7 X 11.5 Long Title.P65

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

7 X 11.5 Long Title.P65 Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-15257-0 - Animal Camouflage: Mechanisms and Function Edited by Martin Stevens and Sami Merilaita Index More information Index acoustic crypsis, 331–336 186, 192, 193, 202, 214, 219, 238, 239, 240, addax, Addax nasomaculatus, 312 241, 245, 246, 248, 260, 261, 265, 266, 275, African hunting dog, Lyacon pictus, 311 299, 300, 307, 309, 314, 321, 338, 342 African linsang, Poiana richardsoni, 311 compromise background matching, 22, 23 Allocyclosa bifurca, 258 compromise strategy, 7 alpine grasshopper, Kosciuscola tristis, 244 general protective resemblance, 7 Amazonian manatee, Trichechus inunguis, 314 specialisation–compromise continuum, 22, 29 American badger, Taxidea taxus, 303 specialist strategy, 7 angelfish, Centropyge bicolor, 200 banded palm civet, Hemilagus derbyanus, 309 angelfish, Pomacanthidae, 198, 199 bark beetle, Dendroctonus pseudotsugata, 345 angelfish, Pygoplites diacanthus, 200 bark beetle, Ips pini, 339 anglerfish, Antennariidae, 193 barracuda, Sphyraena helleri, 205 Angolan black-and-white colobus, Colobus bats, 129, 312, 331, 332, 335 angolensis, 307 constant absolute target direction strategy Antarctic minke whale, Balaenoptera bonaerensis, (CATD), 129 314 behavioural orientation, 114 apatetic coloration, 81, See background matching 102, 113, See also behaviourally mediated Apis mellifica, 262 crypsis aposematism, 4, 227, 230, 242, 259, 298, 299, 300, behaviourally mediated crypsis, 101, 245 301, 302, 303, 304, 305, 306, 308, 309, 310, decoration, See decoration 312, 321 disruptive coloration, 113 apostatic selection, 24 habitat choice, See habitat choice Arabidopsis thaliana, 287 human predator system, tests using, 112 arctic charr, Salvelinus alpinus, 244 in moths, 102–103 arctic fox, Alopex lagopus, 312 orientational behaviour, 101, 114 arctic hare, Lepus arcticus, 312 bell miner, Manorina melanophrys, 334 Argiope aetherea, 255 beluga, Delphinapterus leucas, 313 Argiope aurantia, 256 biology, 3, 4, 150, 186, 221, 261, 267 Argiope bruennichi, 259 bioluminescence, 3, 188, 205 Argiope keyserlingi, 255 Biston robustum, 338 Argiope mascordi, 257 black bear, Ursus americanus, 312 Argiope versicolor, 257 black lemur, Eulemur macaco, 305 art, vi, 4, 11, 63, 87, 91, 92, 98 black pacarana, Dinomys branickii, 308 Asiatic black bear, Ursus thibetanus, 307 blackbuck, Antilope cervicapra, 306, Atlantic humpback whale, Megaptera novaeangliae, 307 314 black-footed cat, Felis nigripes, 310 Atlantic white-sided dolphin, Lagenorhynchus black-footed ferret, Mustela nigripes, 304 acutus, 313, 314 black-shouldered possum, Caluromysiops irrupta, 306 background matching, 5, 6, 7–8, 9, 17–30, 34, 39, black-tailed jackrabbit, Lepus californicus, 311 49, 54, 56, 60, 61, 62, 64, 65, 74, 81, 89, 92, black-tufted-ear marmoset, Callithrix penicillata, 97, 98, 99, 101, 102, 111, 113, 114, 136, 305 147, 150, 151, 152, 158, 168, 169, 170, 178, blennies, Blenniidae, 193 © in this web service Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-15257-0 - Animal Camouflage: Mechanisms and Function Edited by Martin Stevens and Sami Merilaita Index More information 352 Index blue jays Cyanocitta cristata, 8, 23, 102 common tenrec, Tenrec ecaudatus, 301 bontebok, Damaliscus pygargus, 306, 307, 310 complementary colours, 200 braconid parasitoid, Coeloides vancouverensis, 345 compromise coloration, See background matching, brush-tailed possum Trichosurus vulpecula, 311 compromise Brush, George de Forest, 91 computer science, 4, 9 buffy-tufted-ear marmoset, Callithrix aurita, 305, computer vision, 73 311 camouflage assessment and design, 73 bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana, 245 camouflage breaking, 73, 74, 77, 80, 81, 84, 165 Burchell’s zebra, Equus burchelli, 309 convexity detection, 77–78 butterflies, 24 neuronal implementation, 80 butterflyfish, Chaetodontidae, 198 countershading, 82–83 operators for detection of convex domains, 74–77 camouflage conspicuousness, 321 history of, 3–4 costs of camouflage, 66, 212, 223, 245 camouflage officer, 87 Cott, Hugh, 3, 34, 35, 53, 56, 64, 248 candy-stripe spider, Enoplognatha ovata, 259 cotton-topped tamarin, Saguinus oedipus, 305 Cape buffalo, Syncerus caffer, 301 counter-illumination, 206 carotenoids, 260 countershading, 3, 5, 9, 28, 42, 49, 53–68, 73–84, Catocala moths, 8 88, 89, 90, 91, 92, 97, 98, 99, 191, 202, 239, cephalopods, 1, 18, 145, 146, 147, 150, 151, 152, 299, 300, 307, 313, 321 154, 155, 156, 157, 159, 160, 166, 180, 189, body outline obliteration, 66 237, 240, 245, 248 concealing function of, 59–66 body patterns, categories of duck decoys, 88, 89, 90, 95 Mottle vs. Disruptive, 151–152 flattening hypothesis, 64 See also disruptive coloration; cuttlefish in Eurasian perch Perca fluviatilis, 58 deceptive resemblance, 150 in eyed hawkmoth Smerinthus ocellata, larvae, See also masquerade. 59, 62, 63, 65 disruptive function, 151 in grey squirrels Sciurus carolinensis, 63 flounder mimicry, 158 in mammals, 58 general background match, 150 in primates, 58 masquerade and mimicry, 157–158 luminescent countershading, 74 flounder mimicry, 158 obliterative shading, 3, 9, 28, 53, 56 night camouflage, 159 optical flattening, 64 pattern control, feature detection for, 152–156 posture and level of contrast, 58–59 colourblind camouflage, 154–155 protection from abrasion, 67 skin, light sensing in, 156 protection from UV, 66 skin, 156–157 self-shadow concealment, 6, 9, 28, 54, 56, 62, 67, specific background match, 150 74, 78, 314 chameleons, 1, 18, 239, 241, 246, 247 Venus de Milo, 95 cheetah, Acinonyx jubatus, 311 thermoregulation, 67, 323 chemical background matching, 338 crab spiders, 260, 275, 276, 287 chemical camouflage, 336–342 crested rat, Lophiomys imhausi, 301 chemical mimicry, 336 cricket, Telerogryllus oceanicus, 344 Chilean dolphin, Cephalorhynchus eutropia, 315 cuttlefish, 146, 164, 237, 241, 246 chimpanzee, Pan trogolodytes, 305 body patterns, categories of, 147 chiru, Panthlops hodgsoni, 310 Uniform, Mottle and Disruptive (UMD), 146, chromatophore, 237, 245 147, 150, 160 coevolution, 275, 284, 332 body patterns, categories of, 167 colour change, 19, 217, 227, 231, 237–249, 254, Disruptive pattern, 152, 153, 157, 170, 260, 261, 268, 313 177 rapid adaptive camouflage, 145, 157, 159 papillae expression, 154, 178 visual sampling rule, 147 pattern control, edge information, role of, 170 colour mixing, 201 pattern control, feature detection for colour space, 37, 38 contrast, role of, 153 Commerson’s dolphin, Cephalorhynchus Cyclosa argenteoalba, 258 commersonii, 313, 314 Cyclosa confusa, 258 © in this web service Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-15257-0 - Animal Camouflage: Mechanisms and Function Edited by Martin Stevens and Sami Merilaita Index More information Index 353 Cyclosa ginnaga, 259 European mantid, Mantis religiosa, 7 Cyclosa mulmeinensis, 258 European marten, Martes martes, 306 evolving backgrounds, See live backgrounds Dakin, William, 55, 56, 93 Dall’s porpoise, Phocoenoides dalli, 314 feather-tailed possum, Distoechurus pennatus, 304 Dall’s sheep, Ovis dalli, 312 fennec, Vulpes zerda, 312 damselfish, 194, 199 ferret badgers, Melogale, 302 Darwin, Charles, 3 fiddler crab, Uca vomeris, 243 Darwin, Erasmus, 3, 7 figure–ground segregation, 18, 165, 180 dazzle camouflage on ships, 3 fin whale, Balaenoptera physalus, 314 De Brazza’s monkey, Cercopithecus neglectus, flatfish, 18, 193, 239, 241, 248 305 flavonoids, 340 decoration, 11, 212, 213, 214, 215, 216, 217, 219, flicker-fusion camouflage, 10, 11 220, 221, 222, 223, 225, 226, 227, 228, 230, flounder, 193 231, 232, 256, 257, 258, 268 fluorescence, 265 decorator crabs, Majoidea, 212–232 fork-marked dwarf lemur, Phaner furcifer, 304 Derby’s eland, Taurotragus derbianus, 310 four-eyed opossum, Philander opossum, 311 Dictyota menstrualis, 220, 223 Fraser’s dolphin, Lagenodelphis hosei, 314 dik-dik, Madoqua kirkii, 306 fringe-lipped bat, Trachops cirrhosus, 331 disruptive coloration, 5, 8, 9, 10, 26, 27, 34, 37, 101, frogfish, Antennarius commerson, 193 113, 186, 219, 239, 298, 299, 304, 307, 314, 321, 342 Galapagos penguins, 19 coincident disruptive coloration, 9, 34, 43, 45, 47, game theory, 276, 288 48, 49, 50 garden dormice, Eliomys sp., 304 constructive shading, 151 Gasteracantha cranciformis, 256 differential blending, 27, 35, 36, 38, 48, 49, 50, gemsbok, Oryx gazella, 306, 310 151 Geoffroy’s cat, Felis geoffroyi, 311 maximum disruptive contrast, 151, 169 Geoffroy’s marmoset, Callithrix geoffroyi, 305, object recognition, 34, 50, 170, 180, 181, 240, 311 241 giant anteater, Myrmecophaga tridactyla, 303 pictorial relief, 151 giant flying squirrel, Petaurista alborufus, 307 distastefulness, 219 giant panda, Ailuropoda melanoleuca, 304 distractive markings, 5, 10–11, 28, 97, 98, 242, 259, giant tree rat, Mallomys sp., 307 298, 299, 309, 312 giant wood spider, Nephila pilipes, 260 domestic chicks Gallus gallus domesticus, 10 gibbon, Hylobatidae, 306 dottyback, Pictichromis, paccagnellae, 200 gobies, Gobiidae, 193 Douclangur, Pygathrix nemaeus, 310, 316 golden-rumped elephant shrew, Rhynchocyon cirnei, dragonflies, 128, 129 311 Drosophila melanogaster, 255 goral, Naemorhedus goral, 310 dusky dolphin, Lagenorhynchus obscurus, grasshoppers, 22, 24, 53, 276 314 great tit, Parus major, 23 dwarf lemurs, Cheirogalidae, 304 greater glider, Petauroides volans, 312 dwarf minke whales, Balaenoptera acutorostrata, Grevy’s zebra, Equus grevyi, 309 314 grison, Galictis vittata, 306 grivet, Chlorocebus aethiops, 306 eland, Taurotragus oryx, 311 guereza, Colobus guereza, 306 electric
Recommended publications
  • (12) Patent Application Publication (10) Pub. No.: US 2010/0071096 A1 Yamada Et Al
    US 20100071096A1 (19) United States (12) Patent Application Publication (10) Pub. No.: US 2010/0071096 A1 Yamada et al. (43) Pub. Date: Mar. 18, 2010 (54) PLANT DISEASE AND INSECT DAMAGE Publication Classification CONTROL COMPOSITION AND PLANT (51) Int. Cl. DISEASE AND INSECT DAMAGE AOIH 5/10 (2006.01) PREVENTION METHOD AOIN 55/10 (2006.01) AOIN 25/26 (2006.01) (75) Inventors: Eiichi Yamada, Chiba (JP): AOIH 5/00 (2006.01) Ryutaro Ezaki, Shiga (JP); AOIH 5/02 (2006.01) Hidenori Daido, Chiba (JP) AOIH 5/08 (2006.01) AOIP3/00 (2006.01) Correspondence Address: BUCHANAN, INGERSOLL & ROONEY PC (52) U.S. Cl. ............................ 800/295: 514/63; 504/100 POST OFFICE BOX 1404 (57) ABSTRACT ALEXANDRIA, VA 22313-1404 (US) The invention provides a plant disease and insect damage control composition including, as active ingredients, dinote (73) Assignee: Mitsui Chemicals, Inc., Minato-ku furan and at least one fungicidal compound; and a plant (JP) disease and insect damage prevention method that includes applying Such a composition to a plant body, Soil, plant seed, (21) Appl. No.: 12/516,966 stored cereal, stored legume, stored fruit, stored vegetable, silage, stored flowering plant, or export/import timber. The (22) PCT Filed: Nov. 22, 2007 invention provides a new plant disease and insect damage (86). PCT No.: PCT/UP2007/072635 control composition and a plant disease and insect damage prevention method with very low toxicity to mammals and S371 (c)(1), fishes, the composition and method showing an effect against (2), (4) Date: May 29, 2009 plural pathogens and pest insects, including emerging resis tant pathogens and resistant pest insect, by application to a (30) Foreign Application Priority Data plant body, soil, plant seed, stored cereal, stored legume, stored fruit, stored vegetable, silage, stored flowering plant, Nov.
    [Show full text]
  • The Sensory World of Bees
    June 2015 in Australia ccThehh senseeory mmiissttrryy world of bees chemaust.raci.org.au ALSO IN THIS ISSUE: • One X-ray technique better than two • Haber’s rule and toxicity • Madeira wines worth the wait The tale in the Working and sensory lives DAVE SAMMUT g sBY tin of bees 18 | Chemistry in Australia June 2015 Bees have a well-earned reputation for pollination and honey-making, but their lesser known skills in electrocommunication are just as impressive. ccording to the United Nations, ‘of Once the worker bee’s honey stomach is the 100 crop species that provide full, it returns to the hive and regurgitates the 90 per cent of the world’s food, partially modified nectar for a hive bee. The A over 70 are pollinated by bees’ hive bee ingests this material to continue the (bit.ly/1DpiRYF). It’s little wonder, then, that conversion process, then again regurgitates it bees continue to be a subject of study around into a cell of the honeycomb. During these the world. More surprising, perhaps, is that so processes, the bees also absorb water, which much remains to be learned. dehydrates the mixture. Bee-keeping (apiculture) is believed to Hive bees then beat their wings to fan the have started as far back as about 4500 years regurgitated material. As the water evaporates ago; sculptures in ancient Egypt show workers (to as little as around 10% moisture), the blowing smoke into hives as they remove sugars thicken into honey – a supersaturated honeycomb. In ancient Greece, Aristotle either hygroscopic solution of carbohydrates (plus kept and studied bees in his own hives or oils, minerals and other impurities).
    [Show full text]
  • Peppered Moths and the Industrial Revolution: Barking up the Wrong Tree? by Avril M
    NATIONAL CENTER FOR CASE STUDY TEACHING IN SCIENCE Peppered Moths and the Industrial Revolution: Barking up the Wrong Tree? by Avril M. Harder1, Janna R. Willoughby1,2, Jaqueline M. Doyle3 Part I – Hypotheses and Predictions The story of how Britain’s Industrial Revolution led to changes in peppered moth coloration is a classic, textbook ex- ample of evolution in action. You might even already be familiar with the plot and the findings of the main researcher of the phenomenon, H.B.D. Kettlewell. Although the peppered moth story has been told and re-told in thousands of biology classrooms, most narratives leave out an important bit of history: after Kettlewell published his results that described changes in morph population frequencies over time, many of his peers questioned the validity of his data. Your first task in this case study will be to put yourself in the place of one of Kettlewell’s contemporaries, evaluate his experimental methods, and decide whether his data support his assertions. Background Biston betularia is a medium-sized, nocturnal moth. Al- though the peppered moth is found across the world, some of the first research on this species was conducted in Great Britain. Before the Industrial Revolution, tree trunks in unpolluted areas were covered with lichens, and peppered moths with light colored wings were well camouflaged against this background (Figure 1). In this environment, light morphs vastly outnumbered dark morphs. During the Industrial Revolution, increasing air pollution led to the formation of acid rain and inhospitable conditions for the lichens. The death of the lichens combined with soot deposition on tree trunks led to an important environmen- tal shift for the moths.
    [Show full text]
  • Personality, Habitat Selection and Territoriality Kathleen Church A
    Habitat Complexity and Behaviour: Personality, Habitat Selection and Territoriality Kathleen Church A Thesis In the Department of Biology Presented in Partial Fulfilment of the Requirements For the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Biology) at Concordia University Montréal, Québec, Canada July 2018 © Kathleen Church, 2018 iii Abstract Habitat complexity and behaviour: personality, habitat selection and territoriality Kathleen Church, Ph.D. Concordia University, 2018 Structurally complex habitats support high species diversity and promote ecosystem health and stability, however anthropogenic activity is causing natural forms of complexity to rapidly diminish. At the population level, reductions in complexity negatively affect densities of territorial species, as increased visual distance increases the territory size of individuals. Individual behaviour, including aggression, activity and boldness, is also altered by complexity, due to plastic behavioural responses to complexity, habitat selection by particular personality types, or both processes occurring simultaneously. This thesis explores the behavioural effects of habitat complexity in four chapters. The first chapter, a laboratory experiment based on the ideal free distribution, observes how convict cichlids (Amatitlania nigrofasciata) trade-off the higher foraging success obtainable in open habitats with the greater safety provided in complex habitats under overt predation threat. Dominants always preferred the complex habitat, forming ideal despotic distributions, while subordinates altered their habitat use in response to predation. The second chapter also employs the ideal free distribution to assess how convict cichlids within a dominance hierarchy trade-off between food monopolization and safety in the absence of a iv predator. Dominants again formed ideal despotic distributions in the complex habitat, while dominants with lower energetic states more strongly preferred the complex habitat.
    [Show full text]
  • Garden Moth Scheme Report 2017
    Garden Moth Scheme Report 2017 Heather Young – April 2018 1 GMS Report 2017 CONTENTS PAGE Introduction 2 Top 30 Species 2017 3 Scientific Publications 4 Abundant and Widespread Species 8 Common or Garden Moths 11 Winter GMS 2017-18 15 Coordination Changes 16 GMS Annual Conference 16 GMS Sponsors 17 Links & Acknowledgements 18 Cover photograph: Peppered Moth (H. Young) Introduction The Garden Moth Scheme (GMS) welcomes participants from all parts of the United Kingdom and Ireland, and in 2017 received 360 completed recording forms, an increase of over 5% on 2016 (341). We have consistently received records from over 300 sites across the UK and Ireland since 2010, and now have almost 1 ½ million records in the GMS database. Several scientific papers using the GMS data have now been published in peer- reviewed journals, and these are listed in this report, with the relevant abstracts, to illustrate how the GMS records are used for research. The GMS is divided into 12 regions, monitoring 233 species of moth in every part of the UK and Ireland (the ‘Core Species’), along with a variable number of ‘Regional Species’. A selection of core species whose name suggests they should be found commonly, or in our gardens, is highlighted in this report. There is a round-up of the 2017-18 Winter Garden Moth Scheme, which attracted a surprisingly high number of recorders (102) despite the poor weather, a summary of the changes taking place in the GMS coordination team for 2018, and a short report on the 2018 Annual Conference, but we begin as usual with the Top 30 for GMS 2017.
    [Show full text]
  • Waveform Sensors: the Next Challenge in Biomimetic Electroreception
    International Journal of Biosensors & Bioelectronics Mini Review Open Access Waveform sensors: the next challenge in biomimetic electroreception Abstract Volume 2 Issue 6 - 2017 The interest in developing bioinspired electric sensors increased after the rising use Alejo Rodríguez Cattaneo, Angel Caputi and of electric fields as image carriers in underwater robots and medical devices using artificial electroreception (electrotomography and electric catheterism). Electric Ana Carolina Pereira Dept Neurociencias Integrativas y Computacionales, Instituto images of objects have been most often conceived as the amplitude modulation of the de Investigaciones Biológicas Clemente Estable, Uruguay local electric field on a sensory mosaic, but recent research in electric fish indicates that the evaluation of time waveform of local stimulus can increase the electrosensory Correspondence: Ana Carolina Pereira, Department of channels capacities and therefore improve discrimination and recognition of target Neurociencias Integrativas y Computacionales, Instituto de objects. This minireview deals with the present importance of developing electric Investigaciones Biológicas Clemente Estable, Av Italia 3318 sensors specifically tuned to the expected carrier waveforms to improve design of Montevideo, Uruguay, CP11600, Tel 59824871616, artificial bioinspired agents and diagnosis devices. Email [email protected] Keywords: Electroreception, Waveforms sensors Received: June 25, 2017 | Published: July 13, 2017 Introduction signal vanish when
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroethlogy NROC34 Course Goals How? Evaluation
    Course Goals Neuroethlogy NROC34 • What is neuroethology? • Prof. A. Mason – Role of basic biology – Model systems (mainly invertebrate) • e-mail: – Highly specialized organisms – [email protected] – Biomimetics – [email protected] • Primary literature and the scientific process – No textbook – subject = NROC34 – But if you really want one, there are a couple of suggestion on the • Office Hours: Friday, 1 – 4:00 pm, SW566 syllabus • Basic principles of integrative neural function • Weekly Readings: download from course webpage – More on this a bit later www.utsc.utoronto.ca/amason/courses/coursepage/syllabus2012.html NROC34 2012:1 1 NROC34 2012:1 2 How? Evaluation • Case studies of several model systems – Different kinds of questions – Different kinds of research (techniques) • Weekly readings 5% – Sensory, motor, decision-making… • Mid-term 35-45% • Selected papers each week • Final Exam 45-60% – Read before, discuss during: usually readings are challenging and “discussion” means me explaining (so • Presentation (optional) 10% don’t be discouraged) • Most topics include current work • Weekly reading marks: each week several – Take you to the leading edge of research in selected people will be selected at random to areas contribute 3 questions about the papers. NROC34 2012:1 3 NROC34 2012:1 4 1 Information Neuroethology • Who are you people? • The study of the neural mechanisms • What do you expect to learn in this course? underlying behaviour that is biologically • What previous course have you taken in: relevant to the animal performing it. – neuroscience • This encompasses many basic mechanisms – Behaviour of the nervous system. • Is this a req’d course for you? • Combines behavioural analysis and neurophysiology NROC34 2012:1 5 NROC34 2012:1 6 Neurobiology Behavioural Science • What do I mean by “integrative neural function”? • What does the nervous system do? • Ethology • Psychology – detect information in the environment – biological behaviour – abstract concepts (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • How Is the Peppered Moth Evidence of Natural Selection Over a Short Period of Time? Lab # ______
    Name: _______________________________ Date: ________________ How is the peppered moth evidence of natural selection over a short period of time? Lab # ______ Step 1- Background information: Charles Darwin collected a large amount of facts to create the theory of evolution by natural selection. Darwin described natural selection as the “process by which the organisms that are best adapted to a specific environment survive and produce more offspring than organisms that are not as well adapted”. One of his difficulties in proving the theory, however, was the lack of an example of evolution over a short period of time, which could be observed during ones lifetime. Although Darwin was unaware of it, remarkable examples of evolution, which might have helped to persuade people of his theory, were in the countryside of his native England. One such example is the evolution of the peppered moth, Biston betularia. 1) Describe Darwin’s theory of natural selection in your own words. ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________ 2) What was one difficulty Darwin had supporting his theory, even though he had tremendous (large) amounts of evidence? ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________ 3) Why was his theory so
    [Show full text]
  • Why Is the Story of the Peppered Moth So Famous and Important?
    Connects with SciGen Unit L4 Teacher Tune-up Quick Content Refresher for Busy Professionals Why is the story of the peppered moth so famous and important? The story of Biston betularia, the peppered moth, is a staple of many textbooks on evolution. Briefly stated, some mostly light-colored moths got mostly dark colored over many generations; then they got light-colored again. So what’s the big deal? Why is the peppered moth such an evolutionary celebrity? Before 1800, most peppered moths were white, with a sprinkling of dark spots that give the moth its name. They were well camouflaged against lichens that grew on trees with light bark, which helped them avoid predatory birds. Throughout the nineteenth century, England’s industrial revolution saw the rise of steam power. Trains and steam-powered factories (the “dark Satanic Mills” of an 1808 poem by William Blake) burned coal and belched tons of soot into the environment. The black soot killed lichen and settled on the trees where the peppered moths rested. And generation by generation, the moths began to change. In 1811, someone found the first dark, nearly black, peppered moth. By the middle of the century, the frequency of melanism (dark coloration) in the peppered moths had risen dramatically. And by the end of the century, in large areas of England that were coated in dark industrial filth, nearly all peppered moths were dark. Along with changing landscapes and moths, another change was afoot. Naturalists had begun to speculate about something called evolution, based largely on evidence of ancient events: sequences in the fossil record, similarities among different species, vestigial structures in many organisms—that sort of thing.
    [Show full text]
  • Exhibit List
    EXHIBIT LIST Reference No: HOL/00521 Petitioner: Ms Sarah Green on behalf of Arthur Daily Trips (Canal Boat Company) Published to Collaboration Area: Wednesday 23-Nov-2016 Page 1 of 163 No Exhibit Name Page 1 A728 Exhibits List.pdf (A728) 3 2 A729 Exhibit 1 route map google.pdf (A729) 4 3 A730 Exhibit 2 water environment water courses.pdf (A730) 5 4 A731 Exhibit 3 cultural heritage.pdf (A731) 6 - 7 5 A732 Exhibit 4 metropolitan Open Land.pdf (A732) 8 - 9 6 A733 Exhibit 5 dragonflies.pdf (A733) 10 - 15 7 A734 Exhibit 6 quality of water.pdf (A734) 16 8 A735 Exhibit 7 Customer activities.pdf (A735) 17 9 A736 Exhibit 8 pylons.pdf (A736) 18 - 24 10 A737 Exhibit 9 Species-List.pdf (A737) 25 - 84 11 A738 Exhibit 10 magic maps.pdf (A738) 85 - 86 12 A739 Exhibit 11 Leisure and tourism Destination.pdf (A739) 87 13 A740 Exhibit 12 continuity through time.pdf (A740) 88 - 91 14 A741 Exhibit 13 The Plans for Denham Country Park.pdf (A741) 92 - 93 15 A742 Exhibit 14 Enabling Works.pdf (A742) 94 - 95 16 A743 Exhibit 15 Water Framework Directive.pdf (A743) 96 - 97 17 A744 Exhibit 16 Ecological_baseline_data_Mammals.pdf (A744) 98 - 108 18 A745 Exhibit 17 Wetlands_Programmes of measures_170907.pdf (A745) 109 - 117 19 A746 Exhibit 18 Guidance_protection animal species.pdf (A746) 118 - 136 20 A747 Exhibit 19 ODPM Circular 06_2005.pdf (A747) 137 - 151 HOL/00521/0001 EXHIBIT LIST Reference No: HOL/00521 Petitioner: Ms Sarah Green on behalf of Arthur Daily Trips (Canal Boat Company) Published to Collaboration Area: Wednesday 23-Nov-2016 Page 2 of 163 No Exhibit
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals
    Jelle Atema Richard R. Fay Arthur N. Popper William N. Tavolga Editors Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals Springer-Verlag New York Berlin Heidelberg London Paris Tokyo JELLE ATEMA, Boston University Marine Program, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, Massachusetts 02543, USA Richard R. Fay, Parmly Hearing Institute, Loyola University, Chicago, Illinois 60626, USA ARTHUR N. POPPER, Department of Zoology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA WILLIAM N. TAVOLGA, Mote Marine Laboratory, Sarasota, Florida 33577, USA The cover Illustration is a reproduction of Figure 13.3, p. 343 of this volume Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Sensory biology of aquatic animals. Papers based on presentations given at an International Conference on the Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals held, June 24-28, 1985, at the Mote Marine Laboratory in Sarasota, Fla. Bibliography: p. Includes indexes. 1. Aquatic animals—Physiology—Congresses. 2. Senses and Sensation—Congresses. I. Atema, Jelle. II. International Conference on the Sensory Biology - . of Aquatic Animals (1985 : Sarasota, Fla.) QL120.S46 1987 591.92 87-9632 © 1988 by Springer-Verlag New York Inc. x —• All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer-Verlag, 175 Fifth Avenue, New York 10010, U.S.A.), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of Information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, Computer Software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use of general descriptive names, trade names, trademarks, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • 1889Illustratedc00will.Pdf (13.65Mb)
    ^sr -i- ^^, < ^ /\A0A|J|^E1L. Wr'^Pfl^o^E/Co. br»Mt Pcpqnca Pre Co (W« TV.- r^x^w, NOTICE. The vitality of the greater portion of the seeds mentioned in this Catalogue have been thoroughly tested in my nursery and the percentage of each variety, that will vegetate carefully ascertained. No seed will be offered that I do not believe to be perfectly good. My greenhouses and grounds afford facilities that are unsurpassed for experimenting with and testing vegetables and flowers and those best suited to our climate, only, will be recommended, any new varieties that may be introduced and that are really desirable will be produced and offered to my customers. SEEDS BY MAIL. Flower and Vegeteble Seeds in small papers postpaid to all parts of Canada and the United States at the prices quoted in this catalogue. Parcels of Seeds not over four lbs. weight can be forwarded to any Post Office in the Dominion or the United States, at four cents per pound. Where large Parcels are required, they can be forwarded by freight or express to any station, without disappointment or loss. As I do not place Seeds on commission in the hands of dealers, parties at a distance desirous of trying those offered by me are invited to send their orders direct to the House, or to the nearest store keeper who sells my seeds. Ask for Wm. ttvans' seeds and take no others. ATTENTION TO CUSTOMERS. It is the earnest wish of the proprietor that all the requirements and directions of his customers be scrupulously attended to, and the greatest care will be taken to carry out their instructions faithfully.
    [Show full text]