Canine Language the Key Parts of Dog Communication

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Canine Language the Key Parts of Dog Communication Canine Language The key parts of dog communication. Think of it as their dictionary of words. We are going to look at the main areas of the canine body that they use to communicate and how each can be interpreted. Ears, Eyes, Face/Head, Mouth, Tail, Body Position and Vocalization Ears: Ear position often reflects the motivation of the dog. They can give very significant signals about the dogs intentions but should be paired with the whole activity of the dog to get a more clear picture of it’s intentions. Dogs with pricked ears are often seen as more expressionate because the movement of the ears is more noticeable. We’ll look at both pricked and floppy eared dogs. Ears Erect or slightly Forward: Erect ears are an indication of attention, alertness, curiosity, prey/play or an offensive threat. Usually in the context of “What’s that?” ,“This is really interesting.”, “I don’t quite understand” or “I’m gonna get it!” depending on the pairing of signals from the rest of the body. In the case of an offensive threat forward ears are usually paired with a flashing set of teeth! A.1 Ollie’s ears in this photo are erect, alert and focused directly on the ball I am holding. This is an example of play or prey alertness, think of it as “I’m gonna get it!” A.2 In this photo Sketch’s ears are erect and focused directly on the person behind the camera making a silly noise. This is an example of curiosity, a “What is that?”. A.3 In this photo Possum’s ears are erect and focused directly on the cookie. This is an example of focus, “This is interesting”. A.4 In this photo Art’s ears are forward, alert and pointing towards the ball at my feet. This is an example of attention, “There is the ball, it’s right there”. A.5 In this photo Triskett’s ears are forward and erect. They are focused directly on another dog. This is an example of alertness as in “Who is that dog!”. A.6 In this photo Bruiser’s ears are forward and focused on the approaching person. This is an example of an offensive threat, “I am ready to bite you so you better consider your next move carefully.” Ears pulled back or flat against head: This can be a sign of anxiety, tension, fear, stress, caution or in certain situations and appeasing gesture or submission as in “I am no threat”. In some circumstances a dog can flick it’s ears back and forth from erect to pulled back. This is usually a sign that the dog is assessing the situation. This is a good example of ears showing stress in a dog. Ollie is stressed and hot, her ears are flattened on her head. Here is another example of flattened ears but in this case they are showing fear that has turned to aggression. It’s a much different look than the dog in photo A.6 though still not a dog to be messed with. This photo is a great example of submission and fear “I am no threat, please don’t hurt me.” The shepherds ears are flattened as far as they will go. In this photo Sketch’s ears are drawn back in a sign of tension and caution. Sketch often gives this signal when approached by another dog. It’s his, “Are you going to be friendly? I am unsure about this situation.” This photo is an example of appeasement and submission. The dog is being approached by an unknown dog and is reflecting “ I understand you are bigger than me, I am no threat”. This is a great example of a floppy eared Dane with his ears pulled back. He is showing the toy dog just out of the picture that he is no threat to her. This is also an example of a floppy eared dog showing “I am no threat”. Eyes There are a few things to consider when we look at dog’s eyes. The direction of the gaze, the pupils, the shape of the eye and the dog’s brow. Direction of the gaze: The direction of the gaze is one of the most important aspects in canine behavior. Eyes, in humans and most all animals consist of the colored area-the iris, and the sclera-the white part of the eyes. Because of this we can determine the direction of the gaze. Why is this important? Dogs tend to turn their heads to follow their eyes or move in the direction of their eyes, it allows us to see their intentions and where they are focused. Fearful dogs display something called “whale eye” where the dogs eyes are focused on the object they are fearful of but the dogs head and body are frozen in place. This causes the sclera of the eye to become very noticeable like in the eyes of a whale. We most commonly see whale eye in dogs that feel threatened and trapped. This is a good warning sign to back up and give the dog some space. Virtually all animals view direct eye contact or staring as a threat. Dogs especially. Direct eye to eye contact in dogs is a sign that trouble is about to happen. It can be used as an expression of dominance, a threat or an on coming fight. This is not to say that your dog sitting next to you while you eat dinner, staring at your with those puppy dog eyes is threatening you BUT they are trying to control the situation. Some dogs, for instance herding breeds use eye contact to threaten and move the large animals they are herding. Just as eye contact is threatening the aversion of eye contact is seen as a submissive gesture or appeasement. Dogs will often avert their gaze when approaching one another to say “No threat here, I don’t want any trouble.” We call this a “look away”, it can be seen in a multitude of situations. Your dog may stare at you while you are watching TV and then when you turn and look at them they casually glance the other way or look at the floor. The Pupils: The pupils were created to expand and contract to control the amount of light let into the eye. In bright sunlight the pupils will contract and in the dark they will expand. They also communicate a great deal about the emotional state of a dog. Large pupils show intense emotions such as fear, excitement or interest. Smaller pupils often are signs of relaxation, drowsiness and boredom. In cases of anger or aggression in dogs the pupils become smaller and then enlarge to almost the size of the iris. Because some dogs have dark colored eyes it can be difficult to see the pupil so we must rely on the areas of the eye to determine the dogs state of mind. The shape of the eye: The shape or outline of a dog’s eye is one of the easiest things to see when it comes to their eyes. It also expresses strong emotions. The wider or more open the eye the more angry or threatening the dog. The more smaller, closed or narrow the eye the more submissive, fearful or pacifying the gesture in canine language. A dog’s brow: Dogs don’t have eyebrows as humans do but some dogs do have distinct color markings along the brow to make it more noticeable. Solid colored dogs without such markings tend to have fur patterns that create a shadow like appearance which distinguishes the brow of the dog. Dogs use their eyebrows very similarly to humans. When dogs become angry the area between the brows contracts and the brows angle downwards. The hair will also raise up to give a more “human-like” look of the eye brows. When a dog is fearful or being submissive the brows are pulled upward in the center and the outside ends are pulled down and out toward the temples. The fur lays flat so this is not as easy to see as an angry dog’s eyebrow movement. Dogs also use their eyebrows to show confusion or concentration, by pulling their brows together and down without any angles. They can even show surprise as humans do by moving their brows up and outward. Ok let’s look at all of these pieces together. Face & Mouth When looking at a dog’s face it is comprised predominantly of the muzzle or mouth area. A dog’s facial expressions regarding the mouth has many similarities to that of a human expression, however, dog’s lack the ability to consciously move their facial muscles so their expressions are more limited. The key areas to look at are the lips, tongue, teeth and area just about the dog’s nose on the muzzle. The shape of a dog’s mouth signals basic information such as anger, dominance, aggression, fear, interest, attention or relaxation. Mouth relaxed, slightly open; tongue visible and relaxed- This is the sign of a happy, content dog. It’s very close to a human smile. The lips are relaxed, mouth is slightly opened and the tongue is visible or laying just outside the dogs mouth. Art is one Happy Dog! Possum happy and alert! Mouth closed, relaxed lips, no teeth or tongue visible- Just simply closing the mouth changes a dog’s expression completely. A closed mouth with relaxed lips indicates attention or interest in something.
Recommended publications
  • The Social Cognition of Domestic Dogs (Canis Familiaris) During Cross-Species Interactions with Humans
    The Social Cognition of Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) During Cross-Species Interactions with Humans Hannah Kate Worsley School of Environment and Life Sciences University of Salford Submitted in Partial Fulfilment of the Requirements of the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy September 2018 TABLE OF CONTENTS INDEX OF FIGURES ...................................................................................................................... v INDEX OF TABLES ....................................................................................................................... vi AKNOWLEDGEMENTS ............................................................................................................... vii ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................ viii 1. INTRODUCTION....................................................................................................................... 1 1.1 ANIMAL COMMUNICATION AND SOCIAL COGNITION ..................................................... 1 1.1.1 Vocal Communication ................................................................................................ 1 1.1.2 Gestural Communication ............................................................................................ 3 1.1.3 Intentional Cross-species Communication ................................................................. 5 1.2 THE STUDY SPECIES: THE DOMESTIC DOG (Canis familiaris) ..........................................
    [Show full text]
  • Computer Mediated Remote Interaction Between Humans and Dogs
    Rover@Home: Computer Mediated Remote Interaction Between Humans and Dogs Benjamin Ishak Resner B.A. Physics, Cornell University, 1990 Submitted to the Program in Media Arts and Sciences, School of Architecture and Planning in Partial Fulfillment for the degree of Master of Science in Media Arts and Sciences at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology September 2001 © Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2001 Author Benjamin Ishak Resner Program in Media Arts and Sciences August 10, 2001 Ceitified by Dr. Bruce Blumberg Associate Professor of Media Arts & Sciences Accegd by Dr. Andrew Lippman Chair, Department Committee on Graduate Studies Program in Media Arts and Sciences MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY OCT 12 2001 1 LIBRARIES Rover@Home: Computer Mediated Remote Interaction Between Humans and Dogs Benjamin Ishak Resner Submitted to the Program in Media Arts and Sciences, School of Architecture and Planning in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Abstract In this thesis we create a method to allow dogs and humans to interact over the Internet. In particular, we generalize an established dog training technique known as "clicker-training" such that the remote and co-located interactions are reported by dog owners to be similar. In the process of creating this computer-mediated interaction, we learn what it means to design an interface for a creature with very different sensory modalities than humans. Dogs are not "furry humans" but entirely different creatures with very different perceptual, motor, and cognitive systems than humans. This work is significant because by systematically applying HCI design principles to non-humans, we include animals in the HCI community.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Changes in Canine Social Competence
    Review What does it take to become ‘best friends’? Evolutionary changes in canine social competence ´ 1,2 3 Ada´ m Miklo´ si and Jo´ zsef Topa´ l 1 Department of Ethology, Eo¨ tvo¨ s University, Budapest, Hungary 2 MTA-ELTE Comparative Ethology Research Group, Budapest, Hungary 3 Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary The traditional and relatively narrow-focused research offers an exciting possibility for comparative social cogni- on ape–human comparisons has recently been signifi- tion, there are heated debates in the literature and there is cantly extended by investigations of different clades of no scientific consensus on the validity of this approach. animals, including the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Thus, in the light of diverging views on canine social Here, we provide a short overview of how the compar- cognition, it may be useful to develop functional theories ative investigation of canine social behaviour advances for systematising relevant research. our understanding of the evolution of social skills and We review the available evidence and focus on those key argue that a system-level approach to dog social cog- social traits in dogs that are characteristic features of the nition provides a broader view on the ‘human-likeness’ functionally human-like nature of dogs’ social competence. of canine social competence. We introduce the concept We will also provide a concise overview of the different of evolutionary social competence as a collateral notion theories that aim to explain the evolution of the canine of developmental social competence. We argue that mind and the role that domestication plays in the acquisi- such an extended perspective on social competence tion of socio-cognitive skills.
    [Show full text]
  • Communication in Dogs
    animals Review Communication in Dogs Marcello Siniscalchi * ID , Serenella d’Ingeo, Michele Minunno and Angelo Quaranta Department of Veterinary Medicine, Section of Behavioral Sciences and Animal Bioethics, University of Bari “Aldo Moro”, 70121 Bari, Italy; [email protected] (S.d.); [email protected] (M.M.); [email protected] (A.Q.) * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +39-080-544-3948 Received: 30 June 2018; Accepted: 28 July 2018; Published: 31 July 2018 Simple Summary: Communication takes place between members of the same species, as well as between heterospecific individuals, such as the long co-habitation process and inter-dependent relationship present in domestic dogs and humans. Dogs engage in visual communication by modifying different parts of their body; in tactile communication; and also in auditory and olfactory communication, with vocalizations and body odours, respectively. The aim of this review is to provide an overview of the recent literature about dog communication, describing the different nature of the signals used in conspecific and heterospecific interactions and their communicative meaning. Lateralized dog brain patterns underlying basic neural mechanisms are also discussed, for both conspecific and heterospecific social communication. Abstract: Dogs have a vast and flexible repertoire of visual, acoustic, and olfactory signals that allow an expressive and fine tuned conspecific and dog–human communication. Dogs use this behavioural repertoire when communicating with humans, employing the same signals used during conspecific interactions, some of which can acquire and carry a different meaning when directed toward humans. The aim of this review is to provide an overview of the latest progress made in the study of dog communication, describing the different nature of the signals used in conspecific (dog–dog) and heterospecific (dog–human) interactions and their communicative meaning.
    [Show full text]
  • The Intelligence of Dogs a Guide to the Thoughts, Emotions, and Inner Lives of Our Canine
    Praise for The Intelligence of Dogs "For those who take the dog days literally, the best in pooch lit is Stanley Coren’s The Intelligence of Dogs. Psychologist, dog trainer, and all-around canine booster, Coren trots out everyone from Aristotle to Darwin to substantiate the smarts of canines, then lists some 40 commands most dogs can learn, along with tests to determine if your hairball is Harvard material.” —U.S. News & World Report "Fascinating . What makes The Intelligence of Dogs such a great book, however, isn’t just the abstract discussions of canine intelli­ gence. Throughout, Coren relates his findings to the concrete, dis­ cussing the strengths and weaknesses of various breeds and including specific advice on evaluating different breeds for vari­ ous purposes. It's the kind of book would-be dog owners should be required to read before even contemplating buying a dog.” —The Washington Post Book World “Excellent book . Many of us want to think our dog’s persona is characterized by an austere veneer, a streak of intelligence, and a fearless-go-for-broke posture. No matter wrhat your breed, The In­ telligence of Dogs . will tweak your fierce, partisan spirit . Coren doesn’t stop at intelligence and obedience rankings, he also explores breeds best suited as watchdogs and guard dogs . [and] does a masterful job of exploring his subject's origins, vari­ ous forms of intelligence gleaned from genetics and owner/trainer conditioning, and painting an inner portrait of the species.” —The Seattle Times "This book offers more than its w7ell-publicized ranking of pure­ bred dogs by obedience and working intelligence.
    [Show full text]
  • Biological Sciences
    A Comprehensive Book on Environmentalism Table of Contents Chapter 1 - Introduction to Environmentalism Chapter 2 - Environmental Movement Chapter 3 - Conservation Movement Chapter 4 - Green Politics Chapter 5 - Environmental Movement in the United States Chapter 6 - Environmental Movement in New Zealand & Australia Chapter 7 - Free-Market Environmentalism Chapter 8 - Evangelical Environmentalism Chapter 9 -WT Timeline of History of Environmentalism _____________________ WORLD TECHNOLOGIES _____________________ A Comprehensive Book on Enzymes Table of Contents Chapter 1 - Introduction to Enzyme Chapter 2 - Cofactors Chapter 3 - Enzyme Kinetics Chapter 4 - Enzyme Inhibitor Chapter 5 - Enzymes Assay and Substrate WT _____________________ WORLD TECHNOLOGIES _____________________ A Comprehensive Introduction to Bioenergy Table of Contents Chapter 1 - Bioenergy Chapter 2 - Biomass Chapter 3 - Bioconversion of Biomass to Mixed Alcohol Fuels Chapter 4 - Thermal Depolymerization Chapter 5 - Wood Fuel Chapter 6 - Biomass Heating System Chapter 7 - Vegetable Oil Fuel Chapter 8 - Methanol Fuel Chapter 9 - Cellulosic Ethanol Chapter 10 - Butanol Fuel Chapter 11 - Algae Fuel Chapter 12 - Waste-to-energy and Renewable Fuels Chapter 13 WT- Food vs. Fuel _____________________ WORLD TECHNOLOGIES _____________________ A Comprehensive Introduction to Botany Table of Contents Chapter 1 - Botany Chapter 2 - History of Botany Chapter 3 - Paleobotany Chapter 4 - Flora Chapter 5 - Adventitiousness and Ampelography Chapter 6 - Chimera (Plant) and Evergreen Chapter
    [Show full text]
  • An Investigation of the Look Away Behavior in Domestic Dogs Sarah Scott
    Undergraduate Review Volume 9 Article 29 2013 An Investigation of the Look Away Behavior in Domestic Dogs Sarah Scott Follow this and additional works at: http://vc.bridgew.edu/undergrad_rev Part of the Animal Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Scott, Sarah (2013). An Investigation of the Look Away Behavior in Domestic Dogs. Undergraduate Review, 9, 145-150. Available at: http://vc.bridgew.edu/undergrad_rev/vol9/iss1/29 This item is available as part of Virtual Commons, the open-access institutional repository of Bridgewater State University, Bridgewater, Massachusetts. Copyright © 2013 Sarah Scott An Investigation of the Look Away Behavior in Domestic Dogs SARAH SCOTT Sarah Scott returned he unique phylogenetic and ontogenetic history of domestic dogs has had an effect on the way they communicate with one another. to Bridgewater State Research suggests that domestic dogs’ ability to communicate through University to study visual signals may vary by breed (Goodwin, Bradshaw, & Wickens psychology after T1997; Kerswell, Bennet, Butler, & Hemsworth 2009). In the current study, we completing a Bachelor investigate the effect of a visual communication signal, the look away, observed in both domestic dogs and their ancestor, the wolf, in order to examine whether of Science in Business Management or not domesticated dogs respond to this visual signal. Research indicates that from Bridgewater in 2008. She spent domestic dogs respond appropriately to artificial dog models (Leaver & Reimchen, the summer of 2012 conducting 2008). Therefore, we allowed live domestic dogs to approach an artificial model dog as it “looked away,” turning its head approximately 45% to the left, from a research study that investigated the approaching live dog participant.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to Dog Behavior
    Chapter 1 Introduction to dog behavior Julie Hecht1 and Alexandra Horowitz2 1 Department of Psychology, The Graduate Center, City University of New York, Horowitz Dog Cognition Lab, Barnard College, New York, USA 2 Department of Psychology, Barnard College, New York, USA those who reacted without fear or aggression to human Domestic dog evolution and behavior approach. Over 40 generations, he had created foxes Dog evolutionary history which looked and acted in many ways like familiar What is a dog? The answer can come in the form of a domestic dogs (Belyaev 1979; Trut 1999). description of the dog’s characteristic behavior, physical For millennia, dogs were bred for use for tasks (e.g. description, or evolutionary history. We will begin with guarding and hunting) or as companions. Quite recently, the latter. The domestic dog, Canis familiaris, is a member in the 19th century, artificial selection began to be of the Canidae family, genus Canis, along with such driven by an interest in creating pure breed lines, for territorial social carnivores as the gray wolf (Canis lupus), show and competition in dog “fancies,” dog shows. the coyote (Canis latrans), and the jackal (e.g., Canis Thus, the diverse array of breeds seen today is a result of aureus and Canis mesomelas). The dog is the only domesti- specific breeding over the last century and a half for cated species of the genus: that is to say, the only canid physical traits and temperament which suited the newly for whom artificial selection (selective breeding) by formed breed “standards” (Garber 1996). While some humans has usurped natural selection as a prime mover current dog breeds resemble ancient representations of of the species.
    [Show full text]
  • FIDO—Facilitating Interactions for Dogs with Occupations: Wearable Communication Interfaces for Working Dogs
    Pers Ubiquit Comput DOI 10.1007/s00779-014-0817-9 ORIGINAL ARTICLE FIDO—Facilitating interactions for dogs with occupations: wearable communication interfaces for working dogs Melody M. Jackson • Giancarlo Valentin • Larry Freil • Lily Burkeen • Clint Zeagler • Scott Gilliland • Barbara Currier • Thad Starner Received: 10 February 2014 / Accepted: 22 April 2014 Ó Springer-Verlag London 2014 Abstract Working dogs have improved the lives of dog, Roman, senses her oncoming seizure and begins thousands of people throughout history. However, com- nudging her back toward a wall. As Ashley sinks to the munication between human and canine partners is currently floor, Roman bites a small cylinder hanging from his ser- limited. The main goal of the FIDO project is to research vice dog vest, activating Ashley’s cell phone to call 911 fundamental aspects of wearable technologies to support and text her husband with their location. As Ashley loses communication between working dogs and their handlers. consciousness, Roman licks her face, waiting for help that In this study, the FIDO team investigated on-body inter- is already on the way. faces for dogs in the form of wearable technology inte- Charles is engrossed in a movie in his dark home theater grated into assistance dog vests. We created five different when his hearing service dog, Schubert, alerts. ‘‘What is it, sensors that dogs could activate based on natural dog Schubert, the doorbell?’’ Charles signs, and Schubert tou- behaviors such as biting, tugging, and nose touches. We ches one of the four buttons on his vest with his nose. A then tested the sensors on-body with eight dogs previously message appears on Charles’ head-mounted display.
    [Show full text]
  • Program Goals Knowing Dogs 101: Dog Language
    Knowing Dogs 101: Staff Training for Pet Care Centers Dog Language Robin Bennett, CPDT-KA Susan Briggs, CKO This staff workbook belongs to: ii Knowing Dogs 101 Staff Workbook Knowing Dogs 101: Dog Language Staff Workbook By Robin Bennett, CPDT-KA, and Susan Briggs, CKO Published by: The Dog Gurus 1312 Harvard St. Houston, TX 77008 www.TheDogGurus.com All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without written permission from the author, except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review. Copyright © 2017, Robin Bennett and Susan Briggs Bennett, Robin and Briggs, Susan Knowing Dogs 101: Dog Language Robin Bennett, CPDT-KA Susan Briggs, CKO P.cm. Cover by Amanda Nelson at Nvoke Design All other photos courtesy of the authors Limits of Liability and Disclaimer: This book has been published with the intent to provide accurate information in regard to the subject matter within. While every precaution has been taken in preparation of this book, the publisher and authors assume no responsibility for errors or omissions. Neither is any liability assumed for damages resulting from the use of the information herein. Furthermore, the authors and publisher shall not be liable in the event of incidental or consequential damages or loss in connection with, or arising out of the furnishing, performance, or use of the instructions or suggestions contained in this book. 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 iii Program Goals Knowing Dogs 101: Dog Language Welcome to the Knowing Dogs 101: Dog Language, developed and produced by Robin Bennett and Susan Briggs.
    [Show full text]
  • Dog Breed Differences in Visual Communication with Humans
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Dog Breed Differences in Visual Communication with Humans Akitsugu Konno1,2,3*, Teresa Romero1,2, Miho Inoue-Murayama3, Atsuko Saito2, Toshikazu Hasegawa2 1 Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Kojimachi 5-3-1, Chiyoda, Tokyo, Japan, 2 Department of Cognitive and Behavioral Science, The University of Tokyo, Komaba 3-8-1, Meguro, Tokyo, Japan, 3 Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University, Tanaka Sekiden cho 2±24, Sakyo, Kyoto, Japan * [email protected] a11111 Abstract Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) have developed a close relationship with humans through the process of domestication. In human-dog interactions, eye contact is a key element of relationship initiation and maintenance. Previous studies have suggested that canine ability OPEN ACCESS to produce human-directed communicative signals is influenced by domestication history, from wolves to dogs, as well as by recent breed selection for particular working purposes. Citation: Konno A, Romero T, Inoue-Murayama M, Saito A, Hasegawa T (2016) Dog Breed Differences To test the genetic basis for such abilities in purebred dogs, we examined gazing behavior in Visual Communication with Humans. PLoS ONE towards humans using two types of behavioral experiments: the `visual contact task' and 11(10): e0164760. doi:10.1371/journal. the `unsolvable task'. A total of 125 dogs participated in the study. Based on the genetic pone.0164760 relatedness among breeds subjects were classified into five breed groups: Ancient, Herd- Editor: Juliane Kaminski, University of Portsmouth, ing, Hunting, Retriever-Mastiff and Working). We found that it took longer time for Ancient UNITED KINGDOM breeds to make an eye-contact with humans, and that they gazed at humans for shorter Received: October 28, 2013 periods of time than any other breed group in the unsolvable situation.
    [Show full text]
  • Dogs in Animal-Assisted Therapy and Education: a Handbook for Professionals and Dog Handlers
    Dogs in animal-assisted therapy and education: A Handbook for professionals and dog handlers Discover our offers and more at www.DAIC.EU Dogs in animal-assisted therapy and education: A Handbook for professionals and dog handlers Dogs in animal-assisted therapy and education: A Handbook for professionals and dog handlers Warsaw 2019 This publication is developed as part of the Project ‘Preparation and Training of Human-Dog Teams in Dogs Assisted Education and Therapy’, co-funded by Erasmus Plus Program of the European Union. Project Number - 2017-1-PL01-KA202-038652 The publication reflects only the position of its authors. The European Commission and the National Agency of the Erasmus + Program are not responsible for its substantive content. Pubilished by Stowarzyszenie Zwierzęta Ludziom ul. Diamentowa 1 m 7, 05-077 Warszawa - Wesoła www.zwierzetaludziom.pl Creative Commons CC BY-NC-ND 3.0 Project consortium: Stowarzyszenie Zwierzęta Ludziom (SZL) Norwegian Centre of Antrozoology (NCofA) Open University of the Netherlands (OUNL) Authors: Magdalena Nawarecka-Piątek (SZL) Anna Smorczewska-Kiljan (SZL) Christine Olsen (NCofA) Line Sandstedt (NCofA) Marie-Jose Enders (OUNL) Tynke de Winkel (OUNL) In cooperation with: Jo-Ann Fowler ISBN 978-83-955882-0-4 Warsaw 2019 www.humandogteam.eu www.moodle.humandogteam.eu www.facebook.com/HumanDogTeams Spis treści 9 From the authors 11 CHAPTER 1. Introduction to animal-assisted interventions 20 CHAPTER 2. Dogs in animal-assisted interventions 21 AAI with dogs in institutions 22 Dogs in hospitals 26 Dogs in care homes/institutions 28 Dogs in schools - animal-assisted education 36 CHAPTER 3. Welfare and health of dogs in animal-assisted interventions 41 CHAPTER 4.
    [Show full text]