Temporally Graded Retrograde Amnesia Following Separate and Combined Lesions of the Perirhinal Cortex and Fornix in the Rat Kjesten A

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Temporally Graded Retrograde Amnesia Following Separate and Combined Lesions of the Perirhinal Cortex and Fornix in the Rat Kjesten A Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on September 27, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press Temporally Graded Retrograde Amnesia Following Separate and Combined Lesions of the Perirhinal Cortex and Fornix in the Rat Kjesten A. Wiig, 1'2 Leon N. Cooper, ~'3 and Mark F. Bear 4'5 1Institute for Brain and Neural Systems and Departments of 2Neur0science and 3Physics 4H0ward Hughes Medical Institute Brown University Providence, Rhode Island 02912 Abstract 2-week retention interval than control rats. These results suggest that medial temporal The involvement of the perirhinal structures including the perirhinal cortex cortex and the fornix in retrograde and and the fornix are involved in the anterograde amnesia in the rat was consolidation of mnemonic information investigated in this experiment. Male and that their involvement in this process Sprague-Dawley rats were trained on a occurs over a discrete period of time. series of five visual discrimination problems at distinct time intervals prior to receiving bilateral, electrolytic lesions of the perirhinal cortex or the fornix, combined Introduction lesions of both these structures, or sham operations. Following recovery from Throughout the latter half of this century, surgery, rats were retested on the there has been considerable interest in determin- preoperatively learned discrimination ing how memories are encoded and stored within problems, as well as learning a new the mammalian brain. Although the precise mech- discrimination and discrimination reversal. anisms and loci of information storage have re- Results indicated that all animals with mained elusive, some progress has been made to- lesions exhibited temporally graded ward identifying both the brain structures in- retrograde amnesia, whereby memories volved in memory consolidation and the time acquired in the recent past (1-3 weeks) course or duration of this process. were impaired, and memories acquired in Much of our knowledge regarding the organi- the remote past (6-8 weeks) were spared. zation and neural foundations of normal memory There was no difference in the magnitude processes has been obtained from the study of the of retrograde amnesia between the three human amnesic syndrome. Patients who have suf- lesion groups. Animals in the perirhinal, fered damage to the medial temporal region of the fornix, and combined lesion groups were brain, including the hippocampus, amygdala, and able to learn a new discrimination problem surrounding parahippocampal cortical regions at a rate comparable to control rats; typically exhibit severe anterograde amnesia (an however, the animals with lesions were inability to form new memories) and a more vari- impaired at learning the discrimination able retrograde amnesia (loss of memories ac- reversal. The perirhinal, fornix, and quired prior to the precipitating incident). The combined lesion animals also exhibited a phenomenon of retrograde amnesia has had a par- significantly faster forgetting rate over a ticularly large impact on ideas regarding memory consolidation processes. Specifically, retrograde amnesia is often temporally graded, in that mem- SCorresponding author. ories acquired close to the time of the amnesia- LEARNING & MEMORY 3:313-325 9 1996 by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press ISSN1072-0502/96 $5.00 L E A R N I N G & M E M O R Y 313 Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on September 27, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press Wiig et al. inducing incident are lost, whereas memories formance on DNMS (Otto and Eichenbaum 1992; formed in the more distant past are spared. In hu- Mumby and Pinel 1994) and paired-associate tasks mans, memories formed several years prior to the (Bunsey and Eichenbaum 1993). onset of amnesia can be disrupted (Squire et al. Moreover, it has been suggested that the per- 1975; Corkin 1984). This observation suggests irhinal cortex alone may be responsible for much that memory is not fixed at the time of learning of the mnemonic processing required for adequate but, rather, changes and becomes more stable with performance on tasks such as the DNMS. It has the passage of time (McGaugh and Gold 1976). been demonstrated, for example, that lesions re- The development of primate and rodent mod- stricted to the perirhinal cortex in the monkey els of retrograde amnesia has helped to further result in a more severe memory impairment than delineate which medial temporal lobe structures lesions confined to the entorhinal cortex (Meu- are involved in memory consolidation processes. nier et al. 1993). Similarly, bilateral ablations of Much of this research has focused on the hippoc- the perirhinal cortex alone in the rat have been ampal formation, and despite differences in tasks shown to significantly disrupt performance on and species, similar patterns of results have been postoperatively acquired DNMS memory tasks observed across studies. In these experiments, an- (Wiig and Bilkey 1994b, 1995). imals were trained on a task at different time in- Given the importance of the perirhinal cortex tervals prior to receiving bilateral ablations of the to the retention of newly acquired information, it hippocampal formation (Squire and Spanis 1984; is of considerable interest to determine the extent Salmon et al. 1987; Wincour 1990; Zola-Morgan of retrograde amnesia exhibited by animals with and Squire 1990; Kim and Fanselow 1992; Cho et lesions of this area. The following experiment was al. 1995; Kim et al. 1995) or entorhinal cortex designed to compare the retrograde consequences (Cho et al. 1993, 1995; Cho and Kesner 1996). of lesions of the perirhinal cortex and the fornix. The results of these experiments indicated that Rats learned two-choice visual discrimination animals retained information that had been ac- problems at five different time intervals prior to quired in the remote past and lost information that receiving lesions of the perirhinal cortex, fornix, had been acquired more recently. The temporal or both these structures. Retention for the preop- gradient of the retrograde amnesia varied from 10 eratively learned discrimination problems was as- days (Wincour 1990) to several months (Salmon sessed immediately following recovery from sur- et al. 1987). Although it is generally accepted that gery. In addition, the ability of animals with lesions these results cannot be used to determine the time to learn a new discrimination problem and rever- course of memory consolidation, they do support sal and retain this information over a 2-week in- the idea of a gradually changing memory trace, terval was assessed. which is more susceptible to disruption in early phases of memory consolidation. Although the involvement of the hippocampal Materials and Methods formation in retrograde amnesia has been well characterized, little is known about the contribu- SUBJECTS tion of the surrounding medial temporal cortical Twenty-four male Sprague-Dawley rats, regions. Recently, it has been demonstrated that weighing between 150 and 180 grams at the start the rhinal cortex appears to play a particularly im- of behavioral training, served as subjects in this portant role in the retention of newly acquired experiment. Subjects were individually housed in information. It has, for example, been shown that wire-mesh cages and were maintained on a 12-hr- in the monkey, surgical ablations encompassing on, 12-hr-off light-dark cycle. Rats had free access either the perirhinal and parahippocampal corti- to water but were food deprived to 85% of their ces (Zola-Morgan et al. 1989; Suzuki et al. 1993), free-feeding body weight. or the perirhinal and entorhinal cortices (Eacott et al. 1994; Meunier et al. 1993; Murray et al. 1996) result in profound memory deficits on post- APPARATUS operatively acquired delayed-non-match-to-sample (DNMS) and complex discrimination tasks. Simi- The apparatus used in this experiment con- larly, combined lesions of the perirhinal and en- sisted of a perspex runway measuring 60 cm in torhinal cortices in the rat result in impaired per- length and 20 cm in width and with walls 50 cm in L E A R N I N G & M E M O R Y 314 Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on September 27, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press RETROGRADE AMNESIA AND PERIRHINAL CORTEX height (for a more detailed description of this ap- PROCEDURE paratus, see Mumby et al. 1990). At each end of the runway were two identical goal areas, which PREOPERATIVE TRAINING were separated from the central portion of the maze by two opaque perspex guillotine doors. The pretraining phase of the experiment con- Within each goal area were two food wells, which sisted of six 15-min sessions during which the rats were separated from each other by a short divid- were habituated to the apparatus. For the first two ing wall. Food pellets were delivered to the wells sessions, each rat was placed into the apparatus, via funnels that were mounted on the outside with both guillotine doors open, and allowed to walls of the apparatus and connected to the food explore for 15 min. Pellets were scattered liberally wells by means of plastic tubing. throughout the length of the runway and in the A collection of 10 junk objects differing from food-well area. Food wells were rebaited after the one another in size, color, shape, and texture pellets in each one had been consumed. served as stimuli. The objects were large enough For the remaining four habituation session, to cover a food well but were light enough to the rats were trained to run from end to end of the enable a rat to displace them. The food rewards apparatus. Each rat was initially placed in the cen- were 45-mg food pellets (BioServ). ter of the runway, with both guillotine doors closed. After --5 sec, one guillotine door was raised, allowing the rat access to the food wells. After the rat had eaten the pellets placed in both food wells and had begun to move toward the SURGERY center of the runway, the closed guillotine door All animals were anesthetized with sodium was raised.
Recommended publications
  • Lesions of Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cortex That Spare the Amygdala and Hippocampal Formation Produce Severe Memory Impairment
    The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1989, 9(12): 4355-4370 Lesions of Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cortex That Spare the Amygdala and Hippocampal Formation Produce Severe Memory Impairment Stuart Zola-Morgan,’ Larry Ft. Squire,’ David G. Amaral,2 and Wendy A. Suzuki2J Veterans Administration Medical Center, San Diego, California, 92161, and Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, The Salk Institute, San Diego, California 92136, and 3Group in Neurosciences, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093 In monkeys, bilateral damage to the medial temporal region Moss, 1984). (In this notation, H refers to the hippocampus, A produces severe memory impairment. This lesion, which in- to the amygdala, and the plus superscript (+) to the cortical cludes the hippocampal formation, amygdala, and adjacent tissue adjacent to each structure.) This lesion appears to con- cortex, including the parahippocampal gyrus (the H+A+ le- stitute an animal model of medial temporal lobe amnesia like sion), appears to constitute an animal model of human me- that exhibited by the well-studied patient H.M. (Scoville and dial temporal lobe amnesia. Reexamination of histological Milner, 1957). material from previously studied monkeys with H+A+ lesions The H+A+ lesion produces greater memory impairment than indicated that the perirhinal cortex had also sustained sig- a lesion limited to the hippocampal formation and parahip- nificant damage. Furthermore, recent neuroanatomical stud- pocampal cortex-the H+ lesion (Mishkin, 1978; Mahut et al., ies show that the perirhinal cortex and the closely associated 1982; Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1985, 1986; Zola-Morgan et al., parahippocampal cortex provide the major source of cortical 1989a).
    [Show full text]
  • Anterograde and Retrograde Amnesia in Rats with Large Hippocampal Lesions
    HIPPOCAMPUS 11:18–26 (2001) Anterograde and Retrograde Amnesia in Rats With Large Hippocampal Lesions Gordon Winocur,1–4* Robert M. McDonald,3 and Morris Moscovitch1,5 1Department of Psychology, Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 2Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, Ontario, Canada 3Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 4Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 5Department of Psychology, Erindale College, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada ABSTRACT: A test of socially acquired food preferences was used to study durable representation, available for future recall (Mil- the effects of large lesions to the hippocampal formation (HPC) on antero- ner, 1966; Squire, 1992). Two related predictions follow grade and retrograde memory in rats. In the anterograde test, rats with HPC from this position. The first pertains to anterograde lesions normally acquired the food preference but showed a faster rate of forgetting than control groups. When the food preference was acquired memory and asserts that hippocampal damage will result preoperatively, HPC groups exhibited a temporally graded retrograde amne- in impaired memory for events after long delays (long- sia in which memory was impaired when the preference was acquired within term memory, LTM), but not after relatively short delays 2 days of surgery but not at longer delays. The results support the traditional (short-term memory, STM). The rationale is that, in theory that the HPC contributes to the consolidation of newly acquired information into a durable memory trace that is represented in other brain STM, information is held and recalled for a brief period areas.
    [Show full text]
  • The Parahippocampal Region and Object Identification
    The Parahippocampal Region and Object Identification E.A. MURRAY,a T.J. BUSSEY, R.R. HAMPTON, AND L.M. SAKSIDA Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892, USA ABSTRACT: The hippocampus has long been thought to be critical for memory, including memory for objects. However, recent neuropsychological studies in nonhuman primates have indicated that other regions within the medial tem- poral lobe, specifically, structures in the parahippocampal region, are prima- rily responsible for object recognition and object identification. This article reviews the behavioral effects of removal of structures within the parahippo- campal region in monkeys, and cites relevant work in rodents as well. It is ar- gued that the perirhinal cortex, in particular, contributes to object identification in at least two ways: (i) by serving as the final stage in the ventral visual cortical pathway that represents stimulus features, and (ii) by operating as part of a network for associating together sensory inputs within and across sensory modalities. INTRODUCTION This article reviews the contributions of the parahippocampal region to learning and memory in nonhuman primates, with special emphasis on its role in object iden- tification: the knowledge that a particular object or class of objects is one and the same across the different instances in which it is experienced. In macaque monkeys, the parahippocampal region consists of three main cortical fields, located on the ven- tromedial aspect of the temporal lobe: entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, and para- hippocampal cortex (see FIG. 1). Currently, much more information is available regarding the contributions of the perirhinal cortex to learning and memory than for either the entorhinal cortex or parahippocampal cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroimage
    NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroimage. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 January 01. Published in final edited form as: Neuroimage. 2013 January 1; 64C: 32–42. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.071. Predicting the Location of Human Perirhinal Cortex, Brodmann's area 35, from MRI $watermark-text $watermark-text $watermark-text Jean C. Augustinacka,#, Kristen E. Hubera, Allison A. Stevensa, Michelle Roya, Matthew P. Froschb, André J.W. van der Kouwea, Lawrence L. Walda, Koen Van Leemputa,f, Ann McKeec, Bruce Fischla,d,e, and The Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative* aAthinoula A Martinos Center, Dept. of Radiology, MGH, 149 13th Street, Charlestown MA 02129 USA bC.S. Kubik Laboratory for Neuropathology, Pathology Service, MGH, 55 Fruit St., Boston MA 02115 USA cDepartment of Pathology, Boston University School of Medicine, Bedford Veterans Administration Medical Center, MA 01730 USA dMIT Computer Science and AI Lab, Cambridge MA 02139 USA eMIT Division of Health Sciences and Technology, Cambridge MA 02139 USA fDepartment of Informatics and Mathematical Modeling, Technical University of Denmark, Copenhagen, Denmark Abstract The perirhinal cortex (Brodmann's area 35) is a multimodal area that is important for normal memory function. Specifically, perirhinal cortex is involved in detection of novel objects and manifests neurofibrillary tangles in Alzheimer's disease very early in disease progression. We scanned ex vivo brain hemispheres at standard resolution (1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm) to construct pial/white matter surfaces in FreeSurfer and scanned again at high resolution (120 μm × 120 μm × 120 μm) to determine cortical architectural boundaries. After labeling perirhinal area 35 in the high resolution images, we mapped the high resolution labels to the surface models to localize area 35 in fourteen cases.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects on Visual Recognition of Combined and Separate Ablations of the Entorhinal and Perirhinal Cortex in Rhesus Monkeys
    The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1993, 13(12): 5418-5432 Effects on Visual Recognition of Combined and Separate Ablations of the Entorhinal and Perirhinal Cortex in Rhesus Monkeys M. Meunier,” J. Bachevalier,b M. Mishkin, and E. A. Murray Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892 Performance on visual delayed nonmatching-to-sample was amygdalectomy or hippocampectomy. Monkeys with the amyg- assessed in rhesus monkeys with combined and separate dala plus rhinal cortex ablations (A+Rh) were found to be just ablations of the perirhinal and entorhinal cortex, as well as as severely impaired as those with the completeremoval, where- in unoperated controls. Combined (i.e., rhinal cortex) lesions as monkeys with the hippocampus plus rhinal cortex ablations yielded a striking impairment on this task, one almost as (H+Rh) were impaired far less,and, indeed, no more than those severe as that seen after combined amygdalohippocampal with hippocampectomy alone. It was therefore concluded that removals that included some of this subjacent cortex (Mish- a rhinal cortex lesionwas insufficient by itself to produce a severe kin, 1978; Murray and Mishkin, 1984). Ablations of the peri- memory impairment, and that its effect when combined with rhinal cortex alone produced a deficit nearly as severe as amygdalectomy was due to disconnection of the hippocampus that found after rhinal cortex lesions, whereas ablations of from visual input, thereby resulting in a functional A+H lesion. the entorhinal cortex
    [Show full text]
  • Borders and Cytoarchitecture of the Perirhinal and Postrhinal Cortices in the Rat
    THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 437:17–41 (2001) Borders and Cytoarchitecture of the Perirhinal and Postrhinal Cortices in the Rat REBECCA D. BURWELL* Department of Psychology, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island 02912 ABSTRACT Cytoarchitectonic and histochemical analyses were carried out for perirhinal areas 35 and 36 and the postrhinal cortex, providing the first detailed cytoarchitectonic study of these regions in the rat brain. The rostral perirhinal border with insular cortex is at the extreme caudal limit of the claustrum, consistent with classical definitions of insular cortex dating back to Rose ([1928] J. Psychol. Neurol. 37:467–624). The border between the perirhinal and postrhinal cortices is at the caudal limit of the angular bundle, as previously proposed by Burwell et al. ([1995] Hip- pocampus 5:390–408). The ventral borders with entorhinal cortex are consistent with the Insausti et al. ([1997] Hippocampus 7:146–183) description of that region and the Dolorfo and Amaral ([1998] J. Comp. Neurol. 398:25–48) connectional findings. Regarding the remaining borders, both the perirhinal and postrhinal cortices encroach upon temporal cortical regions as defined by others (e.g., Zilles [1990] The cerebral cortex of the rat, p 77–112; Paxinos and Watson [1998] The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates). Based on cytoarchitectonic and histochemical criteria, perirhinal areas 35 and 36 and the postrhinal cortex were further subdivided. Area 36 was parceled into three subregions, areas 36d, 36v, and 36p. Area 35 was parceled into two cytoarchitectonically distinctive subregions, areas 35d and 35v. The postrhinal cortex was di- vided into two subregions, areas PORd and PORv.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perirhinal, Entorhinal, and Parahippocampal Cortices and 1 9 Hippocampus: an Overview of Functional Anatomy and Protocol for Their Segmentation in MR Images
    The Perirhinal, Entorhinal, and Parahippocampal Cortices and 1 9 Hippocampus: An Overview of Functional Anatomy and Protocol for Their Segmentation in MR Images Sasa L. Kivisaari, Alphonse Probst, and Kirsten I. Taylor Abbreviations fi Fimbria gA Gyrus ambiens A Anterior gS Gyrus of Schwalbe Ab Angular bundle (PHg white matter) HB Hippocampal body aCf Anterior calcarine fi ssure Hf Hippocampal fi ssure al Alveus HH Hippocampal head Am Amygdala Hs Hippocampal sulcus bG Band of Giacomini HT Hippocampal tail cf Crus of the fornix I Inferior Cs Collateral sulcus ILg Intralimbic gyrus di Hippocampal digitations Is Isthmus ERc Entorhinal cortex ITg Inferotemporal gyrus Fg Fusiform gyrus L Laterial Lg Lingual gyrus li-gm Limen insulae gray matter S. L. Kivisaari , Ph.D. (*) li-wm Limen insulae white matter Department of Geriatrics , M Medial Memory Clinic, University Hospital Basel , Schanzenstrasse 55, CH-4031 , Basel , Switzerland Mb Mammillary body e-mail: [email protected] MTL Medial temporal lobe A. Probst , M.D. OTs Occipitotemporal sulcus Department of Geriatrics , P Posterior Memory Clinic, University Hospital Basel , Pu Pulvinar Schanzenstrasse 55, CH-4031 , Basel , Switzerland PHc Parahippocampal cortex Department of Neuropathology , PHg Parahippocampal gyrus University Hospital Basel , PRc Perirhinal cortex Schönbeinstrasse 40, CH-4031 , Basel , Switzerland e-mail: [email protected] qgc Quadrigeminal cistern Rs Rhinal sulcus K. I. Taylor , Ph.D. Department of Geriatrics , S Superior Memory Clinic, University Hospital Basel , SAs Semiannular sulcus Schanzenstrasse 55, CH-4031 , Basel , Switzerland SLg Semilunar gyrus Department of Experimental Psychology , Sp Splenium Centre for Speech Language and the Brain, su Subiculum University of Cambridge , TLV Temporal horn of lateral ventricle Downing Street, Cambridge , CB2 3EB , UK e-mail: [email protected] TP Temporal pole S.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perirhinal Cortex
    NE37CH03-Suzuki ARI 26 May 2014 6:59 The Perirhinal Cortex Wendy A. Suzuki1 and Yuji Naya2 1Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003; email: [email protected] 2Department of Psychology, Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences and PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2014. 37:39–53 Keywords The Annual Review of Neuroscience is online at association memory, declarative memory, medial temporal lobe neuro.annualreviews.org by Peking University on 07/21/14. For personal use only. This article’s doi: Abstract 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014207 Anatomically, the perirhinal cortex sits at the boundary between the medial Copyright c 2014 by Annual Reviews. temporal lobe and the ventral visual pathway. It has prominent interconnec- Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2014.37:39-53. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org All rights reserved tions not only with both these systems, but also with a wide range of unimodal and polymodal association areas. Consistent with these diverse projections, neurophysiological studies reveal a multidimensional set of mnemonic sig- nals that include stimulus familiarity, within- and between-domain associ- ations, associative recall, and delay-based persistence. This wide range of perirhinal memory signals not only includes signals that are largely unique to the perirhinal cortex (i.e., object familiarity), consistent with dual-process theories, but also includes a range of signals (i.e., associative flexibility and recall) that are strongly associated with the hippocampus, consistent with single-process theories. These neurophysiological findings have important implications for bridging the gap between single-process and dual-process models of medial temporal lobe function.
    [Show full text]
  • The Rat Perirhinal Cortex: a Review of Anatomy, Physiology, Plasticity, and Function
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by MURAL - Maynooth University Research Archive Library Progress in Neurobiology 93 (2011) 522–548 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Progress in Neurobiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/pneurobio The rat perirhinal cortex: A review of anatomy, physiology, plasticity, and function John Kealy, Sean Commins * Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland Maynooth, Maynooth, Co. Kildare, Ireland ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Article history: The perirhinal cortex is located in a pivotal position to influence the flow of information into and out of Received 19 April 2010 the hippocampal formation. In this review, we examine the anatomical, physiological and functional Received in revised form 28 January 2011 properties of the rat perirhinal cortex. Firstly, we review the properties of the perirhinal cortex itself, we Accepted 10 March 2011 describe how it can be separated into two distinct subregions and consider how it differs from other Available online 21 March 2011 neighbouring regions in terms of cell type, cellular organisation and its afferent and efferent projections. We review the forms of neurotransmission present in the perirhinal cortex and the morphological, Keywords: electrophysiological and plastic properties of its neurons. Secondly, we review the perirhinal cortex in Perirhinal cortex the context of its connections with other brain areas; focussing on the projections to cortical, subcortical Hippocampal formation Anatomy and hippocampal/parahippocampal regions. Particular attention is paid the anatomical and Physiology electrophysiological properties of these projections. Thirdly, we review the main functions of the Electrophysiology perirhinal cortex; its roles in perception, recognition memory, spatial and contextual memory and fear Perception conditioning are explored.
    [Show full text]
  • Lesions of the Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cottices in the Monkey Produce Long-Lasting Memory Impairment in the Visual and Tactual Modalities
    The Journal of Neuroscience, June 1993, 13(6): 2430-2451 Lesions of the Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cottices in the Monkey Produce Long-lasting Memory Impairment in the Visual and Tactual Modalities Wendy A. Suzuki, I,* Stuart Zola-Morgan,1~3 Larry R. Squire,lv3 and David G. AmaraP* ‘Group in Neurosciences, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, “The Salk Institute, San Diego, California 92186, and 3Veterans Affairs Medical Center, San Diego, California 92161 and Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093 Compared to normal animals, monkeys with bilateral lesions man amnesia,the deficit associatedwith H+A+ lesionsin mon- of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices (PRPH lesion) keys is multimodal (Murray and Mishkin, 1984) and long-last- were impaired on both a visual and a tactual version of the ing (Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1985). In addition, animals with delayed nonmatching to sample task. In addition, the mem- H+A+ lesionsexhibit preserved short-term memory (Overman ory deficit was long-lasting, as indicated by the finding of a et al., 1990) intact skill-based memory, and intact habit-like significant deficit when the visual version of the delayed forms of memory (Malamut et al., 1984;Zola-Morgan and Squire, nonmatching to sample task was readministered approxi- 1984) in analogy to the preserved memory abilities of human mately 2 years after surgery. Animals with PRPH lesions amnesicpatients (for reviews, seeSquire and Zola-Morgan, 1991; performed normally on discrimination tasks in the visual and Squire, 1992). tactual modalities. Multimodal and long-lasting memory im- Although the importance of the medial temporal lobe for pairments are defining characteristics of human medial tem- memory function hasbeen known for sometime, a critical issue poral lobe amnesia.
    [Show full text]
  • Temporally-Specific Retrograde Amnesia in Two Cases of Discrete
    HIPPOCAMPUS 9:247–254 (1999) Temporally-Specific Retrograde Amnesia in Two Cases of Discrete Bilateral Hippocampal Pathology Narinder Kapur1,2* and David J. Brooks3 1Wessex Neurological Centre, Southampton General Hospital, Southampton, England 2Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, England 3MRC Cyclotron Unit, Hammersmith Hospital, London, England ABSTRACT: The role of the hippocampus in retrograde amnesia re- tion.’’ As Squire and Alvarez (1995, p 173) have pointed mains controversial and poorly understood. Two cases are reported of out, data relating to the severity and pattern of retro- discrete bilateral hippocampal damage, one of which was a rare case of grade amnesia have implications for theories of memory limbic encephalitis secondary to the human herpes virus 6. Detailed memory testing showed marked anterograde memory impairment, but consolidation. They concluded that ‘‘the facts of tempo- only mild, temporally-limited retrograde amnesia that covered a period of rally-graded retrograde amnesia, and virtually all ac- several years in both autobiographical and factual knowledge domains. counts of this phenomenon that involve the concept of The absence of extensive retrograde amnesia in these two cases points to a memory consolidation, suggest that memory storage and time-limited role for the hippocampus in the retrieval of retrograde memories, and suggests that entorhinal, perirhinal, parahippocampal, or retrieval come eventually to be supported by neocortex neocortical areas of the temporal lobe may be more critical than the as the result of gradual changes in connectivity within hippocampus proper for long-term retrograde memory functioning. Our neocortex.’’ findings offer general support to theories of memory consolidation that Human lesion studies have offered some clues as to propose a gradual transfer of memory from hippocampal to neocortical the role of the hippocampus in retrograde memory, dependency.
    [Show full text]
  • Perirhinal Cortex Represents Nonspatial, but Not Spatial, Information in Rats Foraging in the Presence of Objects: Comparison with Lateral Entorhinal Cortex
    HIPPOCAMPUS 22:2045–2058 (2012) Perirhinal Cortex Represents Nonspatial, But Not Spatial, Information in Rats Foraging in the Presence of Objects: Comparison With Lateral Entorhinal Cortex Sachin S. Deshmukh,1,3* Jeremy L. Johnson,1 and James J. Knierim1,2,3 ABSTRACT: The medial temporal lobe (MTL) is involved in mnemonic the hippocampus, with PRC being involved in non- processing. The perirhinal cortex (PRC) plays a role in object recogni- spatial memory and the hippocampus being involved tion memory, while the hippocampus is required for certain forms of spatial memory and episodic memory. The lateral entorhinal cortex in spatial memory (Ennaceur et al., 1996; Murray (LEC) receives direct projections from PRC and is one of the two major et al., 2007; for a historical perspective on inclusion cortical inputs to the hippocampus. The transformations that occur of PRC in the hippocampal ‘‘memory system’’, see between PRC and LEC neural representations are not well understood. Murray and Wise, 2012). On the other hand, there is Here, we show that PRC and LEC had similarly high proportions of neu- a longstanding debate about whether the hippocampus rons with object-related activity (PRC 52/94; LEC 72/153), as expected from their locations in the ‘‘what’’ pathway into the hippocampus. is specialized for encoding space, or whether space is However, LEC unit activity showed more spatial stability than PRC unit just one of the variables stored in a generalized mem- activity. A minority of LEC neurons showed stable spatial firing fields ory structure in the hippocampus (O’Keefe, 1999; away from objects; these firing fields strongly resembled hippocampal Shapiro and Eichenbaum, 1999).
    [Show full text]