 37:39–53 Keywords The Annual Review of Neuroscience is online at association memory, declarative memory, medial temporal lobe neuro.annualreviews.org by Peking University on 07/21/14. For personal use only. This article’s doi: Abstract 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014207 Anatomically, the perirhinal cortex sits at the boundary between the medial Copyright c 2014 by Annual Reviews. temporal lobe and the ventral visual pathway. It has prominent interconnec- Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2014.37:39-53. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org All rights reserved tions not only with both these systems, but also with a wide range of unimodal and polymodal association areas. Consistent with these diverse projections, neurophysiological studies reveal a multidimensional set of mnemonic sig- nals that include stimulus familiarity, within- and between-domain associ- ations, associative recall, and delay-based persistence. This wide range of perirhinal memory signals not only includes signals that are largely unique to the perirhinal cortex (i.e., object familiarity), consistent with dual-process theories, but also includes a range of signals (i.e., associative flexibility and recall) that are strongly associated with the hippocampus, consistent with single-process theories. These neurophysiological findings have important implications for bridging the gap between single-process and dual-process models of medial temporal lobe function.
    [Show full text]
  • Temporally Graded Retrograde Amnesia Following Separate and Combined Lesions of the Perirhinal Cortex and Fornix in the Rat Kjesten A
    Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on September 27, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press Temporally Graded Retrograde Amnesia Following Separate and Combined Lesions of the Perirhinal Cortex and Fornix in the Rat Kjesten A. Wiig, 1'2 Leon N. Cooper, ~'3 and Mark F. Bear 4'5 1Institute for Brain and Neural Systems and Departments of 2Neur0science and 3Physics 4H0ward Hughes Medical Institute Brown University Providence, Rhode Island 02912 Abstract 2-week retention interval than control rats. These results suggest that medial temporal The involvement of the perirhinal structures including the perirhinal cortex cortex and the fornix in retrograde and and the fornix are involved in the anterograde amnesia in the rat was consolidation of mnemonic information investigated in this experiment. Male and that their involvement in this process Sprague-Dawley rats were trained on a occurs over a discrete period of time. series of five visual discrimination problems at distinct time intervals prior to receiving bilateral, electrolytic lesions of the perirhinal cortex or the fornix, combined Introduction lesions of both these structures, or sham operations. Following recovery from Throughout the latter half of this century, surgery, rats were retested on the there has been considerable interest in determin- preoperatively learned discrimination ing how memories are encoded and stored within problems, as well as learning a new the mammalian brain. Although the precise mech- discrimination and discrimination reversal. anisms and loci of information storage have re- Results indicated that all animals with mained elusive, some progress has been made to- lesions exhibited temporally graded ward identifying both the brain structures in- retrograde amnesia, whereby memories volved in memory consolidation and the time acquired in the recent past (1-3 weeks) course or duration of this process.
    [Show full text]
  • The Rat Perirhinal Cortex: a Review of Anatomy, Physiology, Plasticity, and Function
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by MURAL - Maynooth University Research Archive Library Progress in Neurobiology 93 (2011) 522–548 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Progress in Neurobiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/pneurobio The rat perirhinal cortex: A review of anatomy, physiology, plasticity, and function John Kealy, Sean Commins * Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland Maynooth, Maynooth, Co. Kildare, Ireland ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Article history: The perirhinal cortex is located in a pivotal position to influence the flow of information into and out of Received 19 April 2010 the hippocampal formation. In this review, we examine the anatomical, physiological and functional Received in revised form 28 January 2011 properties of the rat perirhinal cortex. Firstly, we review the properties of the perirhinal cortex itself, we Accepted 10 March 2011 describe how it can be separated into two distinct subregions and consider how it differs from other Available online 21 March 2011 neighbouring regions in terms of cell type, cellular organisation and its afferent and efferent projections. We review the forms of neurotransmission present in the perirhinal cortex and the morphological, Keywords: electrophysiological and plastic properties of its neurons. Secondly, we review the perirhinal cortex in Perirhinal cortex the context of its connections with other brain areas; focussing on the projections to cortical, subcortical Hippocampal formation Anatomy and hippocampal/parahippocampal regions. Particular attention is paid the anatomical and Physiology electrophysiological properties of these projections. Thirdly, we review the main functions of the Electrophysiology perirhinal cortex; its roles in perception, recognition memory, spatial and contextual memory and fear Perception conditioning are explored.
    [Show full text]
  • Lesions of the Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cottices in the Monkey Produce Long-Lasting Memory Impairment in the Visual and Tactual Modalities
    The Journal of Neuroscience, June 1993, 13(6): 2430-2451 Lesions of the Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cottices in the Monkey Produce Long-lasting Memory Impairment in the Visual and Tactual Modalities Wendy A. Suzuki, I,* Stuart Zola-Morgan,1~3 Larry R. Squire,lv3 and David G. AmaraP* ‘Group in Neurosciences, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, “The Salk Institute, San Diego, California 92186, and 3Veterans Affairs Medical Center, San Diego, California 92161 and Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093 Compared to normal animals, monkeys with bilateral lesions man amnesia,the deficit associatedwith H+A+ lesionsin mon- of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices (PRPH lesion) keys is multimodal (Murray and Mishkin, 1984) and long-last- were impaired on both a visual and a tactual version of the ing (Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1985). In addition, animals with delayed nonmatching to sample task. In addition, the mem- H+A+ lesionsexhibit preserved short-term memory (Overman ory deficit was long-lasting, as indicated by the finding of a et al., 1990) intact skill-based memory, and intact habit-like significant deficit when the visual version of the delayed forms of memory (Malamut et al., 1984;Zola-Morgan and Squire, nonmatching to sample task was readministered approxi- 1984) in analogy to the preserved memory abilities of human mately 2 years after surgery. Animals with PRPH lesions amnesicpatients (for reviews, seeSquire and Zola-Morgan, 1991; performed normally on discrimination tasks in the visual and Squire, 1992). tactual modalities. Multimodal and long-lasting memory im- Although the importance of the medial temporal lobe for pairments are defining characteristics of human medial tem- memory function hasbeen known for sometime, a critical issue poral lobe amnesia.
    [Show full text]
  • Perirhinal Cortex Represents Nonspatial, but Not Spatial, Information in Rats Foraging in the Presence of Objects: Comparison with Lateral Entorhinal Cortex
    HIPPOCAMPUS 22:2045–2058 (2012) Perirhinal Cortex Represents Nonspatial, But Not Spatial, Information in Rats Foraging in the Presence of Objects: Comparison With Lateral Entorhinal Cortex Sachin S. Deshmukh,1,3* Jeremy L. Johnson,1 and James J. Knierim1,2,3 ABSTRACT: The medial temporal lobe (MTL) is involved in mnemonic the hippocampus, with PRC being involved in non- processing. The perirhinal cortex (PRC) plays a role in object recogni- spatial memory and the hippocampus being involved tion memory, while the hippocampus is required for certain forms of spatial memory and episodic memory. The lateral entorhinal cortex in spatial memory (Ennaceur et al., 1996; Murray (LEC) receives direct projections from PRC and is one of the two major et al., 2007; for a historical perspective on inclusion cortical inputs to the hippocampus. The transformations that occur of PRC in the hippocampal ‘‘memory system’’, see between PRC and LEC neural representations are not well understood. Murray and Wise, 2012). On the other hand, there is Here, we show that PRC and LEC had similarly high proportions of neu- a longstanding debate about whether the hippocampus rons with object-related activity (PRC 52/94; LEC 72/153), as expected from their locations in the ‘‘what’’ pathway into the hippocampus. is specialized for encoding space, or whether space is However, LEC unit activity showed more spatial stability than PRC unit just one of the variables stored in a generalized mem- activity. A minority of LEC neurons showed stable spatial firing fields ory structure in the hippocampus (O’Keefe, 1999; away from objects; these firing fields strongly resembled hippocampal Shapiro and Eichenbaum, 1999).
    [Show full text]