Mind on the Edge of Humanity

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Mind on the Edge of Humanity Word Count: 7,842 Making Friends, Making Tools and Making Symbols By Matt J. Rossano What kind of mind immediately preceded the human mind? In decades past, it was assumed that the step from the nonhuman mind to the human mind was a qualitative one. The defining characteristics of the human mind such as language, episodic memory, recursion, mentalizing (theory of mind) were, by their very nature, dichotomous (Chomsky 1966: 78; Povinelli 2000; Suddendorf and Corballis 1997). In other words, these mental traits were either present or absent with no intermediate states possible. Recent research, however, has questioned this assumption (e.g. Clayton, Bussey, and Dickson 2003; Slocombe and Zuberbuhler 2006; Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, and Moll 2005). Symbolic thinking has typically been grouped into this same category as another uniquely human and strictly dichotomous mental attribute. Studies with nonhuman primates, however, suggest that a rudimentary symbolic faculty may be present in them as well (Pollick and de Waal 2007; Slocombe and Zuberbuhler 2006). It seems reasonable to assume that our hominin ancestors built upon this rudimentary capacity, incrementally approaching the full-blown symbolism of Homo sapiens. The current paper argues that symbolic thinking can be broken down into a series of progressive stages using the framework proposed by philosopher Charles Peirce. Evidence for the presence of these stages is observable in the archeological record. It is further proposed that the cognitive requirements for increasingly sophisticated tool-manufacture 1 and social cooperation were the key selective forces behind the evolution of symbolic thinking. <A>Incrementally Approaching Symbolism In their book In Search of the Neanderthals, Stringer and Gamble (1993:203) express a strictly dichotomous view of symbolism: “… arranging behaviour according to symbolic codes is an all or nothing situation. The onset of symbolic behaviour can be compared to the flick of a switch…” Symbolism is either there or not, no in-between. This attitude stands in contrast to those expressed recently by two other archeologists. Bar-Yosef (2000:14) has characterized Late Mousterian hominins as possessing a ‘low level of symbolic behavior,’ while Wadley (2001:208) has suggested that Paleolithic image-making varies in its ‘symbolic complexity.’ The conflict between Stringer and Gamble’s view on the one hand, and Bar-Yosef and Wadley’s on the other, may be more apparent than real. It all turns critically on how one defines ‘symbolism.’ One of the most thorough examinations of semiotics (the study of signs and symbols) comes from American philosopher C. S. Peirce (Peirce 1932[1960]; or see Hawkes 1932 for a good discussion). Peirce defined three levels of reference: iconic, indexical, and symbolic. Iconic referents are ones that bear a perceptual or physical resemblance to the things they signify, such as using a round pebble to represent a soccer ball. Indexical referents are ones that indicate (as the name implies) the presence of what they signify based on a temporal or spatial association. For example, a weather vane indicates the wind (when the wind blows, the vane moves), tears indicate sadness, smoke indicates fire, etc. While both iconic and indexical referents can be thought of as ‘symbolic’ in the 2 sense that one thing is standing for another, Peirce reserves the term ‘symbol’ for only those occasions where the relationship between the signifier and the signified is purely arbitrary. For example, the ‘$’ is truly symbolic because its relationship to money is based solely on convention. The $ does not resemble real currency (and therefore is not connected iconicly to its referent) and only rarely occurs in temporal or spatial proximity to currency (and therefore is not connected indexically to its referent). While these levels of reference are distinguishable, they are not necessarily mutually exclusive. A sign can sometimes overlap levels of reference. A smiley face looks like a smiling face, but can also indicate happiness or the intention of conveying a joke (as is the case with electronic messages). The purely arbitrary nature of symbolic associations make them especially taxing to acquire. Deacon (1997:75-92) provides a detailed discussion of the hierarchical relationships among the different levels of reference. He shows how indexical referents are ‘built up’ from iconic ones and symbolic referents are ‘built up’ from indexical ones with each level placing increasing cognitive demands on the organism. Acquired symbolic reference is acutely memory-intensive: To learn the first symbolic relationship requires holding a lot of associations in mind at once while at the same time mentally sampling the potential combinatorial patterns hidden in their higher-order relationships. Even with a very small set of symbols the number of possible combinations is immense, and so sorting out which combinations work and which don’t requires sampling 3 and remembering a large number of possibilities (Deacon 1997:93). Neuroscience studies confirm the notion that extensive ‘brain power’ is required for symbolic function. A broad network of neocortical structures is necessary for the acquisition of the purely arbitrary associations required when learning symbols (Asaad, Rainer, and Miller 1998; Chee, Sriram, Soon, and Lee 2000; Dimitrov et al. 1999; Eacott and Gaffan 1992; Murray, Bussey and Wise 2000; Savage et al. 2000). These structures include the inferior temporal cortex, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and the uncinate fascicles connecting the two. Therefore, while we would expect icons and indices to precede symbols, we would not necessarily expect the move from one to the other to be inevitable, especially when going from indices to symbols. Thus, if by ‘symbolism’ we mean strictly Peircian symbols (arbitrary referents based on cultural convention) then Stringer and Gamble may indeed be correct – this is something that either exists or doesn’t. Peircian symbols, however, do not emerge ex nihlio. Iconic and indexical referents provide the basis for their construction. Bar-Yosef’s ‘low level’ of symbolism and Wadley’s varying degrees of ‘symbolic complexity’ may reflect the presence of iconic and indexical reference systems in the archeological record. All of this raises the possibility that the human mind, with its full-blown capacity for (Peircian) symbolism, was preceded by hominin minds capable of lesser forms of referential thinking (iconic and indexical). There is reason to suspect that this might be the case. Studies of both ape language learning and language development in humans show that they progress from an initial indexical phase to one 4 of symbolic understanding (see discussion in Deacon 1997:85-92,135-142). Thus, we might expect that the emergence of fully symbolic artifacts in the archeological record would be preceded by iconic and indexical ones, providing evidence for the existence of these pre- symbolic hominin minds. <B>Iconic Artifacts Evidence of red ochre and other mineral pigments dates as far back as 300,000 years before present (ybp) in the hominin archeological record (Barham 2002; Clark and Brown 2001; de Lumley 1969; Roebroeks 1986). Barham (2000; 2002), for example, has unearthed over 300 pieces of variously colored mineral pigments dated anywhere from 270,000 – 170,000 ybp from the Twin Rivers site in Zambia. The pigments appear to have been intentionally transported to the site from remote locations and show evidence deliberate modification. Though red ochre and other mineral pigments can have utilitarian uses (Bahn and Vertut 1988; Keeley 1978; 1980), evidence from many of these finds suggests ritual rather than practical use (Knight, Power, and Watts 1995; Watts 2002). Barham (2002:187) notes that the wide variety of different pigments found at the Twin Rivers site argues against purely practical use. If mineral pigments had ritual meaning, then what was the most likely form that meaning took? Our hominin ancestors’ earliest use of pigments, according to Kuhn and Stiner (2007:46), was probably to enhance or alter physical appearance making the wearer more dramatic, impressive or intimidating – as is not uncommon in contemporary traditional societies. Using archeological and anthropological data, Knight, Power, and Watts (1995) have proposed a somewhat different function 5 for early pigments, especially red ochre. In their view, red ochre would have signified menstrual blood and the fertility associated with it. Common to both Kuhn and Stiner’s view and that of Knight et al., is that the pigment’s meaning would have been closely tied to perceptual appearance – either the appearance of the pigment itself (as blood) or the appearance of what it enhances (facial expressions, bodily contours, etc.). All of this suggests that where ritual use of mineral pigments appears likely from the archeological record, its reference value was iconic in nature. In other words, it stood for things by virtue of perceptual cuing. Given that iconic referents are the most basic, it is also not surprising that this is the first form of reference emerging in the archeological record. Mineral pigment use may not be the only example of early iconicity in the archeological record. Though fraught with speculation and controversy, two of the earliest examples of what some researchers have called ‘art’ may also represent iconic referents. A three-million-year-old jasper cobble roughly resembling a human face was uncovered from a site near Makapansgat, South Africa. The nearest source for this item was ten kilometers from the recovery site, leaving open the possibility that it was intentionally transported presumably by a hominin who was intrigue by its anthropomorphic quality (Bednarik 1993; Dart 1974). A similar type artifact was found at Berekhat Ram in the Golan Heights. This was a 233,000 year old human-figure-shaped stone that showed evidence of intentional modification – possibly to enhance its anthropomorphic shape (d'Errico and Nowell 2000; Marshak 1997). The singularity of these finds 6 warrants caution in their interpretation; however, if there is any meaning to them at all it would likely be iconic.
Recommended publications
  • Explanatory Index of Proper Names Other Than Authors Cited
    Quest: An African Journal of Philosophy / Revue Africaine de Philosophie , 24 (2010), 1-2D 1-398 E:planatory inde: of proper names other than authors cited 0xhaustive listing of all proper names other than those of authors cited. 6or names mar2ed with UU also see Inde7 of Authors Cited . Some entries have underlying con- cepts explained under V.v. ( Vuod videre , Esee there=)8 for many other entries this index sei:es the opportunity of explaining details that could not be accommodated in the main text. 1hen a boo2 title is listed, it appears in italic , followed by the author=s name (if any) between parentheses. Inly capitalised text has been processed 9 thus e.g. ECatalyst= is listed but not the many occurrences of Ecatalyst=, Ecatalytic=, etc. Due to last-minute text additions a few page references may be off by 1. identity, 53 227n, 291n8 ,ssyrian ,baris, ,ncient Gree2 Achsenzeit , see ,xial ,ge traces in 9 , 1298 ,rab shaman, 112, 114, 114n ,cragas, 1098 cf. ,grigen- influence on sub- ,b2ha:oids, linguistico- tum Saharan 9, 758 9 and ethnic cluster in the ,dam, Biblical figure, 135 ,ncient Near 0ast, 73, ,ncient 3editerranean, ,donai, .ord, 1608 cf. 1298 9 and North ,mer- 233 ,idoneus ica, 9, 118, 265, 2748 9 ,boriginal ,ustralian, 91, ,egean, Sea and region, and 0urope, 808 and 0ast 1928 cf. Dur2heimUU 115, 141, 151, 175, 294, ,sia, 9 and ,sia, 35, 90, ,braham, Biblical figure, 273n8 9 -,natolian, 226 275, 280, 186n8 1est 162 ,ether, 103-104, 117-118, ,frica, 74, 268, 2818 ,bri du 6acteur, 5pper 130, 137-139, 152, 180- Central ,frica, espe- Palaeolithic site, 189 181, 154n, 165n, 184n8 cially South 9 , 5, 8, 17- ,byss, ,bysmal, 122, ,ether and Day, chil- 18, 31, 43, 62, 64, 70, 164, 236, 101n8 cf.
    [Show full text]
  • Tsodilo Hill, Botswana
    Alec Campbell and Lawrence Robbins Tsodilo Hill, Botswana The Tsodilo Hills in northwestern Botswana were listed as World Heritage in 2001 on account of their prolific rock art, some 4 000 images, 100 000-year occupation by humans, prehistoric specularite mining, early occupation by pastoral Khwe and Black farmers, spiritual importance to their modern residents and unique attributes in a land of endless sand dunes. Tsodilo Hills are situated in a remote region of nium AD farmer villages. Local residents of the the Kalahari Desert in northwest Botswana. Hills, Ju/’hoansi (Bushmen) and Hambukushu The Hills form a 15km-long chain of four (Bantu-speaking farmers) still recognise the quartzite-schist outcrops rising, the highest Hills as sacred. to 400m, above Aeolian sand dunes. The Hills contain evidence of some 100 000 years of Background intermittent human occupations, over 4 000 Tsodilo’s rock paintings, first noted by the rock paintings, groups of cupules, at least 20 outside world in 1898, received legal protec- prehistoric mines, and remains of First Millen- tion only in the late 1930s. The law protecting Fig. 1. Western cliffs that glow a copper colour in evening light giving the Hills their N/ae name, ‘Copper Bracelet of the Evening’. 34 the paintings also prohibited illegal excava- Archaeology tions and damage to, or theft of ‘Bushman Excavations have told us something about (archaeological) relics’, but failed to protect past human occupations of the Hills. White the Hills and their plants or wildlife. Paintings Shelter, excavated to a depth of Research, mainly undertaken by Botswana’s seven metres, has disclosed human-made ar- National Museum, commenced in 1978 with tefacts dating back almost to 100 000 years.
    [Show full text]
  • A Heritage and Cultural Tourism Destination
    MAKING GABORONE A STOP AND NOT A STOP-OVER: A HERITAGE AND CULTURAL TOURISM DESTINATION by Jane Thato Dewah (Student No: 12339556) A Dissertation submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of MAGISTER HEREDITATIS CULTURAEQUE SCIENTIAE (HERITAGE AND CULTURAL STUDIES) (TOURISM) In the Department of Historical and Heritage Studies at the Faculty of Humanities University of Pretoria SUPERVISOR: Prof. K.L Harris December 2014 DECLARATION OF AUTHENTICITY I, the undersigned, hereby declare that the work contained in this thesis is my own original work and has not been submitted previously at any other university for a degree. ............................................... Signature Jane Thato Dewah ................................................ Date ii Abstract The main objective of the study was to identify cultural heritage sites in and around Gaborone which could serve as tourist attractions. Gaborone, the capital city of Botswana, has been neglected in terms of tourism, although it has all the facilities needed to cater for this market. Very little information with regards to tourist attractions around Gaborone is available and therefore this study set out to identify relevant sites and discussed their history, relevance and potential for tourism. It also considers ways in which these sites can be developed in order to attract tourists. Due to its exclusive concentration on wildlife and the wilderness, tourism in Botswana tends to benefit only a few. Moreover, it is mainly concentrated in the north western region of the country, leaving out other parts of the country in terms of the tourism industry. To achieve the main objective of the study, which is to identify sites around the capital city Gaborone and to evaluate if indeed the sites have got the potential to become tourist attractions, three models have been used.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Creating a Better Tourist Experience Through The
    ISSUE SIX (2017) Creating a better tourist experience through the presentation and interpretation of World Heritage Values in Botswana Tshepang Rose Tlatlane Introduction Communities across the world have gradually transformed tourism from time at the beach, sun and sand (Adams 2006), to spending time at World Heritage sites learning about their preservation. There is strong evidence from research that local guides and traditional leaders play a fundamental role in educating tourists about local cultures. Mass tourism regrettably causes a lot of environmental challenges; Ghulam Rabbany et al (2013) confirm the direct impacts of tourism on the environment which include things such as alteration of natural habits, noise and air pollution, loss of biological diversity, and littering. In Botswana, littering is considered one of the eye sore at the famous Okavango Delta. Littering has significantly impacted tourism development in the Okavango Delta and the entire industry in Botswana (Mbaiwa 2004). As tourism is heavily dependent on the quality of the environment, it is crucial for all stakeholders to protect the environment for it to continue being a sustainable economic resource. One of the major issues is the role of mass tourism in environmental protection and sustainable tourism. Is destruction in mass tourism avoidable? A possible answer is Smith’s (1956) market segmentation strategy of grouping tourists in a way that is of most manageable value. Market segmentation results in niche tourism as tourists are divided into smaller groups, according to their needs, behaviours and expectations (Adams 2006). Heritage tourism has observed a significant growth in recognition as a niche in Botswana.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is a Hunter-Gatherer? Variation in the Archaeological Record of Eastern and Southern Africa
    Journal of Archaeological Research, Vol. 13, No. 4, December 2005 (C 2005) DOI: 10.1007/s10814-005-5111-y What Is a Hunter-Gatherer? Variation in the Archaeological Record of Eastern and Southern Africa Sibel B. Kusimba1 This article reviews the recent history of the archaeology of African hunter- gatherers, focusing on debates around the origins of modern humans and the destiny of hunter-gatherers with the advent of food production. African archae- ologists are developing an increasing appreciation for the diversity of African hunter-gatherer societies. Understandings of hunter-gatherers based primarily on ethnography are being successfully challenged and extended. KEY WORDS: Africa; hunter-gatherers; Paleolithic; Middle Stone Age; later Stone Age; food production. INTRODUCTION African examples of hunter-gatherer societies have been paramount in world ethnography (Lee, 1979, 1984; Marshall, 1976; Turnbull, 1962; Woodburn, 1968). African ethnographic cases have also provided useful ethnographic analogy for archaeologists working in many places and time periods (Shott, 1992). Recip- rocally, African archaeologists have sought to provide an ethnographic view of ancient people, focusing on site occupations, settlement patterns, economic activ- ity, and intergroup relationships, at least since the late 1960s (Clark, 1970, p. 80). The effort to excavate “hunter-gatherers” is complicated by the oft-cited poor fit between ethnographic case studies and known archaeological patterns (Parking- ton, 1984; see also Kuhn and Stiner, 2001; Price and Brown, 1985). Ethnographic analogy is still the most important means of interpreting African hunter-gatherer archaeology, although alternatives are well-developed (Winterhalder, 2001). 1Department of Anthropology, Northern Illinois University, De Kalb, Illinois 60115; and The Field Museum, Chicago, Illinois 60605; e-mail: 337 1059-0161/05/1200-0337/0 C 2005 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of Ochre and Painting During the Upper Paleolithic of the Swabian Jura in the Context of the Development of Ochre Use in Africa and Europe
    Open Archaeology 2018; 4: 185–205 Original Study Sibylle Wolf*, Rimtautas Dapschauskas, Elizabeth Velliky, Harald Floss, Andrew W. Kandel, Nicholas J. Conard The Use of Ochre and Painting During the Upper Paleolithic of the Swabian Jura in the Context of the Development of Ochre Use in Africa and Europe https://doi.org/10.1515/opar-2018-0012 Received June 8, 2017; accepted December 13, 2017 Abstract: While the earliest evidence for ochre use is very sparse, the habitual use of ochre by hominins appeared about 140,000 years ago and accompanied them ever since. Here, we present an overview of archaeological sites in southwestern Germany, which yielded remains of ochre. We focus on the artifacts belonging exclusively to anatomically modern humans who were the inhabitants of the cave sites in the Swabian Jura during the Upper Paleolithic. The painted limestones from the Magdalenian layers of Hohle Fels Cave are a particular focus. We present these artifacts in detail and argue that they represent the beginning of a tradition of painting in Central Europe. Keywords: ochre use, Middle Stone Age, Swabian Jura, Upper Paleolithic, Magdalenian painting 1 The Earliest Use of Ochre in the Homo Lineage Modern humans have three types of cone cells in the retina of the eye. These cells are a requirement for trichromatic vision and hence, a requirement for the perception of the color red. The capacity for trichromatic vision dates back about 35 million years, within our shared evolutionary lineage in the Catarrhini subdivision of the higher primates (Jacobs, 2013, 2015). Trichromatic vision may have evolved as a result of the benefits for recognizing ripe yellow, orange, and red fruits in front of a background of green foliage (Regan et al., Article note: This article is a part of Topical Issue on From Line to Colour: Social Context and Visual Communication of Prehistoric Art edited by Liliana Janik and Simon Kaner.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Transmission and Lithic Technology in Middle Stone Age Eastern Africa
    Cultural Transmission and Lithic Technology in Middle Stone Age Eastern Africa by Kathryn L. Ranhorn B.A. in Anthropology, May 2010, University of Florida A Dissertation submitted to The Faculty of The Columbian College of Arts and Sciences of The George Washington University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy August 31, 2017 Dissertation directed by Alison S. Brooks Professor of Anthropology and International Affairs David R. Braun Associate Professor of Anthropology The Columbian College of Arts and Sciences of The George Washington University certifies that Kathryn L. Ranhorn has passed the Final Examination for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy as of May 9th, 2017. This is the final and approved form of the dissertation. Cultural Transmission and Lithic Technology in Middle Stone Age Eastern Africa Kathryn L. Ranhorn Dissertation Research Committee: Alison S. Brooks, Professor of Anthropology and International Affairs, Dissertation Co-Director David R. Braun, Associate Professor of Anthropology, Dissertation Co-Director Francys Subiaul, Associate Professor of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Committee Member Christian A. Tryon, Associate Professor of Anthropology, Harvard University Committee Member ii © Copyright 2017 by Kathryn L. Ranhorn All rights reserved. iii Dedication To my mother, who taught me that evolution requires sometimes copying old ideas, and often creating new ones. Na pia kwa marafiki wangu wa Tanzania na Kenya, hii ndo story zenu. Bado tunapanda. -- “It sometimes appears that all of us treat stone artifacts as infinitely complex repositories of paleocultural information and assume that it is only the imperfections of our present analytical systems that prevent us from decoding them.
    [Show full text]
  • Concerning a Cupule Sequence on the Edge of the Kalahari Desert in South Africa
    Rock Art Research 2015 - Volume 32, Number 2, pp. 163-177. P. B. BEAUMONT and R. G. BEDNARIK 163 KEYWORDS: Cupule – Age estimate – Palaeoenvironment – Tswalu Kalahari Reserve – South Africa CONCERNING A CUPULE SEQUENCE ON THE EDGE OF THE KALAHARI DESERT IN SOUTH AFRICA Peter B. Beaumont and Robert G. Bednarik Abstract. The Tswalu Reserve in the southern Kalahari is an arid place, the present occupation of which is only made possible by means of boreholes that tap patches of fossil water, while semi-permanent surface sources of ~65 m2 extent are confined to three localities within an investigated area of over 1000 km2. Lithic evidence indicates that this vicinity was abandoned by humans during even drier Ice Age intervals, when rainfall fell at times to ~40% of present values, thereby providing a way to refer petroglyphs there to interglacials of known age and intensity in terms of regional and global paleaoclimatic data. By such means, together with microerosion measurements, it then becomes possible to identify three regional cupule production intervals: the earliest with cupules only at ~410–400 ka bp, the next with cupules and outline circles at ~130–115 ka ago, and the most recent, with cupules, geometric motifs and iconic images, at ~8–2 ka bp. Introduction then, but the later dating of various regional sites (Miller Cupules are manmade, roughly semi-hemispherical 1971; Sampson 1974) placed the artefact level between depressions, not normally more than ~8 cm in diame- ~25 and 13 cal ka ago (Weninger and Jöris 2008), and it ter, that were produced on hard rock surfaces by is consequently considered probable that the Chifubwa hammerstone percussion (Kumar and Krishna 2014), petroglyphs were made at some time within that reportedly supplemented or replaced on softer stones interval (Clark 1958).
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Experimental Knapping in Empirically Testing Key Themes in the Evolution of Lithic Technology: Reduction Intensity, Efficiency and Behavioural Complexity
    The role of experimental knapping in empirically testing key themes in the evolution of lithic technology: reduction intensity, efficiency and behavioural complexity Antoine Muller BA (archaeology) BA Honours (archaeology) A thesis submitted for the degree of Master of Philosophy at The University of Queensland in 2017 School of Social Science Abstract Experimental knapping has complimented and stimulated lithic analyses for over a century. Throughout this period, the discipline has witnessed an increase in the scientific rigour and theoretical grounding with which these studies are conducted. This thesis charts these key trends and in doing so establishes a best-practice model of experimental knapping, the veracity of which is in turn tested using four new lithic experiments. These case-studies employ experimental knapping to advance our understanding of flake platform measurement, reduction intensity, technological efficiency, and behavioural complexity. The first case-study, Chapter 3, offers a more accurate and precise calliper-based method of flake platform measurement that relies on simple geometric approximations of platform shape rather than the inflexible and unreliable existing method of multiplying platform width by thickness. In Chapter 4, a new reduction intensity metric for backed blades, a hitherto overlooked tool-type, is developed and tested on the backed blades from an early Neolithic site in Turkey. This new metric allows a reconstruction of the raw material consumption patterns at the site, finding that the backed blades likely contributed to conserving the inhabitants’ scarce lithic raw material. Meanwhile, Chapter 5 outlines the results of a comparison of the raw material efficiency of eight different lithic technologies, finding that lithic technological efficiency was a generally ascending trend over the last 3.3 million years and that the main transition in efficiency occurred between the Lower to Middle Palaeolithic.
    [Show full text]
  • Figures Kristen Et Al Proof
    Originally published as: Kristen, I., Fuhrmann, A., Thorpe, J., Röhl, U., Wilkes, H., Oberhänsli, H. (2007): Hydrological changes in southern Africa over the last 200 Ka as recorded in lake sediments from the Tswaing impact crater. - South African Journal of Geology, 110, 2-3, 311-326, DOI: 10.2113/gssajg.110.2-3.311. Hydrological changes in southern Africa over the last 200 kyr as recorded in lake sediments from the Tswaing impact crater I. Kristen GeoForschungsZentrum Potsdam, Telegrafenberg, D-14473 Potsdam, Germany, [email protected] A. Fuhrmann GeoForschungsZentrum Potsdam, Telegrafenberg, D-14473 Potsdam, Germany Present address: Saudi Aramco, Dhahran 31311, Saudi Arabia, [email protected] J. Thorpe formerly at Department of Geography, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP, Great Britain, [email protected] U. Röhl Center for Marine Environmental Sciences (MARUM), Universität Bremen, Leobener Strasse, 28359 Bremen, Germany, [email protected] H. Wilkes GeoForschungsZentrum Potsdam, Telegrafenberg, D-14473 Potsdam, Germany, [email protected] and H. Oberhänsli GeoForschungsZentrum Potsdam, Telegrafenberg, D-14473 Potsdam, Germany, [email protected] Short working title: Hydrological changes recorded in sediments from Lake Tswaing (South Africa) Abstract Sediments from Lake Tswaing (25°24’30’’ S, 28°04’59’’ E) document hydrological changes in southern Africa over the last 200 kyr. Using high-resolution XRF- scanning, basic geochemistry (TIC, TOC, TN), organic petrology and Rock-Eval pyrolysis, we identify intervals of decreased carbonate precipitation, increased detrital input, decreased salinity and decreased autochthonous (algal and bacterial) organic matter content that represent periods of less stable water column stratification and increased rainfall.
    [Show full text]
  • World Bank Document
    Environmentally Sustainable AFTESWORKING PAPER DevelopmentDivision v- EnvironmentalAssessment Working Paper No.4 Public Disclosure Authorized CULTURAL PROPERTY AND ENVIRONMENTAL ASSESSMENTS IN Public Disclosure Authorized SUB-SAHARAN AFRICA A Handbook Public Disclosure Authorized by June Taboroff and Cynthia C. Cook September 1993 Public Disclosure Authorized Environmentally Sustainable Development Division Africa Technical Department The World Bank EnvironmentalAssessment Working Paper No. 4 Cultural Property and Environmental Assessments in Sub-Saharan Africa: A Handbook by June Taboroff and Cynthia C. Cook September1993 EnvironmentallySustainable Development Division SocialPolicy and Resettlement Division TechnicalDepartment EnvironmentDepartment AfricaRegion TheWorld Bank Thispaper has been prepared for internal use. The views and interpretabonsarethose of the authors and should not be attri- butedto the World Bank, to itsaffiliated organizations, orto any individualacting on their behalf. PREFACE The use of environmentalassessment (EA) to identifythe environmentalconsequences of developmentprojects and to take these consequencesinto account in project design is one of the World Bank's most important tools for ensuring that developmentstrategies are environmentally sound and sustainable. The protectionof cultural heritage - sites, structures, artifacts, and remains of archaeological,historical, religious, cultural, or aesthetic value - is one importantobjective of the EA process. The Bank's EA procedures require its Borrowersto undertakecultural
    [Show full text]
  • WHV – Tsodilo Hills Maintenance Project, BOTSWANA
    WHV – Tsodilo Hills Maintenance Project, BOTSWANA Tsodilo, Botswana 17 June – 05 July 2013 Cultural site inscribed in 2001 With one of the highest concentrations of rock art in the world, Tsodilo has been called the ''Louvre of the Desert''. Over 4,500 paintings are preserved in an area of only 10 km2 of the Kalahari Desert. The archaeological record of the area gives a chronological account of human activities and environmental changes over at least 100,000 years. Local communities in this hostile environment respect Tsodilo as a place of worship frequented by ancestral spirits. © UNESCO Cultural site inscribed in 2001 Project objectives: The project aims at preserving and protecting the culture, such as the signs of origin of the First People of The Kalahari (as the Tsodilo Hills provide evidence to the historical lives of the San). It also aims at conserving the environment, since the Tsodilo Hills provide home to a wide range of birds, plants and other forms of wildlife. Thus conserving the environment is very significant. Furthermore, the project aims at motivating the local communities in remote areas to develop a greater interest in the preservation of culture, history and the environment. Project activities: During the project duration, volunteers will engage in various activities. They will participate in the expansion of service roads to Tsodilo Hills and the Museum, to ensure the access of the site. They will also participate in the re-establishment of trails and footpaths from the Museum to the Hills. The volunteers will have community meetings and various games, and motivate the Tsodilo Community to form local self-help groups that will ensure preservation and protection of culture and the environment.
    [Show full text]