COMPLEX SOUNDS and CRITICAL BANDS1 BERTRAM SCHARF Northeastern University

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

COMPLEX SOUNDS and CRITICAL BANDS1 BERTRAM SCHARF Northeastern University Psychological Bulletin 1961, Vol. 58, No. 3, 205-217 COMPLEX SOUNDS AND CRITICAL BANDS1 BERTRAM SCHARF Northeastern University Studies of the responses of human given by the top curve in Figure 1. observers to bands of noise and other The ordinate gives the width (AF), complex sounds have led to the meas- in cycles per second, of the critical ure of what appears to be a basic unit band; the abscissa gives the center of hearing, the critical band. When frequency. As the frequency at the the frequency spectrum of a stimu- center of a complex sound increases, lating sound is narrower than the the critical band that is measured critical band, the ear reacts one way; around the center frequency becomes when the spectrum is wider, it reacts wider. another way. For example, experi- Not only does the critical band ments show that at values less than have the same values when measured the critical bandwidth, both loud- for several kinds of auditory response, ness and absolute threshold are in- it is also independent of such stimu- dependent of bandwidth; only when lus parameters as the number of the critical bandwidth is exceeded components in the complex (Scharf, do the loudness and the absolute 1959b) and the sound pressure level threshold increase with the width (Feldtkeller, 1955; Feldtkeller & (Gassier, 1954; Zwicker & Feldtkel- Zwicker, 1956). ler, 1955; Zwicker, Flottorp, & Prior to the experimental measures Stevens, 1957). of the critical band, Fletcher (1940) The critical band has also been had hypothesized the existence of a measured in experiments on auditory critical band for masking. He sug- discriminations that seem to depend gested that when a white noise just upon phase (Zwicker, 1952) and in masks a tone, only a relatively nar- experiments on the masking of a row band of frequencies surrounding narrow band of noise by two tones the tone does the masking, energy (Zwicker, 1954). In all four types of outside the band contributing little experiment—loudness, threshold, or nothing. Although attempts to sensitivity to phase, and two-tone test this hypothesis remain incon- masking—the value of the critical clusive, investigators (Bilger & Hirsh, band is the same function of its 1956; Hawkins & Stevens, 1950) have center frequency. The values of the been able to calculate values for the critical band, as a function of the fre- width of these hypothetical masking quency at the center of the band, are bands by assuming that the masking band and the just-masked tone have 1 This paper was completed while the author the same intensity. The calculated held a research grant, B2223, from the Na- values, which are labeled "critical tional Institute of Health, United States Pub- lic Health Service. The author wishes to ratios" in Figure 1, are smaller for the thank S. S. Stevens for detailed criticism and masking band than for the critical advice in the final preparation of the manu- band as measured in the experiments script. A. B. Warren and H. S. Zamansky are also thanked for reading and making sugges- cited above. As we shall see, this dis- tions about the final draft. crepancy is more apparent than real. 205 206 BERTRAM SCHARF 5000 ures of this phenomenon, using many tones and systematically varying the 2000 difference in frequency, AF, between o'ooo the lowest and highest components of the complex sounds.2 He varied the 500 AF by varying the number of equally intense tones, which were spaced at intervals of 20 cps. The number of tones was increased from 1 to 40 or until AF was equal to 780 cps. Each time a tone was added, the threshold CRITICAL RATIO for the whole complex was measured by a "tracking" method (Stevens, 1958). It was necessary, of course, 50 100 ZOO 500 1000 2000 5000 that all the tones in the complex have FREQUENCY IN CYCLES PER SECOND the same threshold when heard FIG. 1. The width, AF, of the critical band singly, for otherwise it would have and of the critical ratio as a function of the been impossible to determine the frequency at the center of the band. (The or- dinate gives the width, in cycles per second, precise cause of a change in the of the critical band—and of the critical ratio— threshold for a complex whose AF for the center frequencies shown on the ab- had been increased by the addition scissa. The top curve gives the values for the of a tone. Thus measurements were critical band which are based upon direct restricted to portions of the fre- measurements in four types of experiment; the bottom curve gives the values for the quency spectrum over which a sub- critical ratio which are calculated from meas- ject's threshold curve was flat. In urements of the masked threshold for pure order to study other portions of the tones in white noise. The points on the bottom spectrum, the multitone complexes curve are from Hawkins and Stevens—1950. This figure is adapted from an article by were presented against a background Zwicker, Flottorp, and Stevens—1957, p. 556 of white noise that had been tailored —which contains also a table of critical-band to raise the threshold for tones at all values.) (Adapted with permission of the the audible frequencies to the same Journal of the Acoustical Society of America') level, thus artificially flattening a subject's threshold curve. EXPERIMENTAL MEASURES OF THE Whether the background was CRITICAL BAND quiet, or consisted of a noise at 0 db. Four types of experiment in which SPL, at 20 db., or at 40 db., the critical bands have been measured same effect was noted: as soon as are reviewed: absolute threshold of AF exceeded a particular value whose complex sounds, masking of a band size depended upon the frequency at of noise by two tones, sensitivity to the center of the complex, the thresh- phase differences, and loudness. old for the multitone complex began to increase. Similar data were re- Threshold of Complex Sounds ported when bands of white noise When two tones, whose frequencies were substituted for the multitone are not too far apart, are presented simultaneously, a subject may report 2 Two or more tones constitute a complex hearing a sound even though either sound, i.e., a sound with energy at more than one frequency in contrast to a single or pure tone by itself is below threshold. tone with most of its energy concentrated at a Gassier (1954) made careful meas- single frequency. COMPLEX SOUNDS AND CRITICAL BANDS 207 complexes. The results indicate that Sensitivity to Phase the total energy necessary for a sound The critical band is also relevant to be heard remains constant so long to phase sensitivity, measured by a as the energy is contained within a comparison between the ear's ability limiting bandwidth. Although differ- to detect amplitude modulation ences between the two observers in (AM) and its ability to detect fre- these experiments were sometimes of quency modulation (FM). This pro- the order of 40%, the average size of cedure requires some explanation. the limiting bandwidths for both When the amplitude of a tone is multitone complexes and bands of modulated—i.e., alternately in- noise is approximated by the critical- 8 creased and decreased—a three-tone band curve of Figure I. complex is produced with the original Two-Tone Masking tone (the "carrier") at the center of the complex and a tone on either The masking of a narrow-band side (side bands). When the frequency noise by two tones provided a second of a tone is modulated over a narrow measure of the critical band. Using range, a three-tone complex is also a tracking method, Zwicker (1954) produced.4 The only important dif- measured the threshold of a narrow- ference between the three-tone com- band noise in the presence of two plex that is produced under AM and tones, one on either side of the noise. the complex that is produced under Increasing the difference in fre- FM concerns the phase relations quency, AF, between the two tones among the components. Conse- left the masked threshold for the quently, any difference in the ear's noise unchanged until a critical AF sensitivity to AM and FM would was reached, whereupon the thresh- presumably depend upon these phase old fell sharply and, in general, con- relations. tinued to fall as AF was increased Zwicker (1952) found, indeed, that further. The two subjects who served in order for a subject to just hear a in this experiment showed the same difference between a modulated and drop in threshold at approximately a pure, unmodulated tone, a smaller the same AF for a given center fre- amount of AM is required than FM. quency regardless of the SPL of the The ear is more sensitive to AM than masking tones. The critical-band to FM, however, only at low rates of curve of Figure 1 gives the approxi- modulation. As the rate of modula- mate values of AF at which the mask- tion is increased, the difference in ing effect of two tones is sharply re- sensitivity to AM and FM gradually duced. disappears. How do these results pertain to the critical band? The rate * GSssler (1954) measured a critical band of at which a tone is modulated deter- 165 cps at 1000 cps. Garner (1947) had writ- ten earlier that "the best estimate ... is that mines the frequency separation, AF, a band of frequencies no wider than 175 cps between the side bands of the three- around 1000 cps is necessary if temporal in- tone complex produced under the tegration of acoustic energy is to be perfect" modulation.
Recommended publications
  • Modeling of the Head-Related Transfer Functions for Reduced Computation and Storage Charles Duncan Lueck Iowa State University
    Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Retrospective Theses and Dissertations Dissertations 1995 Modeling of the head-related transfer functions for reduced computation and storage Charles Duncan Lueck Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd Part of the Electrical and Electronics Commons Recommended Citation Lueck, Charles Duncan, "Modeling of the head-related transfer functions for reduced computation and storage " (1995). Retrospective Theses and Dissertations. 10929. https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/10929 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INFORMATION TO USERS This mamiscr^t has been reproduced fFom the microfilin master. UMI films the text directly from the origmal or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from ai^ type of con^ter printer. Hie qnallQr of this repfodnction is dqiendent opon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, prim bleedthroug^ substandard marginc^ and in^^rqper alignment can adverse^ affect rq)roduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyri^t material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g^ maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to light in equal sections with small overk^ Each original is also photogrs^hed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the bade of the book.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Study Guide for Unit 5 (Hearing, Audition, Music and Speech). Be
    Psychology of Perception Lewis O. Harvey, Jr.–Instructor Psychology 4165-100 Andrew J. Mertens –Teaching Assistant Spring 2021 11:10–12:25 Tuesday, Thursday Study Guide for Unit 5 (hearing, audition, music and speech). Be able to answer the following questions and be familiar with the concepts involved in the answers. Review your textbook reading, lectures, homework and lab assignments and be familiar with the concepts included in them. 1. Diagram the three parts of the auditory system: Outer, middle and inner ear. How is sound mapped onto the basilar membrane? Know these terms: pinna, concha, external auditory canal, tympanic membrane, maleus, incus, stapes, semicircular canals, oval and round windows, cochlea, basilar membrane, organ of Corti, Eustachian tube, inner, outer hair cells. 2. What are the three main physical dimensions of the sound stimulus? What are the three main psychological dimensions of the sound experience? What are the relationships and interdependencies among them? 3. How does the basilar membrane carry out a frequency analysis of the physical sound stimulus? 4. What are roles of the inner and the outer hair cells on the organ of Corti? 5. What is the critical band? Describe three different methods for measuring the critical band. 6. According to Plomp and Levelt (1965), how far apart in frequency must two sine wave tones be in order to sound maximally unpleasant? Why do some musical notes (e.g., the octave or the fifth) sound consonant when played together with the tonic and some other notes (e.g., the second or the seventh) sound dissonant when played with the tonic? 7.
    [Show full text]
  • The Human Ear  Hearing, Sound Intensity and Loudness Levels
    UIUC Physics 406 Acoustical Physics of Music The Human Ear Hearing, Sound Intensity and Loudness Levels We’ve been discussing the generation of sounds, so now we’ll discuss the perception of sounds. Human Senses: The astounding ~ 4 billion year evolution of living organisms on this planet, from the earliest single-cell life form(s) to the present day, with our current abilities to hear / see / smell / taste / feel / etc. – all are the result of the evolutionary forces of nature associated with “survival of the fittest” – i.e. it is evolutionarily{very} beneficial for us to be able to hear/perceive the natural sounds that do exist in the environment – it helps us to locate/find food/keep from becoming food, etc., just as vision/sight enables us to perceive objects in our 3-D environment, the ability to move /locomote through the environment enhances our ability to find food/keep from becoming food; Our sense of balance, via a stereo-pair (!) of semi-circular canals (= inertial guidance system!) helps us respond to 3-D inertial forces (e.g. gravity) and maintain our balance/avoid injury, etc. Our sense of taste & smell warn us of things that are bad to eat and/or breathe… Human Perception of Sound: * The human ear responds to disturbances/temporal variations in pressure. Amazingly sensitive! It has more than 6 orders of magnitude in dynamic range of pressure sensitivity (12 orders of magnitude in sound intensity, I p2) and 3 orders of magnitude in frequency (20 Hz – 20 KHz)! * Existence of 2 ears (stereo!) greatly enhances 3-D localization of sounds, and also the determination of pitch (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • A Biological Rationale for Musical Consonance Daniel L
    PERSPECTIVE PERSPECTIVE A biological rationale for musical consonance Daniel L. Bowlinga,1 and Dale Purvesb,1 aDepartment of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, 1090 Vienna, Austria; and bDuke Institute for Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708 Edited by Solomon H. Snyder, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, and approved June 25, 2015 (received for review March 25, 2015) The basis of musical consonance has been debated for centuries without resolution. Three interpretations have been considered: (i) that consonance derives from the mathematical simplicity of small integer ratios; (ii) that consonance derives from the physical absence of interference between harmonic spectra; and (iii) that consonance derives from the advantages of recognizing biological vocalization and human vocalization in particular. Whereas the mathematical and physical explanations are at odds with the evidence that has now accumu- lated, biology provides a plausible explanation for this central issue in music and audition. consonance | biology | music | audition | vocalization Why we humans hear some tone combina- perfect fifth (3:2), and the perfect fourth revolution in the 17th century, which in- tions as relatively attractive (consonance) (4:3), ratios that all had spiritual and cos- troduced a physical understanding of musi- and others as less attractive (dissonance) has mological significance in Pythagorean phi- cal tones. The science of sound attracted been debated for over 2,000 years (1–4). losophy (9, 10). many scholars of that era, including Vincenzo These perceptual differences form the basis The mathematical range of Pythagorean and Galileo Galilei, Renee Descartes, and of melody when tones are played sequen- consonance was extended in the Renaissance later Daniel Bernoulli and Leonard Euler.
    [Show full text]
  • Laminar Fine Structure of Frequency Organization in Auditory Midbrain
    letters to nature ICC from edge to edge17–19. Anatomical studies indicate that a prominent structural feature of the ICC is a preferred orientation of Laminar fine structure of its principal neurons which are arranged in distinctive, parallel fibrodendritic laminae with a thickness of 120 to 180 mm (refs 20– frequency organization 25). In the cat ICC, this allows for 30 to 45 stacked anatomical laminae. They seem to be of functional significance because laminae in auditory midbrain defined by electrophysiology17–19,25,26, by activity-dependent markers27,28, or by neuroanatomical methods20–25 are of similar Christoph E. Schreiner* & Gerald Langner† spatial orientation and extent. * W. M. Keck Center for Integrative Neuroscience, Sloan Center for Theoretical We consider that a layered but largely continuous frequency Neuroscience, Coleman Laboratory, 513 Parnassus Avenue, University of organization exists in the ICC and that it is the neuronal substrate California at San Francisco, San Francisco, California 94143-0732, USA at which critical bands emerge. The spatial relationship of a † Zoologisches Institut, Technische Hochschule Darmstadt, D-64287 Darmstadt, laminated anatomical structure and a specific distribution of func- Germany tional properties within and across laminae, such as complex local ......................................................................................................................... inhibition and integration networks, appears to be particularly The perception of sound is based on signal processing by a bank
    [Show full text]
  • The Speech Critical Band (S-Cb) in Cochlear Implant Users: Frequency Resolution Employed During the Reception of Everyday Speech
    THE SPEECH CRITICAL BAND (S-CB) IN COCHLEAR IMPLANT USERS: FREQUENCY RESOLUTION EMPLOYED DURING THE RECEPTION OF EVERYDAY SPEECH Capstone Project Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Doctor of Audiology In the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Jason Wigand, B.A. ***** The Ohio State University 2013 Capstone Committee: Approved by: Eric W. Healy, Ph.D. - Advisor Eric C. Bielefeld, Ph.D. Christy M. Goodman, Au.D. _______________________________ Advisor © Copyright by Jason Wigand 2013 Abstract It is widely recognized that cochlear implant (CI) users have limited spectral resolution and that this represents a primary limitation. In contrast to traditional measures, Healy and Bacon [(2006) 119, J. Acoust. Soc. Am.] established a procedure for directly measuring the spectral resolution employed during processing of running speech. This Speech-Critical Band (S-CB) reflects the listeners’ ability to extract spectral detail from an acoustic speech signal. The goal of the current study was to better determine the resolution that CI users are able to employ when processing speech. Ten CI users between the ages of 32 and 72 years using Cochlear Ltd. devices participated. The original standard recordings from the Hearing In Noise Test (HINT) were filtered to a 1.5-octave band, which was then partitioned into sub-bands. Spectral information was removed from each partition and replaced with an amplitude-modulated noise carrier band; the modulated carriers were then summed for presentation. CI subject performance increased with increasing spectral resolution (increasing number of partitions), never reaching asymptote. This result stands in stark contrast to expectation, as it indicates that increases in spectral resolution up to that of normal hearing produced increases in performance.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychoacoustics: a Brief Historical Overview
    Psychoacoustics: A Brief Historical Overview William A. Yost From Pythagoras to Helmholtz to Fletcher to Green and Swets, a centu- ries-long historical overview of psychoacoustics. Postal: Speech and Hearing Science 1 Arizona State University What is psychoacoustics? Psychoacous- PO Box 870102 tics is the psychophysical study of acous- Tempe, Arizona 85287 tics. OK, psychophysics is the study of USA the relationship between sensory per- ception (psychology) and physical vari- Email: ables (physics), e.g., how does perceived [email protected] loudness (perception) relate to sound pressure level (physical variable)? “Psy- chophysics” was coined by Gustav Fech- ner (Figure 1) in his book Elemente der Psychophysik (1860; Elements of Psycho- physics).2 Fechner’s treatise covered at least three topics: psychophysical meth- ods, psychophysical relationships, and panpsychism (the notion that the mind is a universal aspect of all things and, as Figure 1. Gustav Fechner (1801-1887), “Father of Psychophysics.” such, panpsychism rejects the Cartesian dualism of mind and body). Today psychoacoustics is usually broadly described as auditory perception or just hearing, although the latter term also includes the biological aspects of hearing (physiological acoustics). Psychoacoustics includes research involving humans and nonhuman animals, but this review just covers human psychoacoustics. With- in the Acoustical Society of America (ASA), the largest Technical Committee is Physiological and Psychological Acoustics (P&P), and Psychological Acoustics is psychoacoustics. 1 Trying to summarize the history of psychoacoustics in about 5,000 words is a challenge. I had to make difficult decisions about what to cover and omit. My history stops around 1990. I do not cover in any depth speech, music, animal bioacoustics, physiological acoustics, and architectural acoustic topics because these may be future Acoustics Today historical overviews.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychoacoustics & Hearing
    Abstract: In this lecture, the basic concepts of sound and acoustics are introduced as they relate to human hearing. Basic perceptual hearing effects such as loudness, masking, and binaural localization are described in simple terms. The anatomy of the ear and the hearing function are explained. The causes and consequences of hearing loss are shown along with strategies for compensation and suggestions for prevention. Examples are given throughout illustrating the relation between the signal processing performed by the ear and relevant consumer audio technology, as well as its perceptual consequences. Christopher J. Struck CJS Labs San Francisco, CA 19 November 2014 Frequency is perceived as pitch or tone. An octave is a doubling of frequency. A decade is a ten times increase in frequency. Like amplitude, frequency perception is also relative. Therefore, a logarithmic frequency scale is commonly used in acoustics. The normal range for healthy young humans is 20 –20 kHz. The frequency range of fundamental tones on the piano is 27 to 4186 Hz, as shown. The ranges of other instruments and voices are shown. The Threshold of Hearing at the bottom of the graph is the lowest level pure tone that can be heard at any given frequency. The Threshold of Discomfort, at the top of the graph represents the loudest tolerable level at any frequency. The shaded centre area is the range of level and frequency for speech. The larger central area is the range for music. Level Range is also commonly referred to as Dynamic Range. The subjective or perceived loudness of sound is determined by several complex factors, primarily Masking and Hearing Acuity.
    [Show full text]
  • Musical Acoustics the Human
    Musical Acoustics Lecture 11 The Human Ear - I Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 1 Anatomy of Ear The human ear is a highly sensitive sound receptor in which pressure fluctuations in the outer ear are transformed into vibrations of small bones (the ossicles) in the middle ear that are ultimately communicated to the cochlea located in the inner ear, where the vibrations are further transformed by stereocilia (hair cells) into neural impulses distributed by frequency. Middle Ear Outer Ear Inner Ear 2 Outer ear Pinna – (the feather) Pinna matches ear canal to outside world. Meatus – ( the passageway) conducts sound into head. Meatus Tympanum – (the drum) Tympanic transforms pressure membrane fluctuations into displacement. 3 Middle ear – The ossicles (little bones) Malleus ― (the hammer) Incus moved by Tympanum. Malleus Incus ― (the anvil) supported by ligaments that protect against loud percussion. Stapes ― (the stirrup) force multiplied by 1.3 Stapes because of lever action. 4 Tympanum (ear drum) (view from inside) Tympamium Malleus and ligaments 5 The Ossicles Malleus Incus Malleus Stapes Tympanum Incus Stapes Malleus Incus Stapes 6 Inner Ear Cochlea – (the Snail) converts displacement into neural impulses. Semicircular Canals Auditory Nerve – neural impulses to brain Cochlea Oval Window Semicircular canals – detect motion and orientation Round Window Cochlea (micrograph) “The Snail” • o~ oval window • r~ round window 2 mm 7 Structure of Cochlea 1. Spiral cone 2. Divided by Basilar Membrane 3. In on top half 4. Out on bottom 5. “Sloshing “ Basilar Membrane ↑Organ of Corti Auditory Nerve 8 ↑ Structure of Cochlea Stereocilia (Hair Cells) Outer Hair Cell 9 ↑ Structure of Cochlea Connection to nerves Synapse ⇘ Afferent Efferent Outer Vibration Hair Cells Organ of Corti and Basilar Membrane ↑ Inner Hair Cells 10 Detail of Hair Cell Hair cells have nothing to do with hair that grows in the skin.
    [Show full text]
  • Binaural Response Organization Within a Frequency-Band Representation of the Inferior Colliculus: Implications for Sound Localization
    The Journal of Neuroscience April 1986, 6(4): 962-973 Binaural Response Organization Within a Frequency-Band Representation of the Inferior Colliculus: Implications for Sound Localization Jeffrey J. Wenstrup, Linda S. Ross, and George D. Pollak Department of Zoology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712 The auditory system of the mustache hat (Pteronotus parnellii) 1968; Guinan et al., 1972; Rose et al., 1959; Tsuchitani, 1977) contains a disproportionately large representation of a narrow yet their topographically ordered projections converge on a sin- frequency band, corresponding to the dominant, 60 kHz com- gle tonotopic organization in the ICC (Fitzpatrick, 1975; Mer- ponent of its echolocation signal. In the inferior colliculus (IC), zenith and Reid, 1974; Semple and Aitkin, 1979; Serviere et the 60 kHz representation comprises an architectonically dis- al., 1984). Collicular neurons that are most sensitive to nearly tinct region called the dorsoposterior division (DPD), which is the same frequency of sound are grouped within characteristic accessible for detailed physiological study. We examined the laminae of the ICC (Merzenich and Reid, 1974; Morest, 1964; topographic distribution of binaural responses within this one Oliver and Mores& 1984; Rockel and Jones, 1973; Semple and frequency-band representation of the inferior colliculus. Aitkin, 1979; Serviere et al., 1984). These frequency-band lam- We describe two primary results. First, neurons with different inae receive convergent input from the corresponding frequen- binaural response properties are spatially segregated into one cy-band representations of brain stem auditory nuclei (Brunso- of four binaural response-specific regions of the DPD: a large Bechtold et al., 1981; Roth et al., 1978; Zook and Casseday, region of monaural (EO) responses; two regions containing neu- 1982b).
    [Show full text]
  • Neurophysiological Correlates of the Critical Bandwidth in the Human Auditory System
    Brigham Young University BYU ScholarsArchive Theses and Dissertations 2015-11-01 Neurophysiological Correlates of the Critical Bandwidth in the Human Auditory System Grace Ann Bentley Brigham Young University - Provo Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd Part of the Communication Sciences and Disorders Commons BYU ScholarsArchive Citation Bentley, Grace Ann, "Neurophysiological Correlates of the Critical Bandwidth in the Human Auditory System" (2015). Theses and Dissertations. 5619. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/5619 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Neurophysiological Correlates of the Critical Bandwidth in the Human Auditory System Grace Ann Bentley A thesis submitted to the faculty of Brigham Young University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science David McPherson, Chair Richard Harris Martin Fujiki Department of Communication Disorders Brigham Young University November 2015 Copyright © 2015 Grace Ann Bentley All Rights Reserved ABSTRACT Neurophysiological Correlates of the Critical Bandwidth in the Human Auditory System Grace Ann Bentley Department of Communication Disorders, BYU Master of Science The critical bandwidth (CBW) is an auditory phenomenon that has been used to study various aspects of auditory processing, including auditory masking, complex tone processing, and loudness perception. Although the psychoacoustic aspects of the CBW have been well studied, the underlying neurophysiology of the CBW has not been as thoroughly examined. The current study examined the neurophysiology of the CBW in young adults, as well as loudness perception in response to the CBW.
    [Show full text]
  • The Spiral Staircase: Tonotopic Microstructure and Cochlear Tuning
    The Journal of Neuroscience, March 18, 2015 • 35(11):4683–4690 • 4683 Systems/Circuits The Spiral Staircase: Tonotopic Microstructure and Cochlear Tuning X Christopher A. Shera Eaton-Peabody Laboratories, Massachusetts Eye and Ear, Boston, Massachusetts 02114 Although usually assumed to be smooth and continuous, mammalian cochlear frequency-position maps are predicted to manifest a staircase-like structure comprising plateaus of nearly constant characteristic frequency separated by abrupt discontinuities. The height and width of the stair steps are determined by parameters of cochlear frequency tuning and vary with location in the cochlea. The step height is approximately equal to the bandwidth of the auditory filter (critical band), and the step width matches that of the spatial excitation pattern produced by a low-level pure tone. Stepwise tonotopy is an emergent property arising from wave reflection and interference within the cochlea, the same mechanisms responsible for the microstructure of the hearing threshold. Possible relationships between the microstructure of the cochlear map and the tiered tonotopy observed in the inferior colliculus are explored. Key words: basilar membrane; cochlear tonotopy; frequency selectivity; inferior colliculus; otoacoustic emissions Introduction Materials and Methods The cochlear tonotopic map relates the local characteristic fre- BM transfer functions were computed using an active, transmission-line quency (CF) to position within the cochlea. In many animals, model of the cochlea based on the “double-pole” form of the BM admit- including humans, the CF, defined as the frequency of maximum tance obtained by Zweig (1991). Parameter values and other details of the sensitivity (minimum threshold) at low sound levels, is thought implementation have been described previously (Shera and Cooper, to vary quasiexponentially with position, with higher frequencies 2013).
    [Show full text]