Quick viewing(Text Mode)

I the INTERACTION of SEXISM and HETEROSEXISM in LESBIAN

I the INTERACTION of SEXISM and HETEROSEXISM in LESBIAN

THE INTERACTION OF AND IN WOMEN’S

EXPERIENCES WITH INTIMATE PARTNER AND SUBSEQUENT

POSTRAUMATIC REACTIONS

A Dissertation

Presented to

The Graduate Faculty at the University of Akron

In Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Philosophy

Taylor L. Ceroni

August, 2019

i

THE INTERACTION OF SEXISM AND HETEROSEXISM IN LESBIAN WOMEN’S

EXPERIENCES WITH INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE AND SUBSEQUENT

POSTRAUMATIC REACTIONS

Taylor L. Ceroni

Dissertation

Approved: Accepted:

______Advisor Department Chair Dr. Dawn M. Johnson Dr. Paul E. Levy

______Committee Member Interim Dean of College Dr. Margo A. Gregor Dr. Linda M. Subich

______Committee Member Dean of the Graduate Dr. Ronald F. Levant School Dr. Chand Midha ______Committee Member ______Dr. Suzette L. Speight Date

______Committee Member Dr. John F. Zipp

ii

Abstract

Experiences of and experiences of multiple have long been linked to psychological distress. Newer research has been conducted looking at the traumatic effects that oppression can have on mental health including PTSD symptom development. The experience of trauma in women and its links to PTSD have also long been studied but there has been no research to date to look at the experiences of lesbian women who experience IPV and how sexism and heterosexism may affect the development of PTSD symptoms. The current study added to the existing literature by exploring the additive and interactional roles that multiple oppressions play in individual’s lives. Specifically, the present study hypothesized additive effects and interactional effects between , internalized heterosexism, externalized sexism, and externalized heterosexism in predicting PTSD symptom severity.

Additionally, it was predicted that a moderated moderation would exist, where the relationship between externalized oppression and PTSD symptom severity is moderated by with IPV severity moderating the relationship between internalized oppression and PTSD symptom severity. Participants were 209 lesbian women. Hierarchical regression analysis found support for externalized sexism as a unique predictor for PTSD symptom severity. Implications are discussed.

Keywords: sexism, heterosexism, intimate partner violence, PTSD

iii

DEDICATION

I dedicate this work to all sexual minority women. It is my hope that one day we can all love equally and find solidarity in our shared fight for liberation.

iv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

First and foremost, I wish to express my gratitude to Dr. Dawn Johnson, my graduate advisor and mentor. I can’t imagine that I would have been able to accomplish all of this without your endless support, wisdom, and guidance. Thank you, Dawn. My committee members, Drs Margo Gregor, Ronald Levant, Suzette Speight, and John Zipp,

I am thankful for all the ways that you helped me to advance my thinking about this study.

I would like to recognize the unequivocal support of my cohort members,

Kathleen Alto, Maria Pappa, Sarah Sanders, and Rebecca Schlesinger. I am so grateful that you were all a part of this journey. Additionally, I would like to express my gratitude to my internship supervisors at the Southeast Louisiana Veterans Health Care system, Drs

Julie Arseneau, Laurel Franklin, Michelle Hamilton, Amanda Raines, and Karen Slaton, your support and confidence in me has been invaluable to me completing this project.

Thank you for giving me a home in New Orleans.

To my internship cohort member, Chelsea Ennis, I wish to express my heartfelt appreciation for all of your support and friendship. Thank you for always knowing when to push me and when to distract me. To my longtime friend, Ashlee Fielding, there’s nothing I could accomplish without you, including this. Lastly, to my parents, thank you for always telling me that I could be anything I wanted, including a doctor one day.

v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page LIST OF TABLES………………………………………………………………………iv

LIST OF FIGURES……………………………………………………………………....x

CHAPTER

I. INTRODUCTION……………………………………………………………...1

II. A REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE…………………………………………20

Oppression as Trauma…………………………………………………………20

Sex-related Trauma, Sexism as Trauma, and Posttraumatic Reactions……..…28

Heterosexism, Heterosexism as Trauma, and Posttraumatic Reactions……….37

Intimate Partner Violence……………………………………………………...51

Theorizing Multiple Oppressions……………………………………………...53

Research Aims and Hypotheses..……………………………………………....72

III. RESEARCH METHODS……..………………………………………………..77

Participants……………………………………………………………………...77

Measures…………………………………………………………………...... 79

Procedures……………………………………………………………………....96

Analytic Plan…………………………………………………………..………..97

IV. RESULTS……………………………………………………………………..105

Data Screening and Missing Data…………………………………………….105

Descriptive Statistics………………………………………………………….107

vi

Hypothesis 1…………………………………………………………………..108

Hypothesis 2…………………………………………………………………..109

Hypothesis 3…………………………………………………………………..109

Hypothesis 4…………………………………………………………………..110

Hypothesis 5…………………………………………………………………..111

Hypothesis 6…………………………………………………………………..111

V. DISCUSSION…………………………………………………………………113

Hypothesis 1…………………………………………………………………..114

Hypothesis 2…………………………………………………………………..120

Hypothesis 3…………………………………………………………………..125

Hypothesis 4…………………………………………………………………..128

Hypothesis 5…………………………………………………………………..129

Hypothesis 6…………………………………………………………………..129

Clinical Implications…………………………………………………………..131

Implications for Counseling Psychology……………………………………...134

Strengths, Limitations and Future. Directions………………………………...136

Conclusions…………………………………………………………………...144

REFERENCES………………………………………………………………………....146

APPENDICES………………………………………………………………………….208

APPENDIX A. DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE……………………………...208

APPENDIX B. PTSD CHECKLIST FOR THE DSM-5……………………………...209

APPENDIX C. THE SEVERITY OF SCALE..…211

APPENDIX D. SCHEDULE OF SEXIST EVENTS….……………………………...213

vii

APPENDIX E. THE HETEROSEXIST , , AND

REJECTION SCALE………………………………………………………………….216

APPENDIX F. LESBIAN INTERNALIZED SCALE….………….218

APPENDIX G. THE INTERNALIZED SCALE…………………...….221

APPENDIX H. INFORMED CONSENT…………………………………………….223

APPENDIX I. INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD APPROVAL FOR HUMAN

SUBJECTS RESEARCH……………………………………………………………..224

viii

LIST OF TABLES Table Page

1. Conceptual Interactions of Predictors…………………………………………195

2. Demographics……………………………...………………...……………...... 196

3. Correlations Amongst Main Variables……….……..……….……………...... 198

4. Descriptive Statistics for Main Variables……………………….………...…...199

5. Hierarchical Regressions – Hypothesis 1………………….……………...…...200

6. Hierarchical Regressions – Hypothesis 2…………………….……………...... 201

7. Hierarchical Regressions – Hypothesis 3……………………….……………..202

8. Hierarchical Regressions – Hypothesis 4………………….…………………..203

9. Moderated Moderators – Hypothesis 5……………….……………………….204

10. Moderated Moderators – Hypothesis 6……….…………….…………………205

ix

LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page

1. Conceptual Model – Moderated Moderation of Sexism………………………206

2. Conceptual Model – Moderated Moderation of Heterosexism………………..207

x

Chapter I

INTRODUCTION

Oppression is a pervasive and insidious daily occurrence for many individuals with minority statuses. Oppression has been defined in multiple ways over time with the common theme that there is a dominant group and a non-dominant group, with the dominant group keeping needed resources away from the non-dominant group (Bartky,

1990; Mar’i, 1988; Sidanius, 1993; Young, 1990). Bartky (1990) in particular, viewed oppression as not just the dominant group retaining control politically and economically but also psychologically. Bartky (1990) purported that oppression is not purely political or purely psychological but that the political and psychological nature of oppression are inherently linked. Oppression is inherently a state as well as a process of domination by a majority group but also, the insidious psychological control of subjugation of a marginalized group, who believe that they are less than the majority group, deserving of less resources, and ultimately deserving of the oppression they are experiencing (Bartky,

1990; Prilleltensky & Gonick, 1996). Thus, oppression can be defined as “a state of asymmetric power relations characterized by domination, subordination, and resistance, where the dominating persons or groups exercise their power by restricting access to material resources and by implanting in the subordinated persons or groups fear or self-

1

deprecating views about themselves” (Prilleltensky & Gonick, 1996, pp. 129-130). The current study aimed to better understand the additive and interactional perspectives in the experiences of sexism and heterosexism in lesbian women.

Research on oppression has shown the links between the experience of oppression and negative mental health consequences, including but not limited to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Balsam, 2003; Bryant-Davis & Ocampo, 2005; Carter, 2007;

Carter, Forsyth, Mazzula, & Williams, 2005; Neisen, 1993; Root,1992; Sanchez-Hucles,

1998; Szymanski & Balsam, 2011) and argue for a change in the American Psychiatric

Associations Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (2013; DSM-5) in the diagnostic criteria for PTSD to include experiences of oppression. Less is known about how the traumatic experience of oppression, multiple oppressions, and other traumatic incidents in women’s lives influence the development of subsequent PTSD symptoms. The purpose of the current research was to investigate the impact of multiple oppressions and interpersonal trauma on lesbian women’s PTSD symptoms.

Systems of Oppression

Oppression in our society is systemic and is embedded into the structural levels that influence the experience of minority individuals in a variety of ways. Young (1990) describes oppression as structural in nature and minority groups experience oppression as a normalized every day experience with causal underpinnings “embedded in unquestioned norms, habits and symbols, in the assumptions underlying institutional rules and the collective consequences for following those rules” (p. 41). With this type of structural embedment, oppression is normalized and becomes a necessary part of how society functions which not only creates acceptance of oppression but also makes the

2

mechanisms largely invisible, justifying and legitimizing its existence (Hardiman,

Jackson, & Griffin, 2007).

Oppression being structural and embedded into every level of societal experience creates a hierarchy of dominant and subordinate groups (Bell, 1997; 2007). The social hierarchy provides dominant groups with advantages and privilege over necessary resources and power. The system of oppression functions when the hierarchy is continually exercised by all individuals and the majority group continues to maintain power creating social inequity (Fanon, 1963; Freire, 1970; Prilleltensky & Gonick, 1996;

Young, 1990). Further, socialization is an important aspect of individuals enacting their given roles within the system of oppression. Socialization is a process where individuals learn values, rules, and roles that allow them to live in society. These roles may be social and personal, teaching individuals the norms and expectations of them as a member of the majority group or (Fanon, 1967, Harro, 2010, Memmi, 1965). The process of socialization leads minority individuals to believe that their oppression is not only normal but also acceptable furthering the system of oppression (Harro, 2010).

Another mechanism of oppression is internalized oppression, which is an intrapersonal level of oppression that is theorized to be a state as well as a process

(Freire, 1970; Pheterson, 1986; Prilleltensky & Gonick, 1996; Pyke, 2010). Internalized oppression can be defined as “the incorporation and acceptance by individuals within an oppressed group of the against them within the dominant society. Internalized oppression is likely to consist of self-hatred, self-concealment, fear of violence and feelings of inferiority, resignation, isolation, powerlessness, and gratefulness for being allowed to survive.” (Pheterson 1986; p.146). Pheterson (1986; p. 146) further defines

3

internalize oppression as “the mechanism within an oppressive system for perpetuating domination not only by external control but also by building subservience into the minds of the oppressed groups.” Incorporating, accepting, and building subservience shows that internalized oppression is a process that it is a necessary and purposeful part of the system of oppression maintaining the social hierarchy. The state of internalized oppression is the consequence of self-hatred, self-concealment, fear of violence, inferiority, resignation, isolation, powerlessness, and gratefulness for being allowed to survive. Limiting resources and power of minority groups and the acceptance that this is normal leads to the state of internalized oppression (Pharr, 1996, 1997; Pheterson, 1986;).

Brown (1986) discussed internalized homophobia as lesbian, , and bisexual

(LGB) individuals believing they are inferior because they are continuously subjected to messages of their and denied power and privileges that are rewarded to heterosexual individuals. Perez (2005) discusses how this may lead LGB individuals to want to be seen as heterosexual, which is a term called “”. Appearing as heterosexual may make LGB individuals feel safer in social interactions but can further self-hatred as they are not being their authentic selves (David & Derthick, 2014). The action of trying to pass in heterosexual society may also isolate LGB individuals from not only their family, friends, coworkers, but also from interacting with their LGB community, ultimately rejecting their true selves and their culture which can have lasting psychological effects (David & Derthick, 2014; Hill, 2009; Perez, 2005). Nadal and

Medoza (2014) argue for the term “internalized heterosexism” rather than the historic

“internalized homophobia”. The term homophobia does not fully capture the discriminatory experiences of LGB individuals as the word “phobia” implies that it is an

4

irrational fear of LGB individuals. In contrast, the term “internalized heterosexism” better captures LGB discrimination as individuals may not have an irrational fear of LGB individuals but rather a towards favoring opposite- relationships over same- gender relationships, leading to discrimination of LGB individuals (Nadal & Modoza,

2014).

Lesbian women may experience internalized sexism or “internalized misogyny”, which is the internalization of the hatred, degradation, and devaluation of women in our society to maintain men’s power and ultimately, leading to the fear of women and traditional feminine qualities and characteristics (Burch, 1987; O’Neil, 1981; Piggot,

2004; Szymanski & Kashubeck-West, 2008; Szymanski, Gupta, Carr, & Stewart, 2009;

Worrel & Remer, 2003). Consequently, men and women devalue women, which is a construct called “horizontal oppression” (Piggot, 2004; Saakvitne & Pearlman, 1993;

Szymanski et al., 2009). Horizontal oppression may result from internalized misogyny and internalized heterosexism interacting in lesbian women relationships which may lead to enacting oppressive acts and violence, such as IPV (Hines, 2014; Pepper & Sand,

2015). Experiences of oppression and internalized oppression also have lasting psychological consequences and may be traumatic.

Oppression as Trauma

The psychological experience of oppression and internalization of oppression led researchers to look at the impact of oppression as traumatic. Root (1992) coined the term insidious trauma as a way to define the consequences of ongoing daily experiences of political and psychological oppression in minority individuals. Viewing oppression as traumatic has led many researchers and theorists to explore oppression as a form of

5

trauma that could lead to posttraumatic symptoms and posttraumatic stress disorder

(PTSD; Balsam, 2003; Bryant-Davis & Ocampo, 2005; Carter, 2007; Carter, Forsyth,

Mazzula, & Williams, 2005; Neisen, 1993; Root,1992; Sanchez-Hucles, 1998;

Szymanski & Balsam, 2011). These researchers argue that experiences of oppression should be included in the Diagnostic and Statistical manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) as part of Criterion A for posttraumatic stress disorder. Criterion A in the DSM defines what can be included as a trauma.

There is much research to suggest that experiences of oppression are related to negative psychological consequences in individuals and may be perceived as traumatic

(Anderson, 2013; Bandermann & Szymanski, 2014; Cheng & Mallinckrodt, 2015; Díaz,

Ayala, Bein, Henne & Marin, 2001; Pascoe & Richman, 2009; Pieterse, Todd, Neville,

Carter, 2012; Suarez & Gadalla, 2010; Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere, 2015). Anderson

(2013) investigated psychological and physical stress resulting from in racial and ethnic minorities. The researchers found that African American individuals were more likely to experience both emotional and physical stress compared to other racial and ethnic groups. Diaz, et al. (2001) explored the relationship among racism, poverty, and homophobia in Latino gay and bisexual identifying men. They found higher rates of psychological distress, suicidal ideation, anxiety and in Latino gay and bisexual identifying men who also had high rates of poverty and discrimination in employment. Velez, et al. (2015) investigated the impact of multiple oppressions on mental health specifically in sexual minority Latino/a individuals. They found that internalized and externalized racism and heterosexism, which are the acts of racist and heterosexist discrimination, lead to higher rates of psychological distress; whereas solely

6

and heterosexism lead to lower rates of life satisfaction and self- esteem.

Negative Consequences of Sexism and Heterosexism

Everyday Sexism

Much research has been done to show the negative physical health and mental health outcomes of sexism on women (Klonoff & Landrine, 1995; Koss, Bailey, Yan,

Herrera, & Lichter, 2003; Krieger, 1990; Landrine et al., 1995; Moradi & Subich, 2002).

Sexism’s effects on women’s experiences have long been researched and many different subtypes of sexism have been identified, such as everyday, ambivalent, covert, and neosexism (Benokraitis & Feagin's, 1995; Glick & Fiske, 1996; Swim & Cohen, 1997;

Swim, Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001; Tougas, Brown, Beaton, & Joly, 1995).

Swim, Hyers, Cohen, and Hyers (1998) coined the term “everyday sexism” while exploring the psychological impact of everyday incidents of sexism in women. Furthering the research that examined the insidious nature of racism, everyday sexism also looks at the externalized and internalized nature of the oppression of sexism (Bartky, 1990; Lott,

1995; Swim, Cohen, Hyers, 1998). Swim, et al. (2001) discussed how much of the research has focused on retrospective surveys of women’s everyday experiences which accounts for the blatant forms of discrimination and harassment that women experience daily but can miss out on the subtler forms of discrimination, such as sexist jokes or objectification. The researchers also discussed how much of the research might be missing out on everyday sexist acts because women may experience an additive effect of experiences and it’s difficult to discern each incident or which one affected them the most. In Swim et al.’s (2001) study the researchers conducted three daily diary studies

7

with women and men. The results showed that women experienced approximately one to two experiences of sexist acts that affected them each week. The majority of the experiences were in the areas of traditional stereotyping, demeaning and degrading comments, and . Approximately 75% of women reported and distress over the sexist incidents. They were also more likely to have effects on their psychological well-being, increased discomfort and anxiety, higher rates of depression, and lower self-esteem.

Landrine, Klonoff, Gibbs, Manning, and Lund (1995) also found that women who experience more everyday sexist events have higher rates of psychological distress even when taking into account other stressful life events. Klonoff, Landrine, and Campbell

(2000) had similar results, finding that women who experience more lifetime sexist events have increased anxiety, depression, and somatization. Berg (2006) explored everyday sexism and PTSD in heterosexual women and found that the majority of women (77%) reported having serious issues in their romantic relationships and also experienced high rates of emotional and verbal abuse by their romantic partners (56.7%).

Women also faced high rates of discrimination and sexual harassment at their workplace

(45.9%) and 27% of women reported having a serious mental illness in their lifetimes (ie, panic attacks, eating disorders, suicidal ideation, depression, and hospitalizations). Berg

(2006) found that there was a strong relationship between experiences of everyday sexism and PTSD, with the most predictive variable of the relationship being recent sexist degradation, which includes sexual harassment, sexist name calling, sexist jokes, and feelings of anger about sexism. Berg (2006) argues that not only are these experiences of everyday sexism traumatic in their own right, but that there is a

8

cumulative effect of experiences of trauma and experiences of everyday sexism that may lead to higher rates of posttraumatic symptoms and PTSD. It is important to note that in this sample, 90% were heterosexual-identifying women and the majority of studies on everyday sexism and internalized sexism have had mainly heterosexual samples.

Heterosexism

The research indicates that sexual minority individuals more likely to experience heterosexist events, such as harassment, violence, discrimination and but that these experiences are closely related to negative health and psychological distress, including posttraumatic symptoms and PTSD (Herek et al., 1999; Lewis, Derlega,

Berndt, Morris, & Rose, 2001; Mays & Cochran, 2001; Waldo, 1999). Heterosexism affects LGB individuals in many areas of their lives, such as individual, educational, occupational, familial, social, institutional, political, cultural, and religious settings

(Herek, 1995).

Heterosexism can occur in daily areas of lives that are legally protected for other minorities, such as employment and housing. Employment and still take place for other minorities, but legal protection may lead to increased feelings of protection and legitimacy, and less internalization of the discrimination (Herek, 2009).

Mays and Cochran (2001) found that over 50% of LGB individuals reported at least one daily experience of discrimination based on their LGB identity. Herek (2009) found that

10% of participants reported an incident of employment or housing discrimination while approximately half reported verbal harassment. As stated above, there has been a small amount of research that has linked these experiences to PTSD (Herek et al., 1999;

Szymanski & Balsam, 2011). Szymanski and Chung (2003) specifically found that

9

lesbian women who experienced internalized heterosexism also experienced negative mental health outcomes, such as psychological distress, depression, less social support, and greater demoralization. Bandermann and Szymanski (2014) explored coping mediators between heterosexist oppression and PTSD symptom development in lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. Results of the study indicated that heterosexist victimization and heterosexist discrimination had direct and unique links to PTSD symptom severity. Additionally, Szymanski and Balsam (2011) explored heterosexism’s links to PTSD symptoms by building on the previously discussed study by looking at hate crime victimization and heterosexist discrimination’s links to PTSD symptoms. Results indicated that heterosexist discrimination and hate crime victimization were both positive predictors of PTSD symptoms for lesbian women. Consistent with the sexism literature, the experience of internalized heterosexism (internalizing one’s oppression) leads to greater rates of psychological distress, psychological morbidity, and PTSD.

There have been many approaches for researching experiences of multiple oppressions, such as sexism and heterosexism but few have explored how multiple oppressions affect lesbian women’s experiences with sexist and heterosexist discrimination (external oppression) and internalized sexism and heterosexism.

Theoretical Frameworks of Multiple Oppressions

There have been many differing approaches in the study of oppression. The research on heterosexism and other types of oppression have primarily focused on two theoretical frameworks: theory (Meyer, 1995; Meyer, 2003) and

Multicultural-feminist theories (Moradi & Subich, 2002b; Moradi & Subich, 2003).

Further, the research includes three approaches: additive, interactional (multiplicative),

10

and intersectional. The additive perspective describes multiple oppressions in terms of accumulating together with each form of oppression having unique links to negative mental health consequences (Moradi & Subich, 2003). The interactional approach suggests that multiple forms of oppression aren’t simply additive but that they are multiplicative where one form of oppression can increase the link between another form of oppression and negative mental health consequences (Moane, 1999; Mullaly, 2002;

Young, 1990). The intersectional perspective views oppression as not just additive or interactional which focus on solely oppressed identities, rather all of the identities someone experiences (which may be dominant or subordinate) intersect so that it is not the experience of being in one social group but a synthesis of all identities on how individuals understand and experience their oppression (Collins, 2000).

There is support for all three types of approaches to understanding multiple oppressions; current research on the experiences of heterosexism and sexism focus mainly on the additive and interactional approaches (Ragins, Cornwell, & Miller, 2003;

Szymanski, 2005; Szymanski, 2008; Szymanski, 2009; Szymanski, Kashubeck-West,

Meyer, 2008; Szymanski, 2012; Velez, Moradi & DeBlaere, 2015. Szymanski and

Henrichs-Beck (2014) and Velez, Moradi, and DeBlaere (2015) describe both the additive and interactional approaches as they relate to multiple experiences of external and internal oppression. There is support for the additive approach within the literature on both Minority stress theory (Meyers, 2003) and Multicultural- (Szymanski and Henrichs-Beck, 2014). Both theories posit that there is an additive effect of different types of external and internalized oppressions that lead to increased negative mental health outcomes for minority individuals. In the additive perspective, each form of

11

oppression would have unique and direct links between the experience of external oppression and internalized oppression and they combine additively leading to negative mental health consequences. For example, heterosexist discrimination and internalized heterosexism combine and lead to psychological distress and sexist discrimination and internalized sexism combine and lead to psychological distress.

The interactional approach, or multiplicative approach, is also recognized by both minority stress theory and multicultural feminist theory though how the two theories view the interactions differs. In the minority stress theory (Meyer, 2003) external oppression

(i.e. discrimination experiences) and internalized oppression of the same type of oppression will interact and lead to poor health outcomes. For example, sexist experiences would interact with internalized sexism predicting for higher PTSD symptom severity. Minority stress theory posits that this interaction occurs because not only does internalized oppression predispose minority individuals for self-blame, it also may increase the harmful effects of discrimination experiences.

In the multicultural-feminist theory (Moradi & Subich, 2002b; Moradi & Subich,

2003; Szymanski, Kashubeck-West, & Meyer, 2008; Velex, Moradi, & DeBlaere, 2015) there are two different types of interactions recognized. The multicultural-feminist theory also suggests that externalized oppression and internalized oppression lead to negative mental health outcomes but this theory suggests that it’s because of the combining

(sexism and heterosexism) of multiple oppressions that affect people’s experiences. The first form of interactional multiplicative perspective is similar to the additive perspective in that external and internalized oppression both affect mental health but these perspectives see them as additive in one form; where each form of oppression is unique

12

in its links to mental health. For example, internalized sexism and internalized heterosexism combine and lead to negative mental health consequences. The second form of interactional multiplicative perspective suggests that above and beyond unique relationships, that one form of oppressed identity interacts and increases negative mental health outcomes in the other oppressed identity. For example, externalized sexism and internalized heterosexism interact and one increases psychological distress with the other.

Szymanski (2005) found a significant interaction of heterosexism and sexism in lesbian women who had experienced a hate crime which led to psychological distress.

Intersectional models of multiple oppression posits that there is a synthesis of identity and oppression which creates a new and unique experience based on this synthesis for minority individuals (gendered heterosexism). The new and unique experiences that are created from the synthesis can have a negative impact on mental health (Cole 2009; Collins 1991; Szymanski and Moffitt 2012). Much of the research has focused on the experiences of external and internal gendered racism among African

American women (King 2003; Klevens 2008; Thomas, Witherspoon, & Speight, 2004;

2008; Woods et al. 2008). To date, there has been limited qualitative research on the intersectional approach for lesbian women and a lack of quality measures to assess these constructs fully for the lesbian population. Friedman and Leaper (2010) explored the construct of gendered heterosexism in sexual minority women, as well as, experiences of sexism and heterosexism as predictors of social identity and collective action in college.

The researchers tested gendered heterosexism with a measure that they created for their study, which has only been utilized one other time in their follow-up study (Friedman &

Ayres, 2013). Lesbian women had higher levels of heterosexist discrimination than

13

bisexual women, and after controlling for sexism, heterosexism, and their interaction, gendered heterosexism uniquely predicted social identity and commitment to collective action. The gendered heterosexism scale the authors created has not been utilized in a study exploring mental health outcomes in sexual minority women.

The current study utilized the additive and interactional framework when exploring heterosexism and sexism in lesbian women because of the evidenced support for this framework (Szymanski, 2005; Szymanski & Balsam, 2011; Szymanski & Gupta,

2009; Szymanski & Henrichs-Beck, 2014; Szymanski & Meyer, 2008; Thoma &

Huebner, 2013; Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere, 2014). There has been no study to date that has looked at lesbian women’s experiences with internalized sexism, internalized heterosexism, external sexism, and external heterosexism and subsequent PTSD symptoms. There has also been no study to date that has looked at these constructs as it relates to interpersonal trauma in lesbian women, even though lesbian women experience high rates of interpersonal trauma while also experiencing high rates of sexism and heterosexism, not to mention the potential horizontal oppression (Piggot, 2004; Saakvitne

& Pearlman, 1993; Szymanski et al., 2009) which may lead to increased experiences of violence in intimate partner relationships. All of these experiences may interact and increase posttraumatic symptoms.

Interpersonal Trauma

Trauma is a common experience in women’s lives in our society, as approximately five out of every 10 women will experience a traumatic event in their lifetime (National Center for PTSD, 2014). Women also experience specific types of traumatic events more frequently than men, such as sexual harassment, sexual assault,

14

physical assault, and intimate partner violence (Berg, 2006). Further, women who experience these types of traumatic events are more likely to experience psychological distress and physical health consequences, such as posttraumatic stress disorder

(Campbell, Jones, Dienemann, Schollenberger, O’Campo, & Wynne, 2002; Pico-

Alfonso, Garcia-Linares, Celda-Navarro, Blasco-Ros, Echeburua, & Martinez, 2006).

Historically, literature has suggested that women experience disproportionately higher rates of sex-related traumatic events due to sexism in our society (Herman, 1992; Russo,

1995; Szymanski, 2009, Holmes, Facemire, & DaFonseca, 2016). Lesbian women who experience sexism and heterosexism have also been found to experience higher rates of interpersonal trauma, including intimate partner violence (IPV; NISVS; 2010). Further, emerging literature is focusing on how experiences of oppression can lead to post- traumatic reactions and may affect trauma-related symptoms (Herman, 1992; Bryant-

Davis, & Ocampo, 2005). However, there is little research on how interpersonal trauma may impact lesbian women’s experience of oppression and subsequent PTSD symptoms.

Lesbian women experience higher rates of interpersonal violence than their heterosexual counterparts, including high rates of intimate partner violence (IPV). The

National Intimate Partner and Sexual Violence Survey (NIPSVS; 2010) reports that

46.4% of lesbian women report sexual violence in their lifetimes. In intimate partner relationships, sexual abuse has been reported by up to 50% of lesbian women (Waldner-

Haugrad & Gratch, 1997) and approximately 90% of lesbian women have reported at least one incident of psychological abuse by a romantic partner (Burke & Follinstad,

1999). A more recent study (Simpson & Helfrich, 2005), showed a prevalence rate of 41-

68% of lesbian women reporting IPV. There has been much research to show the link

15

between experiences of IPV and PTSD in women (Campbell, 2002; Kemp, Rawlings, &

Green, 1991). For lesbian women, the experience of sexism and heterosexism along with the high rates of IPV may all affect their development of subsequent PTSD symptoms.

IPV is a pervasive issue in women’s lives which may lead to negative mental health consequences and be impacted by experiences of sexism. However, less is known about how experiences of sexism impact lesbian women in general, and specifically impacts lesbian women who have experienced IPV. The most prevalent mental health consequence for women who have experienced intimate partner violence is PTSD

(Kemp, Rawlings, & Green, 1991; Pico-Alfonso et al., 2006). Approximately 64% of women who have experienced IPV meet diagnostic criteria for PTSD (Jones, Hughes, &

Unterstaller, 2001). Lesbian women experiencing higher rates of IPV, PTSD, and the traumatic nature of the experience of multiple oppressions may impact the relationship between oppression and PTSD symptoms. Lesbian women have unique and important considerations when it comes to their experiences of oppression and IPV experiences.

Same-gender violence has long been ignored and the fear that lesbian women may feel due to a heterosexist climate, lack of resources, fears of reporting, lack of consistent legal definitions across states, and variability in arrest practices may lead women to not report

(Duke & Davidson, 2009). Due to heteronormative views of IPV, women are perceived as the victims of abuse and men are viewed as the perpetrators of abuse, which has historically created a culture that holds the view that women can’t be perpetrators minimizing and dismissing lesbian experiences of IPV (Hassouneh & Glass, 2008; Van

Natta, 2005). The oppressive experiences that lesbian women experience, as well as, the oppressive views of IPV may impact their development of PTSD symptoms.

16

To date, no research has specifically investigated how lesbian women’s experience of sexism, heterosexism, and IPV may impact PTSD symptoms. The current study sought to explore the way the experience of the multiple oppressions (sexism and heterosexism) affect lesbian women’s development of PTSD and what role IPV plays in that relationship. A greater understanding of how multiple oppressions and trauma affect lesbian women’s lives is salient to the tenets of counseling psychology.

Importance to Field of Counseling Psychology

Counseling psychology has been committed to recognizing environmental and situational influences and how they shape experiences and issues of and social justice, which as a mission has emphasized the importance of (Munley,

Duncan, McDonnell, & Sauer, 2004). Vera and Speight (2003) argued that counseling psychology should be committed to social justice. The multicultural and social justice emphasis of counseling psychology also includes better understanding of the lived experiences of lesbian women. Vera and Speight (2003) state

social justice is at the heart of multiculturalism in that the existence of

institutionalized racism, sexism, and homophobia is what accounts for the

inequitable experiences of people of color, women, gay, lesbian, and bisexual

people (among others) in the United States. Moreover, discrimination and

prejudice are intimately connected to quality-of-life issues for these groups of

people (p. 254).

It is important for counseling psychologist’s research not only to be grounded in multiculturalism but also to be committed to social justice, not only to better understand marginalized groups but also to conduct research that could lead to the promotion of

17

social justice action (Vera & Speight, 2003). This tenet of counseling psychology is particularly relevant in this study since it is exploring multiple oppressions in a group of individuals who have rarely been researched and historically have unequal access to resources (Roth, 1985). This study sought to help to understand the effects of interpersonal trauma on women but also, the traumatic nature of the experiences of oppression. To date there have been no studies that have explored internalized heterosexism and everyday sexism in lesbian women’s experiences with IPV and their subsequent posttraumatic reactions. The current study sought to fill the gap in the research that has historically ignored lesbian women’s experiences with IPV, PTSD symptoms, and sexism and heterosexism.

The current study seeks to address limitations of previous studies which explored sexual minority individual’s issues by utilizing research done retrospectively and through convenience sampling. This study aimed to explore important constructs, such as experiences of sexism and heterosexism in a population that has been historically overlooked. The current study also explored the theoretical frameworks of additive, minority stress, and multicultural-feminist to better understand how internalized sexism, external sexism, internalized heterosexism, and external heterosexism interact in lesbian women’s daily lives. Utilizing Velez, Moradi, and DeBlaere’s (2014) model, this study expanded the existing research multiple oppressions and its links to PTSD symptoms.

The current study focused on the additive and interactional models of multiple oppressions when exploring heterosexism and sexism.

The current study also sought to expand the research on IPV in women’s lives, specifically in lesbian women’s lives and how it may impact women’s experiences with

18

sexism and heterosexism. This study sought to help better understand the role of IPV and

PTSD symptoms in a context of women experiencing multiple oppressions. Further, the study sought to fill a gap in the IPV literature for a population of women with historically low access to necessary resources, high stigma, and discrimination.

19

CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

The purpose of the first chapter was to give an overview of the issues concerning interpersonal trauma and multiple oppressions in lesbian women’s lives and the development of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). It specifically highlighted the limitations in previous research and the lack of research on the experiences of sexism and heterosexism in lesbian women who experience interpersonal trauma and the development of subsequent PTSD symptoms. The current chapter critically evaluates the literature on oppression, multiple oppressions, sexism and heterosexism as trauma, sexism and heterosexism’s links to psychological distress and PTSD, and the additive and interactional models of how external and internalized oppression may affect lesbian women may lead to PTSD, and what role the most frequent type of interpersonal trauma in lesbian women (i.e., intimate partner violence (IPV)) may play in these relationships.

Oppression as Trauma

Oppression

Generally, oppression is the social inequality that exists due to one group exercising power over another (Fanon, 1963; Freire, 1970). Oppression has been described as a state or a process. Viewing oppression as a state, suggests that there is a

20

dominant group and a non-dominant group, with the dominant group keeping needed resources away from the non-dominant group (Bartky, 1990; Sidanius, 1993; Young,

1990). The state of oppression therefore is an outcome of the domination of the dominant group over the non-dominant group (Pilleltensky & Gonick, 1996). Mar’i (1988) stated

"Oppression involves institutionalized collective and individual modes of behavior through which one group attempts to dominate and control another in order to secure political, economic, and/or social-psychological advantage" (p. 6). The definition that

Mar’I (1988) describes defines oppression as a process. This not only includes a psychological and political component but requires adoption of these in oppressed individuals to maintain and perpetuate the system of oppression (Fanon, 1967;

Freire, 1970; Memmi, 1965).

Fanon (1967) describes the system of colonialism and the psychological effects of the colonized. When the colonized group’s culture is destroyed creating feelings of inferiority about one’s culture. Memmi (1965) also described the process of psychological control through the destruction of one’s culture during colonization as an integral part of perpetuating the oppression of a subordinate group while maintaining the dominant group’s power and domination. Both Fanon (1967) and Memmi (1965) are describing the reluctant acceptance and ultimately internalization of oppression through the process of colonization but also through socialization in oppressed individuals. Freire

(1970) and Miller (1986) suggest that through the process of oppression both subordinate groups and dominant groups are affected by the experience of oppression, with both groups accepting their given roles and perpetuating the system through socialization.

Inherently, oppression contains psychological processes.

21

Historical and contemporary definitions of oppression include psychological factors of oppression as part of the process. In particular, Bartky (1990) viewed oppression as not just the dominant group trying to retain control politically and economically but also psychologically. The concept that oppression is inherently a state as well as a process of not only, domination by a majority group but also the insidious psychological control of making a marginalized group believe that they are less than the majority group, deserving of less resources, and ultimately deserving of the oppression they are experiencing (Bartky, 1990; Prilleltensky & Gonick, 1996). Thus, oppression can be defined as “a state of asymmetric power relations characterized by domination, subordination, and resistance, where the dominating persons or groups exercise their power by restricting access to material resources and by implanting in the subordinated persons or groups fear or self-deprecating views about themselves” (Prilleltensky &

Gonick, 1996, pp. 129-130). Further, Root (1992) discussed how the psychological and political oppression of marginalized groups (ie., women, racial and ethnic minorities, sexual minorities, gender minorities, and disabled individuals) can lead to something the author suggested was insidious trauma. Insidious trauma is the continuous everyday incidents of discrimination that minority groups endure.

Hardiman, Jackson, and Griffin (2007) describes three levels of oppression: (a) personal or individual (b) institutional or structural, and (c) societal or cultural. Each of these different levels of oppression are unique but together creates a system of oppression where prejudice, discrimination, justification of oppression, and the legitimization of oppression are apparent in all facets of society. The individual level of oppression includes the thoughts, attitudes, and behaviors that express prejudice and discrimination.

22

Institutional levels of oppression are the laws and policies that influence and perpetuate oppression while also legitimizing prejudice and discrimination against minority groups.

Finally, cultural levels of oppression are implicit and explicit values and norms that not only influence discrimination and prejudice but also provide a justification for why they exist (Hardiman et al., 2007). For lesbian and gay individuals, this level of oppression may be rooted in religious beliefs and doctrines that justify discrimination, prejudice, and influence institutional methods of discriminating against them (Feldblum, 2006). All of these levels of oppression intertwine and maintain the system of oppression, whereas one level requires the other levels to operate. This system creates a social hierarchy with rules for how each group should act in society, with some groups being rewarded privileges in power, status, and resources while other groups have little to no power, status, or access of resources (Johnson, 2001; Moane, 1999; Mullaly, 2002). These findings continue to suggest that oppression is a process with psychological consequences.

Young (1990) discusses five ways that groups are oppressed: (a) exploitation, or the inequality of compensation through social processes that maintains class differentials,

(b) marginalization, or the process of making a group have a low standard in society or limiting them to the “margins” of society, (c) powerlessness, which is the maintaining of dominant power so that subordinate groups have limited opportunities for advancement,

(d) culture of silence, which is a type of powerlessness where individuals in marginalized groups do not discuss their oppression, and (e) cultural imperialism, or making the dominant group’s culture the norm in society which may lead to internalized oppression.

Young’s (1990) conditions for oppression can be seen as it relates to race, gender, sexual

23

orientation, or ability status. Another construct of oppression that minority individuals experience is internalized oppression.

Internalized Oppression

Experiences of oppression lead to considerable distress in individual’s lives and minority individuals can also experience internalized oppression, where individuals begin to view themselves as undeserving and unimportant (Prilleltensky & Gonick, 1996).

Internalized oppression is a powerful part of the system of oppression as it is an effect of oppression as well as being necessary for the maintenance of oppression (Hardiman &

Jackson, 1997; Moane, 1999; Mullaly, 2002; Pharr, 1996, 1997). Internalized oppression is experienced by subordinate groups as well as dominant groups and can be experienced simultaneously in an individual’s multiple identities in social groups (ie., race, gender, , ability, class, ). Internalized oppression can also be viewed as a state, process, and/or action (Collins, 2000; Pharr, 1996, 1997; Pheterson, 1986; Moane,

1999). The state of internalized oppression can be the psychological or spiritual thoughts, feelings, and characteristics associated with the subordinate group (Collins, 2000;

Pheterson, 1986; Phar, 1996, 1997). The process of internalized oppression is the perpetuation and maintenance of these internalized beliefs through the socialization and the adoption of them (Pheterson, 1986; Phar, 1996, 1997). Internalized oppression as an action is the everyday behaviors that subordinate groups learn and perform as part of the system (Collins, 2000; Sherover-Marcuse, 1994). Internalized oppression as a state, process, and action can occur independently or together, co-occurring in the daily lives of subordinate groups as well as in the maintenance of oppression (Hardiman & Jackson,

1997).

24

This study will utilize the definition of internalized oppression as a process as well as a psychological state. Brown (1986) defines internalized oppression as “as the process by which a member of an oppressed or stigmatized group internalizes into her or his core identity and self-concept all or part of the negative and expectations held by the culture at larger regarding that group.” (Brown, 1986, p. 100). The internalization of oppression occurs through socialization (Harro, 2010), intergenerational transmission and the adoption of the dominant groups beliefs in subordinate groups of the oppressive system (Fanon, 1967; Freire, 1970; Memmi, 1965).

The experience of internalized oppression may be the most harmful part of the system of oppression. Speight (2007) discusses the harmfulness of internalized racism, arguing that this negative consequence of oppression may actually lead to the most damage to an individual’s well-being. Speight (2007) also points out that as everyday experiences of racism become more subtle and covert. The subtle and covert nature of everyday experiences of racism have assisted in normalizing oppression as part of everyday life making the internalization of the dominant groups values easily transmitted and maintained. This is particularly harmful as the normalization of dominant values leads to the destruction of subordinate groups culture suggesting that the dominant group is normal, and the subordinate group is abnormal or inferior (Hardiman & Jackson, 1997;

Speight, 2007). Speight (2007) further discusses that although experiences of discrimination and harassment may assist the internalization of oppression, an individual doesn’t necessarily have to experience an incident of discrimination or harassment to know that they are viewed as inferior and to internalize that message. This phenomenon makes not only experiences of oppression incredibly harmful but also the internalization

25

of oppression harmful, as well. This has lead researchers to look at oppression as a traumatic experience, one that can potentially lead to PTSD symptoms.

Oppression as Trauma

The negative effects of oppression have long been researched (Adams, 1990;

Bloom, 1997; Klonoff & Landrine, 1995; Moradi & Subich, 2002; Pascoe & Richman,

2009, Velez, Moradi, DeBlaere, 2015) and there is literature to suggest that experiences of oppression lead to negative health and mental health consequences in minority groups.

Some trauma researchers have suggested that the negative effects of experiences of oppression should be considered as traumatic (Root, 1992, Vivero & Jenkins, 1999).

Further, Carter (2007) discussed how these experiences of race-related trauma lead to negative mental health consequences including traumatic stress. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; APA, 2013) defines PTSD as the exposure, witnessing, or learning of a traumatic event that threatens ones’ life, serious injury, or physical integrity. After exposure to a traumatic event, PTSD is characterized by the development of posttraumatic symptoms in four different clusters: (a) intrusion,

(b) avoidance, (c) negative alterations in cognitions and mood, and (d) alterations in arousal and reactivity. Carter (2007) discussed how people of color are more likely to have higher rates of PTSD and experience more severe symptoms of PTSD than White individuals. The author also discusses that people of color experience higher rates of violence, which puts them at higher risk for developing posttraumatic symptoms and

PTSD. This discussion suggests that racism may play a part in the development of PTSD and influence the severity of the symptoms.

26

Further research in this area has illustrated that racism in its subtle, blatant, internalized and insidious forms is traumatic and that there is a link between experiencing oppression and the development of PTSD symptoms (Franklin, Boyd-Franklin, & Kelly,

2006; Helms, Nicolas, Green, 2010). This has led some scholars to call for a change in how the DSM defines trauma in a PTSD diagnosis, specifically in criterion A (definition of trauma) to include oppression-based trauma as part of that definition (Holmes,

Facemire, DaFonseca, 2016; Kira, 2001, 2010; Root, 1992; Carlson, 1997; Bryant-Davis

& Ocampo, 2005; Carter, 2007).

Kira (1997, 2001) offers a taxonomy of trauma that includes experiences of trauma that are not in the traditional definition of a criteria A trauma in the DSM-5 (

APA, 2013), including (a) attachment trauma (divorce, abandonment, parental death, loss of significant others), (b) autonomy/identity/individuation (feelings of inadequacy, alienation, incompetence), (c) interdependence or disconnectedness (moving from schools, moving states, loss of lengthy relationships), (d) achievement/self-actualization

(loss of employment, loss of money, loss of health), (e) survival trauma (war, witnessing death, attempted suicide, homicide), (f) factitious trauma (secondary trauma), (g) indirect or vicarious trauma (), (h) one step transmission of trauma (clinicians, fire fighters, police), (h) generational family trauma transmission (family violence, incest), (i) historical trauma (, Holocaust), and (j) multigenerational transmission of structural violence (poverty). Kira (2010) argues that marginalized groups can experience posttraumatic stress but also suggests that one group may not experience oppression the same way as other groups. However, Gilfus (1999) suggests that expanding criteria A to include oppressive experiences stops the victim-blaming that exists and actually

27

externalizes experiences of oppression as not part of the individual’s doing but the larger society. Giflus (1999) furthers that this inclusion would lead to actually addressing the issue of racism, sexism, heterosexism, and other oppressive experiences rather than pathologizing the individual.

Even though there has been research to show oppression is traumatic and has links to PTSD symptoms, there has been no change in regard to oppression in the current definition of a traumatic event in the most current edition of the DSM; the DSM-V (APA,

2013). The research on oppression as trauma and its link to posttraumatic reactions has historically been discussed when examining race-related trauma. However, there is a gap in the literature when considering the traumatic nature of sexism.

Sex-related Trauma, Sexism as Trauma, and Posttraumatic Reactions

Given the high rates of personal violence women experience, there is an extant amount of literature illustrating the negative effects on the psychological health for survivors of trauma (Campbell, 2000; Johnson, Zlotnick, & Perez, 2008; Kemp,

Rawlings, & Green, 1991; Pico-Alfonso et al., 2006). However, less is known about women’s experiences with sexism and the development of PTSD symptoms. It could also be argued that sexism, like racism, can be viewed as traumatic.

There are many different types of sexism described in the literature, Swim et al.

(1995) described old-fashioned sexism which is the belief in traditional gender stereotypes that hold the notion that women are less capable than men. The adherence of these traditional gender role beliefs leads to the differential treatment of women compared to men. Benokraitis (1997) also described old-fashioned sexism as blatant sex discrimination which is the unconcealed and deliberate mistreatment of women based on

28

the belief that women are unequal. Blatant sex discrimination includes overt and purposeful sexist actions, including: sexist language, harassment, and physical violence.

This can also include overt mistreatment in all areas of a woman’s life, such as employment, education, politics, religion, and the family. Old-fashioned sexism is less accepted in our society than modern forms of sexism (Swim et al., 1995) with some researchers believing that the modern forms of sexism have replaced old-fashioned sexism because of an increase in feminist attitudes as well as, a societal belief that blatant sexism is morally wrong and unacceptable (Morrison, Morrison, Pope, & Zumbo, 1998;

Swim & Cohen, 1997; Twenge, 1997).

Benokraitis and Feagin (1995) coined the term “modern sexism”, in an attempt to describe the different types of overt and covert sexism that women experience. Modern sexism is conceptualized as being comprised of four different types of sexist discrimination: everyday (Benokraitis and Feagin's, 1995), ambivalent (Glick & Fiske,

1996), covert (Tougas, Brown, Beaton, & Joly, 1995), and neosexism (Swim & Cohen,

1997; Swim, Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001). Glick and Fiske (1996) conceptualized as having two different constructs: benevolent sexism and hostile sexism. Benevolent sexism is the concept that stereotypical beliefs about gender roles that view women as less competent and weaker than men can be covert and seem positive in nature. For example, the traditional belief that men are physically stronger than women may lead a man to believe that he needs to help women in physical tasks. At a societal level, this seemingly positive behavior can lead to women being discriminated against when seeking jobs that may require them to do highly physical tasks, such as: military careers, police and fire department careers. Conversely, hostile sexism is the concept that

29

traditional views about gender roles leads to prejudicial beliefs about women and mistrust. Unlike benevolent sexism, hostile sexist acts are overt and blatant, ie., degradation, sexual harassment, rape, physical violence. Glick and Fiske (1996) view these constructs as independent yet related as both constructs lead to prejudicial attitudes towards women.

Tougas, Brown, Beaton, and Joly (1995) and Becker and Swim (2011) define neosexism as the belief that discrimination against women doesn’t exist and the resentment towards any equality movements. Neosexism is another subtle form of sexism that leads to continued discrimination against women. Covert sexism is also conceptualized as a subtle form of sexism and focuses on the act of engaging in discriminatory and deleterious treatment of women but doing so in a hidden fashion. For example, individuals may outwardly state that they support gender equality but behave in ways that hinder and sabotage women (Benokraitis & Feagin, 1995; Swim & Cohen,

1997). Covert sexism can be particularly insidious as individuals may outwardly support gender equality while intentionally engaging in sexist behaviors to make women fail.

Swim, Hyers, Cohen, and Ferguson (2001) conceptualized everyday sexism as the mundane everyday experiences of sexism that women encounter. Everyday sexism is another form of subtle sexism and Swim et al. (2001) note that it is also likely that the numerous incidents of sexism that women experience in a day may go unnoticed because the acts can be ambiguous in nature and women may have trouble with identifying them as sexist. These everyday experiences of sexism may have a cumulative effect on women and lead to distress (Swim et al., 2001). Gender microaggressions which can be defined as the intentional or unintentional common daily verbal, behavioral, or environmental

30

incidents that women experience which include derogatory and sometimes hostile insults.

Microaggressions tend to be unconscious to the individual committing them as they don’t necessarily know that they are doing something that is harming to another individual

(Capodilupo, et al., 2010; Nadal, Rivera, & Corpus, 2010; Nadal, 2011). Dovidio and

Gaertner (1998) and Twenge (1997) describe that oppression is becoming more subtle and covert and have become a daily experience in the lives of marginalized groups, including women and sexual minorities.

Even though there are many different terms for the experience of sexism, the more modern forms tend to focus on the perception of the discriminatory experience. The term “perceived discrimination” has debated as of recently, with some researchers finding the term offensive and likening it to the color-blind approach in the race literature. The term “perceived” conveys that discrimination might not actually be occurring or that the individual might be unrealistic with the idea that they are experiencing discrimination, when actually it is just simply discrimination and is a realistic ever-occurring experience

(Neville, Awad, Flores, 2013; Banks, 2014). However, some argue that the literature on perceived discrimination research is attempting to highlight the everyday incidents of discrimination that minority population’s experience and the subsequent distress that it causes.

The small base of literature on the intersection of sexism, trauma, and PTSD symptoms focuses on the perceived type of sexism termed “everyday sexism”. Klonoff and Landrine’s (1995) groundbreaking work on sexist discrimination, explored the concept of subtle everyday experiences of sexism and its effects on women. The researchers described sexual harassment, sexist name-calling, and systemic

31

discrimination, and how these daily experiences in women’s lives lead to psychological distress above and beyond regular life stress. The researchers also introduced the concept of chronic sexist discrimination and noted that the impact of having regular lifetime experiences of sexist discrimination may have different consequences in comparison to women who experience acute forms of discrimination.

Building on this research, Landrine and Klonoff’s (1997) subsequent study examined experiences of everyday sexism and results indicated that of the 652 women and of those women, 99% had experienced at least one sexist experience in their lifetime.

Swim, Hyers, Cohen, and Ferguson (2001) furthered this research by coining the term

“everyday sexism” and highlighting these everyday sexist events as harmful when accumulated over time. The researchers asked a sample of 40 women to keep diaries of their daily experiences of sexist events for two weeks. Results indicated that the women in their sample reported a mean of 2.05 sexist experiences a week with a range from zero to nine sexist experiences. The most common sexist experience reported was in the category of "traditional gender role prejudice and stereotyping", which comprised of incidents such as comments about stereotypical female traits, roles that are appropriate and not appropriate for women, and incidents of double standards for women. The findings suggest that women are experiencing these subtle types of sexist acts at high rates in their daily lives, but they are also experiencing such high rates of blatant overt sexism, that the everyday subtle sexist events may be going unnoticed by women, even though results still show they are leading to distress. The current study will utilize everyday sexism as the accumulated experiences of sexist experiences that lead to greater distress.

32

The role of internalized sexism has also been explored and this byproduct of oppression has been found to have a negative impact on women's psychological health.

Szymanski, Gupta, Carr, and Stewart (2009) explored internalized sexism, self- objectification, and the acceptance of traditional gender roles. With a sample of 274 heterosexual women and found that women who experience more reported sexist events also experienced higher levels of psychological distress. Interestingly, the researchers also found that internalized misogyny, a type of internalized sexism that describes the mistrust and dislike of other women based on an internalized belief that men are the superior gender, was a greater predictor of psychological distress in women. However, when sexist events and internalized misogyny were analyzed together, sexist events became the greater predictor of psychological distress over internalized misogyny. These results indicate that sexist events may lead to increased psychological distress in women and also internalized sexism intensifies the psychological distress women experience.

Additionally, these findings suggest that Speight’s (2007) argument discussed in the previous section about internalized racism in racial minorities may also be true for other marginalized populations, such as women. The comparison can be made between

Szymanski et al.’s (2009) results and Speight’s (2007) argument, since the findings indicate that women’s beliefs that they are less valuable than men actually lead them to experience sexist events in a more distressing way, which may be because they have internalized the sexism they experience and believe the sexist values of the oppressor.

Extending this mechanism, experiencing the distress of internalized sexism may influence and worsen women’s posttraumatic reactions when they experience a more overt traumatic event as well (ie. IPV or sexual assault). These types of overt sexist

33

experiences may affect women’s experiences with trauma and lead to the development of

PTSD symptoms.

Sexism and PTSD

In considering the link between sexism and PTSD, Berg (2006) explored everyday sexism and its effects on PTSD symptoms in 382 women. Berg was influenced by the work of Vivero and Jenkins (1999), who argued that discrimination based on sex and other marginalized identities effects self-esteem and well-being. Further, they argued that “discrimination trauma” be added to the PTSD diagnostic definition of criterion A in the DSM. Berg (2006) asserts that women generally report higher levels of PTSD than men and more severe symptomatology, even when factors such as, the number of traumatic events is controlled for. The researcher was primarily interested in exploring whether women who experience greater numbers of everyday sexist events experience higher levels of traumatic symptoms. Berg (2006) utilized a sample of 382 women who completed a measure of sexist events, a stress checklist based on gender, and a PTSD measurement. She found that 100% of women in her sample had experienced a sexist incident in the past year and within their lifetime. More than a third of the women also reported having been abused verbally, emotionally, or physically, sexually molested, sexually assaulted, or had been in abusive relationships with men. Results also showed that there was a relationship between everyday sexism and PTSD, and that recent sexist degradation (sexual harassment and disrespect) was the most predictive variable for developing PTSD symptoms. The results of this study suggest that sexism both affects women's posttraumatic reactions and their experience with trauma. It could also be argued that the everyday sexist event of sexist degradation may lead to posttraumatic

34

symptoms and PTSD, as well. Future research should seek to further explore the relationship between the everyday sexist act of sexist degradation and the development of posttraumatic symptoms.

Reisner et al.’s (2016) study of 452 individuals and gender nonconforming women explored the transgender individuals’ experiences with discrimination, reasons attributed to discrimination, childhood trauma, intimate partner violence, depression, and PTSD symptom development. Reisner et al. (2016) utilized an everyday discrimination scale and then asked participants to identify the reason they attributed to each incident of discrimination experienced. Results indicated higher rates of

PTSD than the general population, experiences with IPV and childhood trauma even though when controlling for these experiences the high rates of PTSD still existed, indicating that regardless of trauma history, the experience of discrimination was still associated with PTSD. The results of this study suggest the importance of looking at experiences of oppressed identities including gender when looking at the impact on the development of PTSD symptoms. Reisner et al. (2016) also argues that because IPV rates are higher in LGBT women that research should be conducted further exploring the impact of IPV in the experience of oppression, as LGBT individuals experience IPV at higher rates and likely experience it due to discrimination related to their identity.

Balsam and Szymanski (2005) and Szymanski and Stewart (2010) discuss the importance of exploring intersecting identities when examining sexism’s effects on women. The intersection of identities is important to note as the researchers discuss how the more numerous the oppressed identities, the greater the experiences of discrimination and posttraumatic distress. Further, the intersection of these identities can lead to unique

35

forms of discrimination, which in turn may have increased negative psychological effects

(Szymanski, 2005). The researchers explored the concept of insidious trauma on lesbian women and their experiences of sexism and heterosexism, as well as their subsequent posttraumatic symptoms. The researchers explored this by utilizing two types of events: a traumatic event that would meet criterion A for a PTSD diagnosis (ie. heterosexist hate crime victimization) and also sexist and heterosexist discrimination (which does not meet the criterion A definition of trauma), and they examined the subsequent PTSD symptoms.

Results of both studies indicated that the experience of sexist and heterosexist discrimination both predicted for PTSD symptoms in lesbian women regardless of whether they experienced a hate crime or not (Szymanski, 2005; Szymanski & Stewart,

2010).

These findings suggest that sexism and heterosexism were as traumatic as experiencing a criterion A trauma (ie. hate crime victimization) and both lead to the development of PTSD symptoms. This supports the notion that criterion A of the DSM should be modified to include oppressive experiences as a traumatic event. It also suggests that sexist experiences may lead to an increase in PTSD symptomology in women regardless of experience of external oppression, especially women with intersecting minority identities, such as lesbian women. Since sexist external oppression can also include experiences like IPV, rape, and sexual assault, the findings would suggest that the experience of heterosexist external oppression (hate crime victimization) and having multiple identities that are oppressed may have a serious effect on women’s psychological health. These relationships should continue to be explored in future studies. The literature on sexism and its effects on trauma, sexism as a form of trauma,

36

and sexism’s effects on subsequent posttraumatic reactions suggest that sexism as a form of oppression does impact women’s experiences with trauma and posttraumatic reactions.

Heterosexism, Heterosexism as Trauma, and Posttraumatic Reactions

Heterosexism as trauma.

Another form of oppression that can lead to emotional distress and traumatic symptomatology in lesbian women is heterosexism (Otis & Skinner, 1996). Neisen

(1993) defined heterosexism as the continuous traumatic exposure to discrimination and prejudice in the lives of lesbian, gay, and bisexual (LGB) individual’s that influences their psychological well-being. Balsam (2003) explored heterosexism in lesbian and bisexual identifying women and how they experience what the author coined “cultural trauma” based on the traumatic nature of identity development and experiences of homophobia as a sexual minority. There may be a gendered element to heterosexist experiences and psychological trauma for women specifically, as men report more traumatic experiences than women overall but women experience higher rates of violence based on their gender and are also disproportionately more likely to develop PTSD and

PTSD symptoms during their lives (Norris, Foster, & Weishhar, 2002).

Cochran (2001) argues that utilizing the insidious trauma framework discussed in prior sections is also relevant for understanding the experiences and psychological well- being of LGB individuals. The author also reports a large disparity in mental health symptoms and disorders when compared to heterosexual individuals. There is much research that has been conducted that has found a relationship between LGB individual’s experiences with heterosexism, harassment, and violence leading to psychological distress, adverse health-related outcomes, and job-related consequences (Herek et al.,

37

1999; Lewis, Derlega, Berndt, Morris, & Rose, 2002; Mays & Cochran, 2001; Waldo,

1999).

Meyer (1995) explored the stigmatization of LGB identifying individuals and their experiences with oppressive events, coining the term heterosexism. Herek (1995) discusses the impact of heterosexism in LGB individuals’ experience of daily life.

Heterosexism may occur across many domains, including: educational, occupational, familial, social, and religion. An individual’s experience of heterosexism can vary from discrimination to harassment and violence. Mays and Cochran (2001) explored a sample of 73 LGB identifying individuals and 2,844 heterosexual identifying individuals surveying all participants on their daily and lifetime experiences with discrimination. The researchers also assessed for participant’s current and one-year prevalence for psychological distress, depression, anxiety, substance dependence, and overall rating of mental health. LGB identifying individuals reported higher rates of daily and lifetime discrimination. Approximately 73% of LGB individuals reported an incident of discrimination compared to 65% of heterosexual individuals. Furthermore, 42% of LGB individuals attributed their experiences of discrimination to their sexual orientation in contrast to 98% of heterosexual individuals believing their discrimination was not based on their sexual orientation. The researchers also found that perceived discrimination experiences were positively related to psychological distress, quality of life, and psychiatric morbidity. When the researchers controlled for the difference in discrimination experiences, psychiatric morbidity and sexual orientation were related.

The results of this study suggest that perceived heterosexism and experiences of

38

discrimination may lead to higher rates of psychological distress, and higher rates of psychiatric morbidity in LGB individuals.

Exploring prevalence of hate crimes and more severe forms of violence against

LGB individuals is not a new topic of research in the last few decades. The National

Institute of Mental Health in 1989 held an expert panel addressing that prevalence data for antigay violence should be a top priority for research and urged researchers to help collect data to assist the National Coalition of Anti-Violence Programs in the United

States (Herek & Berill, 1990; Harlow, 2005). In the past, data from the Federal Bureau of

Investigation had been used and did show that historically leads to criminal acts on LGB individuals, but they had only utilized convenience samples and failed to show how widespread LGB victimization is (Herek & Sims, 2007). Berill (1992) explored data from 1977 and 1991 including 24 studies, and found that all but one of these studies used a convenience sample of LGB individuals. The author argues that this is unique to heterosexist oppression as compared to other forms of oppression. The researcher found that 9% of the sample reported an aggravated assault with a weapon which they reported being due to their sexual orientation and 17% of respondents reported physical assault without a weapon due to their sexual orientation. Of the respondents, 44% reported threats of violence, 33% reported being chased or followed,

25% had objects thrown in their direction, 13% reported being spit on, and 80% reported being verbally harassed. Of the 24 studies, the majority did not separate data by their sexual orientation or gender (Berill, 1992).

Some research has been conducted focusing on LGB individuals as it relates to their age to explore the rates and pervasiveness of external heterosexism across the

39

lifetime in LGB individuals. Huebner, Rebchook, and Kegeles (2004) found in a sample of 1,248 gay and bisexual men aged 18-27 years that 5% reported experiencing physical violence against them in the last 6 months due to their sexual orientation. Pilkington and

D’Augelli (1995) explored a sample of 194 LGB individuals aged 15-21 years and found that 9% reported at least one aggravated assault with a weapon, 18% had experienced assault, 22% had been sexually assaulted, and 44% had been threatened with violence.

All of the respondents reported their victimizations being due to their sexual orientation.

D’Augelli and Grossman (2001) explored older LGB adults in their study of lifetime occurrence of hate crimes. There sample was 416 LGB individuals over the age of 59 years in the United States. The sample included 16% of individuals that reported being physically attacked in their lifetime, 7% had been sexually assaulted, and 29% had been threatened with violence. Szymasnki (2009) examined the relationship between heterosexist events and psychological distress in gay and bisexual identifying men. The sample included 210 gay and bisexual identifying men and the results indicated that men experienced at least one incident of rejection, harassment, or discrimination in the last year of their lives. Interestingly, rejection about gay and bisexual men was the highest with 52% reporting anti-gay remarks from family members, 34% reporting being rejected by family due to their sexual orientation, and 49% being treated unfairly by their family members. Experiences of heterosexism was related to psychological distress in gay and bisexual men. Further, self-esteem moderated the relationship between heterosexist events and psychological distress with low self-esteem being associated with increased risk of psychological distress.

40

Herek (2009) summarized many of these studies and argued that the way the data were reported was problematic in their categorizations of crimes and time frames, and because the majority of the studies utilized convenience samples, it becomes impossible to get an accurate prevalence rating of victimization against LGB individuals in the

United States. The author addressed the gap in the literature by not only looking at prevalence data for violence against the LGB population but also looking at their experiences with internalized stigma. The author conceptualizes these widespread experiences of violence in the LGB community as a result of sexual stigma, which is another term for heterosexism. Herek (2004, 2007, 2009) defines sexual stigma as the negative view by society for individuals who seem non-heterosexual due to their beliefs, identities, behaviors, relationships, or community. Sexual stigma leads to the belief system that is lesser than and therefore manifests as discrimination at the individual and institutional levels, as well as leading to violent crimes against LGB individuals (Herek, 2009; Herek, Chopp, & Strohl, 2007). Herek

(2009) also explored a form of internalized stigma, which the author named felt stigma.

Stemming from the work by Scrambler & Hopkins (1986) felt stigma is an internalized response to the fear or threat of enacted stigma. The awareness of the threat of enacted stigma may lead LGB individuals to feel a subjective sense of threat which may cause them to act in protective ways to combat potential enacted stigma. For example, LGB individuals may hide their sexual orientation from others. This seemingly protective act can lead to many negative consequences, including feelings of shame, lack of normal social interaction, and potentially not obtaining necessary social support (Herek, 2007;

Herek, 2009).

41

Herek’s (2009) utilized a sample of 662 gay, lesbian, and bisexual identifying individuals in the United States and looked at eight types of enacted sigma in the areas of criminal victimization, harassment and threats, and discrimination (employment). The author assessed for felt stigma by asking three statements: (a) “Most people where I live think less of a person who is gay.” (b) “Most employers where I live will hire openly gay people if they are qualified for the job.” (c) “Most people where I live would not want someone who is openly gay to take care of their children.” The term “gay”, “lesbian”, or

“bisexual” were substituted in the questions based on how the participant initially responded. Results indicated that approximately 20% of LGB individuals had experienced a criminal victimization based on their sexual orientation. When factoring in attempted criminal victimization the percent increases to 25%. Half of the sample reported harassment (verbal abuse) and this was the most widespread experience of LGB individuals in the sample. Also, one out of every 10 in the sample reported housing or employment discrimination based on their sexual orientation. reported the highest rates of physical and property victimization with approximately 38% experiencing physical crimes, property crimes, or both types of criminal victimization.

Lesbian women were more likely than bisexual identifying men and women to experience criminal victimization and harassment. Results of the felt stigma data suggest that approximately 55% of the sample indicated some degree of felt stigma. Felt stigma was overall significantly higher in individuals who had experienced criminal victimization and employment discrimination in their lifetime. This data suggests that some individuals may attribute violent acts to their sexual orientation and more subtle acts (verbal abuse) as less about their sexual orientation because it is more ambiguous

42

(Herek, 2009). Given the retrospective self-report nature of the study no causal relationship can be determined. Although this study was groundbreaking in its exploration of the prevalence of LGB violence and internalized stigma, there are limitations to the study further than just its statistical analysis. Methodologically, the construct of felt stigma may have many more nuances than beliefs and feelings that you are inferior to heterosexual individuals because of your sexual orientation. For instance, the construct of felt stigma may also include expectations of rejection, hiding and concealing, and negative coping strategies (Meyer, 2007).

Results of these studies demonstrate the insidious nature of heterosexism and its effects on LGB identifying individuals ranging from the more subtle forms of verbal abuse, rejection, and harassment to the more blatant forms of physical assault, sexual assault, and aggravated assault. LGB individuals are not just at risk for hate crimes based on their sexual orientation but they are also more likely than heterosexual individuals to experience other forms of traumas in their lifetimes, such as childhood sexual, physical, and psychological abuse (Balsam, Rothblum, & Beauchaine, 2005; Corliss, Cochran, &

Mays, 2002; Hughes, Johnson, & Wilsnack, 2001). The prevalence for LGB individuals to experience sexual, physical, and psychological abuse in their families, as well as, rejection from their family members based on their sexual orientation (Szymanski, 2009) indicates that LGB individuals are at a higher risk for being targets of abuse in their childhood and adolescent by their family based on their sexual orientation.

Further, Herek et al (1999) explored the experiences of LGB-related hate crimes and the adverse mental health consequences of this blatant form of heterosexism.

Historically, LGB discrimination and prejudice has been accepted in our society and is

43

impacted by laws that are implemented in many states that make discrimination against sexual minorities legal and acceptable. The author discusses that when a LGB individual experiences a hate crime they may feel as if it was their fault or they deserve it based on the knowledge that their discrimination is not only legal but acceptable. Depending on where they are in their identity development, the individual may internalize these beliefs and ultimately have psychological distress based on their sexual orientation (Garnets et al., 1990). Much of LGB identity development is done through external sources and interactions, with many LGB individuals receiving negative messages from their family, community, and peers at an early age without learning effective coping skills for these experiences. Consequently, when an LGB individual experiences blatant heterosexist acts, ie hate crimes, they may be more likely to internalize the experience and believe it was just, which may lead to increased psychological distress and trauma-related symptoms (Herek et al, 1999).

Herek et al. (1999) looked at 1170 women and 1089 men who identified as LGB living in California. Approximately 1/4 of the men and 1/5 of the women had experienced an LGB hate crime at least once in their lifetime and approximately 2/5 of men and more than half of the women had experienced a crime that was not due to their sexual orientation, such as burglary and theft. In this study, men were more likely than women to experience an LGB hate crime but were less likely to experience a general crime against them, ie sexual assault. Moreover, individuals who had experienced a hate crime based on their sexual orientation within the past five years were more likely to have experienced increased anger, anxiety, depression, and posttraumatic stress compared

44

to individuals who had experienced non-LGB related crimes and individuals that had no experiences with crimes.

Szymanski and Chung (2003) explored the construct of internalized heterosexism in their study on internalized heterosexism for lesbian woman. This was in response to previous criticism of the definition of homophobia since it is not an irrational fear of lesbian individuals. The authors stated “lesbianism is a real threat to heteropatriarchal values and structures” and thus heterosexism is a better conceptualization of the oppression lesbian women experience (Kitzinger, 1996). The authors discuss the construct of internalized heterosexism broadly as LGB individuals internalizing society’s negative beliefs, values, attitudes, and assumptions about homosexuality (Szymanski &

Chung, 2003). Internalized heterosexism is believed to be something that all LGB individuals will experience in their lifetime and is viewed as a necessary developmental experience as a result of living their daily lives in a heterosexist society and experiencing heterosexist discrimination (Shidlo, 1994).

Previous research on internalized heterosexism in LGB individuals finds that transcending these internalized beliefs and growing a positive internalized identity about their LGB identity is the main construct in identity formation (Cass, 1979; Coleman,

1982; Lewis, 1984). However, until LGB individuals are able to overcome internalized heterosexism it is considered to be a great stressor in their lives and daily experiences

(Shidlo, 1994; Szymanski & Chung, 2003). Literature has found the concept of internalized heterosexism in lesbian and bisexual identifying women to be linked to depression, general distress, demoralization, alcoholism, low self-esteem, loneliness, and decreased social support (Radonsky & Borders, 1996; Herek et al., 1997; DiPlacido,

45

1998; McGregor et al., 2001; Szymanski et al., 2001). McGregor et al. (2001) studied the construct of internalized sexism in lesbian women who were being treated for early detected breast cancer and found that increased internalized heterosexism was related to increased psychological distress. However, the authors recognize the limitation of co- occurring experiences that can lead to psychological distress, such as cancer. None of these studies have specifically looked at factors associated with internalized heterosexism and internalized sexism in lesbian women.

Szymanski and Chung (2003) were the first authors to specifically look at factors associated with internalized heterosexism in lesbian women solely. One of the factors theorized in previous literature but never explored was the relationship between internalized heterosexism and feminist identification, attitudes about , access to resources for coping, and involvement in the feminist community. The sample included

210 lesbian women and the authors found that internalized heterosexism was related to feminist constructs and suggests that lesbian women who also identify with feminist values have less internalized heterosexism and also were found to have more access to coping resources. The authors also argue that feminism is a resource for coping for lesbian women to cope with and combat oppression they experience. The findings are also consistent with previous research that suggests that sexism and heterosexism are linked and intersect to form a unique form of oppression for bisexual and lesbian women

(Herek, 1994).

There have only been three studies to date that explored heterosexism and internalized heterosexism’s effects on psychological health. Rotosky and Riggle’s (2002) study explored external and internalized heterosexism in lesbian and gay identifying

46

individuals in the work place. The researchers found that if an individual had less internalized heterosexism and the job site they worked at had a nondiscrimination policy in place, they were more likely to be more satisfied at their job and also more likely to publically identify themselves as lesbian or gay. Meyer’s (1995) study which focused on gay men and their experiences of discrimination and violence, internalized heterosexism, and perceived stigma, which were all found to be related to greater psychological distress. This study showed that internalized heterosexism moderates the relationship between experiencing external heterosexism (discrimination and violence) and psychological distress in gay men.

There has been only one study to date specifically looking at psychological distress from heterosexist events and internalized heterosexism in lesbian women.

Szymanski (2005) looked at the relationship between heterosexism and internalized heterosexism in 143 lesbian women that had experienced perceived heterosexist harassment, rejection, and discrimination, and their subsequent psychological distress.

The researcher found that lesbian women who had experienced a recent perceived act of externalized heterosexism had higher rates of psychological distress. Inconsistent with previous literature (Meyers, 1995) on gay men, they did not find that internalized heterosexism moderated the relationship between externalized heterosexism and psychological distress. These findings suggest that unlike the literature on gay men, lesbian women experience negative mental health consequences due to external heterosexism regardless of having internalized heterosexist beliefs (Szymanski & Chung,

2003). It is difficult to conclude whether there are gender differences from both of these findings about internalized heterosexism, as the authors used different measures on all of

47

their variables. There has been no research to date to further look at these constructs in lesbian women. Another important construct when exploring heterosexism as trauma is its links to posttraumatic stress disorder.

Heterosexism and PTSD.

From the previous literature discussed, it is clear that heterosexism and internalized heterosexism can be traumatic for LGB individuals and lead to psychological distress. Unlike the literature on sexism’s links to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), even less is known about heterosexism and posttraumatic reactions. Many of the studies on LGB individuals’ psychological distress have focused on increased prevalence of heterosexist violence and mental health as compared to heterosexual individuals

(Cochran, 2001; Meyer, 2003). Mays and Cochran (2001) explored this link between higher rates of heterosexist events and psychological distress in LGB individuals. The researchers compared LGB experiences of daily incidents of discrimination with heterosexual identifying individuals and found that more than three thirds of LGB individuals experienced an incident of discrimination that they perceived was due to their sexual orientation. The researchers also found that 41.8% of LGB individuals indicated that they had any psychiatric disorder in the past year compared to 16.8% of heterosexual individuals. The rates for indicating that they had current high psychological distress for

LGB individuals was 26.8% compared to 16.8% of heterosexual-identifying individuals.

Previous literature has also demonstrated that experiences of LGB hate crimes has led to increased psychological distress in the areas of depression, anxiety, suicidal ideation, and self-esteem (Herek, Gillis, & Cogan, 1999; Otis & Skinner, 1996;

Szymanski, 2005; Waldo et al., 1998). Research has also been found to suggest that

48

experiences of heterosexism are related to psychological distress in racially and ethnically diverse samples of LGB individuals (Szymanski, 2006; Szymanki & Meyer,

2008; Szymanski, 2009; Sung, 2010) even though no research has been conducted to solely look at the intersection of racism and heterosexism in LGB individual’s experiences with traumatic heterosexist events and their posttraumatic reactions.

There have been three studies conducted that have explored PTSD symptoms in

LGB individuals and externalized heterosexism. Herek et al. (1999) which was discussed in the previous section found that LGB individuals who had experienced a hate crime based on their sexual orientation within the last 3-5 years had greater PTSD symptoms than LGB individuals who had not experienced a hate crime. D’Augelli, Grossman, and

Starks (2006) examined LGB individuals experiences with victimization based on their sexual orientation, current mental health, trauma symptoms, and PTSD. Their sample included 528 LGB youth (15-19 years) and they were assessed three times over the course of two-years on victimization and mental health symptoms. Approximately 80% reported verbal abuse, 14% reported physical assault, and 9% reported sexual assault because of their LGB identity. The participants reported verbal abuse beginning at the age of 11 years for males and 14 years for that they related to being perceived as

LGB. The participants also reported that nearly all of their perpetrators were male and males were more likely to report victimization than females. The authors theorize that because they were also exploring “gender atypical behavior” in childhood, that the higher rates in males could be based on society’s acceptance of female nontraditional behavior in youth () compared to male nontraditional behavior (D’Augelli, Grossman, &

Starks, 2006). These findings highlight the importance of looking at sexism as it interacts

49

with heterosexism in LGB individuals. D’Augelli, Grossman, and Starks (2006) also found that 9% of LGB youth met criteria for PTSD and it was related to both physical and verbal violence. Females were significantly more likely to have a PTSD diagnosis than males.

Szymanski and Balsam (2011) is the only study to explore experiences of heterosexism and PTSD symptoms specifically in lesbian women. There is a large literature base that shows that lesbian women experience higher rates of trauma in their lifetimes than heterosexual women, including childhood abuse and abuse in adulthood

(Balsam, Rothblum, & Beauchaine, 2005; Corliss, Cochran, & Mays, 2002; Hughes,

Johnson, & Wilsnack, 2001; Moracco, Runyan, Bowling, & Earp, 2007; Tjaden,

Thoeness, & Allison, 1999). Lesbian-women are also experiencing a wide range of traumatic events based on their lesbian identity or perceived lesbian-identity, as well.

Herek, Gillis, and Cogan (1999) found that 19% of lesbian women reported an incident of lifetime physical assault based on their identity. Herek (2009) that was outlined in the previous section found that 12.5% of lesbian women had experienced a physical assault in their lifetime and 54% had been verbally abused or verbally harassed. With the evidence that lesbian women experience higher rates of traumatic events in their lifetimes, higher rates of violence, and the fact that women in general experience higher rates of PTSD, it is shocking that only one study to date has been done to look at these constructs in lesbian women.

Szymanski and Balsam (2011) had a sample of 247 lesbian women that had experienced either a heterosexist-based hate crime or general heterosexist discrimination

(harassment, rejection) and their subsequent PTSD symptoms. The authors explored both

50

of these types of experiences because a hate crime would meet criteria A for PTSD where heterosexist discrimination would not, arguing that the PTSD diagnosis should include experiences of oppression as traumatic. Both types of heterosexist acts were found to both be predictors of PTSD symptoms in lesbian women. These findings suggest that experiences of heterosexism in general lead to PTSD symptomatology in lesbian women.

Szymanski and Balsam’s (2011) findings also suggest that experiences of oppression are experienced as traumatic and can lead to negative mental health consequences, including PTSD. This adds to a growing literature base (Alessi, Meyer, &

Martin, 2011; Gold, Marx, Soler-Baillo, & Sloan, 2005; Holmes, Facemire, DaFonseca,

2016; Long et al., 2008; Weathers & Keane, 2007) that suggests that Criteria A be adapted to include experiences of discrimination as traumatic and that they can lead to the development of PTSD. Szymanski and Balsam (2011) also found that self-esteem only partially mediated and did not moderate the relationship between heterosexist discrimination and PTSD symptoms. This finding suggests that regardless of how a lesbian woman feels about herself, heterosexist experiences still cause negative mental health consequences.

Intimate Partner Violence

Another form of trauma that lesbian women are likely to experience is intimate partner violence (IPV). Previous literature suggested that LGB experiences with IPV were as common as heterosexual partner violence, and some suggest that IPV rates are even higher in sexual minority women (Farley, 1996; Renzetti, 1992). Similarly to heterosexual women who experience IPV, lesbian women are just as likely to hide the violence they experience (Lobel, 1986). Historically, people have not recognized same

51

gender relationships because intimate partner violence has mostly been portrayed as male violence against women. LGB relationships have exclusively been viewed as having less apparent power differentials as there is no male and female dynamic and therefore people have viewed this as a mutual behavior which has led to the belief that the victim has a part in their abuse (Farley, 1996; Renzetti, 1992). The majority of the research on intimate partner violence in same gender relationships has mostly been done on gay identifying men. Island and Letellier (1991) approximated that around 500,000 gay men are victims of intimate partner violence. There is less known about the prevalence of abuse in lesbian relationships and reports vary widely.

The reports of intimate partner violence in same gender relationships are likely low due to internalized heterosexism, stigma, fear, and a shortage of services for lesbian women (Renzetti, 1996). Renzetti (1996) discusses the fears associated with lesbian women utilizing shelter services. Lesbian women report a fear of rejection from shelters and shelter residents, and a fear that a same gender perpetrator could gain access to them in the shelter system. Research also suggests that protection orders fail in general for victims of intimate partner violence with evidence suggesting that the majority of intimate partner violence perpetrators offend again within two years of having a protective order on them, 29% have been found to offend with severe violence in their subsequent acts (US Department of Justice, 1998). Many states legal systems do not give the same protection to individuals in same gender relationships (DaLuz, 1994; Fray-

Witzer, 1999; Lundy & Leventhal, 1999; NCAVP 1997) and within states that do have protections, it’s likely that law enforcement agents and judges will not enforce them

52

equally to same gender victims as they do with heterosexual partnered victims (Lundy &

Leventhal, 1999).

These findings highlight the importance of examining lesbian women’s experiences with heterosexism and sexism, trauma especially intimate partner violence, and subsequent posttraumatic reactions and PTSD. Research has focused on exploring multiple oppressions either in an additive framework or in an interactional framework.

Relevant research will be discussed.

Theorizing Multiple Oppressions

As outlined above the psychological stress that occurs for being a member of a minority group has long been researched, especially for racial and ethnic minorities

(Allport, 1954; Crocker & Major, 1989). There are three primary approaches to investigating the impact of multiple oppressions: additive, interactional (i.e. multiplicative), and intersectional. The additive perspective suggests that each form of oppression will accumulate (or add together) with each unique form of oppression leading to negative mental health consequences (Moradi & Subich, 2003). The interactional, or multiplicative perspective purports that multiple forms of oppression are multiplicative, in that each experience of oppression does not add together but rather interacts and multiplies the experience of oppression, so that it’s just not the experience of sexism plus heterosexism but also plus sexism multiplied by heterosexism (Moane,

1999; Mullaly, 2002; Young, 1990). Intersectional theory suggests that individuals are a part of multiple social groups, including dominant and subordinate membership and that they can therefore experience privilege and disadvantages simultaneously based on different identities. Therefore, one membership does not define an individual’s

53

experience of oppression but the intertwining nature of all oppressed and privileged identities (Collins, 2000).

Wong, Liu, and Klann (2017) provided three research paradigms for utilizing intersectional theory when conducting social science research: (a) the intergroup paradigm, (b) the interconstruct paradigm, and (c) the intersectional uniqueness paradigm. Levant and Wong (2017) described the intergroup paradigm as including group comparisons of multiple social identities in quantitative research. The authors describe the interconstruct paradigm as research that explores relationships in constructs that are based on multiple social idenitities. Additionally, the researchers define the intersectional uniqueness paradigm which assumes that multiple social identities are inherently linked which creates intertwined experiences based on these social identities.

Levant and Wong (2017) note that though these three paradigms are broad they can be applied to all social science research on theory.

Current research in sexism and heterosexism has focused on the additive and interactional perspective though much research has begun to focus solely on the interactional perspective (Ragins, Cornwell, & Miller, 2003; Szymanski, 2005;

Szymanski, 2008; Szymanski, 2009; Szymanski, 2012; Szymanski & Henrichs-Beck

(2014) Szymanski, Kashubeck-West, & Meyer, 2008; Velez, Moradi & DeBlaere, 2015).

Interactional theory is still primarily used in the study of sexism and heterosexism because gender is seen as the primary force in the oppression of sexual minority individuals (Herek, 1994; Pharr, 1988; Szymanski & Owens, 2009). Szymanski and

Owens (2009) state “heterosexism acts as a weapon of sexism by enforcing rigid gender roles and compulsory heterosexuality”. In one study looking at intersectionality in

54

college-aged sexual minority women’s experiences with discrimination and their relationships with identity and collective action, Friedman and Campbell (2010) found that there were significant interactions between sexual orientation identity and feminist and LGBQ activism, however when including , they found no significance. This finding indicates that gender identity is more complex and may differ in lesbian and bisexual women. Results also indicated lesbian women reported higher rates of heterosexist discrimination and had higher scores in their gender identity measure. Friedman and Campbell (2010) discuss their findings suggest a difference in sexual minority identity and gender identity in lesbian women compared to bisexual women and that future research should be done to understand sexual minority women’s identities with their gender and sexual orientation. Comparing lesbian women to bisexual women and to heterosexual women continues to be an issue in the research base and these results also show the importance of studying lesbian women separately and exploring the interaction of experiences of gender and sexual orientation. Also, the results of this study were primarily correlational, so future research should also to continue to explore the intersectionality of lesbian women’s experiences.

Pharr (1988) also argues that combatting heterosexism must include traditional gender roles because inherently sexism is the driving force. Pharr (1988), Szymanski and

Owens (2009), and Szymanski and Carr (1988) all argue that gender is the root of heterosexism and without challenging traditional gender roles we maintain both forms of oppression. Research (Ragins, Cornwell, & Miller, 2003; Szymanski, 2005; Szymanski,

2008; Szymanski, 2009; Szymanski, 2012; Szymanski & Henrichs-Beck, 2014;

Szymanski, Kashubeck-West, & Meyer, 2008; Velez, Moradi & DeBlaere, 2015)

55

suggests that the interaction between sexism and heterosexism in lesbian women’s experiences may be of more salient to this population in comparison to other identities because of the gender component, whereas lesbian women may be able to experience discrimination or internalize negative messages about themselves based on being a woman or about being a sexual minority, not necessarily solely on being a lesbian.

However, there are criticisms of the additive and interactional models since research (Crenshaw, 1989; King, 2003; Lewis & Neville, 2015; Buchanan, 2005;

Thomas, Witherspoon, & Speight, 2008; Cole, 2009) on other identities, especially in the literature on racism which currently focus on an intersectional approach. Research investigating African American women’s experiences with oppression utilizing an additive and interactional model (Moradi & Subich, 2003; Szymanski & Stewart, 2010) found significant results that sexism and racism lead to increased psychological distress but neither found interaction effects. The interaction perspective when exploring internal and external oppression has had support and criticisms (Feinstein, Goldfried, & Davila,

2012; Meyer, 1995; Rostosky, Riggle, Horne, & Miller, 2009; Szymanski & Ikizler,

2013; Szymanski & Stewart, 2010; Szymanski & Sung, 2010; Velez, Moradi, &

DeBlaere, 2015). Moradi & Grzanka (2017) discuss the importance of intersectionality in the field of counseling psychology as it reflects on the values of the field. It is important to note, that there is also support for exploring interactional theory not only in the sexism and heterosexism literature but also in the racism and heterosexism literature (Szymanski

& Meyer, 2008; Szymanski & Gupta, 2009; Thoma & Huebner, 2013) as well as support for exploring causal paths in external and internalized oppression with mental health outcomes (Hatzenbuehler, 2009; Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere, 2015). Grzanka, Santos, &

56

Moradi (2017) discuss the importance of intersectionality research in the field of counseling psychology to include activism and coalition building, as well as addressing the limitations of the measures currently available that may not be capturing intersectionality. Additionally, the authors discuss the importance of interdisciplinarity in counseling psychology’s research on intersectionality and that researchers should actively challenge the oppressive institutions they research. There has been research (Bauer, 2014;

Bowleg, 2008; Cole, 2008; Grzanka & Frantell, 2017; Hancock, 2007; Luna, 2016;

Mallinckrodt, Miles, & Levy, 2014; Syed, 2010) that have attempted to utilize these approaches individually, however the authors suggest that counseling psychologists strive to conduct research that includes all of these suggestions. Szymanski and Henrichs-Bech

(2013) noted that the lack of quality measures to assess intersectionality for lesbian women and the lack of empirical research exploring these constructs for sexual minority women in general has prevented researchers from being able to explore this perspective fully.

The current study will focus on interactional theory because of the support for utilizing this approach in lesbian women when exploring sexism and heterosexism, as well as results found in much of the research that heterosexism is a tool to further sexism creating an interaction between the two (Pharr, 1988; Szymanski & Carr, 2008;

Szymanski & Owens, 2009). However, future research should explore the unique interactions of the multiple oppressions of lesbian women and their experiences with psychological well-being.

The research on multiple oppressions including heterosexism has focused on two theories: Minority stress theory (Brooks, 1981; Meyer, 1995; 2003) and Multicultural-

57

feminist theory (Cole, 2009; Moradi & Subich, 2002b; Moradi & Subich, 2003), which have incorporated additive, interactional, and intersectional approaches. Though the two theories conceptualize additive similarly, they have distinct differences in how they conceptualize the interactional approach.

Multicultural and Feminist Theories

The interaction of multiple oppressions literature stems from the Multicultural and

Feminist theories (Cole, 2009; Moradi & Subich, 2002b; Moradi & Subich, 2003). For example, Cole (2009) discusses the concept of intersectionality in psychology research focusing on feminist and critical race theories. Historically, researchers have focused on one identity in order to simplify models or by excluding them by controlling for other identities that their participants identify as (Betancourt & Lopez, 1993). Cole (2009) argues that the majority of individuals identify in more than one category of social identities and that these multiple identities may interact and intersect with one another in many ways. Cole (2009) explains the history of intersectionality and places its roots beginning with a group of African American Feminists called the Combahee River

Collective and their manifesto in 1977 and 1995. The authors were expressing their inability to distinguish their race from their gender from their class because “they are most often experienced simultaneously” (p.234). Intersectionality has long been a part of political, liberation, and civil rights movements while more recently becoming a part of academic research and feminist studies (Cole, 2009; McCall, 2005; Risman, 2004).

However, research on sexism and heterosexism differs from the argument that

Cole (2009) was making in that it is argued that gender and sexual orientation are not experienced simultaneously but rather because of strict gender roles that are experienced

58

by being a woman and by being a sexual minority, lesbian women may be able to experience discrimination or internalization of oppression for both identity statuses separately at times as well as together (Pharr, 1988; Szymanski & Carr, 2008; Szymanski

& Owens, 2009). This is a unique experience for lesbian women compared to other minority individuals where both identities may be more salient at any given time.

Multicultural and feminist research has primarily looked at the interaction between sexism and heterosexism rather than the interaction of a combination of identities simultaneously.

Multicultural-Feminist theories have primarily been utilized in the study of sexism solely and as one of the forms of multiple oppressions. However, the theory began with an additive approach to looking at multiple oppressions and then led to an interactional perspective. The additive approach to looking at multiple oppressions suggests that having multiple experiences of internalized oppression and multiple experiences of externalized oppression add together to affect mental health, and that it is the additive nature of having more forms of oppression that ultimately leads to negative mental health consequences (Meyer, 1995; Szymanski, 2006). Gilbert (1992) spoke to counseling psychologists directly arguing that counseling psychologists should have an understanding of how sexism affects every area of a woman’s life. There is much research to show the negative effects that sexism has on women in all domains; ie occupational discrimination, rape and sexual assault, abuse, and other forms of sexist discrimination (Fitzgerald, 1993; Koss, 1993; Koss, Heise, & Russo, 1994; Browne,

1993, Carden, 1994; Landrine & Klonoff, 1997). Landrine et al. (1995) and Klonoff and

Landrine (1995) conceptualized sexist events as distal and proximal predictors of mental

59

health outcomes. In this model, distal predictors are sexist events that a woman has experienced throughout her lifetime, whereas proximal predictors are sexist events that a woman has experienced recently (past year). In Landrine et al. (1995) the authors found that experiences of sexism were related to their psychological symptoms above and beyond their daily life stressors. These types of perceived lifetime and recent sexist events can be viewed as external oppression.

The multicultural-feminist research on racism and sexism in African American women (Beal, 1970; Jeffries and Ransford, 1980; Ladner, 1971) began also with an additive approach to the multiple oppressions of racism and sexism. For example, Beal’s

(1970) suggested that racism and sexism have direct effects on African American women independently but that the combination (additive) of the multiple oppressions are what impact African American women’s lives. Even with the support for the additive approach to exploring multiple oppressions, multicultural-feminist researchers began to theorize that it is not just the added effect of internalized and externalized oppressions that were affecting mental health in minority individuals but an interaction effect between types of oppression. There was a shift in the research to suggest that there is something unique about the experience of internalized oppression versus the experience of externalized oppression. So, researchers started to explore the interaction of types of oppression that worsen experiences of other types of oppression. For example, the internalization of sexism may worsen the link between internalized heterosexism and psychological distress, or the experience of sexist externalized oppression (discrimination) may worsen the link between experiences of heterosexist externalized oppression (discrimination) and psychological distress. Much like the additive perspective, interactional theory is looking

60

at the accumulation of forms of oppression but also how they interact and affect the other form of oppression and mental health consequences (Velez, DeBlaere, & Moradi, 2015).

Landrine, Klonoff, Alcarez, et al. (1995) applied an interactional conceptualization of sexism and racism when they suggested that racism may intensify the impact of sexism and vice versa. However, Moradi & Subich (2003) were the first to explore the additive and interactive links of sexism and racism with psychological distress in African American women. Results indicate that sexism and racism separately, and also a higher number of perceived racist or sexist events all related to higher levels of psychological distress in African American women but when explored additively, only sexist events were a unique predictor of psychological distress and the researchers found no interaction effect between racist and sexist experiences. The researchers suggest that this may be due to the measures they used as participants may have fused their racist and sexist experiences and that women may feel there is no distinction in these identities. The authors argue that future research should look at more subtle forms of oppression rather than solely overt to better capture intersectionality. However, this research did suggest that there may be something unique about types of oppression and how women perceive their experiences of sexism and racism since the results indicated significance with sexism and racism separately.

Feminist theory has also historically focused on sexism as a root of the high rate of IPV in women (Figes, 1970; Millett, 1970). MacKinnon (1987) argued that the pervasiveness of violence against women by men makes the oppression of sexism the most widespread form of oppression globally. Simpson and Helfrich (2014) stated that feminist theory and has helped to recognize “a global epidemic of

61

culturally sanctioned violence against women” (p.443). Historically, the theory has not helped to understand the high levels of intimate partner violence within same-gender relationships (Renzetti, 1998; Ristock, 2003; Simpson & Helfrich, 2014). Carvalho, et al.

(2011) argues that because of minority stressors that are unique to lesbian women may affect the impact of IPV, such as: conflict due to the relationship being out publically, threats to “out” partners to family and employers, lack of social support, absence of a lack of resources, fear of discrimination in the legal system and in shelters, and fear of discrimination in accessing services (Ristock, 2005; Murray et al., 2007; Kulkin et al.

2007).

Balsam’s (2001) reported that 60% of women in the study stayed in the abusive relationship because of a lack of resources and the majority did not go to a shelter. Within

Meyer’s (2003) minority stress model this would include experiences of internalized heterosexism and internalized sexism, which have been found to be related to negative mental health outcomes including PTSD (Hatzenbuehler, 2009; Meyer, 2003; Franklin,

Boyd-Franklin, & Kelly, 2006; Carter, 2007; Helms, Nicolas, Green, 2010). The results

(Reisner et al., 2016) discussed earlier in transgender individual’s experiences with oppression, IPV, and PTSD also suggests that we don’t understand the impact of IPV on

LGBT individual’s experiences with trauma and the trauma of oppression and future

PTSD symptom development. There is no study to date that explores the experiences of internalized and externalized oppression on lesbian women’s development of PTSD symptoms and the impact that experiencing IPV has on that relationship. The other theory primarily utilized in the study of sexism and heterosexism is minority stress theory

(Brooks, 1981; Meyer, 1995, 2003).

62

Minority Stress Theory

Minority stress theory suggests that individuals in stigmatized minority groups experience excessively high rates of stress and negative life events due to the discrimination and prejudice that they experience as a minority individual (Brooks, 1981;

Meyer, 1995, 2003). Meyer (2003) suggests that minority stress is socially-based and chronic, meaning that there is an additive effect to other life stressors that an individual may experience. Minority stress is created from societal institutions and processes not the individual. As it pertains to lesbian women, Brook’s (1981) book reviewing minority stress in lesbian women and defined the construct as a psychological stressor that minority individuals experience solely from being aware of their minority status. Meyer’s

(1995) study was the first to look at minority stress as a construct for gay –identifying men and argued that like other minority groups, gay individuals also experience chronic stress due to discrimination. Historically, the theory of minority stress has its roots in social and psychological orientations and theorizes that there is a conflict that is experienced by individuals in a minority group based on society’s dominant values and the minority group’s values conflicting resulting in difficulties socially for minority group individuals because they possess less social power (Mirowsky & Ross 1989;

Pearlin 1989; Meyer, 1995). Some researchers have argued that the experience of such a conflict for individuals is at the root of all stress that minority individuals experience in society, with some research suggesting that it leads to feelings of alienation and isolation

(Durkheim 1951; Lazarus & Fokman, 1984; Merton, 1968; Moss 1973; Thoits, 1999).

Minority stress theory suggests that the experiences of externalized oppression

(discrimination) leads to the internalization of that oppression in minority individuals

63

which may ultimately lead to psychological distress. This theory suggests that internalized oppression is the concept that is affecting the link between experiences of external oppression and psychological distress, but little is known about these effects on lesbian women.

Brook’s (1981) groundbreaking work on lesbian individuals and minority stress described that LGB identifying individuals live in an “ascribed inferior status” (p.78).

Meyer’s (1995) study provided this framework when looking at internalized homophobia as the amount a gay-identifying individual internalizes and believes the negative attitude society has for gay people. For LGB identifying individuals, the internalization of these negative attitudes can begin when they start exploring and questioning their or when they begin to label themselves (Meyer, 1995). The exploration and labeling as LGB often takes place before an individual “comes out” or publicly informs friends, family, etc of their identity status (Thoits, 1985; Thoits, 1999). The research suggests that this may be the time LGB individuals start internalizing the negative societal views about themselves and the process of chronic minority stress begins

(Brooks, 1981; Stein & Cohen, 1984; Meyer, 1995; Meyer, 2003). However, an LGB individual does not need to self-identify or self-disclose as LGB to experience stigmatization (Brooks, 1981). Another example of how internalized oppression may affect psychological well-being is Meyer’s (1995) study which utilized the framework of minority stress including three conceptualizations: internalized heterosexism, perceived stigma, and experiences of externalized heterosexism (discrimination and violence).

Results indicated that gay men had high rates of minority stress in all three conceptualizations and all three were independently significant. Additionally, all three

64

conceptualizations were overall and independently related to negative mental health outcomes: psychological distress during the past year, demoralization, guilt, sexual problems, suicide, and AIDS-related traumatic stress (distress related to the effects of the

AIDS epidemic). Results suggest that the greater the internalization of the experience of heterosexism with the experience of externalized heterosexism (discrimination and violence) led to worse mental health consequences. This suggests that lesbian women may also experience high rates of internalized heterosexism which may lead to increased mental health consequences.

Meyer’s (2003) follow-up work provided an updated framework of the researcher’s previous work extending the research on internalized oppression by exploring two new conceptualizations of stressors: distal and proximal minority stress.

Stemming from Lazarus and Folkman’s (1984, p. 321) definition of social structures:

Distal concepts whose effects on an individual depend on how they are

manifested in the immediate context of thought, feeling, and action—the proximal

social experiences of a person’s life.

Meyer (2003) proposes that these stressors are on a continuum from distal to proximal.

Distal stressors are the overt discriminatory events that LGB individuals experience that do not need an individual’s perception of them. For example, violence, hate crimes, and harassment base on sexual identity. Distal stressors can also exist at the institutional, procedural, and structural levels; such as, legislation that exists or is enacted to target

LGB individual’s legal rights or ability to gain safe employment, housing, education, and marriage (Rostosky et al., 2010). Meyer (2003) proposes distal stressors as a type of external oppression. Proximal stressors are the subjective stress intra-personally based on

65

an LGB individual’s experiences and self-identity. Proximal stressors can lead to internalized oppression and can manifest itself in expectations of rejection (based on

LGB identity), concealment (hiding LGB identity out of fear of being harmed), or internalized stigma (internalized homophobia). There are many terms to describe the experience of internalized oppression for LGB individuals: internalized homophobia

(Meyer, 1995; 2003), internalized heterosexism (Szymanski, Kashubeck, & Meyer,

2008), and internalized homonegativity (Mohr & Kendra, 2011). Even though, all of these terms are attempting to explain the experience of internalized oppression in LGB individuals, the concept of internalized homophobia in Meyer’s (2003) study is more accurately represented by the construct of internalized homonegativity. Internalized homonegativity refers to the demoralization constructs of feeling one’s self as negative, deserving of derogatory treatment, and denigration of character based on the internalization of heterosexist values and expectations regarding their LGB identity

(2011). So, even though Meyer (2003) is including parts of the construct of internalized heterosexism, the proposed construct focuses more on the self-blaming attributes of internalized heterosexism.

Distal and proximal minority stressors are viewed to be chronic and negatively associated with LGB identifying individual’s well-being and mental health outcomes

(Meyer, 2003). Meyer, (2003) also gives a framework of understanding how multiple oppressions affect each other and how external and internal oppressions together affect

LGB individual’s mental health. This framework conceptualizes heterosexist discrimination (external oppression) and internalized heterosexism (self-blaming attributions) will interact with mental health both individually and combined. Other

66

research has been conducted exploring internalized oppression as a mediator for a causal link with external oppression and mental health outcomes (Hatzenbuehler, 2009).

Meyer’s (2003) and Hatzenbuehler’s (2009) research continues to support the idea that internalized oppression is the concept that is worsening the link between experiences of externalized oppression and negative mental health consequences. However, it has not been consistent since there have been other research that has not found this link

(Szymanski & Ikizler, 2013; Szymanski & Stewart, 2010; Szymanski & Sung, 2010).

There has been more support for the interaction rather than mediation, especially in the racism and heterosexism literature (Szymanski & Gupta, 2009; Szymanski & Meyer,

2008; Thoma & Huebner, 2013), which supports the notion of exploring interactional theory in lesbian women. Since there is support for both multicultural-feminist theory and minority stress theory when exploring LGB individual’s mental health outcomes, the current study will explore both theories and a combination of the two when looking at the interaction between sexism and heterosexism in lesbian women’s subsequent PTSD symptoms.

Integrating Interactional Theories

Both minority stress theory and multicultural-feminist agree that external and internalized oppressions affect individuals in minority groups and their mental health, which includes lesbian women’s experiences with heterosexism and sexism. The distinction between minority stress theory and multicultural-feminist theory is that minority stress theory would conceptualize the negative mental health outcomes as coming from the internalization of one identity affecting the link between experiences of externalized discrimination of the same identity and psychological distress; such as

67

higher internalization of sexism may interact and affect the link between greater perceived incidents of externalized sexism and posttraumatic symptoms. Multicultural- feminist theory conceptualizes that one form of oppression interacts and affects the link of the other form of oppression and psychological distress. For example, the greater internalization of sexism may interact and affect the link of the internalization of heterosexism and posttraumatic symptoms (Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere, 2015).

There is also research (Binning, Unzueta, Huo, & Molina, 2009; Jackson, Yoo,

Harrington, & Guevarra, 2012; Roccas & Brewer, 2002; Shih, Young, & Bucher, 2013) to suggest that a combination of multicularal-feminist and minority stress theory may be a better approach as interactions may be complex due to resilience and protective factors.

For example, if an individual experiences little internalized oppression for one minority identity it may be protective when they experience external discrimination for another minority identity, and there may be little to no mental health outcomes. As the above research and mixed results suggest, the way individuals experience oppression and identity development is extremely complex and some individuals may use an identity that they view as less marginalized to cope with the experiences of the discrimination of the other identity. The research on this “buffering effect” should be considered when exploring a potential combination of minority stress and feminist-multicultural frameworks.

Support for additive and interactive links in sexual minority samples is definitely mixed and the majority of the research has focused on racism and heterosexism. No study to date has compared additive and interactive links with sexism and heterosexism in lesbian women who experience trauma, and specifically looking at their posttraumatic

68

reactions as an outcome. Szymanski’s (2005) study on heterosexism and sexism as correlates of psychological distress in women found support for an interaction of the identities, as the experience of heterosexist victimization in lesbian women strengthened the link between sexist discrimination and psychological distress.

Velez, Moradi, and DeBlaere (2014) explored multiple oppressions and the mental health of sexual minority Latina/o individuals. The study sampled 173 sexual minority Latina/o individuals and investigated the additive and interactive relationships between heterosexism and racism, specifically looking at external oppression as racist discrimination and heterosexist discrimination, and internalized oppression, as internalized racism and internalized heterosexism, with outcomes of psychological distress, lower life satisfaction, and lower self-esteem. The study results showed that there was an additive effect of racist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination, and internalized heterosexism but not internalized racism with psychological distress.

However, internalized racism and internalized heterosexism both had relationships with lower life satisfaction and lower self-esteem. This suggests that the additive framework is supported somewhat but mainly when looking at internalized oppressions links to well- being. The interaction framework results that looked at both minority stress and multi- cultural-feminist theories found support for an interaction between racist discrimination and heterosexist discrimination and both internalized oppressions; internalized racism and internalized heterosexism. These findings suggest that the multicultural-feminist framework of looking at how one type of oppression affects the other form of oppression was better supported for psychological distress than the minority stress model which found that the heterosexist discrimination and internalized heterosexism interaction were

69

all non-significant with psychological distress. However, when looking at self-esteem, they all accounted for unique variance. This would suggest that an integrative framework of both the minority stress model and the multicultural-feminist model, or what the authors call “synthesized” would be better explored when looking at multiple oppressions.

Interestingly, the findings on internalized racism and self-esteem showed that even when perceived racist discrimination was low, Latina/o individuals with either low or high internalized heterosexism all had similar levels of self-esteem (Velez, Moradi, &

DeBlaere, 2014). Further, when there was high racist discrimination, the individuals with lower internalized heterosexism had higher self-esteem compared to those with high internalized heterosexism. When looking at the same concept in heterosexist discrimination and internalized racism, self-esteem stayed the same for individuals who had low or high internalized racism when perceived heterosexist discrimination was low

(Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere, 2014. However, when there was high perceived heterosexist discrimination, self-esteem was higher in those with low internalized racism. These findings suggest that internalized racism did in fact interact with heterosexist discrimination by increasing low self-esteem when individuals experienced high rates of perceived heterosexist discrimination. Whereas, the interaction of racist discrimination and internalized heterosexism suggest an increase in self-esteem for individuals who experience high perceived racist discrimination and low internalized heterosexism. These findings show the complexity of these interactions but do support the above-mentioned literature that some individuals with low internalized oppression protect their self-esteem even when faced with high rates of discrimination on other identities (Velez, Moradi, &

70

DeBlare, 2014). This finding also supports the previous literature that high internalized oppression is linked to low self-esteem in sexual minorities (Meyer, 1995; Meyer, 2003).

Overall, this research supports additive and interactive forms of frameworks when looking at multiple oppressions and highlights the importance for future research on other identities to better understand how experiences of oppression and internalized oppression work together in minority individuals and their experiences with mental health.

The present study furthers the research of Szymanski (2005; 2014) and Velez,

Moradi, & DeBlare (2014) by exploring additive and interactive frameworks with sexism and heterosexism in lesbian individual’s experiences of trauma and their subsequent post- traumatic reactions.

Current Study

There have been no studies to date to look at the interaction of heterosexism and sexism and the role that intimate partner violence (IPV) plays in the link between internalized oppression and PTSD symptoms for lesbian women.

The consideration of oppression as a form of trauma is an important topic in counseling psychology. Multiculturalism and social justice are core principles in our field and understanding the experiences of oppression is part of that principle (Prilliltensky &

Nelson, 2002; Vera & Speight, 2003). There has been much literature on the experiences of racial and ethnic minorities’ experiences of oppression and subsequent mental and physical health consequences. However, there is a lack of research on the traumatic impact of sexist experiences on women and how those experiences can impact women’s experiences of interpersonal trauma and posttraumatic reactions. There is also a gap in the literature considering lesbian women’s multiple minority statuses and the impact of

71

the multiple forms of oppression that these individuals experience. All of these constructs are important to the field of counseling psychology and should continue to be explored and examined. While there is only a small amount, the literature reviewed here suggests that sexism and heterosexism do have an impact on women’s posttraumatic reactions, and additionally shows that the experiences of sexism and heterosexism leading to PTSD symptoms may be impacted by increased internalized sexism and internalized heterosexism, with high levels of intimate partner violence impacting the development of

PTSD symptoms. Taken together these studies illustrate that sexism and heterosexism may lead to more severe experiences with PTSD symptoms and the development of

PTSD symptoms generally.

Research Aims and Hypotheses

The current study aimed to fill the gaps in the literature for lesbian women, their experiences with IPV, and their daily experiences living with multiple oppressions.

Generally, this study sought to better understand the links between the experience of oppression and PTSD. The study also aimed to better understand additive, minority stress, and multicultural-feminist theory’s frameworks in sexual minority women’s experiences with external and internalized sexism and heterosexism. This study tested predictions of minority stress, multicultural-feminist frameworks, and a combination of the two theories of external and internalized sexism and heterosexism in lesbian women and the links to PTSD symptoms. The study also utilized the model that Velez, Moradi,

& DeBlare (2014) tested in their study with Latina/o sexual minority individuals. The present study also explored the role that experiences of IPV plays in the relationship

72

between experiences of oppression and PTSD symptoms. The research questions and hypotheses of the current study are:

Research Question 1: Is there an additive effect of external sexism (sexist discrimination experiences), external heterosexism (heterosexist discrimination experiences), internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism on lesbian women’s symptoms of

PTSD?

• H1: I predicted that perceived sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination,

internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism will each be uniquely related

to higher PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women.

Research Question 2: Does internalized oppression strengthen the relationship between experiences of externalized oppression and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women?

• H2: Minority stress theory proposes that internalized oppression will interact with

externalized oppression (sexist discrimination experiences/heterosexist

discrimination experiences) for sexism and heterosexism. Hence, internalized

sexism will interact with sexist experiences and internalized heterosexism will

interact with heterosexist experiences increasing the link between external

oppression and negative mental health consequences (Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere,

2015). Therefore, I predicted that sexist discrimination would interact with

internalized sexism and that heterosexist discrimination would interact with

internalized heterosexism, with internalized oppression of each type of oppression

increasing the strengthen the relationship between external oppression and PTSD

symptom severity in lesbian women.

73

Research Question 3: Does oppression of one type (sexism external/internal) increase the link of the other type (heterosexism external/internal) and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women?

• H3: Multicultural-feminist theory proposes that externalized oppressions (sexist

discrimination experiences and heterosexist discrimination experiences) interact

and internalized oppressions (internalized sexism and internalized heterosexism)

interact. Therefore, sexist discrimination interacts with heterosexist

discrimination, and internalized sexism interacts with internalized heterosexism,

increasing the link between external oppression experiences and internalized

oppression with negative mental health consequences (Velex, Moradi, &

DeBlaere, 2015). Consequently, I predicted that sexist discrimination would

interact with heterosexist discrimination and that internalized sexism interacts

with internalized heterosexism beyond that accounted for by their unique links,

with each form of oppression increasing the relationship of the other form of

oppression and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women.

Research Question 4: Does one form of externalized oppression strengthen the relationship between the other form of internalized oppression and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women?

• H4: In an attempt to combine minority stress theory and multicultural-feminist

theory, we hypothesize that one form of externalized oppression (sexist

discrimination experiences/heterosexist discrimination experiences) will interact

with the other form of internalized oppression (internalized

heterosexism/internalized sexism). That is, sexist discrimination will interact with

74

internalized heterosexism and heterosexist discrimination will interact with

internalized sexism, in both cases increasing the link between external oppression

of one form and internalized oppression of the other form, with negative mental

health consequences. Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere (2015) named these types of

interactions “synthesized interactions,” where one form of oppression (ie. external

sexism) is interacting with the other form of oppression (ie. internalized

heterosexism). Thus, I predicted that heterosexist discrimination interacts with

internalized sexism and that sexist discrimination interacts with internalized

heterosexism, so that internalized oppression of one type increases the

relationship of external oppression of the other type of oppression and PTSD

symptom severity in lesbian women. I also predicted that this interaction would

account for greater variance and would have a larger effect size than the other

aforementioned interactions (See Table 1 for all conceptual interaction terms for

hierarchical regressions).

Research Question 5: How do lesbian women’s experience of IPV impact their experiences of sexism, internalized sexism, and PTSD symptoms?

• H5: I predicted that there will be a moderated moderation, where the relationship

between externalized sexism and PTSD symptom severity is moderated by

internalized sexism with severity of IPV moderating the relationship between

internalized sexism and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women. I also

predicted a significant positive relationship between externalized sexism and

PTSD symptom severity for those who have higher internalized sexism and

greater severity of IPV.

75

Research Question 6: How do lesbian women’s experience of IPV impact their experiences of heterosexism, internalized heterosexism, and PTSD symptoms?

• H6: I predicted that there will be a moderated moderation, where the relationship

between externalized heterosexism and PTSD symptom severity is moderated by

internalized heterosexism with severity of IPV moderating that relationship in

lesbian women. I also predict a significant positive relationship between

externalized heterosexism and PTSD symptom severity for those who have higher

internalized heterosexism and greater severity of IPV.

76

CHAPTER III

METHOD

The current study hoped to extend the literature on the experiences of sexism and heterosexism in lesbian women and their subsequent posttraumatic symptoms. This study explored the additive and multiplicative models to better understand how the multiple oppressions of sexism and heterosexism combine or interact in women’s lives and their effects on mental health. The current study explored the role of experiences of intimate partner violence in lesbian women’s lives and how it affects the relationship between externalized oppression experiences, internalized oppression, and subsequent PTSD symptoms. The current chapter outlines the methods conducted in the study. I discuss the participants, measures (see appendices), procedures, and the statistical analytic plan of the study.

Participants

Data were collected from 422 women. After removing participants who did not meet minimum inclusion criteria (i.e., self-identified as a woman, over 18 years old, sexual identity criteria) and/or had too much missing data (see Chapter IV description of

Data Screening and Missing Data), 209 participants remained. The initial power analysis that was conducted indicated a minimum of 123 participants should be sampled for a

77

power of .80, an f2 = .15, and α = .05. Thus, it was determined that the current study was sufficiently powered for the proposed analyses. Cohen (1988) describes f2 as a medium effect size and it is utilized in regression analyses to assess the magnitude of the effect independent of the sample size. Moradi, Mohr, Worthington, and Fassinger (2009) discuss the challenges when methodologically sampling sexual minority individuals. For example, we anticipated that not every woman who is attracted to women will identify as a “lesbian” and it is important when studying sexual minority individuals to be aware of the importance of language. Taking into account the diversity of sexuality expressions, we gave participants the in the demographics (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin,

1948) and utilized individuals who identify as a five (predominantly attracted to the same gender, but more than incidentally attracted to the other gender), six (predominantly attracted to the same gender, only incidentally attracted to the other gender) or seven

(exclusively attracted to the same gender) so to capture women who are mostly attracted to the same gender at the time of recruitment. Klein, Sepekoff, and Wolf (1985) discussed the importance of utilizing measures of sexual orientation that allow for sexual exploration and changes throughout the lifetime and on a continuum.

The sample ranged in age 18 to 56 (M=28.46, SD=8.67). The majority of the sample identified as White (78%), exclusively attracted to the same gender (65.1%), and lesbian (87.6%). The majority of the sample identified as exclusively attracted to the same gender (65.1%), 27.8% identified as predominantly attracted to the same gender, only incidentally attracted to the other gender, and 72.5% identified as predominantly attracted to the same gender, only incidentally attracted to the other gender. Additionally, the majority of participants (46.9%) identified their socioeconomic status (SES) as, “my

78

family has no problem buying the things we need and sometimes we also buy special things.” Please see Table 2 for demographic characteristics of the sample.

Measures

Demographic Questionnaire. A brief demographics questionnaire (see Appendix

A) was given and participants responded to five questions that assess their age, gender, race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and sexual orientation.

PTSD Symptom Severity

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for the DSM-5 (PCL-5; Blevins,

Weathers, Davis, Witte, & Domino, 2015). The PCL-5 (See Appendix B) is a self- report measure developed to assess the twenty symptoms of PTSD in the Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

This scale was utilized in the current study to measure symptoms of PTSD in participants. Sample items of the PCL-5 include, “In the past month, how much were you bothered by: Repeated, disturbing, and unwanted memories of the stressful experience?” and “In the past month, how much were you bothered by: Feeling very upset when something reminded you of the stressful experience?” (Blevins, Weathers,

Davis, Witte, & Domino, 2015). The scale consists of 20 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). Participants are prompted to answer all items for how much they have been bothered by each symptom in the past month. A participant’s score is derived by summing all items for an overall PTSD symptom score, which can range from 0-80. A higher score indicates higher PTSD symptoms. A total score for PTSD symptoms in the last month was utilized in the present study as was outlined by Blevins,

Weathers, Davis, Witte, & Domino (2015). There is no empirical support that we are

79

aware of for the use of total scores for the PCL-5 (i.e., through confirmatory factor analytic results supporting either a bifactor, hierarchical or unidimensional model), which is a limitation of the measure, although total scores are what are most often used in research (Dardis, Dichter, & Iverson, 2018; Dardis, Amoroso, & Iverson, 2017; Iverson,

Dardis, & Pogoda, 2017; Ovrebo, et.al., 2018; Straub, McConnell, & Messman-Moore,

2018).

Blevins, Weathers, Davis, Witte, and Domino (2015) conducted two studies to test the psychometric properties of the PCL-5. The first study utilized a sample of 278 college students who had experienced incidents of self-reported trauma. The sample included 197 females (70.9%) and 81 males (29.1%) aged 18-54 years. The majority of the sample identified as Caucasian (81.30%) and African American (11.5%). The PCL-5 scores indicated strong internal consistency (α=.94), which was comparable to other measures of PTSD symptoms, including the PTSD Checklist (PCL), the Posttraumatic

Diagnostic Scale (PDS), and the Detailed Assessment of Posttraumatic Stress Scale

(DAPS). PCL-5 test-retest reliability was explored in a subset of 53 participants who were given the initial battery during study two if they volunteered, retest interval was approximately a week (M=6.14 days). Total scores showed strong test-retest reliability

(r=.82, 95% CI [.71, .89), as did the total scores for the other measures of PTSD symptoms respectively rs = .85, 95% CI [.75, .91], .80, 95% CI [.68, .88], and .91, 95%

CI [.85, .95].

Convergent validity was explored with the PCL-5 and other PTSD measures and strong correlations were found between the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist

(PCL; Herman, Huska, & Keane, 1993; Weathers, Litz, Herman, Huska, & Keane, 1993;

80

Weathers, 2008), r = .85, p < .01, Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS; Foa,

1995), r = .85, p < .01, and Detailed Assessment of Posttraumatic Stress (DAPS; Briere,

2001), ), r = .84, p < .01.Discriminant validity was assessed with related constructs and unrelated constructs. Depression, a related construct was found to have a correlation of r

= .60; 36% overlapping variance and Mania, an unrelated construct had a correlation of r

= .31; 10% overlapping variance, indicating good discriminant validity. Predicted correlations and λ values were used to calculate construct validity effect sizes. The PCL-

5 had large effect sizes specifying a strong similarity between predicted and observed correlations (ralerting-CV = .90, rcontrast-CV = .92).

A common factors model was estimated though confirmatory factor analysis for the four-factor model of PTSD symptoms in the DSM-5 (Re-experiencing; Avoidance;

Negative Alterations in Cognitions and Mood; Hyperarousal), which had questionable fit, particularly with CFI and TLI: χ2(164) = 455.83, p < .001, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) = .07, root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA) =

.08, comparative fit index (CFI) = .86, and Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = .84. Analyses showed good fit for the items of the PCL-5 with the 4-factor model and superior fit for the six-factor model (Liu et al., 2014; reexperiencing, avoidance, negative alterations in cognitions and mood, anhedonia, anxious arousal, and dysphoric arousal factors) and seven factor (Armour et al., 2015; reexperiencing, avoidance, negative affect, anhedonia, externalizing behavior, anxious arousal, and dysphoric arousal factors) models, χ2 (164)

= 318.37, p < .001, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .06, CFI = .92, and TLI = .90, and 7-factor,

χ2 (164) = 291.32, p < .001, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .06, CFI = .93, and TLI = .91,

81

models. The 6 and 7 factor models had a significantly better fit than the DSM-5 4-factor model.

Blevins, Weathers, Davis, Witte, and Domino (2015) second study had a sample of 558 college students that reported a traumatic incident from the same university as the first study. The sample included 419 females (75.2%) and 138 males (24.8%). The majority of the participants identified as Caucasian (85.5%) and African American

(7.7%). In this study, analyses showed similar results to the first study, with strong reliability and validity. The PCL-5 still had high internal consistency (α=.95). Interitem correlations were ranged .25 to .77 (M=.51) which is in the recommended range.

Convergent and Discriminant validity also indicated strong correlations for the PCL-5 and DAPS (r=.94, p<.001), and the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI; Morey,

2007) (r=.92, p<.001) and with the pattern of the correlations in Study 1. Additionally, the PCL-5 had large effect sizes specifying a strong similarity between predicted and observed correlations 5 (ralerting-CV = .94, rcontrast-CV = .81). The confirmatory factor analysis of the DSM-5 4-factor model had mostly adequate fit to the data (with the exception of TLI): χ2 (164) = 558.18, p < .001, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .07, 90% CI

[.06, .07], CFI = .91, and TLI = .89. The 6-factor model, χ2(155) = 389.02, p < .001,

SRMR = .04, RMSEA = .05, 90% CI [.05, .06], CFI = .94, and TLI = .93, and 7-factor model, χ2(149) = 352.26, p < .001, SRMR = .04, RMSEA = .05, 90% CI [.04, .06], CFI =

.95, and TLI = .94 both had superior fit compared to the 4-factor DSM-5 model but they did not significantly differ using the scaled Chi-Square difference test, χ2 (6) = 0.91, p = n.s..

82

Intimate Partner Violence

Severity of Violence against Women Scale (SVAW: Marshall, 1992). The

SVAW (See Appendix C) is a self-report measure of recent threats and incidents of physical violence and assault. The SVAW was utilized in the current study to measure experiences of intimate partner violence. Participants are instructed to answer how often each item occurred in the past year. There are three categories of the SVAW: (a) threats of violence, including mild violence, moderate violence, and serious violence, (b) actual violence mild, minor, and serious, and (c) sexual violence. A sample item in the threats of mild violence category include: “acted like a bully towards you.” A threats of moderate violence item includes: “threatened to destroy property.” A sample item of threats of serious violence includes: “threatened to kill you.” The actual violence mild, minor, and serious category includes items such as; “pushed or shoved you” for mild violence. Minor violence items include: “scratched you.” A moderate violence item example is: “slapped you around the face and head.” A serious violence item example is: “beat you up”. The sexual violence category includes items such as; “physically forced you to have sex”

(Marshall, 1992). The scale consists of forty-six items of threats of violence, actual violence, and sexual violence rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (4 or more times). Scores are summed for an overall severity of violence score. Higher scores indicate greater incidents of violent events. In the present study, a total score was used for items indicated in the past year which has been typically used in research (Sansone, Chu,

& Weiderman, 2007; Schafer et al., 2012; Thomas & Weston, 2018; Wiist & McFarlane,

1998) and is the method described by Herman (1992). However, there is no empirical support that we are aware of for the use of total scores for the SVAW (i.e., through

83

confirmatory factor analytic results supporting either a bifactor, hierarchical or unidimensional model), which is a limitation of the measure.

Items were created by Marshall (1992) to better capture the experiences of violence and their severity level. The Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS; Strauss. 1979) had been the main scale used for measuring IPV and critics of the scale argued that it was not sensitive enough and did not measure the intensity and consequence of acts of violence.

For example, “hit or tried to hit with an object.” Unlike the CTS, the SVAW allows researchers to measure perceived intent of violence by assessing for threats of violence, as well as actual incidents of violence experienced and the severity of violence.

Marshall (1992) utilized a sample of 707 college student women ranging from ages 17 to 72 (M=20.39, SD=5.76) to explore the dimensionality using exploratory factor analysis of 49 physically abusive, sexual abuse, and threats of abuse. These acts included: symbolic violence, threats of physical violence, actual violence, and sexual violence. Three items were included in the initial study that were later not included in the final SVAW. Participants rated all items on a 10-point Likert scale on how serious, aggressive, abusive, threatening, and violent each act was if done to a woman. The participants also rated how much physical, psychological, and emotional harm it would cause a woman. Marshall (1992) discussed that the intent was to have at least four factors when exploring a list of behaviors of IPV (symbolic acts, threats, physical, and sexual violence). The author discussed the desire for comprehensiveness in the scale and since there could be perceived threat levels (minor, moderate, and serious), as well as, actual violence (minor, moderate, and serious violence), the author decided to explore a 8, 9, and 10-factor solution though the 9-factor model was ultimately selected. Means were

84

calculated for severity scores and a 9-factor solution was found: (a) minor violence

(56.8% variance), (b) serious violence (10.1% variance), (c) threats of serious violence

(4.7% variance), (d) sexually violent acts (4.3% variance), (e) symbolic violence (1.6% of variance), (f) threats of moderate violence (1.2% of variance), (g) threats of mild violence (1% of variance), and (h) mild violence (0.8% of variance).

Alpha coefficients, mean severity scores for each dimension, and correlations were utilized to test the 9-factor model. Alphas were .92 for symbolic violence and .96 for moderate threats, mild, moderate, and serious violence. Mean severity scores were:

7.10 for symbolic violence, 5.50 for mild threats, 7.29 for moderate threats, 8.50 for serious threats, 7.65 for minor violence, 7.71 for mild violence, 8.53 for moderate violence, 9.06 for sexual violence, and 9.24 for serious violence. All 36 t-tests for differences were significant. Correlations ranged from .34 to .88. A mean correlation of

.69 was found for severity. Correlations for within dimensions rather than between were also calculated. Mean correlations ranged from .73 (sexual violence) to .90 (moderate violence) with a mean score of .80. Symbolic violence had a similar correlation with the between dimensions mean score. All other dimensions the mean correlation was higher than between dimensions. Marshall (1992) provided no fit statistics for the factor analysis in Study one of the SVAW.

Marshall (1992) conducted a second study of the SVAW to better explore whether the levels of violence found could be generalized to women outside a college sample. The second study utilized a sample of 208 women in the Dallas-Fort Worth Metroplex area who ranged in age from 19 to 75 (M=40.80, SD=13.03). Both studies occurred in the same time period, so the original 49 behaviors were also utilized in this study. Amount of

85

threat and violence were not included in this survey. Mean severity scores were calculated and an exploratory factor analysis using maximum likelihood extraction with oblique rotation. The 9-factor solution accounted for 80.6% of the variance in severity ratings. The symbolic violence factor (1.6% variance). The threats of mild, moderate, and serious violence factors (1%, 1.2%, and 4.7% of variance respectively). The mild, minor, moderate, and serious violence factors (0.8%, 56.8%, .7%, and 10.1% of variance respectively). Finally, the sexual violence factor (4.3% of variance). Coefficient alphas ranged from .89 (symbolic violence) to .96 (mild and serious violence).

A second order confirmatory factor analysis was also utilized, and the four violent acts were included as first order factors: (a) symbolic, (b) threats, (c) physical, and (d) sexual. Results indicated two higher order factors: (a) minor, moderate, serious, mild, and sexual violence (67.5% of variance), and (b) threats of violence, which included serious and moderate threats, symbolic violence, and mild threats (8.0% of variance). Mean severity scores were calculated and were similar to study one though minor and mild violence were reversed in study two’s sample. Mean severity scores were 7.91 for symbolic violence, 6.55 for mild threats, 8.05 for moderate threats, 8.95 for serious threats, 8.55 for mild violence, 8.98 for minor violence, 9.27 for moderate violence, 9.79 for serious violence, and 9.48 for sexual violence. When comparing the dimensions all but serious threats and mild violence differed significantly. Correlations between dimensions were similar to study 1 with a range of .60 to .74 (sexual violence to minor violence), with an overall mean of .67 which was lower than the .69 mean found in the student sample. Correlations were also calculated for each act and ranged from .61 to .85

(serious threats to mild and moderate violence) and the mean correlations of .73 in this

86

study is compared to .79 in the college student sample. Forty-six acts were ultimately selected for the SVAW, however Marshall (1992) provided no fit statistics for study two of the SVAW.

Sexism & Heterosexism

Schedule of Sexist Events (SSE: Klonoff & Landrine, 1995). The SSE (See

Appendix D) is a self-report measure of recent and lifetime sexist events and is utilized as a measure of everyday sexism. The SSE was utilized in the current study to measure experiences of everyday sexism. The SSE can be used to measure sexist events in that participants experience in their lifetime (SSE-Lifetime) and in the past year (SSE-

Recent). The current study will utilize the SSE-Recent. Sample items of the SSE include,

“How many times have people failed to show you the respect that you deserve because you are a woman?” and “How many times have you been called a sexist name like bitch, cunt, chick, or other names?” (Klonoff & Landrine, 1995). The scale consists of twenty items of sexist behavior and they are rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (the event never happened) to 6 (the event happens all of the time) for recent (past year). Scores are summed for frequency of recent sexist events. Higher scores indicate greater incidents of sexist events. The current study utilized a total score of past year sexist experiences, which is typically used in research (Klonoff, Landrine, & Campbell, 2000; Moradi &

Subich, 2002; Moradi & Subich, 2004) and was the original method described by Klonoff and Landrine (1995). However, there is no empirical support that we are aware of for the use of total scores for the SSE (i.e., through confirmatory factor analytic results supporting either a bifactor, hierarchical or unidimensional model), which is a limitation of the measure.

87

Items were created by Klonoff and Landrine (1995) for an unpublished qualitative study investigating women’s answers to the question, “what’s the worst thing that has ever happened to or has been done to you because you are a woman?” The measure was then studied for its psychometric properties in a sample of 631 women. The majority of the sample included women who identified as Caucasian (64%) and the ages ranged from

18-73 years old. The majority of the sample was community-based (53%) and the rest of the sample were college students. Exploratory factor analysis indicated four factors for were adequate: Sexist Degradation and its Consequences, Sexist Discrimination in

Distant Relationships, Sexism in Close Relationships, and Sexist Discrimination in the

Workplace. Results indicated Chronbach’s alphas for the SSE-Recent (α =.90) and split- half reliability was (r = .83). The test-retest reliability of the SSE was conducted; however the researchers discussed how this form of reliability testing was not an adequate way to assess reliability since the participants may experience new sexist events daily. Test-retest reliability was conducted on 50 college women with the SSE-recent on the initial administration and then again 2-weeks later. The results for the test-retest reliability were r=.63 for the SSE-Recent.

Klonoff and Landrine (1995) also tested the validity of the SSE in their study, initially looking at how the SSE correlated with the Hassles-F (Kanner, Coyne, Schaeffer,

& Lazarus, 1981) and the Peri-Life Events Scale (PERI-LES) (Dohrenwend, Krasnoff,

Askenasy, & Dohrenwend, 1978). The SSE-Recent correlated with the other stressful life events measures. Landrine and Klonoff (1997) adapted the SSE to the SSE-Appraisal by including an intensity rating. Participants are prompted with the question, “How stressful was this for you?” and the item is rated on a 6-point Likert scale from 1 (Not at all

88

stressful ) to 6 (Extremely stressful). Psychometric properties were also tested for the new adaptation. The sample included 652 women ranging from 17 to 73 years of age. The majority of the sample was White (n=405) and Latinas (n=136), Asian/Pacific Islander

(n=45), Black (n=42), and Other Ethnic groups (n=24). Two-thirds of the sample were college students and the majority indicated that they were single (58.6%). Similarly, to their 1995 study, reliability (α =.93) and split-half reliability (r =.89) were both highly reliable. The SSE-Appraisal was found to have good validity and reliability in this sample, as well as in their 1995 study.

Heterosexist Harassment, Rejection, and Discrimination Scale (HHRDS;

Szymanski, 2006). The HHRDS (See Appendix E) is a self-report measure that was developed by Szymanski (2006) to measure everyday heterosexist events in the author’s study on heterosexist events and lesbian’s psychological distress. In the current study, the

HHRDS was used to measure experiences of externalized heterosexism. The scale was designed to be similar to the other everyday measures of oppressive events, such as the

SSE which was also utilized in the current study. The HHRDS asks participants to answer items about the frequency of incidents of heterosexist harassment, rejection, and discrimination in their lifetime and in the past year. The present study had participants answer items for the past year. Sample items of the HHRDS include, “How many times have you been verbally insulted because you are a lesbian?” and How many times have you been treated unfairly by your employer, boss, or supervisors because you are a lesbian?” (Szymanski, 2006). The scale consists of 14 items rated on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (the event has never happened to you) to 6 (the event happened almost all the time; more than 70% of the time). Mean scores are calculated and higher

89

scores on the HHRDS indicates greater the heterosexist experiences in the past year. The present study utilized an overall mean score of heterosexist experiences in the past year, which is typically used in research (Denton, Rostosky & Danner, 2014; Szymanski &

Ikizler, 2013; Szymanski & Meyer, 2008) and was the method used by Szymanski

(2006). However, there is no empirical support that we aware of for the use of total scores for the HHRDS (i.e., through confirmatory factor analytic results supporting either a bifactor, hierarchical or unidimensional model), which is a limitation of the measure.

There are three subscales of the HHRDS (Szymanski, 2006): The Harassment and

Rejection subscale, the Workplace and School Discrimination subscale, and Other

Discrimination subscale. The Harassment and Rejection subscale explores recent perceived rejection and harassment in areas such as friends, family members, verbal rejection, physical violence or threats of physical violence, and verbal harassment. A sample item includes, “How many times have you been rejected by family members because you are a lesbian?” The Workplace and School Discrimination subscale looks at discrimination specifically in the areas of employment, the work place, and academic pursuits. A sample item includes, “How many times have you been treated unfairly by your employer, boss, or supervisors because you are a lesbian?” The Other

Discrimination subscale also looks at situation specific events of heterosexist discrimination in areas such as, interactions with strangers, service workers, and medical professionals. A sample item includes, “How many times have you been treated unfairly by strangers because you are a lesbian?” Szymanski (2006) recommends using the measure as a whole as some participants may not experience certain types of

90

discrimination based on certain individual circumstances, ie., not employed, not currently enrolled in academics.

Szymanski (2006) explored the psychometric properties of the HHRDS with a sample of 143 lesbian women. The sample was predominantly White (90%) and highly educated (74% had a minimum of a Bachelor’s degree). Ages ranged from 19 to 70 years old. Factor analysis indicated a good fit for the three-factor model: Harassment and

Rejection (α =.89), Workplace and School Discrimination (α =.84), and Other

Discrimination (α =.78). Overall, the measure had high internal consistency (α =.90).

Inter-scale correlations had a ,42 to .56 range. Subscales of the HHRDS found moderate correlations with each other however good internal consistency was found. Correlations had a range from .73 to .90 for the total and subscales. Construct validity was also strong with positive correlations between the HRDS and other measures of psychological symptoms.

Internalized Sexism & Internalized Heterosexism

Lesbian Internalized Homophobia Scale (LIHS; Szymanski & Chung, 2001).

The LIHS (See Appendix F) is a self-report measure developed by Szymanski and Chung

(2001) to address the need for an internalized heterosexism scale for women rather than for gay men. The researchers argue that because the existing scales were created based on theories and empirical literature about gay men, it is problematic to assume that they would be appropriate for lesbian women based on simply same sex attraction. The authors further argue that there is a difference between gay men and lesbian women culture and that the development of the LIHS fills the need of a measure that has been created and validated on sexual minority women. The LIHS total score was used in the

91

current study to measure internalized heterosexism. Sample items of the LIHS include, “I hate myself for being attracted to other women?” and “I live in fear that someone will find out I am a lesbian?” (Szymanski & Chung, 2001). The LIHS consists of 52 items that are rated on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 7 (Strongly agree).

The LIHS includes reverse-scored items and the scores are averaged and totaled as well as averaged subscales are utilized, with higher scores indicating greater internalized heterosexism. The present study utilized an overall total score for internalized heterosexism which has been typically used in research (Amadio & Chung, 2004;

Szymanski, 2006) and was the method suggested by Szymanski & Chung (2001).

However, there is no empirical support that we are aware of for the use of total scores for the LIHS (i.e., through confirmatory factor analytic results supporting either a bifactor, hierarchical or unidimensional model), which is a limitation of the measure.

There are 5 subscales of the LIHS which were derived from previous literature

(Burisch, 1984; Gartrell, 1984; Herek, 1984; Pearlman, 1987; Pharr, 1988; Sophie, 1987) on internalized oppression and grounded in theory. The items were then created based on these dimensions. The five dimensions are: Connection with the Lesbian Community,

Public Identification as a Lesbian, Personal Feelings about Being a Lesbian, Moral and

Religious Attitudes Toward Lesbianism, and Attitudes Toward Other . The

Connection with the Lesbian Community subscale measures the connection with the larger lesbian community and how frequently the participants engage in contact with other lesbian women. A sample item includes: “When interacting with members of the lesbian community, I often feel different and alone, like I don’t fit in.” The Public

Identification as a Lesbian subscale measures how much the participants worry about

92

being identified as a lesbian in public as well as how much the participants want the ability to “come out”. A sample item includes: “I live in fear that someone will find out I am a lesbian.” The Personal Feelings about Being a Lesbian subscale measures the participant’s self-esteem and feelings of guilt about their identity. A sample item includes: I hate myself for being attracted to other women.” The Moral and Religious

Attitudes Toward Lesbianism subscale measures how much the participants have internalized religious messages that their lesbianism is wrong and atypical. A sample item includes: “Female homosexuality is a sin.” Finally, the Attitudes Toward Other

Lesbians subscale measures the participant’s feelings and attitudes about other lesbian women particularly in the areas of their adherence to stereotypical lesbian norms and judgments about other lesbian women’s adherence to the same norms. A sample item includes: “My feelings toward other lesbians are often negative.”

Psychometric properties were tested on a sample of 157 lesbian women ages ranging from 18 to 74 years old. The sample identified Caucasian (86%), African

American (8%), Hispanic/Latina (3%), Asian American/Pacific Islander (1%),

Multiracial (2%), and Other (1%). The five subscales indicated strong internal reliabilities: Connection with the Lesbian Community (α =.87), Public Identification as a

Lesbian (α =.92), Personal Feelings about Being a Lesbian (α =.79), Moral and Religious

Attitudes Toward Lesbianism (α =.74), and Attitudes Toward Other Lesbians (α =.77).

Interscore correlations ranged from .60 to .87. The authors also measured the test-retest reliability of the LIHS in a two-week period and total scores indicated a Chronbach’s alpha of α =.93. The subscales Chronbach’s alphas were as follows: Connection with the

Lesbian Community (α =.91), Public Identification as a Lesbian (α =.93), Personal

93

Feelings about Being a Lesbian (α =.88), Moral and Religious Attitudes Toward

Lesbianism (α =.75), and Attitudes Toward Other Lesbians (α =.87). To explore content validity the authors used five expert raters who indicated support (Szymanski & Chung,

2001b). Construct validity was also supported when correlations were explored between the LIHS and measures of depression, self-esteem, social support, loneliness, passing for straight, LGB group membership, and sexual orientation conflict (Szymanski & Chung,

2001; Szymanski, Chung, & Balsam, 2001). Subsequent studies have translated the scale into the Italian language and also modified it for use with other sexual minority women, ie bisexual women (Balsam & Szymanski, 2005; Montano, 2000; Rowan, 2004).

The LIHS measure unique aspects of internalized heterosexism but research has also suggested that the five dimensions that the LIHS measures have not been cohesively included in other internalized heterosexism measures and have been created for gay men

(Szymanski & Chung, 2001). Szymanski & Chung (2001) argue that these dimensions are a combined experience in lesbian women’s lives and important constructs to be measured together when assessing for internalized heterosexism in women. The LIHS is currently the only published scale for internalized heterosexism in lesbian women.

Szymanski & Chung (2001) found a total scale alpha of .94 for the LIHS. Further,

Szymanski & Chung (2001) found correlations between total and subscale scores from

.60 to .87 which also indicated construct validity for the LIHS.

The Internalized Misogyny Scale (IMS; Piggot, 2004). The Internalized

Misogyny Scale (See Appendix G) is a self-report measure created by Piggot (2004) to measure the construct of internalized sexism. The term internalized misogyny is another way to describe the experience of internalizing the sexism based on the greater

94

patriarchal culture in our society. The author argues that misogyny is a better term for the internalization of sexism because the degradation and devaluation of women is at the core of the power differential between men and women in our society (Johnson, 2014; Piggot,

2004; Szymanski & Henrichs-Beck, 2014). In the current study the IMS was used to measure experiences of internalized sexism. Sample items of the IMS include, “It is generally safer not to trust other women too much” and “Generally, I prefer to work with men.” The IMS consists of 17 items that are completed with a 7-point Likert scale from 1

(Strongly disagree) to 7 (Strongly agree). Scores can be totaled or averaged with higher scores indicating higher levels of internalized sexism. The current study used an overall mean score of internalized sexism which has been typically utilized in research

(Szymanski & Henrichs-Beck, 2014; Szymanski, Ikizler, & Dunn, 2016; Szymanski &

Kashubeck-West, 2008; Szymanski & Stewart, 2010). However, there is no empirical that we are aware of to support the use of total scores for the IMS (i.e., through confirmatory factor analytic results supporting either a bifactor, hierarchical or unidimensional model), which is a limitation of the measure.

The three factors of the IMS are: Devaluing of Women, Distrust of Women, and

Gender Bias in Favor of Men. An exploratory factor analysis using maximum likelihood extraction with oblique rotation was utilized and a 3-factor solution with the 17-items was adequately supported. The five-factor model accounted for 42.22% of the total variance. Full scale and subscales showed good reliability with Chronbach’s alphas ranging from .88 to .90 (Piggott, 2004; Szymanski et al., 2009). The scale has shown good construct validity with strong correlations with the IMS and measures of internalized heterosexism, modern sexism, depression, psychosexual adjustment, self-

95

esteem, and social desirability (Piggot, 2004; Shelby, 2014). Piggot (2004) completed a confirmatory factor analysis from a sample of 803 sexual minority women in five different countries showing strong reliability and validity across a cross-cultural sample.

No fit statistics were provided. Results indicated a significant positive relationship with internalized sexism and internalized homophobia across all countries.

The measure has also been validated with a sample of 171 heterosexual women and has proven to be a valid and reliable measure for both heterosexual and lesbian women (Szymanski et al., 2009). Szymanski and Henrichs-Beck (2014) also utilized the

IMS in their sample of 473 sexual minority women who identified as lesbian, bisexual, and not sure. The researchers used a modified version of the Kinsey scale when exploring identity and as was expected, there was much variation of identity. 39% of women indicated being attracted to only women, 39% indicated being attracted more to women than men, 11% indicated an attraction to both sexes, and 10% were attracted more to men than women. The sample ranged from 18 to 74 years and participants identified as White

(81%), African American/Black (3%), Asian American/Pacific Islander (5%),

Hispanic/Latina (5%), Native American (1%), Multiracial (4%), and Other (2%). This study replicated the reliability analysis of the earlier studies and showed strong reliability with a Chronbach alpha of α =.88. The IMS has been shown to be a good measure for internalized sexism in sexual minority women and a lesbian sample.

Procedures

Once Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval was obtained, participants were recruited from the social media and the online forum sites: Facebook and LGBT-specific

Reddit groups, ie. r/, r/ainbow, r/actuallesbians, r/lesbians, r/olderlesbians, r/,

96

and r/MeetLGBT, as well as snowball sampling. Potential participants were also asked to further post the link to the survey including all of the measures discussed above on their own social media/forum sites for individuals who meet study criteria. Potential participants were provided a link to Qualtrics to complete the study. All participants were given the optional offer to be entered into a raffle to win one of six $25 gift cards and their name and email address were collected but be de-identified from their responses.

The American Psychological Association (APA, 2002) ethical guidelines for research participation were taken into accord in the present study and all participants were treated ethically by these guidelines.

Participants who met inclusion criteria of being at least 18 years of age, identify as a woman or a transgender woman, and as “predominantly attracted to the same gender” or “exclusively attracted to the same gender” (5, 6, or 7 on the modified Kinsey scale in the sexual orientation demographics) were included in the study. Participants were provided an informed consent form (see Appendix I) and completed the demographic measure first with participants who met inclusion criteria continuing to the rest of the measures which were given at random (see Appendices A – G). A debriefing was provided at the conclusion of the study which also included information about intimate partner violence and mental health websites and resources available to participants.

Analytic Plan

Prior to analysis, data were inspected to determine if there were patterns in missing data and if the data violated any assumptions required for analyses. The data was also screened for univariate and multivariate normality (skewness, kurtosis) suggested by

97

Tabachnick and Fidell (2001). Missing data was handled utilizing available item analysis which has been found to be commensurate with multiple imputation and multivariate analyses (Parent, 2013). Means were calculated for participants who have completed at least 80% of a scale (Parent, 2013).

After completing the initial data screening, descriptive statistics and scale reliabilities were measured (i.e. means, standard deviations, Cronbach’s alpha). Bivariate correlations were also be completed in SPSS. SPSS and PROCESS (Hayes, 2013) was utilized to explore the interaction effects and moderated moderator models in the hypotheses. Hayes (2013) PROCESS macro for SPSS provides estimations of conditional effects in moderation relationships. Bootstrapping confidence intervals were utilized to analyze effect sizes and indirect effect sizes. Ferguson (2009) suggests when interpreting effect sizes that significant effects are identified by confidence intervals that do not contain a zero. The Bonferroni correction was also used in data analyses to minimize the chance of type 1 errors (Armstrong, 2014). Research questions and hypotheses were addressed as follows:

Research Question 1: Is there an additive effect of external sexism (sexist discrimination experiences), external heterosexism (heterosexist discrimination experiences), internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism on lesbian women’s symptoms of

PTSD?

• H1: I predicted that perceived sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination,

internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism would each be uniquely related

to higher PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women.

98

• Data Analysis Plan: In SPSS, a regression was conducted with PTSD severity

(PCL-5 scores) regressed on the four different predictors: sexist discrimination

(SSE scores), heterosexist discrimination (HHRDS scores), internalized sexism

(IMS scores), and internalized heterosexism (LIHS scores). The amount of

variance accounted for by the set of predictors was also explored as well as

individually. Squared semipartial correlation coefficients were analyzed to

examine the unique variance in PTSD severity by each predictor variable.

Research Question 2: Does internalized oppression strengthen the relationship between experiences of externalized oppression and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women?

• H2: Minority stress theory proposes that internalized oppression will interact with

externalized oppression (sexist discrimination experiences/heterosexist

discrimination experiences) for sexism and heterosexism. Hence, internalized

sexism will interact with sexist experiences and internalized heterosexism will

interact with heterosexist experiences increasing the link between external

oppression and negative mental health consequences (Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere,

2015). Therefore, I predicted that sexist discrimination would interact with

internalized sexism and that heterosexist discrimination would interact with

internalized heterosexism, with internalized oppression of each type of oppression

increasing the strengthen the relationship between external oppression and PTSD

symptom severity in lesbian women.

• Data Analysis Plan: A hierarchical multiple regression was conducted with PTSD

symptom severity (PCL-5 scores) as the dependent variable and each predictor

variable (sexist discrimination (SSE scores), heterosexist discrimination (HHRDS

99

scores), internalized sexism (IMS scores), and internalized heterosexism (LIHS

scores). Each predictor variable were centered prior to the creation of interaction

terms. The interaction terms were: heterosexist discrimination X internalized

heterosexism and sexist discrimination X internalized sexism. In step 1 of the

hierarchical regression, we included each predictor variable [(sexist

discrimination (SSE scores), heterosexist discrimination (HHRDS scores),

internalized sexism (IMS scores), and internalized heterosexism (LIHS scores)].

In step 2, we included the two interaction terms: heterosexist discrimination X

internalized heterosexism and sexist discrimination X internalized sexism.

Significant R2 change as well as regression coefficients were used to determine if

there were significant interaction effects. A simple slope analysis would be

conducted in PROCESS SPSS Model 1(Hayes, 2012) to analyze any significant

interaction effects.

Research Question 3: Does oppression of one type (sexism external/internal) increase the link of the other type (heterosexism external/internal) and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women?

• H3: Multicultural-feminist theory proposes that externalized oppressions (sexist

discrimination experiences and heterosexist discrimination experiences) interact

and internalized oppressions (internalized sexism and internalized heterosexism)

interact. Therefore, sexist discrimination interacts with heterosexist

discrimination, and internalized sexism interacts with internalized heterosexism,

increasing the link between external oppression experiences and internalized

oppression with negative mental health consequences (Velex, Moradi, &

100

DeBlaere, 2015). Consequently, I predicted that sexist discrimination would

interact with heterosexist discrimination and that internalized sexism interacts

with internalized heterosexism beyond that accounted for by their unique links,

with each form of oppression increasing the relationship of the other form of

oppression and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women.

• Data Analysis Plan: Following the guidelines of Aiken and West (1991) and

Velez, Moradi, and DeBlaere (2015), a hierarchical multiple regression will be

conducted with PTSD symptom severity (PCL-5 scores) as the dependent variable

and each predictor variable (sexist discrimination (SSE scores), heterosexist

discrimination (HHRDS scores), internalized sexism (IMS scores), and

internalized sexism (LIHS scores)) were centered prior to creating an interaction

term. The interaction terms were: heterosexist discrimination X sexist

discrimination and internalized sexism X internalized heterosexism. In step 1 of

the hierarchical regression, we included each predictor variable [(sexist

discrimination (SSE scores), heterosexist discrimination (HHRDS scores),

internalized sexism (IMS scores), and internalized heterosexism (LIHS scores)].

In step 2, we included the two interaction terms: heterosexist discrimination X

sexist discrimination and internalized sexism X internalized heterosexism.

Significant R2 change as well as regression coefficients were used to determine if

there are significant interaction effects. A simple slope analysis would be

conducted in PROCESS SPSS Model 1(Hayes, 2012) to analyze any significant

interaction effects.

101

Research Question 4: Does one form of externalized oppression strengthen the relationship between the other form of internalized oppression and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women?

• H4: In an attempt to combine minority stress theory and multicultural-feminist

theory, we hypothesize that one form of externalized oppression (sexist

discrimination experiences/heterosexist discrimination experiences) would

interact with the other form of internalized oppression (internalized

heterosexism/internalized sexism). That is, sexist discrimination would interact

with internalized heterosexism and heterosexist discrimination would interact

with internalized sexism, in both cases increasing the link between external

oppression of one form and internalized oppression of the other form, with

negative mental health consequences. Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere (2015) named

these types of interactions “synthesized interactions”, where one form of

oppression (ie. external sexism) is interacting with the other form of oppression

(ie. internalized heterosexism). Thus, I predicted that heterosexist discrimination

interacts with internalized sexism and that sexist discrimination interacts with

internalized heterosexism, so that internalized oppression of one type increases

the relationship of external oppression of the other type of oppression and PTSD

symptom severity in lesbian women. I also predicted that this interaction will

account for greater variance than the other aforementioned interactions (See Table

1 for all conceptual interaction terms for hierarchical regressions).

• Data Analysis Plan: Following the guidelines of Aiken and West (1991) and

Velez, Moradi, and DeBlaere (2015), a hierarchical multiple regression were

102

conducted with PTSD symptom severity (PCL-5 scores) as the dependent variable

and each predictor variable [(sexist discrimination (SSE scores), heterosexist

discrimination (HHRDS scores), internalized sexism (IMS scores), and

internalized heterosexism (LIHS scores)] were centered prior to the creation of an

interaction term. The interaction terms were: sexist discrimination X internalized

heterosexism and heterosexist discrimination X internalized sexism. In step 1, we

included each predictor variable (sexist discrimination (SSE scores), heterosexist

discrimination (HHRDS scores), internalized sexism (IMS scores), and

internalized heterosexism (LIHS scores). In step 2, we included the two

interaction terms: sexist discrimination X internalized heterosexism and

heterosexist discrimination X internalized sexism. Significant R2 change as well

as regression coefficients were used to determine if there are significant

interaction effects. A simple slope analysis would be conducted in PROCESS

SPSS Model 1(Hayes, 2012) to analyze any significant interaction effects.

Research Question 5: How do lesbian women’s experience of IPV impact their experiences of sexism, internalized sexism, and PTSD symptoms?

• H5: I predicted that there will be a moderated moderation, where the relationship

between externalized sexism (x) and PTSD symptom severity (y) is moderated by

internalized sexism (M) with severity of IPV (Z) moderating the relationship

between internalized sexism and PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women. I

also predicted a significant positive relationship between externalized sexism and

PTSD symptom severity for those who have higher internalized sexism and

greater severity of IPV.

103

• Data Analysis Plan: A moderated moderation was tested utilizing Hayes’ (2013)

PROCESS Model 3. The moderating moderation analysis tested the three-way

interaction between external sexism (predictor), internalized sexism (moderator),

and severity of IPV (moderating moderator) as predictors of PTSD symptom

severity (See Figure 1).

Research Question 6: How do lesbian women’s experience of IPV impact their experiences of heterosexism, internalized heterosexism, and PTSD symptoms?

• H6: I predicted that there would be a moderated moderator, where the relationship

between externalized heterosexism (x) and PTSD symptom severity(y) is

moderated by internalized heterosexism (M) with severity of IPV (Z) moderating

the relationship between internalized heterosexism and PTSD symptom severity

in lesbian women. I also predicted a significant positive relationship between

externalized heterosexism and PTSD symptom severity for those who have higher

internalized heterosexism and greater severity of IPV.

• Data Analysis Plan: A moderated moderation was tested utilizing Hayes’ (2012)

PROCESS Model 3. The moderating moderation analysis tested the three-way

interaction between external heterosexism (predictor), internalized heterosexism

(moderator), and severity of IPV (moderating moderator) as predictors of PTSD

symptom severity (See Figure 2).

104

Chapter IV

Results

Data Screening and Missing Data

Five hundred and twenty-six participants initiated the survey; however, 154 only consented to the survey and did not complete any additional items or completed only the demographic portion of the survey and did not complete any primary measure. The initial dataset consisted of 422 participants; however, 187 (44.31%) participants were omitted because they did not meet inclusion criteria. Specifically, 140 participants were omitted because they: (a) identified as heterosexual, (b) exclusively attracted to the other gender,

(c) predominantly attracted to the other gender, (d) only incidentally attracted to the same gender, or e) identified as male. Four individuals were omitted for being under 18-years- old, and 43 women were removed for identifying as bisexual both on the sexual orientation demographic question and the modified Kinsey scale. Of the remaining participants, 26 individuals (11.06%) did not complete at least 80% of each primary measure, and thus were also omitted, leaving a final sample size of 209 participants.

Research by Downey and King (1998) indicates that scale reliability is unlikely to be biased when the scale is at least 80% completed. All means, minimums, and maximums for each scale were explored to ensure they fell within the expected range.

105

To assess for normality, histograms were used to examine the shape of the distribution (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2012). The data appeared normal for all variables except internalized sexism and IPV. Internalized sexism was both skewed and kurtotic

(skewness = 2.16, kurtosis = 6.75). Data were examined for outliers, which were identified as values that exceeded +/- 3 SD of the variable mean, and values identified as outliers were brought to the 3 SD range. Following adjustment of two identified outliers on the internalized sexism variable, distributions for all variables were reexamined for significant skewness (≤ ± 2) and kurtosis (≤ ±7), which were both in the recommended range for internalized sexism (Curran, West, and Finch, 1996). IPV was both skewed and kurtotic (skew skewness = 5.81, kurtosis = 38.67). Logarithm transformations were used due to the severity of skewness and kurtosis. All variables were reassessed and were in the normal range, including IPV (skewness = 1.28, kurtosis = .92). Estimating a

Mahalonobis distance revealed no multivariate outliers. To assess bivariate multicollinearity, zero-order correlations were examined among all variables (see Table

3). Multivariate multicollinearity and singularity were assessed by running collinearity diagnostics. The condition index was below .30 for each dimension, which is in the acceptable range (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2012).

Missing data were handled utilizing available item analysis, which has been found to be commensurate with multiple imputation (Parent, 2013). Means were calculated for participants who completed at least 80% of a scale (Parent, 2013). The Bonferroni correction was also used in data analyses to minimize the chance of type 1 errors

(Armstrong, 2014). After calculating the Bonferroni correction, significance for all analyses is p<0.005.

106

Descriptive Statistics

Means, standard deviations, measurement ranges, and Cronbach’s alphas for each measure can be found in Table 4. Alphas for the measures ranged from .87 to .95. In the current sample, the mean score for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptom severity on the PCL-5 was 26.52 (SD = 19.21), which is slightly below the cutoff score of

33 for clinically significant symptoms of PTSD (Blevins, Weathers, Davis, Witte, &

Domino, 2015). In the present sample, 71 participants (33.9%) had scores above the clinically significant cut-off score of 33 on the PCL-5. The mean score for externalized heterosexism on the HHRDS was 15.60 (SD = 16.03), the mean score of externalized sexism on the SSE was 31.09 (SD = 19.74), the mean score for internalized heterosexism on the LIHS was 107.68 (SD = 35.82) and the mean score for internalized sexism on the

IMS was 28.45 (SD = 12.71). Lastly, the current sample’s mean score of IPV severity on the SVAW was 3.60 (SD = 11.25). Subtypes of the SVAW, including threat of violence, physical violence, and sexual violence, had mean scores of 2.04 (SD = 5.99), 1.14 (SD =

4.91), and .43 (SD = 1.65), respectively.

Please see Table 3 for zero-order correlations between all variables. Externalized heterosexism and externalized sexism were significantly and positively correlated with a higher endorsement of posttraumatic stress symptoms (r (207)= .22, p < .001 and r

(207)= .33, p < .001, respectively). More experiences of externalized heterosexism was significantly correlated with higher experiences of externalized sexism (r (207)= .62, p <

.001). Greater internalized heterosexism was significantly and positively correlated with internalized sexism (r (207)= .31, p < .001). The strongest positive correlation for PTSD symptoms was with externalized sexism (r = .33, p < .001). Surprisingly, neither

107

internalized heterosexism nor internalized sexism was significantly correlated with PTSD symptoms. Internalized sexism correlated negatively with PTSD symptoms (r (207)= -

.05, p =.49). An examination of zero-order correlations for demographic variables and

PTSD symptoms revealed that SES was significantly correlated with PTSD symptoms.

As such, SES was utilized as a covariate for all subsequent analyses.

Primary Analyses

To test hypothesis 1, a hierarchical multiple regression analysis was used to predict PTSD symptoms from sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism (see Table 5). Given that bivariate correlations revealed SES to be significantly associated with PTSD symptoms, SES was entered into Step 1 of the model as a covariate in order to examine whether the hypothesized predictor variables contributed additional variance above and beyond SES.

Step 2 included each of the hypothesized predictor variables, including perceived sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism. Step 1 of the model indicated a significant relationship, with SES accounting for 4% of the variance in PTSD symptoms (F(1,207) = 9.75, p=.002). SES was significantly negatively related to PTSD symptoms in Step 1 (β = -.21, t = -3.12, p=.002, sr2 = .04). Step 2 of the model indicated that as a group, the hypothesized predictors were a significant addition to the model and accounted for 8.9% additional variance in PTSD symptoms, above and beyond SES (Fchange (4) = 5.21, p=.001).

Externalized sexism was significantly related to PTSD symptoms (β = .31, t = 3.56, p <

.001, sr2 = .05). However, heterosexist discrimination (β = -.00, t = -.04, p =.97 sr2 =.00),

108

internalized sexism (β = .03, t = .42, p=.68, sr2 = .00), and internalized heterosexism (β =

-.02, t = .33, p =.74, sr2 = .00) were not significantly related to PTSD symptoms.

Hypothesis 2 postulated that sexist discrimination would interact with internalized sexism and that heterosexist discrimination would interact with internalized heterosexism in how they related to PTSD symptom severity (see Table 6). More specifically, it is hypothesized that when internalized oppression is more severe, higher levels of external oppression will more strongly predict a high severity of PTSD symptoms. A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted where SES was entered into Step 1 of the model as a covariate and the centered predictor variables (i.e., sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism) were entered in Step 2. Variance accounted for by Step 1 and Step 2 were identical to the first analysis. Consistent with the first analysis, only the main effect of sexist discrimination was significantly associated with PTSD symptoms (β = .33, t = 3.77, p < .001, sr2 = .06).

Step 3, which included the two centered interaction terms, accounted for 1.1% additional variance in PTSD (F(7,201) = 4.87, p<.001), although the increase in explained variance was not statistically significant Fchange (2) = 1.34, p = .26. Hypothesis 2 was not supported as neither of the interaction effects in Step 3 of the model were significant: sexist discrimination and internalized sexism interaction (β = .15, t = 1.41, p =.16, sr2

=.00) and heterosexist discrimination and internalized heterosexism interaction (β = .13, t

= .58, p=.57, sr2 =.00).

The analysis for Hypothesis 3 tested whether sexist discrimination interacts with heterosexist discrimination and internalized sexism interacts with internalized heterosexism in predicting PTSD symptoms (see Table 7). More specifically, it is

109

believed that more severe levels of one type of oppression (i.e., heterosexism) will more strongly predict higher PTSD symptom severity when the other type of oppression (i.e., sexism) is also present at higher levels. Similar to the previous analysis, a hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted where SES was entered into Step 1 of the model as a covariate and the centered predictor variables (i.e., sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism) were entered in Step 2. Step 3, which included the centered interaction terms, accounted for

1.9% additional variance in PTSD (F(7,201) = 5.17, p<.001), which was a nonsignificant change from the previous step Fchange (2) = 2.25, p = .11. Hypothesis 3 was not supported as neither of the interaction effects in Step 3 of the model were significant: sexist discrimination and externalized heterosexism interaction (β = -.29, t = -2.07, p

=.04, sr2 =.02) and internalized sexism and internalized heterosexism was not interaction

(β = .06, t = .29, p =.77, sr2 =.00).

Hypothesis 4 was tested by examining whether heterosexist discrimination interacted with internalized sexism and sexist discrimination interacted with internalized heterosexism in predicting PTSD symptom severity (see Table 8). Specifically, it is hypothesized that when external oppression is more severe, higher levels of internalized oppression will more strongly predict higher PTSD symptom severity. Consistent with the previous models, SES was entered into Step 1 of the model as a covariate and the centered predictor variables (i.e., sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism) were entered in Step 2. Step 3, which included the centered interaction terms, accounted for 0.7% additional variance in PTSD

(F(7,201) = 4.68, p<.001), a change from the previous step that was not statistically

110

significant Fchange (2) = 0.78, p = .46. Hypothesis 4 was not supported as neither of the interaction terms in Step 3 of the model were significant: sexist discrimination and internalized heterosexism interaction (β = .17, t = .80, p=.43, sr2 =.00) and heterosexist discrimination and internalized sexism interaction (β = .06, t = .89, p=.38, sr2 =.00).

Hypothesis 5 posited that the relationship between sexist discrimination and

PTSD symptom severity would be moderated by internalized sexism and the strength of moderation would depend on the severity of IPV. Hayes (2013) Model 3 PROCESS macro was utilized and PTSD severity scores were regressed on experiences of sexist discrimination, internalized sexism, IPV severity, and the following interaction terms: sexist discrimination x internalized sexism, sexist discrimination x IPV severity,

Internalized sexism x IPV severity, and sexist discrimination x internalized sexism x IPV severity while controlling for SES. The overall model was significant (F(7, 201) = 5.13, p<.001), however, the three-way interaction was not significant (F(1,201) =- 87, p=.39) and accounted for 0.3% additional variance in PTSD above and beyond the other variables. None of the variables in the model, except the covariate, was an independent predictor of PTSD and no significant interactions were found (see Table 9).

Hypothesis 6 was similarly tested by exploring whether the strength of the moderating role of internalized heterosexism on the relationship between heterosexist discrimination and PTSD symptom severity varied depending on severity of IPV while controlling for SES. Hayes (2013) Model 3 PROCESS macro was also utilized to test this hypothesis, regressing PTSD severity scores on heterosexist discrimination, internalized heterosexism, IPV severity, and these interaction terms: heterosexist discrimination x internalized heterosexism, heterosexist discrimination x IPV symptom severity,

111

internalized heterosexism x IPV symptom severity, and heterosexist discrimination x internalized heterosexism x IPV symptom severity. The overall model was significant

(F(7,201) = 2.05, p=.04) though the three-way interaction was not significant (F(1,201) =

.44, p=.50), with 0.2% additional variance in PTSD accounted for by the final interaction above and beyond the variance accounted for by the other variables. Additionally, only the covariate emerged as a significant predictor of PTSD. No significant interactions or other independent predictors were found (see Table 10).

112

CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION

The present study adds to the existent literature on sexism, heterosexism, intimate partner violence, and posttraumatic stress in many ways. Consistent with other studies that have found that sexual minority women experience sexist and heterosexist events

(DeBlaere & Bertsch, 2013; Mason, Lewis, Winstead, & Dertega, 2015; Szymanski,

2005; Szymanski & Henrichs-Beck, 2013), the current results demonstrated that greater exposure to sexist events and heterosexist events were associated with higher levels of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms (PTSD). Additionally, the present study found that at low levels of sexist experiences, greater heterosexist experiences was associated with PTSD symptom severity. Conversely, a relationship was also found, wherein low heterosexist experiences and high sexist experiences was also associated with PTSD symptom severity. This is inconsistent with prior research (Szymanski, 2005) that found that greater heterosexist experiences were associated with greater sexist experiences when predicting for psychological distress. These findings extend the existing literature on interactional theory and suggest that future research should explore these constructs intersectionally for lesbian women. The present study also did not find support for either

IPV hypotheses, which may be due to the low rates of IPV reported by participants in the

113

present study. This chapter discusses these findings in detail and addresses their implications for future research and practice.

Hypothesis 1

Hypothesis one predicted that that perceived sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism would each be uniquely related to higher PTSD symptom severity in lesbian women. Consistent with prior research, (e.g., DeBlaere & Bertsch, 2013; Szymanski, 2005; Szymanski & Owens,

2009), sexist discrimination was a unique predictor of psychological distress in lesbian women, specifically sexist discrimination related to higher PTSD symptom severity.

However, inconsistent with the prior research (Mason, Lewis, Winstead, & Dertega,

2015; Piggot, 2004; Szymanski, 2005; Szymanski & Kashubeck-West, 2008; Szymanski

& Owens, 2009; Szymanski & Moffitt, 2012, heterosexist discrimination, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism were not found to be uniquely related to psychological distress. Contrary to expectation, the additive perspective of multiple oppression was not supported which postulates an accumulative effect of minority identities and that each identity is uniquely important and additively influence mental health outcomes (Nelson and Probst, 2004; Warner, 2008).

This finding is inconsistent with previous research conducted by Szymanski

(2005) that found that heterosexist events, sexist events, and internalized heterosexism were uniquely related to psychological distress in lesbian women. Szymanski and

Henrichs-Beck (2013) found support for the additive perspective in sexual minority women finding that heterosexist events, internalized heterosexism, sexist events, and internalized sexism were significant and unique predictors of psychological distress in

114

sexual minority women. Velez, Moradi, and DeBlaere (2014) found that racist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination, and internalized heterosexism uniquely predicted for psychological distress in sexual minority Latina/o individuals. Additionally,

Szymanski and Balsam (2011) found that heterosexist discrimination was a significant positive predictor of lesbians’ PTSD symptoms. The low levels of internalized oppression in the current sample is one possible explanation for why they did not emerge as unique predictors of PTSD in this study. Additionally, there was a higher number of women who experienced sexist discrimination than heterosexist discrimination. In the current sample,

99.5% of lesbian women reported at least one incident of sexist discrimination in the past year as compared to 83.5% for heterosexist discrimination. Overall, the sample identified more experiences of sexist discrimination which could account for heterosexist discrimination not being a unique predictor of PTSD symptom severity. Additionally, experiences of sexism could be more readily perceived by lesbian women as they are more frequently discussed and confronted, compared to heterosexist experiences.

However, the finding that sexist discrimination was uniquely related to higher rates of PTSD symptoms builds upon the current literature on sexism and the development of PTSD (Berg, 2002; Berg, 2006). This finding challenges the current

DSM conceptualization of trauma since sexist events were measured including experiences of discrimination and prejudice that would not currently meet diagnostic criteria A for PTSD. There are only two items on the SSE that could indicate a criteria A traumatic event for PTSD: “How many times have people made inappropriate or unwanted sexual advances to you because you are a woman” and How many times have you been made fun of, picked on, pushed, shoved, hit, or threatened with harm because

115

you are a woman?”, which were experienced at relatively lower rates in the study’s sample. Lesbian women in the present study experienced being treated unfairly by people in service jobs, teachers/professors, coworkers, and strangers, as well as, being called sexist names (ie. Bitch, cunt, chick, or other names) at higher rates. These findings attest to the impact of sexism on women’s lives broadly, as well as, lesbian women. For lesbian women in particular, it may be that many of their experiences of their sexual orientation are inherently gendered based on how others perceive their and thus their sexual orientation. Daley, Solomon, Newman, and Mishna (2007) discussed an intersectional approach for studying sexual minority individuals by describing a potential interaction of sexism and racism and heterosexism when addressing racialized sexual harassment. Within this framework, it may be that for lesbian women sexism was the unique predictor for PTSD because they view most of their experiences whether sexist or heterosexist through the lens of their gender (gendered heterosexism).

The means and frequencies for the items on externalized heterosexism, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism were low in comparison to the maximum point on these scales which could explain the non-significant findings for this hypothesis (see Table 4). This would suggest that participants experienced less heterosexist events and less internalized oppression. However, a number of participants did endorse heterosexist events and internalized oppression, just at lower rates than sexist events. This is consistent with previous research (Szymanski, 2005) that found relatively low rates of heterosexist events and sexist events. However, this finding is inconsistent with the same research since even at low rates sexist and heterosexist experiences have been found to be unique predictors for psychological distress (2005). Although the

116

frequency of sexist and heterosexist events wasn’t high, experiencing at least one sexist or heterosexist experience in the past year was common for the sample. The most frequent heterosexist event experienced was hearing anti-lesbian/anti-gay/anti-bisexual remarks from family members and being rejected by family members because they were a sexual minority. The most frequently endorsed item on the internalized heterosexism measure was strongly disagree on the item “I believe female homosexuality is a sin” though many women endorsed other items of internalized heterosexism. Interestingly, women reported lower rates of internalized sexism compared to internalized heterosexism.

The majority of the participants were sampled from LGBTQ online forums where there is a community of other sexual minority women. Perhaps, women on LGBTQ women forums are less likely to respond affirmatively to items related to the internalization of messages related to their gender and sexual orientation. However, these results should be interpreted carefully as they do not imply that lesbian women don’t experience heterosexist events or internalized oppression. Piggot (2004) found that lesbian women in the later stages of identity development (i.e. identity pride and identity synthesis) had lower levels of internalized heterosexism compared to lesbian women in beginning stages of identity development (i.e. identity confusion, comparison, tolerance, and acceptance). Mildner (2001) found that internalized heterosexism correlated negatively with aspects of sexual identity development and phase of group membership identity development in lesbian women. This finding suggests that higher identification with lesbian communities was related to lower internalized heterosexism. Perhaps, because this study collected the majority of its sample of sexual minority women from

117

LGBTQ online forums on Reddit where there may be a sense of community, lesbian women in this sample reported less internalized oppression. Future studies should continue to explore internalized oppression and heterosexist discrimination to better understand sexual minority women’s experiences.

Socioeconomic status was also found to be significant throughout all analyses as a unique predictor for PTSD symptom severity for lesbian women. Intersectionality theory posits that it is important to consider the unique intersections of all the social identities an individual is a member of (Bowleg, 2008). Sexual orientation research has largely focused on white, middle-class, gay men which has left out the diverse experiences of the sexual minority community (McGarrity, 2014). Socioeconomic status includes education, income, and occupational status which have been related to many health-related outcomes (Adler & Stewart, 2010). Adler et al., (1994) discussed the importance of understanding socioeconomic status as an important social category that can affect many areas of an individual’s health.

Gallo & Matthews (2003) developed the reserve capacity model which proposes that lower-socioeconomic status individuals have less tangible, interpersonal, and intrapersonal resources to handle stressful life experiences compared to individuals with a higher socioeconomic status. Additionally, the authors note that lower-socioeconomic status individuals may have more frequent stressful life events which could cause a diminishing reserve capacity, wherein they use all available resources and can’t replenish in time for the next stressful experience. Overall, lower socioeconomic status individuals have been found to have less social support and less community engagement compared to higher socioeconomic individuals, which are both important resources for resiliency

118

(Adler & Snibbe, 2003). Coping styles have also been found to be different for lower socioeconomic status individuals compared to higher socioeconomic individuals. Lower socioeconomic status has been found to be related to emotion-focused or avoidant coping, which can lead to psychological distress (Holohan & Moos, 1987; Billings &

Moos, 1981).

McGarrity (2014) discusses the lack of research on socioeconomic status for sexual minority individuals due to a societal assumption that sexual minority individuals have increased education and higher financial success that heterosexual individuals

(Albelda, Badgett, Schneedbaum, & Gates, 2009). Shugart (2003) notes that the myth of gay affluence is largely due to media portrayal of a white middle-class sexual minority individuals, though this has more frequently been applied to gay men rather than lesbian women. Recent media portrayals of sexual minority individuals highlight them as having professional careers, spending large amounts of money, having stable relationships, and having children, which has been a successful strategy in trying to attain social and political rights. However, this portrayal also marginalizes single, non-monogamous, lower socioeconomic status, and racial and ethnic minority lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. Recent research has begun to attempt to disprove the myth of gay affluence to include all sexual minority experiences (Albelda et al., 2009; McGarrity, 2014).

In regard to experiences of discrimination, research has found that lower socioeconomic status has been related to increased levels of negative life events, chronic stress, and more perceived discrimination (Brady & Matthews, 2002; Forman, Williams,

& Jackson, 1997; Gary, 1995; McLeod & Kessler, 1990; Matthews et al., 2000;

Matthews, Gallo, & Taylor, 2010; Sigelman & Welch, 1991). Gamarel et al.’s (2012)

119

study found that gay and bisexual men with higher socioeconomic status had less discrimination experiences and reported that experiences of discrimination had less impact on their lives compared to lower socioeconomic status gay and bisexual men.

Lower socioeconomic status has also been associated with IPV and PTSD (Cavanah,

Hansen, & Sullivan, 2010; Kolltvelt, Lange-Nielsen, & Thabet, 2012; Schmidt, 2014;

Vogel & Marshall, 2001) and though psychological research has typically used socioeconomic status as a control variable, there is evidence to suggest that this construct should be explored in the experiences of sexual minority individuals. Less is known about socioeconomic status and PTSD symptoms for lesbian women, the present study’s findings suggest that future research should explore this important social identity.

Hypothesis 2

Hypothesis two predicted that sexist discrimination would interact with internalized sexism and that heterosexist discrimination would interact with internalized heterosexism in how they related to PTSD symptom severity. More specifically, when internalized oppression is more severe, higher levels of external oppression will more strongly predict a high severity of PTSD symptoms. Contrary to prediction, neither interaction between sexist discrimination and internalized sexism nor heterosexist discrimination and internalized heterosexism predicted higher PTSD symptom severity.

This is finding is in opposition of previous research conducted by Mason et al. (2015) that found main effects between heterosexist events and internalized heterosexism positively though weakly in their study exploring the moderating roles of social constraints and collective self-esteem in sexual minority women. Additionally, Velez,

Moradi, and DeBlaere (2014) found a significant interaction effect between racist

120

discrimination and internalized racism in predicting life satisfaction and self-esteem in sexual minority Latina/o individuals.

Contrary to Meyer’s (1995) research on the interaction of internalized heterosexism and heterosexist discrimination and violence in gay men, the study found an interaction between internalized heterosexism and heterosexist discrimination and violence predicting demoralization, guilt, and psychological distress. Further, higher internalized heterosexism with high heterosexist experiences have more of an effect on mental health outcomes than that of low internalized heterosexism and high heterosexist discrimination in gay men. The present sample had relatively low rates of internalized heterosexism compared to heterosexist discrimination (see Table 4 for observed ranges and mean scores) which could explain the non-significant findings. Additionally,

Szymanski (2008) describes many studies (Bennett & O’Conner, 2002; Herek et al.,

1998; Meyers, 1995; Szymanski & Chung, 2001; Szymanski et al., 2001) on internalized heterosexism that had low levels of internalized heterosexism which had less of an impact on mental health factors compared to high levels of internalized heterosexism.

These findings should be interpreted with caution in drawing conclusions about lesbian women compared to research on gay men and samples that included all sexual minority women, as their experiences may be inherently different.

Furthermore, these findings suggest, as in previous research (Brewster et al.,

2013; Meyer, 1995; Szymanski & Meyer, 2008; Szymanski & Sung, 2010) that internalized oppression does not influence an individual’s report of experiences of external oppression nor conversely does the experience of external oppression directly lead to internalized oppression. However, much of the research has been cross-sectional

121

and not longitudinal to see if there is a time effect. Overall, these findings and previous research on the interaction between forms of oppression in sexual minority women, sexual minority people of color, and African American women (Buchanan & Fitzgerald

2008; Moradi & Subich 2003; Szymanski & Gupta 2009; Szymanski & Kashubeck-West

2008; Szymanski & Meyer 2008; Szymanski & Owens 2008; Szymanski & Stewart

2010) have not supported the interaction perspective when predicting mental health outcomes. Future researchers should explore the intersectionality multiple oppression perspective. in lesbian women’s experiences with oppression and psychological distress, including PTSD symptom severity. However, there is a lack of measures for these constructs for sexual minority women and much of the research has focused on gendered racism and internalized gendered racism in African American women (King 2003;

Klevens 2008; Thomas, Witherspoon, & Speight, 2004; 2008; Woods, Buchanon, &

Settles, 2008).

Shin, et al. (2017) noted that there is a lack of research in counseling psychology on intersectionality and urges researchers to explore social identity not as independent but as inseparable. Moving towards an intersectional perspective is essential for understanding lesbian women’s experiences and to confront the hegemonic heteronormative norms that are dominant in our society (Phillips, 2010). Grzanka,

Santos, & Moradi (2017) noted that counseling psychologists have a responsibility to confront the ways intersectionality can inform theory and modernize research on social identity. Additionally, Moradi and Grzanka (2017) discussed that counseling psychologists can unintentionally minimize the effects of power in social inequality by not exploring social identity in an intersectional way. The authors also critiqued

122

measuring social identity as a demographic variable which offers a static view of how social identity may function (2017). Levine and Breshears, (2019) also discussed that intersectionality research should be focusing on political goals and structural inequities to modernize the research and create change. Additionally, Sarno, Mohr, Jackson, and

Fassinger (2015) discuss the importance of creating measures that allow researchers to utilize intersectionality in their research for sexual minority individuals. The findings of this study support the need for measures that explore these constructs in sexual minority women utilizing an intersectional perspective (gendered heterosexism; Friedman &

Leaper, 2010), as well as, the need to explore potential other variables that could influence the relationship between internalized oppression and externalized oppression when predicting for PTSD symptom severity.

Additionally, the present sample’s low levels of internalized oppression could be representative of resilience factors in lesbian women. Szymanski (2008) discusses that the historic view of internalized heterosexism has some concerns due to pathologizing sexual minority individual’s distress due to oppression, and thus make sexual minority individuals responsible for the distress they may experience rather than society. This may lead sexual minority individuals to feel that they need to change rather than society.

Additionally, the conceptualization of internalized heterosexism ignores the inherent resilience that sexual minority individuals have shown historically even with pervasive heterosexism. Lesbian women in the present sample may have a multitude of resources for coping with the oppression they experience. Factors such as, social support, outness, connection to community, and access to resources may be accounting for lower levels of

123

internalized oppression in the present sample of lesbian women. Future studies should explore resilience factors when exploring internalized oppression for lesbian women.

Another explanation is that the present sample is not representative of the more diverse sexual minority community. The present sample was mostly white, a mode age of

23-years old, and above average socioeconomic status. The present sample may not reflect the experiences of oppression and internalized oppression for all lesbian women.

For example, research has found that sexual minority people of color are more likely to experience externalized discrimination and stigma, less likely to be out, less likely to identify as gay, and have increased rates of internalized oppression (Huang et al., 2010;

Moradi et al., 2010; Parks, Hughes, & Matthews, 2004). Future studies should explore externalized oppression, internalized oppression, IPV, and PTSD symptom severity in more representative samples of lesbian women.

Additionally, help-seeking and access to mental health treatment may be another explanation for the low levels of internalized oppression and PTSD symptoms in the present sample. Historically, research has shown that sexual minority individuals access mental health treatment at higher rates compared to heterosexual individuals (Bradford,

Ryan, & Rothblum, 1994; Hughes, Haas, Razzano, Cassidy, & Matthews, 2000; Liddle,

1997; Murphy, Rawlings, & Howe, 2002). Kashubeck-West, Szymanski, & Meyer

(2008) discuss the importance of counseling when addressing internalized oppression and psychological distress. The authors suggest that utilizing a feminist approach of facilitating awareness of internalized heterosexism, exploring the negative impact of heterosexism on their lives, challenging internalized heterosexism, and building skills for confronting oppression can help minimize the negative effects of external and

124

internalized oppression in sexual minority women’s lives. It is possible that the present sample not only may be connected to mental health resources but may be engaging in some of these behaviors within their communities and on the sexual minority online forums that they were recruited from. The forums are largely used as a social support resource and may be one way the present sample utilizes social support to confront the oppressive experiences they endure. Future research should continue to explore the help- seeking behaviors and access to mental health resources for lesbian women. It may be important to capture a sample of women that is more representative of the larger community, many of which may not have access to community or resources.

Hypothesis 3

Hypothesis three predicted that sexist discrimination interacts with heterosexist discrimination and internalized sexism interacts with internalized heterosexism in predicting PTSD symptoms (see Table 7). More specifically, it is believed that more severe levels of one type of oppression (i.e., heterosexist discrimination and internalized heterosexism) will more strongly predict higher PTSD symptom severity when the other type of oppression (i.e., sexist discrimination and internalized sexism) is also present at higher levels. Contrary to previous research (Thoma & Huebner, 2013; Szymanski, 2005;

Szymanski & Stewart, 2010) that found interactions between externalized forms of oppression, this prediction was not supported. However, previous research has been limited in finding a significant interaction between sexism and heterosexism experiences in sexual minority women when predicting for psychological distress. Szymanski

(2005)’s study did find a significant interaction with heterosexist victimization strengthening the link between sexist discrimination and psychological distress for

125

lesbian women. However, the researcher measured heterosexist events by hate crime victimization and the present study included heterosexist experiences that include experiences of discrimination and prejudice. Additionally, sexist discrimination had main effects in hypotheses 1-4, experiences of sexism and its links to PTSD symptoms should continue to be explored in lesbian women. Future research should also continue to explore heterosexist discrimination which does not only include hate crimes and heterosexist victimization but rather explores the experiences of discrimination and prejudice that lesbian women may experience that though may be overt, are not necessarily including physical violence.

The present study’s non-significant findings may be due to lesbian women perceiving experiences in a potentially intersectional way, whereas they are perceiving their experiences as inherently linked (gendered heterosexism; Friedman & Leaper,

2010). Lesbian women’s experiences with heterosexism are largely sexist in nature in many ways. Historically, research has found that sexual minority women who have a gender expression in a non-conforming way or attempt to refuse sexual advances from men have reported being assaulted because of violating traditional gender norms

(Fernald, 1995; Friedman, Ayres, & Leaper, 2008; Rich, 1980). Research has also found that lesbian women are perceived as more masculine than heterosexual women (Lehavot

& Lambert, 2007) and women who don’t conform to gender norms are considered less acceptable than those who do conform because they are violating their gender norms

(Horn, 2007; Lehavot & Lambert, 2007). Therefore, stereotypes for lesbian women based on gender and sexual orientation are linked and the discrimination and prejudice they experience is perceived as both sexist and heterosexist. Further, the concept of gendered

126

heterosexism intersectionally would suggest that lesbian women experience discrimination not additively nor interactionally but in a new and unique experience of discrimination which impact psychological outcomes (Cole, 2009). The literature

(Bowleg, 2013; Bowleg, Teti, Malebranche, & Tschann, 2013; Grzanka, Santos, &

Moradi, 2017; Levine & Breshears, 2019; Moradi & Grzanka, 2017; Rosenthal & Lobel,

2011; 2016; Sarno, Mohr, Jackson, & Fassinger, 2015; Settles, 2006; Shin, et al, 2017;

Thomas et al., 2011) supports utilizing an intersectional approach when exploring social identities. These findings suggest that exploring intersectionality in sexual minority women is an important future step and show the need to explore other variables that may be affecting the relationship between sexist discrimination, heterosexist discrimination and PTSD symptoms.

One variable that may impact lesbian women’s experiences with sexist and heterosexist discrimination is gender expression. Levitt and Horne (2002) explored discrimination experiences of lesbian women based on the identities of “butch,”

,” “androgynous,” or “other.” The researchers found that butch-identifying women and androgynous-identifying women had higher rates of heterosexist discrimination than femme-identifying or other-identifying women. Butch-identifying women also reported that they became aware of their sexual orientation at younger ages

(approximately 15-years of age compared to 22-years of age) and were more likely to experience heterosexist discrimination during their adolescence. Hequembourg and

Brallier (2009) found similar results where butch-identifying women were more likely to have experienced conflict particularly with heterosexual men compared to femme- identifying women. Butch women were also more likely to believe heterosexual men

127

were threatened by their . Interestingly, femme-identifying women were more likely to perceive conflict with heterosexual men when their sexual orientation was based on their physical appearance. Femme women were also more likely to feel marginalized by the lesbian community based on a feminine gender expression. Levitt, Gerrish, and

Hiestand (2003) and Levitt and Hiestand (2004) also found butch women to have higher rates of harassment by strangers and femme women had higher rates of sexual harassment. It may be that lesbian women may experience and perceive discrimination differently based on their gender expression which could be impacting the interaction between sexist discrimination and heterosexist discrimination predicting PTSD symptom severity. Future research should explore the role of gender expression in lesbian women’s experiences of sexism and heterosexism, and the development of PTSD symptoms.

Hypothesis 4

Hypothesis four predicted that heterosexist discrimination interacts with internalized sexism and that sexist discrimination interacts with internalized heterosexism predicting PTSD symptom severity. Specifically, it was predicted that when external oppression is more severe, higher levels of internalized oppression will more strongly predict higher PTSD symptom severity. Contrary to prediction, neither interaction was significant. This is inconsistent with Velez, Moradi, & DeBlaere’s (2015) finding that found significant interactions between racist discrimination and internalized heterosexism and Heterosexist discrimination and internalized racism predicting for self-esteem in sexual minority Latina/o individuals. It is also inconsistent with previous research

(Adams, Cahill, & Ackerlind, 2005; Bowleg, Huang, Brooks, Black, & Burkholder,

2003; Meyer & Ouellette, 2009; Della, Wilson & Miller, 2002) that suggests a that low

128

internalized oppression in one identity may be protective when experiencing discrimination based on another marginalized identity.

The present sample had relatively low internalized oppression (see Table 4) and though the internalized sexism scale correlated with both sexist discrimination and internalized heterosexism, the internalized heterosexism scale correlated only with internalized sexism not heterosexist discrimination. The correlational finding suggests that internalized oppression is not related to experiences of discrimination necessarily and can occur without an experience of discrimination. Overall, the findings suggest the need to continue to explore these constructs intersectionally rather than interactionally to better understand the psychological distress that occurs based on multiple oppressions (Cole,

2009; Collins 1991; Szymanski and Moffitt 2012). Contrary to previous studies, internalized sexism and psychological distress was not significant in any prediction which suggest that it may operate differently for lesbian women compared to heterosexual women and other sexual minority women. Researchers should be cautious about making inferences about lesbian women based on research on heterosexual women and other sexual minority identities.

Hypothesis 5 - 6

Hypothesis five and six predicted the relationship between sexist discrimination or heterosexist discrimination and PTSD symptom severity would be moderated by internalized sexism or internalized heterosexism and the strength of moderation would depend on the severity of IPV. Contrary to either prediction, internalized sexism, internalized heterosexism, or IPV had significant moderator effects. Though this is the first study to explore the moderating effects of internalized oppression and IPV on the

129

relationship between externalized oppression and PTSD symptom severity, previous research has found an impact of internalized oppression on experiences of IPV.

Girshick (2002) found that internalized heterosexism along with dependency, jealousy, a history of childhood abuse, and alcohol and drug use were all important factors in same-gender IPV. Szymanski et al. (2008) described two factors that contribute to levels of internalized heterosexism: the significance of individuals that communicate heterosexism in sexual minority individual’s lives (e.g., family, friends, clergy) and having a lack of access to resources or information that could counter negative messages about sexual minorities. Abusive partners may be able to use an individual’s own internalized oppression as a way to control them (Duke & Davidson; Erbaugh, 2007).

Additionally, reporting intimate partner violence in same gender relationships may also be low due to internalized heterosexism, stigma, fear, and a shortage of services for lesbian women (Renzett, 1996). Future studies should continue to explore internalized oppression in sexual minority women’s experiences with IPV and PTSD symptoms.

The present sample had low incidence of recent IPV experiences (see Table 4) and was also limited by the measure of IPV utilized. The SVAW does not include many items associated with emotional abuse but rather includes more items on physical and sexual violence. Future researchers should use measures that include physical, sexual, and emotional abuse. Additionally, newer research has introduced the concept of identity abuse which could be particularly salient in lesbian women, as abusive partners could use systems of oppression to abuse them (Ard & Makadon, 2011; West, 2012; Woulfe &

Goodman, 2018). Future research should also explore identity abuse as it relates to lesbian women and the development of PTSD symptoms.

130

Clinical Implications

The scientific and clinical study of mental health concerns and the impact of oppression and trauma among lesbian women is of great importance. Lesbian women have historically been denied power and privileges that have been given to heterosexual individuals. They are also continuously subjected to messages that they are inferior and deviant compared to heterosexual individuals (Brown, 1986). Contrary to popular opinion that The United States Supreme Court ruling that delivered marriage equality for same- gender couples in 2015 had increased acceptance of the LGBTQ community, recent reports actually show a decrease in acceptance with the LGBTQ community. The Gay and Lesbian Alliance Against organization’s (GLAAD) results of their

“Accelerating Acceptance” survey found that overall acceptance had decreased in all areas measured with 51% of non-LGBTQ adults reporting that they are “somewhat” or

“very” uncomfortable with LGBTQ people and there was an 11% increase in reported discrimination (55%) based on sexual orientation or gender identity from the previous year (2018). Recent political movements to legalize discrimination against sexual minority individuals based on religious beliefs may impact sexual minority individuals’ access to mental health services and healthcare in general. The current research is timely and has important implications for researchers, scholars, mental health providers, and advocates.

The findings from the present study challenge the current DSM definition of what constitutes a criterion A traumatic event. Consistent with Kira’s (2001) taxonomy of trauma as being complex, cumulative, and ongoing. Mental health professional should take into account the taxonomies offered by Kira (2001) as it provides a better

131

understanding of how traumatic stressors occur throughout the lifetime and should include the deleterious impact of heterosexism and sexism along with other forms of oppression on our client’s lives. Clinicians should assess client’s trauma history including experiences of sexist and heterosexist discrimination when assessing for PTSD symptoms.

Much like the research that exists on the traumatizing impact of racism and its influence on the development of PTSD symptoms, mental health professionals should also not fail to explore the impact of sexism and heterosexism in their client’s lives.

Though the current study found that sexist discrimination was significant throughout, it does not mean that heterosexist discrimination did not impact lesbian women in the present sample. Additionally, the failure to discuss lesbian women’s oppressed identities may be oppressive itself (Carter, 2007). Historically, LGB individuals have more frequently utilized mental health services (Bieschke, McClanahan, Tozer, Grzegorek, &

Park, 2000) and have been found to have more help seeking behaviors compared to heterosexual individuals (Morgan, 1992). However, even with the increase of multicultural courses for graduate mental health professionals, providers have reported having insufficient training in LGB-related issues and treatment. There is also a lack of continued training in therapists as it relates to the treatment of sexual minority individuals and LGB-issues (American Psychological Association (APA), 2000; Murphy, Rawlings,

& Howe, 2002; Ponterotto, 1996). This can have a negative impact on the treatment LGB clients are receiving. Additionally, sexual minorities have a history of being pathologized by the field of psychology and though homosexuality is no longer a psychological disorder, there are still treatment protocols that continue to use pathological and

132

oppressive methods (ie. ). The findings in this study suggest that sexism and heterosexism discrimination are perceived as traumatic. Mental health professionals should seek education on how to treat LGB individuals generally but also using affirming methods as to not additionally oppress lesbian women.

The justification in society to pathologize sexual minority individuals and in some states to legalize the discrimination of sexual minority individuals in mental health care is largely rooted in religion (Israel & Mohr, 2004). Homosexuality is still widely condemned in most around the world and is some places is punishable by death

(LeVay & Novas, 1995; Morrow & Tyson, 2006). Mental health professionals should be aware that they also are exposed to these messages like their sexual minority clients and may develop negative views that can impact their treatment of lesbian women. Mental health professionals should continue to explore their when working with all clients, including lesbian women.

The ethics codes, multicultural counseling competencies (2017), and guidelines for psychotherapy with lesbian, gay, and bisexual clients (2000) all state that competent practice should include an examination of overt and hidden biases for mental health professionals and additionally, that they understand the cultural norms and systemic factors that affect diverse clients. The negative messages about lesbian women not only impacts their experiences with discrimination and prejudice but also their development of internalized oppression. Mental health professionals even without intention can promote these messages and it may lead to heterosexist or sexist microaggressions which can impact the therapeutic relationship, as well as, negatively affect the client. Though the present study did not have significant amounts of internalized oppression, participants

133

still reported some internalized sexism and internalized heterosexism. Mental health professionals should be knowledgeable about internalized oppression so not to unintentionally promote negative messages for their sexual minority clients.

Additionally, the current research points to the need for culturally competent interventions for sexual minority individuals. However, Becares and Priest (2015) discussed that since social stratification continues based on gender and , any intervention created won’t be completely effective. Since much of the current research and practice on the treatment of trauma has largely focused on the medical model ignoring the core tenets of counseling psychology, which includes issues of diversity, social justice, and advocacy (Albee, 2000).

Implications for Counseling Psychology

The current research has clinical implications but is also important in understanding the unique experiences of sexism and heterosexism for lesbian women in society. Counseling psychology’s mission of recognizing the importance of multiculturalism and the impact that environmental and situational influences have on issues related to diversity and social justice (Munley, Duncan, McDonnell, & Sauer,

2004). It is important that research be grounded in multiculturalism and be committed to social justice, promoting social justice action, and advocacy (Vera & Speight, 2003).

The present study added to the literature on multiple oppressions in lesbian women who have historically rarely been researched and have an unequal access to resources (Roth, 1985). Additionally, the present study aimed to fill the gap in the literature exploring the impact of multiple oppression on lesbian women who experience

IPV and their subsequent PTSD symptoms. Though the present study did not find a

134

significant amount of IPV in the sample, it is still important for counseling psychologist to continue to do research to understand the prevalence and impact of IPV on women’s lives broadly and on sexual minority women specifically. This is consistent with counseling psychology’s tenet of social justice in that it will assist in understanding all women who experience trauma and the impact that oppression has on these experiences and the development of PTSD symptoms.

The results that lesbian women are experiencing significant amounts of sexism which was linked to higher amounts of PTSD symptom severity also informs potential advocacy efforts. Counseling psychologists are in a unique position to be able to utilize these results to inform their care of clients and future research directions. The results support that sexist external events that oppressed individuals experience lead to greater psychological distress, which challenges placing the source of distress within the individual as the medical typically does (Albee, 2000; Prilleltensky, 1989). Counseling psychologists can advocate for their clients, as well as, use these results to inform social justice action and advocacy efforts.

The American Psychological Association (APA) emphasizes the need for psychology to have a role in social justice work in order to advance the field and promote education and welfare (APA, 2008). The continued discrimination of LGBTQ individuals within our society has largely been accepted with continued religious based discrimination, as well as, newer legal efforts to make discrimination possible in the medical and mental health fields based on religious beliefs. Additionally, there are still psychological interventions that promote that being a sexual minority is pathological (ie.

Conversion Therapy). There also continues to be discrimination in employment, housing,

135

adoption, and there are no laws to protect against the continued of LGBTQ youth in our society. Counseling psychologists have many opportunities for advocacy in these areas as the results of this study show that lesbian women continue to experience sexism and heterosexism at high rates which leads to higher rates of posttraumatic symptoms. Advocating for the eradication of psychological interventions, such as:

Conversion Therapy is one opportunity for advocacy that would align with Counseling

Psychology’s core tenets of prevention, advocacy, and social justice.

Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions

There are a number of strengths to the current study, the first of which is that it represented a sample of lesbian women and also included women who might not have identified with the term “lesbian” but were still mostly attracted to the same gender.

However, representation continues to be a challenge when methodologically sampling sexual minority women since attraction is fluid and identity can evolve throughout an individual’s lifetime (Moradi, Mohr, Worthington, & Fassinger, 2009). The importance of utilizing person-centered language and varying identity descriptions when sampling sexual minority women should continue to be accounted for in future studies. It is also a strength of the study that the sample included an age range of 18 to 56 (M=28.46). The current study adds to the literature and show that experiences of sexist discrimination continue to affect sexual minority women’s psychological well-being. Limitations and future directions for research are discussed below.

There are several limitations that need to be considered with interpreting the results of the present study. The participants were overwhelmingly White and above average socioeconomic status, with 78% of the sample identifying as White and 46.9%

136

reporting that “my family has no problem buying the things we need and sometimes we can also buy special things.” Thus, it is difficult to generalize the results to lesbian women of color and who are at differing socioeconomic statuses. Future research should intentionally explore the impact of race, sexism, and heterosexism in lesbian women of color broadly, and additionally their experiences with IPV and subsequent PTSD symptom development. Additionally, the present study utilized total scores for multidimensional measures which has been critiqued in the literature (Hammer, Heath, &

Vogel, 2018; Heath, Brenner, Vogel, Lannin, & Strass, 2018; Iwamoto, Cheng, Lee,

Takamatsu, & Gordon, 2011; Levant, Hall, Weigold, & McCurdy, 2015; Wong, Ho,

Wang, & Miller, 2017). Hammer, Heath, and Vogel (2018) noted a limitation of utilizing total scores for measures that are multidimensional as the different constructs measured may not be related which would result in a non-valid total score. Reise, Bonifay, and

Haviland (1995) also noted that confirmatory analytic studies of psychological measures which result in item responses being multidimensional does not mean that a total score is inadequate. The majority of research on these measures have used a total score so the present study was consistent with the prior literature for all scales utilizing a total score.

Additionally, all measures were found to have good internal consistency in the present study. Future studies should take the critiques about utilizing total scores under consideration when understanding the validity of their measures (2018).

The present study was also cross sectional so we cannot draw causal conclusions based on the findings. Participants were also prompted to reflect on sexist, heterosexist, and IPV events that occurred in the past year and not when they occurred. Participants may have not accurately recalled all of their past year experiences which could impact the

137

results collected, as well as, their PTSD symptom severity. The sample was also largely collected from online LGBTQ forums where lesbian women may feel a sense of community, have greater degrees of outness, have more access to community resources, group-level coping resources, and be at later stages of sexual identity development which could explain the non-significant findings for internalized oppression, and could affect

PTSD development. The sample may not be representative of women who are or who are not connected to LGBTQ community and outness and connection to the LGBTQ community may provide a protective buffering effect against internal and externalized oppression. Participants also self-selected to the current study, who could have intense beliefs, opinions, and experiences about sexism and heterosexism. Future studies should explore internalized oppression and PTSD symptoms in lesbian women at all stages of sexual identity development, as well as, sampling from other places to better be able to generalize to all lesbian women.

The present study also had low rates of IPV which is not consistent with previous research on the increased rates of IPV in the LGBT women community. One potential explanation is the choice of IPV measure used. The SVAW measures recent threats and incidents of physical and sexual violence but does not include items related to emotional abuse, because of this IPV may be underreported in the current sample. Future research should use more comprehensive measures of abuse, including: physical, sexual, emotional, financial, and identity abuse to better understand the prevalence and impact of

IPV on lesbian women.

Another potential explanation for the non-significant results is the use of the additive and interactionist models of multiple oppressions. The use of intersectional

138

multiple oppression perspective (Bowleg, 2013; Bowleg, Teti, Malebranche, & Tschann,

2013; Cole 2009; Collins 1991; Grzanka, Santos, & Moradi, 2017; Levine & Breshears,

2019; Moradi & Grzanka, 2017; Sarno, Mohr, Jackson, & Fassinger, 2015; Rosenthal &

Lobel, 2011; 2016; Settles, 2006; Shin, et al, 2017; Szymanski and Moffitt 2012; Thomas et al., 2011) should be utilized in future studies. Presently, intersectionality theory has largely been empirically supported in the experiences of external and internalized gendered racism (King 2003; Klevens 2008; Thomas et al. 2004; 2008; Woods et al.

2008). There is a lack of quality measures and empirical research to measure these unique experiences in lesbian women. Future research should develop and measure external and internal gendered heterosexism in sexual minority women broadly, and lesbian women specifically (Mohr, Jackson, & Fassinger, 2015; Szymanski, 2014). Currently, there is only one known gendered heterosexism scale (Friedman & Leaper, 2010) and when initially tested, did explain sexual orientation identity above sexism and heterosexist experiences. The measure was used in a follow-up study (Friedman & Ayres, 2013) exploring gendered heterosexism as a predictor for feminist activism in college women.

However, there have been no other known studies to utilize the measure. Future research could explore intersectionality with this scale and mental health outcomes.

Parent, DeBlaere, & Moradi (2013) offered suggested intersectional approaches to research on gender, LGBT, and racial and ethnic identities. Furthering the contributions in the literature (Cole 2009; Purdie-Vaughns & Eibach 2008; Shields 2008) that suggest that it is important to explore intersectionality in psychological research, the authors posit that without exploring oppressed experiences of multiple social identities as a fused intersection, we can’t fully understand the experiences of gender, sexual orientation, or

139

race/ethnicity. Suggestions also include exploring gender and sexual orientation as a continuous variable rather than categorical, qualitative research, and using a within-group approach rather than between group comparisons. These suggestions should be taken into consideration when conducting research on sexual minority women in the future.

Additionally, there may be another variable impacting the relationship between sexism, heterosexism, and internalized oppression predicting for PTSD symptoms in lesbian women. As discussed above, gender expression is one such explanation for differing experiences with sexism and heterosexism in lesbian women. It may also be part of intersectionality whereas lesbian women are experiencing sexist and heterosexist discrimination as inherently linked based on how they express their gender. Traditionally feminine expressing women may experience more perceived sexist discrimination rather than heterosexist discrimination. Conversely, butch or androgynous expressing women may experience more perceived heterosexist discrimination rather than sexist. However, this does not mean that all gender expressing lesbian women aren’t experiencing both sexist and heterosexist experiences, it suggests that their experiences of discrimination are intersectional in nature because they are both interlocked and inherently gendered in nature. The potential of gender expression being a potential construct affecting women’s experiences highlights the need to explore these constructs intersectionally. Bowleg

(2012) discusses the challenges inherent in measuring intersectional theory though argued that it is necessary as everyone has intersecting identities that may include macro-level and/or micro-level identities, and that one identity alone is not adequate in explaining discrimination without the other intersecting identities. Future studies should explore

140

other constructs that could be impacting lesbian women’s experience with oppression, including gender expression and explore these constructs intersectionally.

The present study also utilized self-report measures. Various data collection methods such as interviews should be used in future research so not to rely solely on one type of data collection, as well as, to decrease socially desirable responding in participants. Interviews may also include more information about experiences of internalized and externalized oppression for lesbian women that goes beyond a self-report measure. Qualitative research may also be beneficial to better understand these constructs and how they impact lesbian women’s mental health outcomes. The Clinician-

Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5; Weathers et. al., 2018) is the gold standard structured interview for the assessment and diagnosis of PTSD. Future research may want to consider utilizing the CAPS-5 when studying PTSD-related to oppressive experiences.

The present study was also limited in the measures available to measure experiences of heterosexist oppression and internalized heterosexism for lesbian women.

Research has been extant in measuring the experiences of sexism but not heterosexism. A limitation of the HHRDS is that it measures overt experiences of heterosexist discrimination, however lesbian women experience subtler forms of heterosexist experiences daily. It may not fully be capturing the daily experiences of heterosexist that range from overt, covert, and subtle. Additionally, the LIHS measures experiences of internalized oppression for sexual minority women in five domains: (a) connection with the lesbian community, (b) public identification as a lesbian, (c) personal feelings about being a lesbian, (d) moral and religious attitudes toward lesbianism, and (e) attitudes

141

toward other lesbians. Though capturing many different aspects of the different domains of experiences of internalized oppression for lesbian women, it may not fully capture all factors that lead to the internalization of oppression (ie. socialization). Both measures have primarily been utilized in research studies that comprise of mainly white middle- class samples of lesbian women and may not reflect the experiences of all lesbian women. These measures also don’t take into account the intersectional experience of lesbian women (gendered heterosexism), future research should be done to create more comprehensive measures of experiences and internalized experiences for lesbian women.

Moreover, measures utilizing intersectional theory may better explain the experiences of multiple oppression for lesbian women.

Additionally, the present findings show the need to replicate the current study in other groups, as well as, to expand the way trauma is conceptualized. The present study expanded the definition of trauma that may not necessarily meet criteria for PTSD’s

Criterion A in that experiences of sexism for lesbian women that don’t include being exposed to threatened death, death, actual or threatened serious injury, or actual or threatened sexual violence predicted for PTSD symptoms (APA, 2013). The present study included sexist events of discrimination and prejudice that would not meet this narrow definition of trauma and there has been previous research that have demonstrated that non-Criterion A experiences of oppression are positively associated with PTSD and have predicted for PTSD symptoms greater than Criterion A traumas (Berg, 2006; Loo et al., 2001; Szymanski & Balsam, 2011). Additionally, unmeasured variables such as lifetime stressful experiences and past traumas, including previous IPV that was not in the past year that are not necessarily related to sexism and heterosexism may impact the

142

relationships of variables in the present study. Future research should continue to assess the traumatic nature of oppression broadly and subtle everyday experiences of discrimination and prejudice specifically and the development of PTSD.

Future research should also continue to explore other factors associated with lesbian women’s experiences of oppression, internalized oppression, and psychological distress. Factors, such as socioeconomic status, resiliency, outness, coping strategies, help-seeking behaviors, and access to mental health treatment should be explored specifically for lesbian women. Historically, research has focused on these factors in sexual minority individuals as a whole, but research has also found differences between gay men and lesbian women (Garnets, Hancock, Cochran, Goodchilds, & Peplau, 1991;

Liddle, 1996). Future research should explore these constructs specifically for lesbian women. Less is known about the experience of these factors and how they relate to PTSD broadly, and IPV-related PTSD specifically. Future research should continue to explore all of these constructs for lesbian women who experience IPV.

Overall, future research should continue to explore the experience of multiple oppressions in lesbian women, though the present study suggests that utilizing an intersectional approach may be more beneficial to understanding the unique experiences of lesbian women when exploring mental health outcomes. Future research should also explore sexist discrimination and heterosexist discrimination that may be subtle rather than overt, such as; gender-based microaggressions. Lesbian women who may be less out and less connected to the sexual minority community which have been found to be protective factors for psychological distress, should also be conducted. Other minority identities should also be a focus in future research to better understand the experience of

143

all lesbian women beyond the White and well-educated women in the present study.

Future research should continue to explore internalized oppression and PTSD symptoms, though the present study did not find a relationship, there is much research to show its importance in mental health outcomes. Additionally, the present sample had relatively low rates of IPV and this should be continued to be explored in the experiences of oppression and PTSD symptom development. Future research should utilize IPV scales for all types of abuse rather than just physical, sexual, and threats of violence, including the newer researched construct of identity abuse. Future research should also continue to explore the links between oppression and PTSD symptoms. As discussed above, the construct of gender expression should be explored in future research as it may affect lesbian women’s experiences of discrimination and internalized oppression.

Ultimately, research should continue to explore the influences of sexism, heterosexism, and IPV in lesbian women and the effects of multiple oppression in all marginalized individuals. As a field, we should continue to strive for multiculturalism, social justice, and advocacy for all those marginalized, including sexual minority individuals. With acceptance of LGBTQ individuals dropping due to current conservative political efforts, as well as, the continued of the LGBTQ community, it is necessary for research to continue addressing the unique issues of sexual and gender minorities to be able to address the negative impact of oppression for the

LGBTQ community in the future.

Conclusions

The current study adds to the current literature with its findings that sexism discrimination uniquely predicted for PTSD above and beyond heterosexist

144

discrimination, internalized sexism, and internalized heterosexism. The results of the study demonstrate the need for continued research on the impact of multiple oppressions on lesbian women and the development of PTSD symptoms. The results also indicate the need to assess for oppressive experiences when mental health professionals are exploring clients stressful and traumatic experiences. Future research should replicate these results, as well as, develop measures and utilize intersectional theory to better understand all social identities that lesbian women may have and their impact on mental health outcomes. Future research should also explore other variables, such as gender expression that may be affecting lesbian women’s experiences of sexist and heterosexist discrimination and internalized oppression and subsequent PTSD symptom development.

Lastly, IPV should continue to be explored in lesbian women and the impact of oppression on their experiences with interpersonal trauma and PTSD symptoms.

145

REFERENCES

Adams, P. L. (1990). Prejudice and exclusion as social traumata. Oxford, England: John

Wiley & Sons.

Adams, E. M., Cahill, B. J., & Ackerlind, S. J. (2005). A qualitative study of Latino

lesbian and gay youths’ experiences with discrimination and the career

development process. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66(2), 199-218.

Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R. L., & Syme,

S. L. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health. The challenge of the gradient.

American Psychologist, 49, 15–24. http://dx .doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.49.1.15

Adler, N. E., & Snibbe, A. C. (2003). The role of psychosocial processes in explaining

the gradient between socioeconomic status and health. Current Directions in

Psychological Science, 12, 119 –123. http://dx.doi .org/10.1111/1467-8721.01245

Adler, N. E., & Stewart, J. (2010). Preface to the biology of disadvantage:

Socioeconomic status and health. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,

1186, 1– 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009 .05385.x

Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and

interpreting interactions. Sage.

Albelda, R., Badgett, M. V. L., Schneebaum, A., & Gates, G. (2009). Poverty in the

lesbian, gay, and . Los Angeles, CA: The Williams Institute.

Retrieved from http://www.law.ucla.edu/

146

williamsinstitute/pdf/LGBPovertyReport.pdf

Albee, G. W. (2000). The Boulder model's fatal flaw. American Psychologist, 55(2), 247.

Alessi, E. J., Meyer, I. H., & Martin, J. I. (2013). PTSD and sexual orientation: An

examination of criterion A1 and non-criterion A1 events. Psychological Trauma:

Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 5(2), 149.

Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Amadio, D. M., & Chung, Y. B. (2004). Internalized homophobia and substance use

among lesbian, gay, and bisexual persons. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social

Services, 17(1), 83-101.

American Psychological Association. (1992). Guidelines for providers of psychological

services to ethnic, linguistic, and culturally diverse populations. Washington, DC:

Author.

American Psychological Association. (2000). Guidelines for psychotherapy with lesbian,

gay, and bisexual clients. American Psychologist, 55, 1440–1451.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Anderson, E. (2013). Streetwise: Race, class, and change in an urban community.

University of Chicago Press.

Ard, K. L., & Makadon, H. J. (2011). Addressing intimate partner violence in lesbian,

gay, bisexual, and transgender patients. Journal of general internal

medicine, 26(8), 930-933.

Armstrong, R. A. (2014). When to use the Bonferroni correction. Ophthalmic and

Physiological Optics, 34(5), 502-508.

147

Balsam K. F. (2003). Traumatic victimization in the lives of lesbian and bisexual women:

A contextual approach. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 7, 1-14.

Balsam, K. F., Rothblum, E. D., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2005). Victimization over the life

span: a comparison of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and heterosexual siblings. Journal of

consulting and clinical psychology, 73(3), 477.

Balsam, K. F., & Szymanski, D. M. (2005). Relationship quality and

in women's same‐sex relationships: the role of minority stress. Psychology of

Women Quarterly, 29(3), 258-269. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00220.x

Bandermann, K. M., & Szymanski, D. M. (2014). Exploring coping mediators between

heterosexist oppression and posttraumatic stress symptoms among lesbian, gay,

and bisexual persons. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender

Diversity, 1(3), 213.

Banks, K. H. (2014). “Perceived” discrimination as an example of color-blind racial

’s influence on psychology. American Psychologist, 69(3), 311-313.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035734

Bargad, A., & Hyde, J. S. (1991). Women's studies. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 15(2), 181-201.

Bartky, S. L. (1990). and domination: Studies in the phenomenology of

oppression. Psychology Press.

Bauer, G. R. (2014). Incorporating intersectionality theory into population health research

methodology: Challenges and the potential to advance health equity. Social

Science & Medicine, 110, 10 –17. http://dx.doi.org/

10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.03.022

148

Beal, F. M. (1970). Double jeopardy: To be African American and female. In T. Cade

(Ed.), The African American woman: An anthology (pp. 90–100). New York:

Signet.

Bécares, L., & Priest, N. (2015). Understanding the influence of race/ethnicity, gender,

and class on inequalities in academic and non-academic outcomes among eighth-

grade students: Findings from an intersectionality approach. PloS one, 10(10),

e0141363.

Becker, J. C., & Swim, J. K. (2011). Seeing the unseen: Attention to daily encounters

with sexism as way to reduce sexist beliefs. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 35(2), 227-242.

Bell, L. A. (1997). Theoretical foundations for social justice education. In M.Adams,L.

A. Bell, & P. Griffin (Eds.), Teaching for diversity and social justice: A

sourcebook (pp.3-15). New York: Routledge. Brown.

Bell, L. A. (2007). Theoretical foundations of social justice education. In M. Adams, L.

A. Bell, & P. Griffin (Eds.), Teaching for diversity and social justice (2nd ed., pp.

1-14). New York, NY: Routledge.

Bennett, J., & O’Connor, R. (2002). Self-harm among lesbians. Health Psychology

Update, 11(4), 32-38

Benokraitis, N. V., & Feagin, J. R. (1995). Modern sexism: Blatant, subtle, and covert

discrimination. Pearson College Div.

Berrill, K. T., & Violence, A. G. (1992). Victimization in the United States: An

Overview. Hate Crimes: Confronting Violence against Lesbians and Gay

Men, 19, 19-25.

149

Berg, S. H. (2002). Everyday sexism and post-traumatic stress disorder in women: A

correlational study.

Berg, S. H. (2006). Everyday Sexism and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in Women A

Correlational Study. Violence Against Women, 12(10), 970-988. doi:

10.1177/1077801206293082

Betancourt, H., & López, S. R. (1993). The study of culture, ethnicity, and race in

American psychology. American Psychologist, 48(6), 629.

Bieschke, K. J., McClanahan, M., Tozer, E., Grzegorek, J. L., & Park, J. (2000).

Programmatic research on the treatment of lesbian, gay, and bisexual clients: The

past, the present, and the course for the future.

Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping responses and social resources

in attenuating the stress of life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 139 –

157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ BF00844267

Binning, K. R., Unzueta, M. M., Huo, Y. J., & Molina, L. E. (2009). The interpretation of

multiracial status and its relation to social engagement and psychological well‐

being. Journal of Social Issues, 65(1), 35-49.

Black, M. C., Basile, K. C., Breiding, M. J., Smith, S .G., Walters, M. L., Merrick, M. T.,

… Stevens, M. R. (2011). The National Intimate Partner and Sexual Violence

Survey: 2010 summary report. Retrieved from the Centers for Disease Control

and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control:

http://www.cdc.gov/ViolencePrevention/pdf/NISVS_Report2010-a.pdf

150

Blake, D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Charney, D. S., & Keane, T.

M. (1997). Clinician administered PTSD scale (revised). Boston: Behavioral

Science Division, Boston National Center for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder.

Blevins, C. A., Weathers, F. W., Davis, M. T., Witte, T. K., & Domino, J. L. (2015). The

posttraumatic stress disorder checklist for DSM‐5 (PCL‐5): Development and

initial psychometric evaluation. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 28(6), 489-498.

Bloom, L. R. (1997). Locked in uneasy sisterhood: Reflections on feminist methodology

and research relations. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 28(1), 111-122.

doi:10.1525/aeq.1997.28.1.111

Bowleg, L. (2013). “Once you’ve blended the cake, you can’t take the parts back to the

main ingredients”: Black gay and bisexual men’s descriptions and experiences of

intersectionality. Sex Roles, 68, 754 –767. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-

0152-4

Bowleg, L. (2008). When Black woman lesbian Black lesbian woman: The

methodological challenges of qualitative and quantitative intersectionality

research. Sex Roles, 59, 312–325. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1007/s11199-008-9400-z

Bowleg, L., Huang, J., Brooks, K., Black, A., & Burkholder, G. (2003). Triple jeopardy

and beyond: Multiple minority stress and resilience among Black

lesbians. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 7(4), 87-108.

Bowleg, L., Teti, M., Malebranche, D. J., & Tschann, J. M. (2013). “It’s an uphill battle

everyday”: Intersectionality, low-income Black heterosexual men, and

implications for HIV prevention research and interventions. Psychology of Men

& Masculinity, 14, 25–34. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/a0028392

151

Bradford, J., Ryan, C., & Rothblum, E. D. (1994). National Lesbian Health Care Survey:

implications for mental health care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

psychology, 62(2), 228.

Brady, S. S., & Matthews, K. A. (2002). The influence of socioeconomic status and

ethnicity on adolescents' exposure to stressful life events. Journal of Pediatric

Psychology, 27(7), 575-583.

Breines, J. G., Crocker, J., & Garcia, J. A. (2008). Self-objectification and well-being in

women’s daily lives. Personality & Social Psychological Bulletin, 34(5), 583-

598.

Brewster, M. E., Moradi, B., DeBlaere, C., & Velez, B. L. (2013). Navigating the

borderlands: The roles of minority stressors, bicultural self-efficacy, and cognitive

flexibility in the mental health of bisexual individuals. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 60, 543-556. doi:10.1037/a0033224

Bridges, F.S., Tatum, K. M., & Kunselman, J.C. (2008). Domestic violence statutes and

rates of intimate partner and family homicide: A research note. Criminal Justice

Policy Review, 19(1), 117-130.

Brooks, V. R. (1981). Minority stress and lesbian women. Free Press.

Brown, L. (1986). Confronting internalized oppression in sex therapy with lesbians.

Journal of Homosexuality, 12(3/4), 99-107.

Brown, L.S. (1994). Subversive dialogues. New York: Basic Books.

Browne, A. (1993). Violence against women by male partners: Prevalence, outcomes,

and policy implications. American psychologist, 48(10),1077.

152

Browne, K. (2005). Snowball sampling: using social networks to research non‐

heterosexual women. International journal of social research methodology, 8(1),

47-60.

Bryant-Davis, T., & Ocampo, C. (2005). The trauma of racism: Implications for

counseling, research, and education. Counseling Psychologist, 33(4), 574. doi:

10.1177/0011000005276581

Buchanan, N. T. (2005). The nexus of race and gender domination: The racialized sexual

harassment of African American women. In the company of men: Re-discovering

the links between sexual harassment and male domination, 294-320.

Buchanan, N. T., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2008). Effects of racial and sexual harassment on

work and the psychological well-being of African American women. Journal of

occupational health psychology, 13(2), 137.

Bulhan, H. A. (1985). Black Americans and psychopathology: An overview of research

and theory. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 22(2S), 370.

Burch, B. (1987). Barriers to intimacy: Conflicts over power, dependency, and nurturing

in lesbian relationships. Lesbian psychologies: Explorations and challenges, 126-

141.

Burke, Leslie K., & Follingstad, Diane R. (1999). Violence in lesbian and gay

relationships: theory, prevalence, and correlational factors. Clinical Psychology

Review, 19 (5), 487-512.

Campbell, J. C. (2002). Health consequences of intimate partner violence. Lancet, 359,

1331- 1336. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(02)08336-8

153

Campbell, J., Jones, A. S., Dienemann, J., Kub, J., Schollenberger, J., O'Campo, P., ... &

Wynne, C. (2002). Intimate partner violence and physical health consequences.

Archives of internal medicine, 162(10), 1157-1163.

doi:10.1001/archinte.162.10.1157

Cavanaugh, C. E., Hansen, N. B., & Sullivan, T. P. (2010). HIV sexual risk behavior

among low-income women experiencing intimate partner violence: The role of

posttraumatic stress disorder. AIDS and Behavior, 14(2), 318-327.

Capodilupo, C. M., Nadal, K. L., Corman, L., Hamit, S., Lyons, O. B., & Weinberg, A.

(2010). The manifestation of gender microaggressions. Microaggressions and

marginality: Manifestation, dynamics, and impact, 193-216.

Carden, A. D. (1994). Wife abuse and the wife abuser: Review and

recommendations. The Counseling Psychologist, 22(4), 539-582.

Carlson, E. B. (1997). Traumatic assessments: Clinician’s Guide. New York: Guilford.

Carter, R. T. (2007). Racism and psychological and emotional injury: Recognizing and

assessing race-based traumatic stress. The Counseling Psychologist, 35, 13-105.

doi:10.1177/0011000006292033

Carter, R. T., Forsyth, J. M., Mazzula, S. L., & Williams, B. (2005). Racial

discrimination and race-based traumatic stress: An exploratory

investigation. Handbook of racial-cultural psychology and counseling: Training

and practice, 2, 447-476.

Carvalho, A. F., Lewis, R. J., Derlega, V. J., Winstead, B. A., & Viggiano, C. (2011).

Internalized sexual minority stressors and same-sex intimate partner

violence. Journal of Family Violence, 26(7), 501-509.

154

Cass, V. C. (1979). Homosexuality identity formation: A theoretical model. Journal of

homosexuality, 4(3), 219-235.

Catalano, S. (2013). Intimate Partner Violence: Attributes of Victimization, 1993- 2011.

(Report No. NCJ 243300). Retrieved from Bureau of Justice Statistics:

http://www.bjs.gov/content/pub/pdf/ipvav9311.pdf

Cheng, H. L., & Mallinckrodt, B. (2015). Racial/ethnic discrimination, posttraumatic

stress symptoms, and alcohol problems in a longitudinal study of Hispanic/Latino

college students. Journal of counseling psychology, 62(1), 38.

Cochran S. D. (2001). Emerging issues in research on lesbians’ and gay men’s mental

health: Does sexual orientation really matter? American Psychologist, 56, 931-

947.

Cole, E. R. (2009). Intersectionality and research in psychology. American

psychologist, 64(3), 170.

Coleman, E. (1982). Developmental stages of the process. Journal of

homosexuality, 7(2-3), 31-43.

Collins, P. H. (2000). Gender, , and black political economy. The Annals

of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 568(1), 41-53.

Crenshaw, K. (1989). Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: A black feminist

critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory and antiracist politics. U.

Chi. Legal F., 139.

Corliss, H. L., Cochran, S. D., & Mays, V. M. (2002). Reports of parental maltreatment

during childhood in a United States population-based survey of homosexual,

bisexual, and heterosexual adults. Child abuse & neglect, 26(11), 1165-1178.

155

Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). and self-esteem: The self-protective

properties of stigma. Psychological review, 96(4), 608.

Curran, P. J., West, S. G., & Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to

nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor

analysis. Psychological methods, 1(1), 16.

Da Luz, C. M. (1994). A legal and social comparison of heterosexual and same-sex

domestic violence: Similar inadequacies in legal recognition and response. S. Cal.

Rev. L. & Women's Stud., 4, 251.

Daley, A., Solomon, S., Newman, P. A., & Mishna, F. (2007). Traversing the margins:

Intersectionalities in the bullying of lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender

youth. Journal of gay & lesbian social services, 19(3-4), 9-29.

Dardis, C. M., Amoroso, T., & Iverson, K. M. (2017). Intimate partner stalking:

Contributions to PTSD symptomatology among a national sample of women

veterans. Psychological trauma: theory, research, practice, and policy, 9(S1), 67.

Dardis, C. M., Dichter, M. E., & Iverson, K. M. (2018). , PTSD and

revictimization among women who have experienced intimate partner

violence. Psychiatry research, 266, 103-110.

David, E. J. R., & Derthick, A. O. (2014). What is internalized oppression, and so what?

(pg. 1-30). In E. J. R. David (Ed.). Internalized Oppression: The Psychology of

Marginalized Groups. New York: Springer.

DeBlaere, C., & Bertsch, K. N. (2013). Perceived sexist events and psychological distress

of sexual minority women of color: The moderating role of

. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(2), 167-178.

156

Della, B., Wilson, M., & Miller, R. L. (2002). Strategies for managing heterosexism used

among African American gay and bisexual men. Journal of Black

Psychology, 28(4), 371-391.

Denton, F. N., Rostosky, S. S., & Danner, F. (2014). Stigma-related stressors, coping

self-efficacy, and physical health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual

individuals. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61(3), 383.

Diaz, R. M., Ayala, G., Bein, E., Henne, J., & Marin, B. V. (2001). The impact of

homophobia, poverty, and racism on the mental health of gay and bisexual Latino

men: findings from 3 US cities. American journal of public health, 91(6), 927.

D'Augelli, A. R., & Grossman, A. H. (2001). Disclosure of sexual orientation,

victimization, and mental health among lesbian, gay, and bisexual older

adults. Journal of interpersonal violence, 16(10), 1008-1027.

D’Augelli, A. R., Grossman, A. H., & Starks, M. T. (2006). Childhood gender

atypicality, victimization, and PTSD among lesbian, gay, and bisexual

youth. Journal of interpersonal violence, 21(11), 1462-1482

DeBlaere, C., & Bertsch, K. N. (2013). Perceived sexist events and psychological distress

of sexual minority women of color: The moderating role of

womanism. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(2), 167-178.

DiPlacido, J. (1998). Minority stress among lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals: A

consequence of heterosexism, homophobia, and stigmatization. Sage Publications,

Inc.

157

Dohrenwend, B. S., Askenasy, A. R., Krasnoff, L., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (1978).

Exemplification of a method for scaling life events: The PERI Life Events

Scale. Journal of health and social behavior, 205-229.

Dovidio, J.F., & Gaertner, S.L. (1998). On the nature of contemporary prejudice: The

causes, consequences, and challenges of . In J. Eberhardt & S.T.

Fiske (Eds.), Confronting racism: The problem and the response (pp. 3–32).

Newbury Park, CA: Sage

Downing, N. E., & Roush, K. L. (1985). From passive acceptance to active commitment:

A model of feminist identity development for women. The Counseling

Psychologist, 13(4), 695-709.

Duke, A., & Davidson, M. M. (2009). Same-sex intimate partner violence: Lesbian, gay,

and bisexual affirmative outreach and advocacy. Journal of Aggression,

Maltreatment & Trauma, 18(8), 795-816.

Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide: A study in sociology (JA Spaulding & G. Simpson,

trans.). Glencoe, IL: Free Press.(Original work published 1897).

Enns, C. Z. (1997). Feminist theories and feminist psychotherapies: Origins, themes, and

variations. Harrington Park Press/The Haworth Press.

Essed, P. (1991). Understanding everyday racism: An interdisciplinary theory(Vol. 2).

Sage.

Fanon, F. (1963). The wretched ofthe earth. New York: Grove.

Fanon, F. (1967). Black skin, white masks. New York, NY: Grove Press.

158

Farley, N. (1996). Same sex domestic violence. In E. Dworkin &F. J. Gutierrez (Eds.),

Gay men and lesbians: Journey to the end of the rainbow (pp. 231-242).

Alexandria, VA: American Association for Counseling Development.

Feinstein, B. A., Goldfried, M. R., & Davila, J. (2012). The relationship between

experiences of discrimination and mental health among lesbians and gay men: An

examination of internalized homonegativity and rejection sensitivity as potential

mechanisms. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 80(5), 917.

Feldblum, C. R. (2006). Moral conflict and liberty: Gay rights and religion. Brook. L.

Rev., 72, 61.

Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers.

Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40, 532-538.

doi:10.1037/a0015808

Fernald, J. L. (1995). Interpersonal heterosexism. In B. Lott & D. Maluso (Eds.), The

of interpersonal discrimination (pp. 80–117). New York:

Guilford.

Figes, E. (1970). Patriarchial Attitudes. Greenwich, Conn.

Finkelhor, D., Hotaling, G., Lewis, I. A., & Smith, C. (1990). Sexual abuse in a national

survey of adult men and women: Prevalence, characteristics and risk factors.

Child Abuse & Neglect 14, 19-28. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(90)90077-7

Fischer, A. R., Tokar, D. M., Mergl, M. M., Good, G. E., Hill, M. S., & Blum, S. A.

(2000). Assessing women’s feminist identity development: Studies of convergent,

discriminant, and structural validity. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24(1), 15-

29.

159

Fitzgerald, L. F. (1993). Sexual harassment: Violence against women in the

workplace. American Psychologist, 48(10), 1070.

Fleury, R. E., Sullivan, C. M., & Bybee, D. I. (2000). When Ending the Relationship

Does Not End the Violence Women's Experiences of Violence by Former

Partners. Violence against women, 6(12), 1363-1383.

doi:10.1177/10778010022183695

Forman, T. A., Williams, D. R., & Jackson, J. S. (1997). Race, place, and discrimination.

Perspectives on Social Problems, 9, 231–261.

Franklin, A. J., Boyd-Franklin, N., & Kelly, S. (2006). Racism and invisibility: Race-

related stress, emotional abuse and psychological trauma for people of color.

Journal of Emotional Abuse, 6(2-3), 9-30.

Fray-Witzer, E. (1999). Twice abused: Same-sex domestic violence and the law. Same-

sex domestic violence, 19-41.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward

understanding women's lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of

women quarterly, 21(2), 173-206.

Friedman, C. K., & Ayres, M. (2013). Predictors of feminist activism among sexual-

minority and heterosexual college women. Journal of Homosexuality, 60(12),

1726-1744.

Friedman, C. K., Ayres, M. M., & Leaper, C. (2008). Gendered heterosexism and

women’s experiences with discrimination. Poster session presented at the biennial

Gender Development Research Conference, San Francisco

Friedman, C., & Leaper, C. (2010). Sexual-minority college women's experiences with

160

discrimination: Relations with identity and collective action. Psychology of

Women Quarterly, 34(2), 152-164.

Freire, P. (1970). Pedagogy of the oppressed. New York, NY: Continuum.

Gallo, L. C., & Matthews, K. A. (2003). Understanding the association between

socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions play a role?

Psychological Bulletin, 129, 10 –51. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0033-

2909.129.1.10

Gamarel, K. E., Reisner, S. L., Parsons, J. T., & Golub, S. A. (2012). Association

between socioeconomic position discrimination and psychological distress:

Findings from a community-based sample of gay and bisexual men in New York

City. American Journal of Public Health, 102, 2094 –2101.

http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2012.300668

García Linares, M. I., Martínez, M., Celda Navarro, N., Picó Alfonso, M. A., Blasco Ros,

C., & Echeburúa, E. (2006). The impact of physical, psychological, and sexual

intimate partner violence on women's mental health: depressive symptoms,

posttraumatic stress disorder, state anxiety, and suicide. Psicologia, 61.

Gary, L. E. (1995). African American men’s perceptions of racial discrimination: A

sociocultural analysis. Social Work Research, 19, 207–217.

Garnets, L., Hancock, K. A., Cochran, S. D., Goodchilds, J., & Peplau, L. A. (1991).

Issues in psychotherapy with lesbians and gay men: A survey of

psychologists. American psychologist, 46(9), 964.

161

Garnets, L., Herek, G. M., & Levy, B. (1990). Violence and victimization of lesbians and

gay men: Mental health consequences. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 5(3),

366-383.

Gilbert, L. A. (1992). Gender and counseling psychology: Current knowledge and

directions for research and social action.

Gilfus, M. E. (1999). The price of the ticket: A survivor-centered appraisal of trauma

theory. Violence Against Women, 5(11), 1238-1257.

Girshick, L. B. (2002). No sugar, no spice: Reflections on research on woman-to-woman

sexual violence. Violence Against Women, 8(12), 1500-1520.

GLAAD. (2018). Accelerating Acceptance 2018: A Harris Poll survey of Americans’

acceptance of LGBTQ people. https://www.glaad.org/publications/accelerating-

acceptance-2018

Glick, P., & Fiske, S.T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating

hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

70(3), 491-512. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.491

Gold, S. D., Marx, B. P., Soler-Baillo, J. M., & Sloan, D. M. (2005). Is life stress more

traumatic than traumatic stress?. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(6), 687-698.

Gray, M. J., Litz, B. T., Hsu, J. L., & Lombardo, T. W. (2004). Psychometric properties

of the life events checklist. Assessment, 11(4), 330-341.

Grzanka, P. R., & Frantell, K. A. (2017). Counseling psychology and reproductive

justice: A call to action. Counseling Psychologist, 45, 326 –352.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0011000017699871

Grzanka, P. R., & Miles, J. R. (2016). The problem with the phrase “intersecting

162

identities”: LGBT affirmative therapy, intersectionality, and neoliberalism.

Sexuality Research & Social Policy, 13, 371–389.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13178-016-0240-2

Grzanka, P. R., Santos, C. E., & Moradi, B. (2017). Intersectionality research in

counseling psychology. Journal of counseling psychology, 64(5), 453.

Hammer, J. H., Heath, P. J., & Vogel, D. L. (2018). Fate of the total score:

Dimensionality of the Conformity to Masculine Norms Inventory-46 (CMNI-

46). Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 19(4), 645.

Hancock, A.-M. (2007). When multiplication doesn’t equal quick addition: Examining

intersectionality as a research paradigm. Perspectives on Politics, 5, 63–79.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1537592707070065

Hardiman, R., Jackson, B., & Griffin, P. (2007). Conceptual foundations for social justice

education.

Harlow, C. W. (2005). Hate crime reported by victims and police. Washington, DC: US

Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Bureau of Justice Statistics.

Harro, B. (2010). The cycle of socialization. In M. Adams, W. J. Blumenfeld, C.

Castañeda, H. W. Hackman, M. L. Peters, & X. Zúñiga (Eds.), Readings for

diversity and social justice (2nd ed., pp. 45-51). New York, NY: Routledge.

Hatzenbuehler, M. L. (2009). How does sexual minority stigma “get under the skin”? A

psychological mediation framework. Psychological bulletin, 135(5), 707.

Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable

mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling.

163

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process

analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Press.

Heath, P. J., Brenner, R. E., Vogel, D. L., Lannin, D. G., & Strass, H. A. (2017).

Masculinity and barriers to seeking counseling: The buffering role of self-

compassion. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64, 94 –103.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cou0000185

Hegarty KL. Measuring a multidimensional definition of domestic violence: Prevalence

of partner abuse in women attending general practice [PhD thesis]. Brisbane:

Department of Social and Preventive Medicine., University of Queensland; 1999.

Hegarty, K., Bush, R., & Sheehan, M. (2005). The composite abuse scale: further

development and assessment of reliability and validity of a multidimensional

partner abuse measure in clinical settings. Violence and victims, 20(5), 529.

Hegarty, K., Sheehan, M., & Schonfeld, C. (1999). A multidimensional definition of

partner abuse: development and preliminary validation of the Composite Abuse

Scale. Journal of family violence, 14(4), 399-415.

Helms, J. E., Nicolas, G., & Green, C. E. (2010). Racism and ethnoviolence as trauma:

Enhancing professional training. Traumatology, 16(4), 53.

Hequembourg, A. L., & Brallier, S. A. (2009). An exploration of sexual minority stress

across the lines of gender and sexual identity. Journal of homosexuality, 56(3),

273-298.

Herek, G. M. (1994). Assessing heterosexuals' attitudes toward lesbians and gay men: A

review of empirical research with the ATLG scale.

164

Herek, G. M. (1995). Psychological heterosexism in the United States. Lesbian, gay, and

bisexual identities over the lifespan: Psychological perspectives, 321-346.

Herek, G. M., Gillis, J. R., & Cogan, J. C. (1999). Psychological sequelae of hate-crime

victimization among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults. Journal of consulting and

clinical psychology, 67, 945-951.

Herek, G. M. (2004). Beyond “homophobia”: Thinking about sexual prejudice and

stigma in the twenty-first century. Sexuality Research & Social Policy, 1(2), 6-24.

Herek, G. M., Chopp, R., & Strohl, D. (2007). Sexual stigma: Putting sexual minority

health issues in context. In The health of sexual minorities (pp. 171-208). Springer

US.

Herek, G., & Sims, C. (2007). Sexual orientation and violent victimization: Hate crimes

and intimate partner violence among gay and bisexual males in the United States.

In R. J. Wolitski, R. Stall, & R. O. Valdiserri (Eds.), Unequal opportunity: Health

disparities among gay and bisexual men in the United States (pp. 35-71). New

York: Oxford University Press.

Herek, G. M. (2009). Hate crimes and stigma-related experiences among sexual minority

adults in the United States: Prevalence estimates from a national probability

sample. Journal of interpersonal violence, 24(1), 54-74.

Herek, G. M., & Berrill, K. T. (1990). Documenting the victimization of lesbians and gay

men: Methodological issues. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 5(3), 301-315.

Herek, G. M., Chopp, R., & Strohl, D. (2007). Sexual stigma: Putting sexual minority

health issues in context. In The health of sexual minorities (pp. 171-208). Springer

US.

165

Herek, G. M., Gillis, J. R., & Cogan, J. C. (1999). Psychological sequelae of hate-crime

victimization among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults. Journal of consulting and

clinical psychology, 67, 945-951.

Herek, G., & Sims, C. (2007). Sexual orientation and violent victimization: Hate crimes

and intimate partner violence among gay and bisexual males in the United States.

In R. J. Wolitski, R. Stall, & R. O. Valdiserri (Eds.), Unequal opportunity: Health

disparities among gay and bisexual men in the United States (pp. 35-71). New

York: Oxford University Press.

Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books.

Hill, N. L. (2009). Affirmative practice and alternative sexual orientations: Helping

clients navigate the coming out process. Clinical Social Work Journal, 37(4), 346-

356.

Hines, J. M. (2014). Internalized Heterosexism, Outness, Relationship Satisfaction, and

Violence in Lesbian Relationships (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at

Chicago).

Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1987). Personal and contextual determinants of coping

strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 946 –955.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.5.946

Holmes, S. C., Facemire, V. C., & DaFonseca, A. M. (2016). Expanding criterion a for

posttraumatic stress disorder: Considering the deleterious impact of

oppression. Traumatology, 22(4), 314.

Horn, S. S. (2007). Adolescents’ acceptance of same-sex peers based on sexual

166

orientation and gender expression. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 363–

371.

Huang, H. T. M. (2010). From Glass Clique to Tongzhi Nation: Crystal Boys, Identity

Formation, and the Politics of Sexual Shame. positions: east asia cultures

critique, 18(2), 373-398.

Huebner, D. M., Rebchook, G. M., & Kegeles, S. M. (2004). Experiences of harassment,

discrimination, and physical violence among young gay and bisexual

men. American Journal of Public Health, 94(7), 1200-1203.

Hughes, T. L., Haas, A. P., Razzano, L., Cassidy, R., & Matthews, A. (2000). Comparing lesbians' and heterosexual women's mental health: A multi-site survey. Journal of Gay &

Lesbian Social Services, 11(1), 57-76.

Hughes, T. L., Johnson, T., & Wilsnack, S. C. (2001). Sexual assault and alcohol abuse:

A comparison of lesbians and heterosexual women. Journal of substance

abuse, 13(4), 515-532.

Island, D., & Letellier, P. (1991). Men who beat the men who love them: Battered gay

men and domestic violence. Psychology Press.

Israel, T., & Mohr, J. J. (2004). Attitudes toward bisexual women and men: Current

research, future directions. Journal of , 4(1-2), 117-134.

Iverson, K. M., Dardis, C. M., & Pogoda, T. K. (2017). Traumatic brain injury and PTSD

symptoms as a consequence of intimate partner violence. Comprehensive

psychiatry, 74, 80-87.

Iwamoto, D. K., Cheng, A., Lee, C. S., Takamatsu, S., & Gordon, D. (2011). “Man-ing”

167

up and getting drunk: The role of masculine norms, alcohol intoxication and

alcohol-related problems among college men. Addictive Behaviors, 36, 906 –911.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh .2011.04.005

Jackson, K. F., Yoo, H. C. B., Guevarra Jr, R., & Harrington, B. A. (2012). Role of

identity integration on the relationship between perceived racial discrimination

and psychological adjustment of . Journal of counseling

psychology, 59(2), 240.

Jeffries, V., & Ransford, H. E. (1980). Social stratification: A multiple hierarchy

approach. Allyn & Bacon.

Johnson, A. G. (2001). Power, privilege, and difference. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.

Johnson, S. (2014). Perfectionism, the Thin Ideal, and Disordered Eating: Does

Internalized Misogyny Play a Role?.

Johnson, D. M., Zlotnick, C., & Perez, S. (2008). The relative contribution of abuse

severity and PTSD severity on the psychiatric and social morbidity of battered

women in shelters. Behavior Therapy, 39(3), 232-241.

doi:10.1016/j.beth.2007.08.003

Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Comparison of two

modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life

events. Journal of behavioral medicine, 4(1), 1-39.

Kashubeck-West, S., Szymanski, D., & Meyer, J. (2008). Internalized heterosexism:

Clinical implications and training considerations. The Counseling

Psychologist, 36(4), 615-630.

Kellermann, N. P. (2001). Transmission of Holocaust trauma-An integrative

168

view. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 64(3), 256-267.

Kemp A, Rawlings EI, Green BL. (1991). Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in

battered women: A shelter sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 14, 137–148. doi:

10.1002/jts.2490040111

King, K. R. (2003). Racism or sexism? Attributional ambiguity and simultaneous

membership in multiple oppressed groups. Journal of Applied Social

Psychology, 33(2), 223-247.

Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Sloan, S. (1948). Sexual behavior in the

human male.

Kira, I. A. (2001). Taxonomy of trauma and trauma assessment. Traumatology, 7(2), 73-

86.

Kitzinger, C. (1996). Speaking of oppression: Psychology, politics, and the language of

power. Preventing heterosexism and homophobia, 3-19.

Klein, F., Sepekoff, B., & Wolf, T. J. (1985). Sexual orientation: A multi-variable

dynamic process. Journal of homosexuality, 11(1-2), 35-49.

Klevens, C. L. (2008). Coping style as a moderator between gendered racism and

emotional eating and binge eating in African American women. Dissertation

Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 68(10-B),

6968.

Klonoff, E.A., & Landrine, H. (1995). The Schedule of Sexist Events: A measurement of

lifetime and recent sexism discrimination in women’s lives. Psychology of

Women Quarterly, 19, 439-472. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1995.tb00086.x

169

Klonoff, E. A., Landrine, H., & Campbell, R. (2000). Sexist discrimination may account

for well-known gender differences in psychiatric symptoms. Psychology of

Women Quarterly, 24(1), 93-99.

Kolltveit, S., Lange‐Nielsen, I. I., Thabet, A. A. M., Dyregrov, A., Pallesen, S., Johnsen,

T. B., & Laberg, J. C. (2012). Risk factors for PTSD, anxiety, and depression

among adolescents in Gaza. Journal of traumatic stress, 25(2), 164-170.

Koss, M. P. (1993). Rape: Scope, impact, interventions, and public policy

responses. American Psychologist, 48(10), 1062.

Koss, M. P., Heise, L., & Russo, N. F. (1994). The global health burden of

rape. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18(4), 509-537.

Koss, M., Bailey, J., Yuan, N., Herrera, V., & Lichter, E. (2003). Depression and PTSD

in survivors of male violence, research and training initiatives to facilitate

recovery. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 130–142

Krieger, N. (1990). Racial and gender discrimination: risk factors for high blood

pressure?. Social science & medicine, 30(12), 1273-1281.

Kubany, E. S., Leisen, M. B., Kaplan, A. S., Watson, S. B., Haynes, S. N., Owens, J. A.,

& Burns, K. (2000). Development and preliminary validation of a brief broad-

spectrum measure of trauma exposure: the Traumatic Life Events

Questionnaire. Psychological assessment, 12(2), 210.

Kulkin, H. S., Williams, J., Borne, H. F., de la Bretonne, D., & Laurendine, J. (2007). A

review of research on violence in same-gender couples: a resource for

clinicians. Journal of Homosexuality, 53(4), 71-87.

Ladner, J. (1971). Tomorrow's tomorrow: The black woman. Garden City, NY.

170

Landrine, H., Klonoff, E. A., Alcaraz, R., Scott, J., & Wilkins, P. (1995). Multiple

variables in discrimination.

Landrine, H., Klonoff, E. A., Gibbs, J., Manning, V., & Lund, M. (1995). Physical and

psychiatric correlates of gender discrimination. Psychology of women

quarterly, 19(4), 473-492.

Landrine, H., & Klonoff, E. A. (1997). Discrimination against women: Prevalence,

consequences, remedies. Sage Publications.

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Coping and adaptation. The handbook of

behavioral medicine, 282-325.

Lehavot, K., & Lambert, A. J. (2007). Toward a greater understanding of antigay

prejudice: On the role of sexual orientation and gender role violation. Basic and

Applied Social Psychology, 29, 279–292.

Levant, R. F., & Wong, Y. (2017). The psychology of men and . American

Psychological Association.

Levant, R. F., Hall, R. J., Weigold, I. K., & McCurdy, E. R. (2015). Construct

distinctiveness and variance composition of multidimensional instruments: Three

short-form masculinity measures. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 62, 488 –

502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ cou0000092

LeVay S. & Nonas, E. (1995), City of Friends: A Portrait of the Gay and Lesbian

Community in America. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Levine, A., & Breshears, B. (2019). Discrimination at every turn: An intersectional

ecological lens for rehabilitation. Rehabilitation psychology, 64(2), 146.

Levitt, H., Gerrish, E., & Hiestand, K. (2003). The misunderstood gender: A model of

171

modern femme identity. Sex Roles, 48, 99–113.

Levitt, H., & Hiestand, K. (2004). A quest for authenticity: A contemporary butch

gender. Sex Roles, 50(9/10), 605–621.

Levitt, H. M., & Horne, S. G. (2002). Explorations of lesbian-queer : Butch,

femme, androgynous or “other”. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 6(2), 25-39.

Lewis, L. A. (1984). The coming-out process for lesbians: Integrating a stable

identity. Social Work, 29(5), 464-469.

Lewis, R. J., Derlega, V. J., Berndt, A., Morris, L. M., & Rose, S. (2002). An empirical

analysis of stressors for gay men and lesbians. Journal of homosexuality, 42(1),

63-88.

Lewis, J. A., & Neville, H. A. (2015). Construction and initial validation of the Gendered

Racial Microaggressions Scale for Black women. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 62(2), 289.

Liddle, B. J. (1997). Gay and lesbian clients' selection of therapists and utilization of

therapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, research, practice, training, 34(1), 11.

Lobel, K. (1986). Naming the violence: Speaking out about lesbian battering. Seal Press.

Long, M. E., Elhai, J. D., Schweinle, A., Gray, M. J., Grubaugh, A. L., & Frueh, B. C.

(2008). Differences in posttraumatic stress disorder diagnostic rates and symptom

severity between Criterion A1 and non-Criterion A1 stressors. Journal of anxiety

disorders, 22(7), 1255-1263.

Loo, C. M., Fairbank, J. A., Scurfield, R. M., Ruch, L. O., King, D. W., Adams, L. J., &

Chemtob, C. M. (2001). Measuring exposure to racism: Development and

validation of a race-related stressor scale (RRSS) for Asian American Vietnam

172

veterans. Psychological Assessment, 13(4), 503.

Lott, B. (1995). Distancing from women: Interpersonal sexist discrimination. In Earlier

versions of this chapter were presented as colloquia at the psychology

departments of Yale U, the U of California at Berkeley, Stanford U, and the U of

Rhode Island.. Guilford Press.

Luna, Z. (2016). “Truly a women of color organization”: Negotiating sameness and

difference in pursuit of intersectionality. Gender & Society, 30, 769 –790.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0891243216649929

Lundy, S., & Leventhal, B. (1999). Same-sex domestic violence: Strategies for

change (Vol. 15). Sage.

MacKinnon, C. A. (1987). Feminism unmodified: Discourses on life and law. Harvard

university press.

Mallinckrodt, B., Miles, J. R., & Levy, J. J. (2014). The scientistpractitioner-advocate

model: Addressing contemporary training needs for social justice advocacy.

Training and Education in Professional Psychology, 8, 303–311.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tep0000045

Mar'i, S. K. (1988). Challenges to minority counselling: Arabs in Israel. International

Journal for the Advancement of Counselling, 11(1), 5-21.

Mason, T. B., Lewis, R. J., Winstead, B. A., & Derlega, V. J. (2015). External and

internalized heterosexism among sexual minority women: The moderating roles

of social constraints and collective self-esteem. Psychology of Sexual Orientation

and Gender Diversity, 2(3), 313.

Matthews, K. A., Gallo, L. C., & Taylor, S. E. (2010). Are psychosocial factors mediators

173

of socioeconomic status and health connections? A progress report and blueprint

for the future. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1186, 146 –173.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749- 6632.2009.05332.x

Matthews, K. A., Räikkönen, K., Everson, S. A., Flory, J. D., Marco, C. A., Owens, J. F.,

& Lloyd, C. E. (2000). Do the daily experiences of healthy men and women vary

according to occupational prestige and work strain? Psychosomatic Medicine, 62,

346 –353. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1097/00006842-200005000-00008

Mays, V. M., & Cochran, S. D. (2001). Mental health correlates of perceived

discrimination among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the United

States. American Journal of Public Health, 91(11), 1869-1876.

McCall, L. (2005). The complexity of intersectionality. Signs: Journal of women in

culture and society, 30(3), 1771-1800.

McGarrity, L. A. (2014). Socioeconomic status as context for minority stress and health

disparities among lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. Psychology of Sexual

Orientation and Gender Diversity, 1(4), 383.

McGregor, B. A., Carver, C. S., Antoni, M. H., Weiss, S., Yount, S. E., & Ironson, G.

(2001). Distress and internalized homophobia among lesbian women treated for

early stage breast cancer. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25(1), 1-9.Meyer, I.

H. (1995). Minority stress and mental health in gay men. Journal of health and

social behavior, 38-56.

McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The objectified body consciousness scale

development and validation. Psychology of women quarterly, 20(2), 181-215.

McLeod, J. D., & Kessler, R. C. (1990). Socioeconomic status differences in

174

vulnerability to undesirable life events. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 31,

162–172. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2137170

Memmi, A. (1965). The colonizer and the colonized. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.

Merton, R. K. (1968). Social theory and social structure. Simon and Schuster

Meyer, I. H. (1995). Minority stress and mental health in gay men. Journal of health and

social behavior, 38-56.

Meyer, I. H. (2003). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and

bisexual populations: conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychological

bulletin, 129(5), 674.

Meyer, I. H., & Ouellette, S. C. (2009). Unity and purpose at the intersections of

racial/ethnic and sexual identities. The story of sexual identity: Narrative

perspectives on the gay and lesbian life course, 79-106.

Mildner, C. A. (2001). Sexual minority identity formation and internalized homophobia

in lesbians: A validation study of a new instrument and related

variables (Doctoral dissertation, ProQuest Information & Learning).

Miller, A.G. (1986). The obedience experiments. New York: Praeger.

Miller, J. B. (1986). Toward a new psychology of women. Boston: Beacon Press

Millett, K. (1970). Sexual politics. New York: Ballantine.

Miner-Rubino, K., Twenge, J. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2002). Trait self-objectification

in women: Affective and personality correlates. Journal of Research in

Personality, 36(2), 147-172.

Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (1989). Psychiatric diagnosis as reified

measurement. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 11-25.

175

Moane, G. (1999). Gender and colonialism: A psychological analysis of oppression and

liberation. London, England: Macmillian.

Mohr, J. J., & Kendra, M. S. (2011). Revision and extension of a multidimensional

measure of sexual minority identity: the Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identity

Scale. Journal of counseling psychology, 58(2), 234.

Montano, A. (2000). Psicotrapia con clienti omosessuali. [Psychotherapy with the female

homosexual client] Milano, Italy: McGraw-Hill.

Moracco, K. E., Runyan, C. W., Bowling, J. M., & Earp, J. A. L. (2007). Women’s

experiences with violence: A national study. Women's Health Issues, 17(1), 3-12.

Moradi, B. (2010). Addressing gender and cultural diversity in :

Objectification theory as a framework for integrating theories and grounding

research. Sex Roles, 63(1-2), 138-148.

Moradi, B., Dirks, D., & Matteson, A. V. (2005). Roles of sexual objectification

experiences and internalization of standards of beauty in

symptomatology: A test and extension of Objectification Theory. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 52(3), 420.

Moradi, B., & Grzanka, P. R. (2017). Using intersectionality responsibly: Toward critical

epistemology, structural analysis, and social justice activism. Journal of

counseling psychology, 64(5), 500.

Moradi, B., Mohr, J. J., Worthington, R. L., & Fassinger, R. E. (2009). Counseling

psychology research on sexual (orientation) minority issues: Conceptual and

methodological challenges and opportunities. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 56(1), 5.

176

Moradi, B., & Subich, L.M. (2002). Perceived sexist events and feminist identity

development attitudes: Links to women’s psychological distress. The Counseling

Psychologist, 30(1), 44-65. doi:10.1177/0011000002301003

Moradi, B., Subich, L. M., & Phillips, J. C. (2002). Beyond revisiting: Moving feminist

identity development ahead. The Counseling Psychologist, 30(1), 111-117.

Moradi, B., & Subich, L. M. (2003). A concomitant examination of the relations of

perceived racist and sexist events to psychological distress for African American

women. The Counseling Psychologist, 31(4), 451-469.

Moradi, B., & Subich, L. M. (2004). Examining the Moderating Role of Self-Esteem in

the Link Between Experiences of Perceived Sexist Events and Psychological

Distress. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(1), 50.

Morgan, K. S. (1992). Caucasian lesbians use of psychotherapy: A matter of

altitude? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 16(1), 127-130.

Morrison, M.A., Morrison, T.G., Pope G.A., & Zumbo, B.C. (1999). An investigation of

measures of modern and old-fashioned sexism. Social Indicators Research, 48,

39-50

Morrow, D. F., & Tyson, B. (2006). Religion and spirituality. Sexual orientation and

gender expression in social work practice: Working with gay, lesbian, bisexual,

and transgender people, 384-404.

Moss, G. E. (1973). Illness, immunity, and social interaction: the dynamics of biosocial

resonation. New York: Wiley.

177

Muehlenkamp, J. J., & Saris-Baglama, R. N. (2002). Self-objectification and its

psychological outcomes for college women. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 26(4), 371-379.

Mullaly, B. (2002). Challenging oppression: A critical social work approach. New York,

NY: Oxford University Press.

Murphy, J. A., Rawlings, E. I., & Howe, S. R. (2002). A survey of clinical psychologists

on treating lesbian, gay, and bisexual clients. Professional Psychology: Research

and Practice, 33(2), 183.

Murray, C. E., Mobley, A. K., Buford, A. P., & Seaman-DeJohn, M. M. (2007). Same-

sex intimate partner violence: Dynamics, social context, and counseling

implications. Journal of LGBT Issues in Counseling, 1(4), 7-30.

Munley, P. H., Duncan, L. E., Mcdonnell, K. A., & Sauer, E. M. (2004). Counseling

psychology in the United States of America. Counselling Psychology

Quarterly, 17(3), 247-271.

Nadal, K. L. (2011). The Racial and Ethnic Microaggressions Scale (REMS):

construction, reliability, and validity. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58(4),

470.

Nadal, K. L., & Medoza, R. J. (2014). Internalized oppression and the Lesbian, Gay,

Bisexual and Transgender community (pg. 227-252). In E. J. R. David (Ed.).

Internalized Oppression: The Psychology of Marginalized Groups. New York:

Springer.

Nadal, K. L., Rivera, D. P., Corpus, J. H., & Sue, D. W. (2010). Sexual orientation and

transgender microaggressions. Microaggressions and marginality: Manifestation,

178

dynamics, and impact, 217-240.

National Center for PTSD (2014) . Women, Trauma, and PTSD. Washington, D.C., U.S.

Department of Veteran Affairs.

National Sexual Violence Resource Center. (2011). Child sexual abuse prevention:

Overview. Retrieved from

http://www.nsvrc.org/sites/default/files/Publications_NSVRC_Overview_Child-

sexual-abuse-prevention_0.pdf

National Center for PTSD (2015) . Sexual Assault Against Females. Washington, D.C.,

U.S. Department of Veteran Affairs; 2014.

National Sexual Violence Resource Center. (2015). Statistics about sexual violence.

Retrieved from http://www.nsvrc.org/sites/default/files/ publications_ nsvrc_

factsheet_media-packet_statistics-about-sexual-violence_0.pdf

Neisen J. H. (1993). Healing from cultural victimization: Recovery from shame due to

heterosexism. Journal of Gay and Lesbian Psychotherapy, 2, 49-63.

Nelson, N. L., & Probst, T. (2010). Multiple minority individuals: Multiplying the risk of

workplace harassment and discrimination. The psychology of prejudice and

discrimination: A revised and condensed edition, 97-111.

Neville, H. A., Awad, G. H., Brooks, J. E., Flores, M. P., & Bluemel, J. (2013). Color-

blind racial ideology: Theory, training, and measurement implications in

psychology. American Psychologist, 68(6), 455. doi:10.1037/a0033282

Noll, S. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). A mediational model linking self-

objectification, body shame, and disordered eating. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 22(4), 623-636.

179

Norris F. H., Foster J. D., Weisshaar, D. L. (2002). The epidemiology of sex differences

in PTSD across developmental, societal, and research contexts. In Kimerling

R., Ouimette P., Wolfe J. (Eds.), Gender and PTSD (pp. 3-42). New

York: Guilford Press.

O'Neil, J. M. (1981). Patterns of gender role conflict and strain: Sexism and fear of

femininity in men's lives. Journal of Counseling & Development, 60(4), 203-210.

Otis, M. D., Skinner W. F. (1996). The prevalence of victimization and its effect on

mental well-being among lesbian and gay people. Journal of Homosexuality,

30, 93-122.

Ovrebo, E., Brown, E. L., Emery, H. E., Stenersen, M., Schimmel-Bristow, A., &

Steinruck, R. E. (2018). Bisexual Invisibility in Trauma: PTSD Symptomology,

and MentalHealthcare Experiences Among Bisexual Women and Men versus

Lesbians and Gay Men. Journal of Bisexuality, 18(2), 168-185.

Parent, M. C. (2013). Handling item-level missing data: Simpler is just as good. The

Counseling Psychologist, 41(4), 568-600.

Parent, M. C., DeBlaere, C., & Moradi, B. (2013). Approaches to research on

intersectionality: Perspectives on gender, LGBT, and racial/ethnic identities. Sex

Roles, 68(11-12), 639-645.

Parks, C. A., Hughes, T. L., & Matthews, A. K. (2004). Race/ethnicity and sexual

orientation: Intersecting identities. Cultural diversity and ethnic minority

psychology, 10(3), 241.

Pascoe, E. A., & Smart Richman, L. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: a meta-

analytic review. Psychological bulletin, 135(4), 531. doi:10.1037/a0016059

180

Pearlin, L. I. (1989). The sociological study of stress. Journal of health and social

behavior, 241-256.

Pepper, B. I., & Sand, S. (2015). Internalized homophobia and intimate partner violence

in young adult women’s same-sex relationships. Journal of Aggression,

Maltreatment & Trauma, 24(6), 656-673.

Perez, A. (2005). Internalized oppression: How it affects members of the LGBT

community. The Diversity Factor, 13(1), 25-29.

Pharr, S. (1996). In the time of the right: Reflections on liberation. Berkeley, CA:

Chardon Press.

Pharr, S. (1997). Homophobia: A weapon of sexism. Berkeley, CA: Chardon Press.

Pheterson, G. (1986). Alliances between women: Overcoming Internalized oppression

and internalized domination. Signs, 12, 146-160.

Phillips, J. C. (2010). Eight articles, eight journals, 8 years: Selected disappointments and

celebrations from an outstanding major contribution. The Counseling

Psychologist, 38(3), 434-441.

Pico-Alfonso, M. A., Garcia-Linares, M. I., Celda-Navarro, N., Blasco-Ros, C.,

Echeburúa, E., & Martinez, M. (2006). The impact of physical, psychological,

and sexual intimate male partner violence on women's mental health: depressive

symptoms, posttraumatic stress disorder, state anxiety, and suicide. Journal of

Women's Health, 15(5), 599-611. doi:10.1089/jwh.2006.15.599

Pieterse, A. L., Todd, N. R., Neville, H. A., & Carter, R. T. (2012). Perceived racism and

mental health among Black American adults: a meta-analytic review.

Piggot, M. (2004). Double jeopardy: Lesbians and the legacy of multiple stigmatized

181

identities. Unpublished thesis, Psychology Strand at Swinburne University of

Technology, Australia.

Pilkington, N.W., & D'Augelli, A.R. (1995). Victimization of lesbian, gay, and bisexual

youth in community settings. Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 34-56.

Prilleltensky, I. (1989). Psychology and the status quo. American Psychologist, 44(5),

795.

Prilleltensky, I., & Gonick, L. (1996). Politics change, oppression remains: On the

psychology and politics of oppression. Political Psychology, 17(1), 127-138. doi:

10.2307/3791946

Ponterotto, J. G. (1997). Multicultural counseling training: A competency model and

national survey. In D. B. Pope-Davis & H. L. K. Coleman (Eds.), Multicultural

aspects of counseling series: Vol. 7. Multicultural counseling competencies:

Assessment, education and training, and supervision (pp. 111–130). Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Purdie-Vaughns, V., & Eibach, R. P. (2008). Intersectional invisibility: The distinctive

advantages and disadvantages of multiple subordinate-group identities. Sex

Roles, 59(5-6), 377-391.

Pyke, K. D. (2010). What is internalized oppression and why don’t we study it?

Acknowledging racism’s hidden injuries. Sociological Perspectives, 53(4), 55-

72.

Radonsky, V. E., & Borders, L. D. (1996). Factors influencing lesbians' direct disclosure

of their sexual orientation. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Psychotherapy, 2(3), 17-37.

182

Ragins, B. R., Cornwell, J. M., & Miller, J. S. (2003). Heterosexism in the workplace: Do

race and gender matter?. Group & Organization Management, 28(1), 45-74.

Reisner, S. L., White Hughto, J. M., Gamarel, K. E., Keuroghlian, A. S., Mizock, L., &

Pachankis, J. E. (2016). Discriminatory experiences associated with posttraumatic

stress disorder symptoms among transgender adults. Journal of counseling

psychology, 63(5), 509.

Renzetti, C. M. (1992). Violent betrayal: Partner abuse in lesbian relationships. Sage

Publications.

Renzetti, C. M. (1998). Lesbian Relationships. Issues in intimate violence, 117.

Rich, A. (1980). Compulsory heterosexuality and lesbian existence. Signs, 5, 631–60.

Risman, B. J. (2004). Gender as a social structure: Theory wrestling with

activism. Gender & society, 18(4), 429-450.

Ristock, J. L. (2003). Exploring dynamics of abusive lesbian relationships: Preliminary

analysis of a multisite, qualitative study. American journal of community

psychology, 31(3-4), 329-341.

Roccas, S., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Social identity complexity. Personality and Social

Psychology Review, 6(2), 88-106.

Root, M. P. (1992). Reconstructing the impact of trauma on personality. Personality and

psychopathology: Feminist reappraisals, 229-265.

Rosenthal, L., & Lobel, M. (2011). Explaining racial disparities in adverse birth

outcomes: Unique sources of stress for Black American women. Social Science

and Medicine, 72, 977–983.

Rosenthal, L., & Lobel, M. (2016). Stereotypes of Black American women related to

183

sexuality and motherhood. Psychology of Women Quarterly. Advance online

publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0361684315627459

Rostosky, S. S., & Riggle, E. D. (2002). " Out" at work: The relation of actor and partner

workplace policy and internalized homophobia to disclosure status. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 49(4), 411.

Rostosky, S. S., Riggle, E. D., Horne, S. G., Denton, F. N., & Huellemeier, J. D. (2010).

Lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals’ psychological reactions to amendments

denying access to civil marriage. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 80(3),

302.

Roth, S. (1985). Psychotherapy with lesbian couples: Individual issues, female

socialization, and the social context. Journal of Marital and Family

Therapy, 11(3), 273-286.

Rowan, K.S. (2004). Emotional intelligence and well-being: Implications for lesbian and

bisexual women. Unpublished master's thesis, Virginia Commonwealth

University, Richmond.

Russo, N. F. (1995). Women’s mental health: Research agenda for the twenty-first

century. In C. Willie, Sidanius, J., Levin, S., & Pratto, F. (1996). Consensual

social dominance orientation and its correlates within the hierarchical structure of

American society. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20(3), 385-

408.

Saakvitne, K. W., & Pearlman, L. A. (1993). The impact of internalized misogyny and

violence against women on feminine identity.

Sanchez-Hucles, J. V. 1998. Racism: Emotional abusiveness and psychological trauma

184

for ethnic minorities. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 1: 69–87.

Sansone, R. A., Chu, J., & Wiederman, M. W. (2007). Self‐inflicted bodily harm among

victims of intimate‐partner violence. Clinical Psychology &

Psychotherapy, 14(5), 352-357.

Sarno, E. L., Mohr, J. J., Jackson, S. D., & Fassinger, R. E. (2015). When identities

collide: Conflicts in allegiances among LGB people of color. Cultural Diversity

and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 21(4), 550.

Scambler, G., & Hopkins, A. (1986). Being epileptic: Coming to terms with stigma.

Sociology of Health and Illness, 8, 26-43.

Schafer, K. R., Brant, J., Gupta, S., Thorpe, J., Winstead-Derlega, C., Pinkerton, R., ... &

Dillingham, R. (2012). Intimate partner violence: a predictor of worse HIV

outcomes and engagement in care. AIDS patient care and STDs, 26(6), 356-365.

Schmidt, F. (2014). Intimate Partner Violence (IPV) Within the South Asian Community

in the West (Doctoral dissertation, City University of Seattle).

Settles, I. H. (2006). Use of an intersectional framework to understand Black women’s

racial and gender identities. Sex Roles, 54, 589 – 601.

Sherover-Marcuse, R. (1994). Liberation theory: Axioms and working assumptions about

the perpetuation of social oppression. The politics of liberation, Dubuque, IA:

Kendall/Hunt.

Shidlo, A. (1994). Internalized homophobia: Conceptual and empirical issues in

measurement. In Some of the information in this chapter was presented at the

meeting of the American Psychological Assn, New York, 1987.. Sage Publications,

Inc.

185

Shih, M., Young, M. J., & Bucher, A. (2013). Working to reduce the effects of

discrimination: Identity management strategies in organizations. American

Psychologist, 68(3), 145.

Shin, R. Q., Welch, J. C., Kaya, A. E., Yeung, J. G., Obana, C., Sharma, R., ... & Yee, S.

(2017). The intersectionality framework and identity intersections in the Journal

of Counseling Psychology and The Counseling Psychologist: A content

analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64(5), 458.

Shields, S. A. (2008). Gender: An intersectionality perspective. Sex roles, 59(5-6), 301-

311.

Sidanius, J. (1993). The psychology of group conflict and the dynamics of oppression: A

social dominance perspective.

Sidanius, J., Levin, S., & Pratto, F. (1996). Consensual social dominance orientation and

its correlates within the hierarchical structure of American society. International

Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20(3), 385-408.

Sigelman, L., & Welch, S. (1991). Black Americans’ views of racial inequality. New

York, NY: Cambridge University Press

Simpson, E. K., & Helfrich, C. A. (2005). Lesbian survivors of intimate partner violence:

Provider perspectives on barriers to accessing services. Journal of Gay & Lesbian

Social Services, 18(2), 39-59.

Speight, S. L. (2007). Internalized racism: One more piece of the puzzle. The Counseling

Psychologist, 35(1), 126-134.

Stein. T. S. & Cohen, C.J. (1984). Psychotherapy with Gay Men and Lesbians: An

Examination of Homophobia, Coming-Out. and Identity. Pp. 59-73 in Innovations

186

in Psychotherapy with Homosexuals, edited by E.S. Helrick and T.S. Stein.

Washington. D,C.: American Psychiatric Association Press.

Straub, K. T., McConnell, A. A., & Messman-Moore, T. L. (2018). Internalized

heterosexism and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: The mediating role of

shame proneness among trauma-exposed sexual minority women. Psychology of

Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 5(1), 99.

Suarez, E., & Gadalla, T. M. (2010). Stop blaming the victim: A meta-analysis on rape

myths. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25(11), 2010-2035.

Swim, J.K., Aikin, K.J., Hall, W.S., & Hunter, B.A. (1995). Sexism and racism: Old-

fashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

68 (2), 199-214.

Swim, J.K., & Cohen, L.L. (1997). Overt, covert, and subtle sexism: A comparison

between the attitudes toward women and the modern sexism scales. Psychology of

Women Quarterly, 21, 103-118.

Swim, J. K., Cohen, L. L., & Hyers, L. L. (1998). Experiencing everyday prejudice and

discrimination. In Prejudice (pp. 37-60). Academic Press.

Swim, J.K., Hyers, L.L., Cohen, L.L., & Ferguson, M.J. (2001). Everyday sexism:

Evidence for its incidence, nature, and psychological impact from three daily

diary studies. Journal of Social Issues, 57 (1), 31-53. doi:10.1111/0022-

4537.00200

Syed, M. (2010). Disciplinarity and methodology in intersectionality theory and research.

American Psychologist, 65, 61– 62. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/a0017495

187

Szymanski, D. M. (2005). Heterosexism and sexism as correlates of psychological

distress in lesbians. Journal of Counseling & Development, 83(3), 355-360.

Szymanski, D. M. (2009). Examining potential moderators of the link between

heterosexist events and gay and bisexual men's psychological distress. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 56(1), 142.

Szymanski, D. M., & Balsam, K. F. (2011). Insidious trauma: Examining the relationship

between heterosexism and lesbians’ PTSD symptoms. Traumatology, 17(2), 4-13.

Szymanski, D. M., & Carr, E. R. (2008). The roles of gender role conflict and

internalized heterosexism in gay and bisexual men's psychological distress:

Testing two mediation models. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 9(1), 40.

Szymanski, D. M., & Chung, Y. B. (2001). The lesbian internalized homophobia scale: A

rational/theoretical approach. Journal of homosexuality, 41(2), 37-52.

Szymanski, D. M., & Chung, Y. B. (2003). Feminist attitudes and coping resources as

correlates of lesbian internalized heterosexism. Feminism & Psychology, 13(3),

369-389.

Szymanski, D. M. (2005). A feminist approach to working with internalized heterosexism

in lesbians. Journal of College Counseling, 8(1), 74-85.

Szymanski, D. M. (2006). Does internalized heterosexism moderate the link between

heterosexist events and lesbians' psychological distress?. Sex Roles, 54(3), 227-

234.

Szymanski, D. M., Gupta, A., Carr, E. R., & Stewart, D. (2009). Internalized misogyny as

a moderator of the link between sexist events and women’s psychological distress.

Sex Roles, 61(1-2), 101-109. doi:10.1007/s11199-009-9611-y

188

Szymanski, D. M., & Henning, S. L. (2007). The role of self-objectification in women’s

depression: A test of objectification theory. Sex Roles, 56(1-2), 45-53.

Szymanski, D. M., & Henrichs-Beck, C. (2014). Exploring sexual minority women’s

experiences of external and internalized heterosexism and sexism and their links

to coping and distress. Sex Roles, 70(1-2), 28-42.

Szymanski, D. M., & Ikizler, A. S. (2013). Internalized heterosexism as a mediator in the

relationship between gender role conflict, heterosexist discrimination, and

depression among sexual minority men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 14(2),

211.

Szymanski, D. M., Kashubeck-West, S., & Meyer, J. (2008). Internalized heterosexism:

A historical and theoretical overview. The Counseling Psychologist, 36(4), 510-

524.

Szymanski, D. M., & Meyer, D. (2008). Racism and heterosexism as correlates of

psychological distress in African American sexual minority women. Journal of

LGBT Issues in Counseling, 2(2), 94-108.

Szymanski, D. M., & Moffitt, L. B. (2012). Sexism and heterosexism. In N. A. Fouad, J.

A. Carter, & L. M. Subich (Eds.), APA handbooks in psychology. APA handbook

of counseling psychology, Vol. 2. Practice, interventions, and applications (pp.

361-390). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association

.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/13755-015

Szymanski, D. M., & Owens, G. P. (2008). Do coping styles moderate or mediate the

relationship between internalized heterosexism and sexual minority women's

psychological distress?. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32(1), 95-104.

189

Szymanski, D. M., & Stewart, D. N. (2010). Racism and sexism as correlates of African

American women’s psychological distress. Sex Roles, 63(3-4), 226-238.

Szymanski, D. M., & Sung, M. R. (2010). Minority Stress and Psychological Distress

Among Asian American Sexual Minority Persons 1Ψ7. The Counseling

Psychologist, 38(6), 848-872.

Szymanski, D. M., & Balsam, K. F. (2011). Insidious trauma: Examining the relationship

between heterosexism and lesbians’ PTSD symptoms. Traumatology, 17(2), 4-13.

Szymanski, D. M., & Henrichs-Beck, C. (2014). Exploring sexual minority women’s

experiences of external and internalized heterosexism and sexism and their links

to coping and distress. Sex Roles, 70(1-2), 28-42.

Szymanski, D. M., Kashubeck-West, S., & Meyer, J. (2008). Internalized heterosexism:

A historical and theoretical overview. The Counseling Psychologist, 36(4), 510-

524.

Tabachnick, B. G., Fidell, L. S., & Osterlind, S. J. (2001). Using multivariate statistics.

Thoits, P. A. (1985). Social support and psychological well-being: Theoretical

possibilities.

In Social support: Theory, research and applications (pp. 51-72). Springer

Netherlands.

Thoits, P. A. (1999). Sociological approaches to mental illness. A handbook for the study

of mental health, 121-138.

Thoma, B. C., & Huebner, D. M. (2013). Health consequences of racist and antigay

discrimination for multiple minority adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic

Minority Psychology, 19(4), 404.

190

Thomas, R. A., & Weston, R. (2018). Gender Differences in the Association of Intimate

Partner Violence and Relationship Values among College Students. Journal of

Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 1-20.

Thomas, A. J., Witherspoon, K. M., & Speight, S. L. (2004). Toward the development of

the Stereotypic Roles of Black Women Scale. Journal of Black Psychology, 30 ,

426–442. doi:10.1177/ 0095798404266061.

Thomas, A. J., Witherspoon, K. M., & Speight, S. L. (2008). Gendered racism,

psychological distress, and coping styles of African American women. Cultural

Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14(4), 307.

Tjaden, P., Thoennes, N., & Allison, C. J. (1999). Comparing violence over the life span

in samples of same-sex and opposite-sex cohabitants. Violence and victims, 14(4),

413.

Tiggemann, M., & Kuring, J. K. (2004). The role of body objectification in disordered

eating and depressed mood. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43(3), 299-

311.

Tiggemann, M., & Slater, A. (2001). A test of objectification theory in former dancers

and non‐dancers. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25(1), 57-64.

Tolin, D. F., & Foa, E. B. (2006). Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress

disorder: a quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychological

bulletin, 132(6), 959.

Tougas, F., Brown, R. Beaton, A.M., & Joly, S. (1995) Neosexism: Plus ça change, plus

c’est pareil. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(8), 842-849.

doi:10.1177/0146167295218007

191

Twenge, G.M. (1997). Attitudes toward women, 1970-1995: A meta-analysis.

Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 35-51.

Velez, B. L., Moradi, B., & DeBlaere, C. (2015). Multiple oppressions and the mental

health of sexual minority Latina/o individuals. The Counseling Psychologist, 43,

7-38. doi:10.1177/0011000014542836

Vera, E. A., & Speight, S. L. (2003). Multicultural competence, social justice, and

counseling psychology: Expanding our roles. The Counseling Psychologist, 31,

253-272. doi: 10.1177/0011000003031003001

Violence Policy Center. (2012). American roulette: Murder-suicide in the United States.

Retrieved from www.vpc.org/studies/amroul2012.pdf.

Vivero, V. N., & Jenkins, S. R. (1999). Existential hazards of the multicultural

individual: Defining and understanding" cultural homelessness.". Cultural

Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 5(1), 6. doi:10.1037//1099-9809.5.1.6

Vogel, L. C., & Marshall, L. L. (2001). PTSD symptoms and partner abuse: Low income

women at risk. Journal of Traumatic Stress: Official Publication of The

International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, 14(3), 569-584.

Waldo, C. R. (1999). Working in a majority context: A structural model of heterosexism

as minority stress in the workplace. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46(2),

218.

Walters, M.L., Chen J., & Breiding, M.J. (2013). The National Intimate Partner and

Sexual Violence Survey (NISVS): 2010 Findings on Victimization by Sexual

Orientation. Retrieved from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,

192

National Center for Injury Prevention and Control:

http://www.cdc.gov/ViolencePrevention/pdf/NISVS_SOfindings.pdf

Waldner-Haugrud, Lisa K., & Vaden Gratch, Linda. (1997). Sexual coercion in

gay/lesbian relationships: Descriptives and gender differences. Violence and

Victims, 12 (1), 87-98.

Warner, L. R. (2008). A best practices guide to intersectional approaches in

psychological research. Sex roles, 59(5-6), 454-463.

Weathers, F. W., & Keane, T. M. (2007). The Criterion A problem revisited:

Controversies and challenges in defining and measuring psychological

trauma. Journal of traumatic stress, 20(2), 107-121.

Weathers, F. W., Litz, B. T., Keane, T. M., Palmieri, P. A., Marx, B. P., & Schnurr, P. P.

(2013). The ptsd checklist for dsm-5 (pcl-5). Scale available from the National

Center for PTSD at www. ptsd. va. gov.

West, C. M. (2012). Partner abuse in ethnic minority and gay, lesbian, bisexual, and

transgender populations. Partner Abuse, 3(3), 336-357.

Wiist, W. H., & McFarlane, J. (1998). Utilization of police by abused pregnant Hispanic

women. Violence Against Women, 4(6), 677-693.

Wong, Y. J., Ho, M. R., Wang, S.-Y., & Miller, I. S. K. (2017). Metaanalyses of the

relationship between conformity to masculine norms and mental health-related

outcomes. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64, 80 –93.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cou0000176

Wong, Y. J., Liu, T., & Klann, E. M. (2017). The intersection of race, ethnicity, and

masculinities: Progress, problems, and prospects. In R. F. Levant & Y. J. Wong

193

(Eds.), The psychology of men and masculinities (pp. 261-288). Washington, DC,

US: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0000023-010

Woods, K. C., Buchanan, N. T., & Settles, I. H. (2008). Sexual harassment across the

color line: Experiences and outcomes of cross versus intra-racial sexual

harassment among black women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority

Psychology, 15, 1–11. doi:10.1037/ a0013541.

Worell, J., & Remer, P. (2003). Feminist perspectives in therapy. Hoboken

Woulfe, J. M., & Goodman, L. A. (2018). Identity abuse as a tactic of violence in

LGBTQ communities: Initial validation of the identity abuse measure. Journal of

interpersonal violence, 0886260518760018.

Young, I. M. (1990). Justice and the politics of difference. Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press.

194

Table 1.

Conceptual Interactions of Predictors with PTSD Symptom Severity for Hierarchical

Regressions

Step Variable

1 Sexist Discrimination Heterosexist Discrimination Internalized Sexism Internalized Heterosexism

2 Sexist Discrimination x Internalized Sexism Heterosexist Discrimination x Internalized Heterosexism

Sexist Discrimination x Heterosexist Discrimination Internalized Sexism x Internalized Heterosexism

Sexist Discrimination x Internalized Heterosexism Heterosexist Discrimination x Internalized Sexism

195

Table 2.

Demographic Characteristics of Participants (N =209)

n %

Gender

Woman 201 96.2%

Transgender Woman 8 3.8%

Race

Arab 3 1.4%

Asian/Pacific Islander 14 6.7%

African American or Black 5 2.4%

Caucasian or White 163 78%

Hispanic 4 1.9%

Indigenous 1 0.5%

Latina 6 2.9%

Multiracial 8 3.8%

Other 5 2.4%

Sexual Orientation

Lesbian/gay 183 87.6%

Other 26 4%

Kinsey Gender Attraction

Same gender somewhat more 15 7.2%

196

Table 2 (continued). n %

Demographic Characteristics of Participants 58 27.8%

Same gender mostly

Same gender only 136 65.1%

House Income

Hard time buying the things we need 23 11%

Just enough money for the things we need 66 31.6%

No problem buying things 98 46.9%

Enough money to buy anything 22 10.5%

Note. % = percentage of entire sample.

197

Table 3.

Correlations amongst Main Variables (N =209)

Note. **p <0.01, two-tailed; * p <0.05, two-tailed. PCL = Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist; HHRDS = The Heterosexist Harassment, Discrimination, and Rejection Scale; SSE = Schedule of Sexist Events; LIHS = Lesbian Internalized Homophobia Scale; IMS = The Internalized Misogyny Scale; SVAW = Severity of Violence Against Women Scale; SES = Socioeconomic Status; Kinsey = Gender Attraction; SO = Sexual Orientation.

198

Table 4.

Descriptive Statistics for Main Variables (N =209)

Variable M(SD) α Potential Observed Range Range PTSD 26.52 (19.21) .95 0-80 0-71

HHRDS 15.60 (16.03) .90 1-84 1-80

SSE 31.09 (19.74) .94 1-120 1-96

LIHS 107.68(35.82) .92 1-364 40-199

IMS 28.45 (12.71) .87 1-119 17-84

SVAW 3.60(11.25) .95 1-184 1-88

Note. HHRDS = externalized heterosexism; SSE = externalized sexism; LIHS = internalized heterosexism; IMS = internalized sexism; SVAW = intimate partner violence.

199

Table 5.

Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Posttraumatic Stress Symptom Severity (Hypothesis 1; N =209)

Variable B SE B β t p Step 1

SES -.4.94 1.58 -.22 -3.12 .002**

Step 2 SES -3.74 1.57 -.16 -2.38 .02* HHRDS -.06 1.43 -.00 -.04 .97 SSE .30 .08 .31 3.56 <.001** LIHS .01 .04 .02 .33 .74 IMS .77 1.84 .03 .42 .67 Note. **p <0.01, *p<.05 two-tailed. SES = Socioeconomic Status; HHRDS = The Heterosexist Harassment, Discrimination, and Rejection Scale; SSE = Schedule of Sexist Events; LIHS = Lesbian Internalized Homophobia Scale; IMS = The Internalized Misogyny Scale.

200

Table 6.

Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Posttraumatic Stress Symptom Severity (Hypothesis 2; N =209)

Variable B SE B β t p Step 1

SES -4.94 1.58 -.21 -3.12 .002**

Step 2 SES -3.72 1.57 -.16 -2.37 .02* HHRDS -.04 1.43 -.00 -.03 .98 SSE .30 .08 .31 3.54 <.001** LIHS .01 .04 -.03 .03 .36 IMS .61 1.86 .02 .33 .74

Step 3 SES -3.70 1.57 -.16 -2.35 .02* HHRDS -2.14 3.73 -.13 -.57 .57 SSE .32 .09 .33 3.77 <.001** LIHS -.01 .05 -.02 -.19 .85 IMS -2.31 2.75 -.09 -.84 .40 HHRDSxLIHS .02 .03 .13 .58 .57 SSExIMS .14 .10 .15 1.41 .16

Note. **p <0.01, p<0.05 two-tailed. SES = Socioeconomic Status; HHRDS = The Heterosexist Harassment, Discrimination, and Rejection Scale; SSE = Schedule of Sexist Events; LIHS = Lesbian Internalized Homophobia Scale; IMS = The Internalized Misogyny Scale.

201

Table 7.

Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Posttraumatic Stress Symptom Severity (Hypothesis 3; N =209)

Variable B SE B β t p Step 1

SES -4.94 1.58 -.21 -3.12 .002**

Step 2 SES -3.72 1.57 -.16 -2.37 .02* HHRDS -.04 1.43 -.00 -.03 .98 SSE .30 .08 .30 3.54 .001** LIHS .01 .04 .03 .36 .72 IMS .61 1.86 .02 .34 .74

Step 3 SES -3.76 1.57 -.16 -2.41 .02* HHRDS 4.06 2.46 .24 1.65 .10 SSE .32 .08 .33 3.78 <.001** LIHS -.01 .04 .02 .26 .79 IMS -.78 5.37 -.03 -.15 .88 HHRDSxSSE -.10 .05 -.29 -2.07 .04* LIHSxIMS .01 .04 .06 .29 .77

Note. **p <0.01, *p<0.05, two-tailed. SES = Socioeconomic Status; HHRDS = The Heterosexist Harassment, Discrimination, and Rejection Scale; SSE = Schedule of Sexist Events; LIHS = Lesbian Internalized Homophobia Scale; IMS = The Internalized Misogyny Scale.

202

Table 8.

Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Posttraumatic Stress Symptom Severity (Hypothesis 4; N =209)

Variable B SE B β t p Step 1

SES -4.94 1.58 -.21 -3.12 .002**

Step 2 SES -3.72 1.57 -.16 -2.37 .02* HHRDS -.04 .04 1.43 -.00 .98 SSE .30 .08 .31 3.54 .001** LIHS .01 .04 .03 .36 .72 IMS .61 1.86 .02 .33 .74

Step 3 SES -3.81 1.59 -.16 -2.39 .02* HHRDS -.22 1.45 -.01 -.15 .88 SSE .17 .19 .18 .90 .37 LIHS -.03 .06 -.06 -.50 .62 IMS .93 1.88 .04 .50 .62 HHRDSxIMS .00 .00 .17 .80 .43 SSExLIHS 1.34 1.52 .06 .89 .38

Note. **p <0.01, two-tailed. SES = Socioeconomic Status; HHRDS = The Heterosexist Harassment, Discrimination, and Rejection Scale; SSE = Schedule of Sexist Events; LIHS = Lesbian Internalized Homophobia Scale; IMS = The Internalized Misogyny Scale.

203

Table 9

Moderated Moderator Analyses for Variables Predicting Posttraumatic Stress Symptom Severity (Hypothesis 5; N =209)

Variable B SE B t p SSE -.05 .17 -.38 .78 IMS -2.04 2.41 -.84 .40 SVAW -.64 10.55 -.06 .95 SSExIMS .18 .10 1.77 .08 SSExSVAW .45 .34 1.32 .19 IMSxSVAW -.40 3.81 -.10 .92 SSExIMSxSVAW -.14 .15 -.94 .35 SES -3.46 1.37 -2.53 .01 Note. **p <0.01, two-tailed. SES = Socioeconomic Status; SSE = Schedule of Sexist Events; IMS = The Internalized Misogyny Scale; SVAW = Severity of Violence Against Women Scale.

204

Table 10

Moderated Moderator Analyses for Variables Predicting Posttraumatic Stress Symptom Severity (Hypothesis 6; N =209)

Variable B SE B t p HHRDS -3.07 4.50 -.68 .50 LIHS -.03 .05 -.64 .52 SVAW -5.34 11.69 -.46 .65 HHRDSxLIHS .05 .04 1.31 .19 HHRDSxSVAW 13.19 8.65 1.52 .13 LIHSxSVAW .07 .10 .76 .45 HHRDSxLIHSxSVAW -.10 .07 -1.40 .16 SES -3.71 1.40 -2.66 .00** Note. **p <0.01, two-tailed. SES = Socioeconomic Status; HHRDS = The Heterosexist Harassment, Discrimination, and Rejection Scale; LIHS = Lesbian Internalized Homophobia Scale; SVAW = Severity of Violence Against Women Scale.

205

Figure 1. Conceptual model of the moderated moderation of experiences of sexism, internalized sexism, PTSD symptom severity, and intimate partner violence.

206

Figure 2. Conceptual model of the moderated moderation of experiences of heterosexism, internalized heterosexism, PTSD symptom severity, and intimate partner violence.

207

APPENDIX A

DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE

Please answer the following questions about yourself: What is your age?

What is your gender? A. Female B. Transgender Woman C. Genderqueer D. Male E. Transgender Man How would you classify yourself? A. Arab B. Asian/Pacific Islander C. Black/African American D. Caucasian/White E. Hispanic F. Indigenous or Aboriginal G. Latino H. Multiracial I. Other Which of the following describes your A. My family has a hard time buying the family’s income? things we need. B. My family has just enough money for the things we need. C. My family has no problem buying the things we need and sometimes we can also buy special things. D. My family has enough money to buy pretty much anything we want. What best describes your sexual A. Exclusively attracted to the other orientation? gender B. Predominantly attracted to the other gender, only incidentally attracted to the same gender C. Predominantly attracted to the other gender, but more than incidentally attracted to the same gender D. Bisexual E. Predominantly attracted to the same gender, but more than incidentally attracted to the other gender F. Predominantly attracted to the same gender, only incidentally attracted to the other gender G. Exclusively attracted to the same gender H. Asexual

208

APPENDIX B

PTSD CHECKLIST FOR THE DSM-5

Instructions: Below is a list of problems that people sometimes have in response to a very stressful experience. Please read each problem carefully and then circle one of the numbers to the right to indicate how much you have been bothered by that problem in the past month.

Not A In the past month, how much were Quite at little Moderately Extremely you bothered by: a bit all bit 1. Repeated, disturbing, and unwanted memories of the stressful 0 1 2 3 4 experience? 2. Repeated, disturbing dreams of the stressful experience? 0 1 2 3 4 3. Suddenly feeling or acting as if the stressful experience were actually happening again (as if you were 0 1 2 3 4 actually back there reliving it)? 4. Feeling very upset when something reminded you of the stressful 0 1 2 3 4 experience? 5. Having strong physical reactions when something reminded you of the stressful experience (for example, 0 1 2 3 4 heart pounding, trouble breathing, sweating)? 6. Avoiding memories, thoughts, or feelings related to the stressful 0 1 2 3 4 experience? 7. Avoiding external reminders of the stressful experience (for example, people, places, conversations, 0 1 2 3 4 activities, objects, or situations)? 8. Trouble remembering important parts of the stressful experience? 0 1 2 3 4 9. Having strong negative beliefs 0 1 2 3 4 about yourself, other people, or the 209

world (for example, having thoughts such as: I am bad, there is something seriously wrong with me, no one can be trusted, the world is completely dangerous)? 10. Blaming yourself or someone else for the stressful experience or what 0 1 2 3 4 happened after it? 11. Having strong negative feelings such as fear, horror, anger, guilt, or 0 1 2 3 4 shame? 12. Loss of interest in activities that you used to enjoy? 0 1 2 3 4 13. Feeling distant or cut off from other people? 0 1 2 3 4 14. Trouble experiencing positive feelings (for example, being unable to feel happiness or have loving feelings 0 1 2 3 4 for people close to you)? 15. Irritable behavior, angry outbursts, or acting aggressively? 0 1 2 3 4 16. Taking too many risks or doing things that could cause you harm? 0 1 2 3 4 17. Being “superalert” or watchful or on guard? 0 1 2 3 4 18. Feeling jumpy or easily startled? 0 1 2 3 4 19. Having difficulty concentrating? 0 1 2 3 4 20. Trouble falling or staying asleep? 0 1 2 3 4

210

APPENDIX C

SEVERITY OF VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN SCALE

1 ______2______3______4 ______never once ………. a few times ……….. … 4 or more times

In the past year, how often did your partner… ____ shake a finger at your partner (you) ____ make threatening gestures or faces ____ shake a fist at your partner (you) ____ act like a bully ____ grab your partner (you) suddenly or forcefully ____ hit or kick a wall, door or furniture ____ threaten to harm/damage things your partner (you) cares about ____ destroy something belonging to your partner (you) ____ throw, smash or break an object ____ threaten to destroy property ____ drive dangerously with your partner (you) in the car ____ throw an object at your partner (you) ____ threaten to hurt your partner (you) ____ threaten suicide ____ threaten someone your partner (you) cares about _____ threaten to kill your partner (you) ____ act like you (your partner) wanted to kill your partner (you) ____ threaten your partner (you) with a club-like object ____ threaten your partner (you) with a weapon ____ threaten your partner (you) with a knife or gun

____ hold your partner (you) down, pinning in place ____ push or shove your partner (you) ____ shake or roughly handle your partner (you) ____ spank your partner (you) ____ twist your partner’s (your) arm ____ pull your partner’s (your) hair ____ scratch your partner (you) ____ bite your partner (you) ____ kick your partner (you) ____ slap your partner (you) with the palm of a hand ____ slap your partner (you) with back of a hand ____ punch your partner (you) ____ slap your partner (you) repeatedly around the face and head ____ hit your partner (you) with an object ____ stomp on your partner (you) ____ choke your partner (you) 211

____ beat your partner up (beat you up) ____ burn your partner (you) with something ____ use a club-like object on your partner (you) ____ use a knife or gun on your partner (you) ____ demand sex whether your partner (you) wanted it or not ____ make your partner (you) have sexual intercourse against his/her will

____ make your partner have oral sex against his/her will ____ physically force your partner (you) to have sex ____ make your partner (you) have anal sex against his/her will ____ use an object on your partner (you) in a sexual way

212

APPENDIX D

SCHEDULE OF SEXIST EVENTS

Instructions: Please think carefully about your life as you answer the questions below.

For each question, read the question and then answer for what the

PAST YEAR has been like. Circle the number that best describes events in the PAST

YEAR, using these rules:

Circle 1 = If the event has NEVER happened to you

Circle 2 = If the event happened ONCE IN A WHILE (less than 10% of the time)

Circle 3 = If the event happened SOMETIMES (10-25% of the time)

Circle 4 = If the event happened A LOT (26-49% of the time)

Circle 5 = If the event happed MOST OF THE TIME (50-70% of the time)

Circle 6 = If the event happed ALMOST ALL OF THE TIME (more than 70% of the time)

1. How many times have you been treated unfairly by teachers or professors because

you are a woman?

2. How many times have you been treated unfairly by your employer, boss, or

supervisors because you are a woman?

3. How many times have you been treated unfairly by your co-workers, fellow

students, or colleagues because you are a woman?

213

4. How many times have you been treated unfairly by people in service jobs (by

store clerks, waiters, bartenders, waitresses, bank tellers, mechanics and others)

because you are a woman?

5. How many times have you been treated unfairly by strangers because you are a

woman?

6. How many times have you been treated unfairly by people in helping jobs (by

doctors, nurses, psychiatrists, case workers, dentists, school counselors, therapists,

pediatricians, school principals, gynecologists, and others) because you are a

woman?

7. How many times have you been treated unfairly by neighbors because you are a

woman?

8. How many times have you been treated unfairly by your boyfriend, husband, or

other important men in our life because you are a woman?

9. How many times were you denied a raise, a promotion, tenure, a good

assignment, a job or other such thing at work that you deserved because you are a

woman?

10. How many times have you been treated unfairly by your family because you are a

woman?

11. How many times have people made inappropriate or unwanted sexual advances to

you because you are a woman?

12. How many times have people failed to show you the respect that you deserve

because you are a woman?

13. How many times have you wanted to tell someone off for being sexist?

214

14. How many times have you been really angry about something sexist that was

done to you?

15. How many times were you forced to take drastic steps (such as filing a grievance,

filing a lawsuit, quitting a job, moving away, and other actions) to deal with some

sexist thing that was done to you?

16. How many times have you been called a sexist name like bitch, cunt, chick, or

other names?

17. How many times have you gotten into an argument or a fight about something

sexist that was done or said to you or done to somebody else?

18. How many times have you been made fun of, picked on, pushed, shoved, hit, or

threatened with harm because you are a woman?

19. How many times have you heard people making sexist jokes, or degrading sexual

jokes?

20. How different would your life be now if you HAD NOT BEEN treated in a sexist

and unfair way?

1 = the same as it is now

2 = a little different

3 = different in a few ways

4 = different in a lot of ways

5 = different in most ways

6 = totally different

215

APPENDIX E

THE HETEROSEXIST HARASSMENT, DISCRIMINATION, AND REJECTION

SCALE

Instructions: We are interested in your experiences with heterosexism/homophobia during the past year. For Items 1-14, please answer on a scale from 1 to 6:

1 2 3 4 5 6 The event The has never event happened to happene you d almost all of the time, more than 70% of the time 1. In the past year, how many times have you been treated unfairly by teachers or professors because you are Lesbian? 2. In the past year, how many times have you been treated unfairly by your employer, boss, or supervisors because you are Lesbian? 3. In the past year, how many times have you been treated unfairly by your co-workers, fellow students or colleagues because you are Lesbian? 4. In the past year, how many times have you been treated unfairly by people in helping jobs (by doctors, nurses, psychiatrists, caseworkers, dentists, school counselors, therapists, pediatricians, school principals, gynecologists, and others) because you are Lesbian? 5. In the past year, how many times have you been treated unfairly by people in service jobs (by store clerks, waiters, bartenders, waitresses, bank tellers, mechanic, and other) because you are Lesbian?

216

HETEROSEXISM AND SEXISM EFFECTS ON PTSD SYMPTOMS

6. In the past year, how many times have you been treated unfairly by strangers because you are Lesbian? 7. In the past year, how many times were you denied a raise, promotion, tenure, a good assignment, a job, or other such thing at work that you deserved because you are Lesbian? 8. In the past year, how many times have you been treated unfairly by your family because you are Lesbian? 9. In the past year, how many times have you been called a HETEROSEXIST name like , lezzie, or other names?

10. In the past year, how many times have you been made fun of, picked on, pushed, shoved, hit, or threatened with harm because you are Lesbian? 11. In the past year, how many times have you been rejected by family members because you are Lesbian? 12. In the past year, how many times have you been rejected by friends because you are Lesbian? 13. In the past year, how many times have you heard anti-lesbian/anti-gay remarks from family members?

14. In the past year, how many times have you been verbally insulted because you were Lesbian?

217

HETEROSEXISM AND SEXISM EFFECTS ON PTSD SYMPTOMS

APPENDIX F

LESBIAN INTERNALIZED HOMOPHOBIA SCALE

Please indicate your agreement or disagreement with each of the following statements by selecting the appropriate response from the scale below. There are no right or wrong answers; however, for the data to be meaningful, you must answer each statement given below as honestly as possible. Your responses are completely anonymous. Please do not leave any statement unmarked. Some statements may depict situations that you have not experienced; please imagine yourself in those situations when answering those statements.

1. I try not to give signs that I am a lesbian. I am careful about the way I dress, the. jewelry I wear, the places, people and events I talk about.

2. I can't stand lesbians who are too "butch". They make lesbians as a group look bad.

3. Attending lesbian events and organizations is important to me.

4. I hate myself for being attracted to other women.

5. I believe female homosexuality is a sin.

6. I am comfortable being an "out" lesbian. I want others to know and see me as a lesbian.

7. I have respect and admiration for other lesbians.

8. I wouldn't mind if my boss knew that I was a lesbian.

9. If some lesbians would change and be more acceptable to the larger society, lesbians as a group would not have to deal with so much negativity and discrimination.

10. I am proud to be a lesbian.

11. I am not worried about anyone finding out that I am a lesbian.

12. When interacting with members of the lesbian community, I often feel different and alone, like I don't fit in. 218

HETEROSEXISM AND SEXISM EFFECTS ON PTSD SYMPTOMS

13. I feel bad for acting on my lesbian desire.

14. I feel comfortable talking to my heterosexual friends about my everyday home life with my lesbian partner/lover or my everyday activities with my lesbian friends.

15. Having lesbian friends is important to me.

16. I am familiar with lesbian books and/or magazines.

17. Being a part of the lesbian community is important to me.

18. It is important for me to conceal the fact that I am a lesbian from my family.

19. I feel comfortable talking about homosexuality in public.

20. I live in fear that someone will find out I am a lesbian.

21. If I could change my sexual orientation and become heterosexual, I would.

22. I do not feel the need to be on guard, lie, or hide my lesbianism to others.

23. I feel comfortable joining a lesbian social group, lesbian sports team, or lesbian organization.

24. When speaking of my lesbian lover/partner to a straight person I change pronouns

so that others will think I'm involved with a man rather than a woman.

25. Being a lesbian makes my future look bleak and hopeless.

26. If my peers knew of my lesbianism, I am afraid that many would not want to be friends

with me.

27. Social situations with other lesbians make me feel uncomfortable.

28. I wish some lesbians wouldn't "flaunt" their lesbianism. They only do it for shock

and it doesn't accomplish anything positive.

29. I don't feel disappointment in myself for being a lesbian.

30. I am familiar with lesbian movies and/or music.

219

HETEROSEXISM AND SEXISM EFFECTS ON PTSD SYMPTOMS

31. I am aware of the history concerning the development of lesbian communities and/or the lesbian/gay rights movement.

32. I act as if my lesbian lovers are merely friends.

33. I feel comfortable discussing my lesbianism with my family.

34. I could not confront a straight friend or acquaintance if she or he made a

homophobic or heterosexist statement to me.

35. I am familiar with lesbian music festivals and conferences.

36. When speaking of my lesbian lover/partner to a straight person, I often use

neutral pronouns so the sex of the person is vague.

37. Lesbians are too aggressive.

38. I frequently make negative comments about other lesbians.

39. I am familiar with community resources for lesbians (i.e., bookstores, support groups, bars, etc.).

220

HETEROSEXISM AND SEXISM EFFECTS ON PTSD SYMPTOMS

APPENDIX G

THE INTERNALIZED MISOGYNY SCALE

221

HETEROSEXISM AND SEXISM EFFECTS ON PTSD SYMPTOMS

222

HETEROSEXISM AND SEXISM EFFECTS ON PTSD SYMPTOMS

APPENDIX H

INFORMED CONSENT

Informed Consent for Participation in a Research Project to Understand Sexual

Minority Women’s Experiences with Oppression and Trauma

My name is Taylor Ceroni, a doctoral student at the University of Akron conducting a research project on sexual minority women’s experiences with oppression and interpersonal trauma. I am under the direction of my advisor, Dr. Dawn Johnson, Ph.D. We want to better understand the experiences and beliefs that sexual minority women have based on their marginalized identities. Procedures: Your participation is voluntary and the survey should take approximately 30-45 minutes to complete. You must be at least 18 years old to participate in this project and identify as a sexual minority woman.

Benefits: All responses are confidential and anonymous. Participants will be redirected at the end of the survey to a separate website to enter into a raffle to win 1 of 6, $25 gift cards. This information will not be attached to your responses to the survey. Your responses will also help us to better understand the experience of sexual minority women in today’s society.

Risks and Discomforts: This survey may contain some questions that cause discomfort based on experiences of oppression and trauma. The survey is completely voluntary and you may stop at any time. Additional resources will be provided at the end if discomfort is experienced.

Right to Refuse or Withdraw: This study is completely voluntary and you may refuse participation or refuse to answer any questions that you do not want to answer. No penalty is given for not completing the survey.

Confidentiality: All responses to the survey are anonymous and confidential. All records of this study will be kept private and no identifying information will be included in any reports or publications based on this survey.

Who to Contact with Questions/Concerns: If you have any questions about this study, you may contact the investigator, Taylor Ceroni, M.A. ([email protected]). This project has been reviewed and approved by The University of Akron Institutional Review Board (IRB). Any serious concerns about participant rights can be directed to the IRB who can be reached at (330) 972-7666.

Advancing to the next page to begin the study indicates that you have read and understood the information provided and voluntarily agree to participate in the study.

Those who consent by clicking the button will then be taken to the survey. Thank you for participating! 223

HETEROSEXISM AND SEXISM EFFECTS ON PTSD SYMPTOMS

APPENDIX I

INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD APPROVAL FOR HUMAN SUBJECTS RESEARCH

224