Peppered Moths
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Icons of Evolution? Why Much of What Jonathan Wells Writes about Evolution is Wrong Alan D. Gishlick, National Center for Science Education PEPPERED MOTHS HOW MANY MOTHS CAN DANCE ON THE TRUNK OF A TREE? THE STORY OF THE PEPPERED MOTH DISTRACTION BY IRRELEVANT DATA ndustrial melanism in peppered moths is ells disagrees with the results of the one of the most frequently used examples research on industrial melanism in Iof natural selection in action. This is large- Wthe peppered moth, and manipulates ly because of its pedagogical simplicity — it is the literature and the data to fit his views. He a straightforward example that is visual and points out that the “problem” of the peppered dynamic — and its copious documentation. moths is far from simple. His discussion cen- Industrial melanism refers to the darkening of ters on three points where he believes text- color that occurred in a number of species of books are in error, alleging that (1) the daytime insects following the Industrial Revolution. resting places of peppered moths invalidates This change appears to be related to the Kettlewell’s experimental results; (2) the pho- increase in pollutants in the environment. tos of the moths are “staged”; and (3) the Before the Industrial Revolution, individuals recovery patterns of populations dominated by of the moth species Biston betularia (com- light moths after the levels of pollution were monly called the “peppered moth”) were pre- reduced do not fit the “model,” although he is dominantly white with black speckles. By the unclear as to what the “model” is. All three of end of the 1800s, they were predominantly these objections are spurious. They are distrac- charcoal grey. This change was well docu- tions from the general accuracy of the story mented and led Tutt (1896) to hypothesize that and its value in showing the effects of natural this change was a result of pollution-stained selection on genetic variability in natural pop- trees’ affecting the camouflage potential of the ulations. moths. This change was termed “industrial First, Wells argues that the story is seriously melanism.” In the 1950s, Bernard Kettlewell flawed because “peppered moths in the wild decided to test the hypothesis that natural don’t even rest on tree trunks” (Wells, selection was working on the differential cam- 2000:138). He repeats this point throughout ouflage of the moths. In order to do this, he the chapter. However, it is both false and irrel- released marked light and dark moths into pol- evant, and only serves as a distraction to lead luted and non-polluted forests. He found that the reader away from the actual story of the birds appear to prey selectively on light moths moths. Contrary to Wells’s assertions, data in polluted forests and on dark moths in non- given by Majerus (1998:123) indicate that the polluted forests and so documented the idea of moths do indeed rest on the trunks of trees natural selection of these color patterns in 25% of the time. The rest of the time moths moths by birds. After anti-pollution laws took rest in branches (25%) or at branch-trunk junc- effect and the bark lightened, the moth popula- tions (50%). The facts have been pointed out tions in formerly polluted areas returned to repeatedly to Wells; his response has been previous color distributions. mostly to claim that moths don’t rest on “exposed” tree trunks (Wells, 2002 www.dis- covery.org/viewDB/index.php3?program=CR SC&command=view&id=1144). But this is not what he said in the text of Icons, which 52 Icons of Evolution? Why Much of What Jonathan Wells Writes about Evolution is Wrong Alan D. Gishlick, National Center for Science Education remains flatly wrong. Moths are found all over words, birds hunt the prey they can see and trees, which is not a surprise (Clarke et al., hunt it where it is, not where it isn’t. Therefore, 1994) and it is mentioned in the references that no matter where the moth rests in the tree, it is Wells cites. visible to predatory birds, and thus its differen- To clear up any confusion, no researcher tial camouflage is important. doubts that the peppered moth rests in trees The purpose of Wells’s distraction is to put (Clarke et al. 1994; Majerus 1998), which the actual experiments into question and make means that the resting substrate is bark. Entire it sound as if the textbook authors are either trees are stained by pollution — the leaves, mistaken, or intentionally trying to fool stu- twigs, branches, trunks, and the surrounding dents. The insinuation is that because ground (Kettlewell, 1973) — and so the colors Kettlewell released the moths during the day, of the moths are relevant no matter where on they did not find “normal” resting places. the tree they rest — trunks, trunk-branch junc- Whether or not this is so, the release and cap- tions, branches, twigs, and even the leaves. ture experiments took place over a number of Wells’s argument implies that predatory birds days, so the moths were able to take up posi- can only see moths that are on exposed trunks. tions of their choosing, even if the first day By making this argument, however, Wells was not perfectly “natural” (Kettlewell, 1955, shows an apparent ignorance of the ecology of 1956, 1973). Kettlewell’s experiments were birds and woodland ecosystems. If you walk not perfect — few field experiments are — and into any forest, you can see that the birds fly they may have magnified the degree of selec- from tree to tree, branch to branch, and hunt at tion, but all serious researchers in the field all levels of the forest. Woodland species of agree that they were certainly not so flawed as birds that prey on moths and other insects live to invalidate his conclusion. and hunt in the canopy (the leafy part of the In his second objection, Wells ties the trees). These birds are not hunting from out- Kettlewell experiments to textbooks by con- side, soaring above the trees like hawks, as stantly repeating the statement that the illustra- Wells’s argument would require. tive photos were “staged” (Wells, 2000:150); In the scientific literature, there is extensive the important issue here is not how the photos discussion of the hunting behavior of birds, were made, but rather their intent. Wells including those that hunt peppered moths. implies that the photos purport to show a “life- Ornithologists have shown the woodland like” condition to prove that moths rest on ecosystem to be vertically stratified by compe- trunks. This is not the case. The photos are tition between different bird species. This meant to demonstrate the visibility of the dif- zonation means that there are skilled predators ferent forms of the moth on polluted and patrolling all levels of the forest: the trunks, unpolluted trees. It is absurd to expect a pho- trunk-branch joints, branches, and higher tographer to just sit around and wait until two canopy (Colquhoun and Morley, 1943; differently colored moths happen to alight side Hartley, 1953). Further, birds learn to distin- by side. Further, how the photos were pro- guish their prey against various backgrounds duced does not change the actual data. Birds and preferentially hunt prey in locations where eat moths and they eat the ones that they see they have found it in the past and that birds more easily first. The textbook photos never selectively prey on the more visible moths claim to depict a real-life situation, and it is (Pietrewitz and Kamil, 1977, 1981). In other improper to imply otherwise. 53 Icons of Evolution? Why Much of What Jonathan Wells Writes about Evolution is Wrong Alan D. Gishlick, National Center for Science Education The third criticism, and the only scientific GREY AREA GRADES one that Wells levels, deals with the recovery ike the grading schemes for the other of the light form of the moth following the “icons,” this one is stacked against the institution of pollution control laws. The main textbooks as well. Even books that have thrust of his argument is that because the L more extensive discussions of the problems recovery of light-colored lichens does not cor- and details (such as Miller and Levine) can at relate with the recovery of the light form of the best earn a D. Like the grading schemes for moths, the entire story is incorrect. Wells Miller-Urey and Haeckel’s embryos, it is based exploits the fact that the original researchers largely on the presence or absence of pictures thought that the camouflage of the light moths (Figure 15). Explaining the peppered moth depended on the presence of lichen. However, story without photos (as in Biggs et al.), gar- the light forms recovered before the lichens ners a peculiar X grade. In order to get an A or did; therefore, Wells concludes, natural selec- B, a book must contain pictures of moths in tion has nothing to do with the story. Although “natural” resting places. Given Wells’s expla- it is true that the moths are well-camouflaged nation that these are unknown, presenting against lichens, and lichens are destroyed by those would be impossible. How can textbooks pollution, nevertheless the camouflage of the be expected to do that? A C would be awarded moths ultimately depends upon the color of the to a textbook that (1) used “misleading” pic- trees, which reflect the amount of soot staining tures, but (2) referred to them as “staged,” and the trees. Although lichens play a role in cam- (3) stated that the results of the experiment are ouflage, they are not necessary.