Beyond the Big Five: the Dark Triad and the Supernumerary Personality Inventory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Beyond the Big Five: the Dark Triad and the Supernumerary Personality Inventory ARTICLE AVAILABLE ONLINE Twin Research and Human Genetics Volume 14 | Number 2 | pp. 158–168 Beyond the Big Five: The Dark Triad and the Supernumerary Personality Inventory Livia Veselka,1 Julie Aitken Schermer2 and Philip A. Vernon1 1 Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, Canada 2 Management and Organizational Studies, University of Western Ontario, Canada The Dark Triad of personality, comprising Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy, was investigated in relation to the Supernumerary Personality Inventory (SPI) traits, because both sets of variables are predomi- nantly distinct from the Big Five model of personality. Correlational and principal factor analyses were conducted to assess the relations between the Dark Triad and SPI traits. Multivariate behavioral genetic model- fitting analyses were also conducted to determine the correlated genetic and/or environmental underpinnings of the observed phenotypic correlations. Participants were 358 monozygotic and 98 same-sex dizygotic adult twin pairs from North America. As predicted, results revealed significant correlations between the Dark Triad and most SPI traits, and these correlations were primarily attributable to common genetic and non-shared envi- ronmental factors, except in the case of Machiavellianism, where shared environmental effects emerged. Three correlated factors were extracted during joint factor analysis of the Dark Triad and SPI traits, as well as a herita- ble general factor of personality — results that clarified the structure of the Dark Triad construct. It is concluded that the Dark Triad represents an exploitative and antisocial construct that extends beyond the Big Five model and shares a theoretical space with the SPI traits. ■ Keywords: personality, Dark Triad, Supernumerary Personality Inventory, behavioral genetics Antisocial and socially aversive personality traits — stable the Dark Triad, however, it remains enigmatic, particularly human characteristics that do not comply with established given its uncertain place in personality theory. Specifically, social and ethical codes and norms (Hott, 1979) — have the Dark Triad has shown inconsistent correlations with been subject to extensive research, first in clinical samples the Big Five model (e.g., Lee & Ashton, 2005; Vernon et al., (e.g., Macaskill, 1980; Raskin & Hare, 1978), and later in 2008b) — the conventional framework of personality. community populations (e.g., Moore & Katz, 1995; These inconsistencies suggest that the Dark Triad may lie Takahashi, 2007). In these latter community-based studies, outside the realm of the Big Five structure, although no three traits in particular have received considerable atten- evidence has yet been provided to support this notion. tion: Machiavellianism, as exhibited through cold and The inconsistencies also make it difficult to further manipulative behaviors (Christie & Geis, 1970), narcis- explore the Dark Triad because it has not shown reliable sism, as defined by excessive self-love and unrelenting correlations with available models, and has therefore not feelings of superiority (Raskin & Hall, 1979), and psy- progressed as far as it might have in its development as a chopathy, as characterized by high thrill-seeking behaviors personality construct. paired with diminished empathy (Hare, 1985). With the recent development of the Supernumerary Most recently, Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psy- Personality Inventory (SPI) by Paunonen (2002), which chopathy have been grouped together into a single construct, labeled the Dark Triad by Paulhus and Williams (2002), who first reported strong associations between these subclinical traits. Since this initial analysis, several RECEIVED 22 October, 2010; ACCEPTED 25 November, 2010. studies have confirmed the existence of this Dark Triad ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE: Philip A. Vernon, Department of construct (e.g., Jakobwitz & Egan, 2006; Lee & Ashton, Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, 2005; Vernon et al., 2008b). Despite evidence validating Canada N6A 5C2. E-mail: [email protected] 158 Veselka, L., Aitken Schermer, J., & Vernon, P. A. (2011). Beyond the big five: The dark triad and the supernumerary personality inventory. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14, 2, 158–168. DOI 10.1375/twin.14.2.158 Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 170.106.202.126, on 01 Oct 2021 at 01:34:06, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.14.2.158 Dark Triad Versus the Supernumerary Personality Inventory measures 10 traits that lie beyond the Big Five model Behavioral Genetic Analyses of the Dark Triad (conventionality, seductiveness, manipulativeness, thrifti- and SPI Traits ness, humorousness, integrity, femininity, religiosity, Twin studies have been used in the assessment of Dark risk-taking, egotism) it may now be possible to elaborate Triad traits to determine the extent to which variance in upon our understanding of the Dark Triad traits. these traits is affected by genes and/or the environment. Specifically, the purpose of the present study is to clarify Behavioral genetic assessments of psychopathy, narcissism, the nature of the Dark Triad construct by identifying the and Machiavellianism have suggested that variance in the SPI traits with which it correlates. Further, by assessing the occurrence and stability of these traits can be accounted for strength of these correlations, and determining the extent by both genetic and non-shared environmental factors to which phenotypic correlations between the Dark Triad (e.g., Blonigen et al., 2006; Larsson et al., 2006; Livesley et traits and the SPI traits are attributable to common al., 1993; Taylor et al., 2003; Vernon et al., 2008b). genetic and/or environmental factors, the present study Correlations between the three Dark Triad traits have also also aims to establish whether the Dark Triad has empiri- been shown to be moderate (Paulhus & Williams, 2002), cal ties to a model that lies beyond the Big Five. although Vernon et al. (2008b) did report a non-significant correlation between Machiavellianism and narcissism. Relations Between the Dark Triad Traits To date, there have been no behavioral genetic investi- and the Traits Measured by the SPI gations carried out on all of the variables measured by the Several SPI traits, when examined individually, have exhib- SPI. However, some univariate behavioral genetic investi- ited correlations with all three Dark Triad variables. gations or assessments of heritability have been conducted Specifically, narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism on select traits that are presently subsumed by the SPI. have all shown positive associations with seductiveness Results from these investigations offer initial evidence (e.g., Foster et al., 2006; Grann, 2000; McHoskey, 2001; about the etiology of the ten traits that have supposedly Wilson et al., 1998), manipulativeness (e.g., Austin et al., evaded conventional personality models. 2007; Paulhus & Williams, 2002; Shepperd & Socherman, Past twin studies have noted that genetic and non- 1997; Watson et al., 1984), risk-taking or the related shared environmental effects primarily account for concept of sensation-seeking (e.g., Emmons, 1981; Rim, individual differences in conventionality (e.g., Bouchard et 1966; Vitacco & Rogers, 2001), and egotism (e.g., Paulhus al., 2003; Hatemi et al., 2007), seductiveness as derived & Williams, 2002). They have all further correlated neg- from studies of sociosexuality (Bailey et al., 2000), manip- atively with integrity (e.g., Ashton et al., 2000; Blair et ulativeness (Larsson et al., 2006), humorousness (Vernon al., 2008). et al., 2008a), femininity (e.g., Loehlin et al., et al., 2005; In addition to these common correlates, the Dark Mitchell et al., 1989), and risk-taking (e.g., Anokhin et al., Triad traits have also exhibited unique patterns of corre- 2009; Stoel et al., 2006). In addition to these genetic and lations with other variables presently subsumed by the non-shared environmental effects, shared environmental SPI. For instance, both Machiavellianism and narcissism influences have also been found to underlie variance in have shown positive associations with femininity (e.g., religiosity (e.g., Bradshaw & Ellison, 2008; D’Onofrio et Jackson et al., 1992; Nigro & Galli, 1985). Furthermore, al., 1999). The traits of thriftiness, integrity, and egotism Machiavellianism alone has correlated positively with a (in a non-narcissistic context) have not been exposed to preference for conservative values focusing on the mainte- behavioral genetic investigation. nance of tradition, which can be seen as a representation Present Study of SPI’s conventionality dimension (e.g., Krampen, 1980; The purpose of the present study is to gain a greater Shafiq, 1982; Stone & Russ, 1976), and only narcissism has understanding of the nature of the Dark Triad, and of its shown negative relations with religiosity (e.g., Ghorbani et place in the overall framework of human personality by al., 2004) and with thriftiness (Rose, 2007). Lastly, all three examining its association with the SPI, which has been Dark Triad traits have exhibited varied correlations with developed to measure 10 personality traits that exist different humor styles — ways in which humor is pro- beyond the Big Five structure. Correlations between the duced and understood (Martin et al., 2003). Specifically, Dark Triad and SPI variables will be
Recommended publications
  • Why Susie Sells Seashells by the Seashore: Implicit Egotism and Major Life Decisions
    ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Why Susie Sells Seashells by the Seashore: Implicit Egotism and Major Life Decisions Brett W. Pelham, Matthew C. Mirenberg, and John T. Jones State University of New York at Buffalo Because most people possess positive associations about themselves, most people prefer things that are connected to the self (e.g., the letters in one’s name). The authors refer to such preferences as implicit egotism. Ten studies assessed the role of implicit egotism in 2 major life decisions: where people choose to live and what people choose to do for a living. Studies 1–5 showed that people are disproportionately likely to live in places whose names resemble their own first or last names (e.g., people named Louis are disproportionately likely to live in St. Louis). Study 6 extended this finding to birthday number preferences. People were disproportionately likely to live in cities whose names began with their birthday numbers (e.g., Two Harbors, MN). Studies 7–10 suggested that people disproportionately choose careers whose labels resemble their names (e.g., people named Dennis or Denise are overrepresented among dentists). Implicit egotism appears to influence major life decisions. This idea stands in sharp contrast to many models of rational choice and attests to the importance of understanding implicit beliefs. What role do people’s thoughts and feelings about themselves important role in major life decisions. For example, only a handful play in their important day-to-day decisions and behaviors? Con- of studies have examined whether self-regulation processes influ- temporary research on the self-concept suggests many answers to ence people’s choice of relationship partners.
    [Show full text]
  • Relationships and the Self: Egosystem and Ecosystem
    C HAPTER 4 RELATIONSHIPS AND THE SELF: EGOSYSTEM AND ECOSYSTEM Jennifer Crocker and Amy Canevello Most social scientists assume that people are funda- Crocker & Canevello, 2008, 2012b;ASSOCIATION Crocker, mentally self-interested, that they do what they Olivier, & Nuer, 2009). We first describe this perceive to be advantageous to themselves (Miller, system in general terms that apply to most, if not all, 1999). Unsurprisingly, this view pervades research types of relationships. Then, because romantic rela- on the self, in which people are depicted as self- tionships are the focus of a great deal of interest and enhancing and self-protective, seeking validation research, we describe the principles of romantic and affirmation, and taking credit for successes but relationships driven by egosystem motivation. dodging blame for failures. Perhaps more surprising, We next describe an emerging alternate view, in this view also pervades a great deal of research on which people have the capacity to transcend self- relationships, which presumably involve shared interestPSYCHOLOGICAL and care deeply about people and things bonds between people and feelings such as close- beyond themselves, which we call ecosystem motiva- ness, caring, affection, or love. Many researchers tion (Crocker, 2008; Crocker & Canevello, 2008, assume that people in relationships, as in the rest of 2012b; Crocker et al., 2006). In relationships, their lives, are fundamentally self-centered and people driven by ecosystem motivation seek to pro- self-serving. In this view, people want to AMERICANbe in rela- mote the well-being of the relationship partner not tionships to promote their own ends, ©they use rela- out of selfish motives to obtain benefits in return, tionship partners to satisfy their own needs, and but because they care about the partner or because they sacrifice and compromise in relationships to both people care about the well-being of someone or keep their relationship partners happy so they can something beyond themselves.
    [Show full text]
  • Origins of Narcissism in Children
    Origins of narcissism in children Eddie Brummelmana,b,1, Sander Thomaesb,c, Stefanie A. Nelemansd, Bram Orobio de Castrob, Geertjan Overbeeka, and Brad J. Bushmane,f aResearch Institute of Child Development and Education, Department of Educational Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1001 NG, The Netherlands; bDepartment of Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht 3584 CS, The Netherlands; cCenter for Research on Self and Identity, Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton SO17 1BJ, England; dResearch Centre Adolescent Development, Department of Youth and Family, Utrecht University, Utrecht 3584 CS, The Netherlands; eDepartment of Communication and Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210-1339; and fDepartment of Communication Science, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1081 HV, The Netherlands Edited by Susan T. Fiske, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved February 12, 2015 (received for review November 7, 2014) Narcissism levels have been increasing among Western youth, and (9) and “are under a compulsion to ascribe every perfection to contribute to societal problems such as aggression and violence. the child—which sober observation would find no occasion to The origins of narcissism, however, are not well understood. Here, do” (10). Consequently, children might internalize the belief that we report, to our knowledge, the first prospective longitudinal they are special individuals who are entitled to privileges. In evidence on the origins of narcissism in children. We compared contrast, psychoanalytic theory holds that children are likely to two perspectives: social learning theory (positing that narcissism is grow up to be narcissistic when their parents lack warmth toward cultivated by parental overvaluation) and psychoanalytic theory them (11, 12).
    [Show full text]
  • The Dirty Dozen: a Concise Measure of the Dark Triad
    Psychological Assessment © 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 22, No. 2, 420–432 1040-3590/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019265 The Dirty Dozen: A Concise Measure of the Dark Triad Peter K. Jonason Gregory D. Webster University of West Florida University of Florida There has been an exponential increase of interest in the dark side of human nature during the last decade. To better understand this dark side, the authors developed and validated a concise, 12-item measure of the Dark Triad: narcissism, psychopathy, Machiavellianism. In 4 studies involving 1,085 participants, they examined its structural reliability, convergent and discriminant validity (Studies 1, 2, and 4), and test–retest reliability (Study 3). Their measure retained the flexibility needed to measure these 3 independent-yet-related constructs while improving its efficiency by reducing its item count by 87% (from 91 to 12 items). The measure retained its core of disagreeableness, short-term mating, and aggressiveness. They call this measure the Dirty Dozen, but it cleanly measures the Dark Triad. Keywords: Dark Triad, narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy, measurement The Dark Triad is a term used to describe a constellation of quiring scores on each measure to be standardized (Jonason, Li, three socially undesirable personality traits: narcissism, psychop- Webster, & Schmitt, 2009). athy, and Machiavellianism (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Research Second, assessing the Dark Triad’s 91 items is inefficient, on the Dark Triad has increased exponentially over the last decade. time-consuming, and may cause response fatigue in some partic- An analysis of Google Scholar hit counts for “Dark Triad” in ipants. When studying the Dark Triad and one or more other scientific works reveals an explosive increase from one in 2002 to measures of interest (e.g., self-esteem, Big Five personality traits), at least 38 in 2009.
    [Show full text]
  • Impulsivity and the Self-Defeating Behavior of Narcissists
    Personality and Social Psychology Review Copyright © 2006 by 2006, Vol. 10, No. 2, 154–165 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Impulsivity and the Self-Defeating Behavior of Narcissists Simine Vazire Department of Psychology The University of Texas at Austin David C. Funder Department of Psychology University of California, Riverside Currently prominent models of narcissism (e.g., Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001) primarily explain narcissists’self-defeating behaviors in terms of conscious cognitive and affec- tive processes. We propose that the disposition of impulsivity may also play an impor- tant role. We offer 2 forms of evidence. First, we present a meta-analysis demonstrat- ing a strong positive relationship between narcissism and impulsivity. Second, we review and reinterpret the literature on 3 hallmarks of narcissism: self-enhancement, aggression, and negative long-term outcomes. Our reinterpretation argues that impulsivity provides a more parsimonious explanation for at least some of narcissists’ self-defeating behavior than do existing models. These 2 sources of evidence suggest that narcissists’ quest for the status and recognition they so intensely desire is thwarted, in part, by their lack of the self-control necessary to achieve those goals. Narcissists are a puzzle. Their bragging and arro- This article begins by proposing that the widely ac- gance interferes with the attainment of the status and cepted cognitive–affective processing model presented recognition they so poignantly desire. Why do they by Morf and Rhodewalt (2001) be extended to include continually undermine themselves in this way? The re- dispositional impulsivity. We then review the empirical search literature appears to have achieved some con- evidence for our proposal by presenting a sensus about the nature of sub-clinical narcissism1 meta-analysis of the relationship between narcissism with respect to underlying cognitive, social, and affec- and impulsivity, including unpublished results from tive processes (e.g., Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001).
    [Show full text]
  • Intellectual Humility and the Curse of Knowledge
    Forthcoming in Arrogance and Polarization. Eds. A. Tanesini and M. Lynch. Routledge. Intellectual Humility and the Curse of Knowledge Michael Hannon University of Nottingham This chapter explores an unappreciated psychological dimension of intellectual humility. In particular, I argue there is a plausible connection between intellectual humility and epistemic egocentrism. Epistemic egocentrism is a well-known cognitive bias – often called ‘the curse of knowledge’ – whereby an agent attributes his or her own mental states to other people. I hypothesize that an individual who exhibits this bias is more likely to possess a variety of traits that are characteristic of intellectual humility. This is surprising because intellectual humility is often regarded as an antidote to cognitive biases, whereas I claim that a particular cognitive bias (namely, the curse of knowledge) may help foster an intellectual virtue. Introduction Intellectual humility is widely considered a virtue.1 Although there is no consensus within philosophy or psychology on a precise definition of intellectual humility, it has been linked to an assortment of desirable traits. For example, intellectual humble people have been said to: not think too highly of themselves; not ascribe to themselves greater excellence than they possesses; show low concern for how special their talents are; take complaints and criticism seriously; generously acknowledge the contributions of others; and not demand special treatment, even 1 For example, see Church and Barrett (2016), Hazlett (2012), Roberts and Wood (2003), Samuelson et al. (2015), Tanesini (2018), and Whitcomb et al. (2017). when deserving. This virtue has been studied by philosophers, psychologists, and theologians, and it seems to involve a mix of cognitive, emotional, behavioural, and motivational components.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem in Relation to the Dark Triad
    © 2014 WUPJ, September 2014, Volume 2 Personality Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem in Relation to the Dark Triad Lauren Stenason* Individuals with characteristics encompassing the three domains of the Dark Triad (psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism) are often thought to possess inflated levels of self-esteem however, there has been controversy as to whether this inflated self-esteem represents their true underlying level of self-esteem. The present study investigated the relationships between the Dark Triad, explicit, and implicit self-esteem. Participants consisted of 231 individuals from The University of Western Ontario with an age range of 17 to 45 years (M = 19.390, SD = 2.908). 81% of participants were female and 19% of participants were male. Participants completed an online survey consisting of the Short Dark Triad, Rosenberg Self- Esteem Scale, and the Name Letter Task. The analysis revealed that Machiavellianism showed a weak negative correlation with last name initial. However, Machiavellianism and explicit self-esteem were not significantly correlated with first name initial. Narcissism showed a moderate positive correlation with explicit self-esteem and a weak positive correlation with last name initial but no significant relationship between narcissism and first name initial. Psychopathy did not reveal any significant correlations with either explicit or implicit self-esteem. Multiple regression analysis revealed that narcissism and psychopathy added significantly to the prediction of explicit self-esteem. Self-esteem is involved in many domains The components of the Dark Triad are regarding personality, psychological disorders, considered maladaptive personality traits that and overall mental health. Different types of include narcissism, psychopathy, and personality characteristics involve varying Machiavellianism.
    [Show full text]
  • Threatened Egotism, Narcissism, Self-Esteem, and Direct and Displaced Aggression: Does Self-Love Or Self-Hate Lead to Violence?
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1998, Vol. 75, No. 1, 219-229 0022-3514/98/S3.00 Threatened Egotism, Narcissism, Self-Esteem, and Direct and Displaced Aggression: Does Self-Love or Self-Hate Lead to Violence? Brad J. Bushman Roy F. Baumeister Iowa State University Case Western Reserve University It has been widely asserted that low self-esteem causes violence, but laboratory evidence is lacking, and some contrary observations have characterized aggressors as having favorable self-opinions. In 2 studies, both simple self-esteem and narcissism were measured, and then individual participants were given an opportunity to aggress against someone who had insulted them or praised them or against an innocent third person. Self-esteem proved irrelevant to aggression. The combination of narcissism and insult led to exceptionally high levels of aggression toward the source of the insult. Neither form of self-regard affected displaced aggression, which was low in general. These findings contradict the popular view that low self-esteem causes aggression and point instead toward threat- ened egotism as an important cause. How do people's thoughts and feelings about themselves in- of personal and social problems (e.g., California Task Force, fluence their propensities to perform acts of aggression against 1990). Consistent with this view, it has been widely asserted others? Multiple answers to this question can be suggested. For that low self-esteem is a cause of violence (e.g., Kirschner, decades, clinical psychologists have subscribed to a conven- 1992; Long, 1990; Oates & Forrest, 1985; Schoenfeld, 1988; tional view that low self-esteem underlies aggression.
    [Show full text]
  • Our Vulnerable Dark Side—Two Laboratory Approaches
    International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health Article Our Vulnerable Dark Side—Two Laboratory Approaches Lena Lämmle 1,* and Matthias Ziegler 2 1 Department of Psychology, Medical School Hamburg, Faculty of Human Sciences, MSH Medical School Hamburg, 20457 Hamburg, Germany 2 Department of Psychology, Faculty of Life Sciences, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: The Dark Triad of personality has been associated with aggression against others as a reaction to perceived provocations. However, previous work has also shown that such responsive aggression even occurs if it means harming oneself. The first of two laboratory studies aimed to investigate whether this relation between the Dark Triad and self-harming behavior also occurs in situations where no others are affected but self-harm is likely. The second laboratory study considered two different settings in a within-participants design in order to analyze the stability of self-harming behavior and to what extent the Dark Triad constructs influence this behavior. The sample for study 1 consisted of 151 students (45.7% female) with a mean age of 21.40 years (SD = 2.19); the sample for study 2 consisted of 251 students (76.0% female) with a mean age of 22.21 years (SD = 3.90). Aside from the Dark Triad’s common core, depending on how self-harm was triggered (ego-threat (mainly narcissism), being alone with one’s own thoughts (mainly psychopathy), or reward condition (mainly Machiavellianism)), the Dark Triad traits differed in their responsiveness but were stable over the last two conditions, thereby suggesting a vulnerable side of the Dark Triad.
    [Show full text]
  • The Empathy Paradox: Increasing Disconnection in the Age of Increasing Connection
    The Empathy Paradox: Increasing disconnection in the age of increasing connection Cite as: Konrath, S. (in press, 2012) The empathy paradox: Increasing disconnection in the age of increasing connection. In Handbook of Research on Technoself: Identity in a Technological Society, Rocci Luppicini (Ed.), IGI Global. Sara Konrath Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, United States 426 Thompson Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48104 Email: [email protected] Phone: 734-763-1512 Author bio: Sara Konrath received her PhD in Social Psychology from the University of Michigan in 2007. She is currently an Assistant Research Professor at the Institute for Social Research at University of Michigan, with affiliations in Social Psychology (University of Michigan) and Psychiatry (University of Rochester). Konrath is the director of the Interdisciplinary Program on Empathy and Altruism Research (iPEAR), a research lab with a primary focus on empathy, altruism, and other-focused behavior. Her work examines causes, consequences, and changes in empathy and related traits from a variety of perspectives, including psychophysiological and neurological. Konrath’s work has been funded by the American Association for University Women, the Hay Group, and the Templeton Foundation. See sarakonrath.com for recent publications. 1 ABSTRACT The purpose of this chapter is to summarize changes in personality traits that have co-occurred with the rise of new social media, and to evaluate the plausibility of the hypothesis that new social media are a partial explanation for these dramatic changes. Studies have found a rise in social disconnection among recent generations of young Americans. Self-esteem and narcissism have been rising in college students from the late 1970s to 2010, with simultaneous declines in empathy.
    [Show full text]
  • On Within-Defense Variability and Defenses in Male Federal Offenders
    On Within-Defense Variability and Defenses in Male Federal Offenders A thesis submitted to the College of Graduate Studies and Research, in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Psychology University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon © Michael Sheppard, November 2009, all rights reserved Within-defense variability i PERMISSION TO USE In presenting this dissertation in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a Postgraduate degree from the University of Saskatchewan, I agree that the Libraries of this university may make it freely available for inspection. I further agree that permission for copying of this dissertation in any manner, in whole or in part, for scholarly purposes may be granted by the professor or professors who supervised my dissertation work, or, in their absence, by the Head of the Department or the Dean of the College in which my thesis work was done. It is understood that any copying or publication or use of this dissertation or parts thereof for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. It is also understood that due recognition shall be given to me and to the University of Saskatchewan in any scholarly use which may be made of any material in my dissertation. DISCLAIMER Reference in this dissertation to any specific commercial products, process, or service my trade name, trademark, or otherwise, does not constitute or imply its endorsement, recommendation, or favouring by the University of Saskatchewan. The views and opinions of the author expressed herein do not state or reflect those of the University of Saskatchewan, and shall not be used for advertising or product endorsement purposes.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Shame in General, Intimate and Sexual Violence Perpetration
    See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/264897258 The role of shame in general, intimate and sexual violence perpetration Chapter · January 2014 READS 2,626 4 authors, including: Lauren Hansen Aaron Kivisto University of Indianapolis University of Indianapolis 3 PUBLICATIONS 0 CITATIONS 26 PUBLICATIONS 137 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Aaron Kivisto letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 07 August 2016 Running head: SHAME AND VIOLENCE PERPETRATION The Role of Shame in General, Intimate, and Sexual Violence Perpetration Devona Gruber, B.A. Lauren Hansen, B.A. Katrina Soaper, M.A. Aaron J. Kivisto, Ph.D. University of Indianapolis School of Psychological Sciences Recommended Citation: Gruber, D., Hansen, L., Soaper, K., & Kivisto, A.J. (2014). The role of shame in general, intimate, and sexual violence perpetration. In K.G. Lockhart (Ed.), Psychology of Shame: New Research. New York: Nova Science. Address correspondence to: Aaron J. Kivisto, Ph.D., Assistant Professor of Clinical Psychology, University of Indianapolis, School of Psychological Sciences, 1400 East Hanna Avenue, Indianapolis, IN 46227. Phone: 317-788-3541. Email: [email protected]. SHAME AND VIOLENCE PERPETRATION 2 The Role of Shame in General, Intimate, and Sexual Violence Perpetration The relationship between violence and shame is complex, bidirectional, and interactive, although research has tended to emphasize certain aspects of this association and to deemphasize others. In particular, much has been written about shame as a consequence of violence victimization, suggesting that victims of interpersonal violence are at greater risk for developing global self-devaluations (cf.
    [Show full text]