A Sentence Completion Measure of Kohut And

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Sentence Completion Measure of Kohut And A SENTENCE COMPLETION MEASURE OF KOHUT AND WOLF’S NARCISSISTIC PERSONALITY TYPES by Terence David Estrin B.A., University of British Columbia, 1987 M.A., Simon Fraser University, 1995 Dissertation Submitted in Partial Fulfilment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Psychology © Terence David Estrin, 2003 SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY April, 2003 All rights reserved. This work may not be reproduced in whole or part, by photocopy or other means, without permission of the author. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. National Library Bibliotheque nationale of Canada du Canada Acquisitions and Acquisisitons et Bibliographic Services services bibliographiques 395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington Ottawa ON K1A 0N4 Ottawa ON K1A 0N4 Canada Canada Your file Votre reference ISBN: 0-612-81583-8 Our file Notre reference ISBN: 0-612-81583-8 The author has granted a non­ L'auteur a accorde une licence non exclusive licence allowing the exclusive permettant a la National Library of Canada to Bibliotheque nationale du Canada de reproduce, loan, distribute or sell reproduire, preter, distribuer ou copies of this thesis in microform, vendre des copies de cette these sous paper or electronic formats. la forme de microfiche/film, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format electronique. The author retains ownership of theL'auteur conserve la propriete du copyright in this thesis. Neither thedroit d'auteur qui protege cette these. thesis nor substantial extracts from it Ni la these ni des extraits substantiels may be printed or otherwise de celle-ci ne doivent etre imprimes reproduced without the author's ou aturement reproduits sans son permission. autorisation. Canada Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. PARTIAL COPYRIGHT LICENCE I hereby grant to Simon Fraser University the right to lend my thesis, project or extended essay (the title of which is shown below) to users of the Simon Fraser University Library, and to make partial or single copies only for such users or in response to a request from the library of any other university, or other educational institution, on its own behalf or for one of its users. I further agree that permission for multiple copying o f this work for scholarly purposes may be granted by me or the Dean of Graduate Studies. It is understood that copying or publication of this work for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. Title of Thesis/Project/Extended Essay: A Sentence Completion Measure of Kohut and Wolf’s Narcissistic Personality Types Author: (Signature) Terence David Estrin >■ ( I f 0 1 (Date) Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPROVAL Name: Terence David Estrin Degree: Doctor of Philosophy (Psychology) Title of Dissertation: A Sentence Completion Measure o f Kohut and Wolfs Narcissistic Personality Types Examining Committee: Chair: Dr. John McDonald Assistant Profe: ^ Dr. Jame rcia Profess Senio Dr. Ray Koo Associate Pr Dr. Andrea Kowaz Registrar, British Columbia College of Psychologists Dr. RobeiTTev InternaTExternal Examiner Associate Professor Department of Psychology Simon Fraser University )r. Michael Cnandler External E^Mniner Professor Department of Psychology University of British Columbia Date Approved: 0 3 ^ 1 ^ 2-oq_£ Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ABSTRACT The present study reconstructed a Sentence Completion test and scoring manual (Estrin, 1994) originally designed to measure Kohut and W olfs (1978) mirror-hungry, ideal-hungry, and twnship-hungry subclinical narcissistic personalities in a student sample. In order to extend the test’s range, 140 student and 20 psychotherapy client participants were recruited. Operational definitions were revised and subscales constructed according to theory-driven rules, resulting in a 24-item test with 3 subscales representing the behavioral expression of the narcissistic need (ml, H, t1), and 3 representing narcissistic vulnerability (m2, i2, t2). Inter-rater reliability was satisfactory for all subscales except H. Student participants demonstrated greater ideal- hunger and grandiose narcissism. Females scored higher than males on narcissistic vulnerability. An analysis of scoring patterns determined that, as expected, mirror-hunger was most prevalent, and twinship-hunger the least. A principal components analysis resulted in 4 oblique factors, suggesting both vulnerable and grandiose mirror-hungry narcissism, and the attraction and disappointment dynamics underlying twinship-hunger. Aggregated content validity findings were consistent with theoretical models for mirror- and twinship-hunger, but not ideal-hunger. All subscales except H demonstrated convergence with self-rating measures of the personality constructs, but predicted associations between subscales and corresponding other-rating scales completed by therapists of clinical participants were not supported. Convergent validity was not demonstrated for the H subscale, whereas i2 was related to defensiveness. Twinship-hungry scales were related to dependency and reassurance-seeking (for t1), and risk-avoidance (for t2). The combination of the m l and m2 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. IV subscales conformed to Kohut & Wolfs (1978) description of the mirror-hungry type as being both attention- and recognition-seeking, and defensive. As predicted, the m l (attention-seeking) subscale was related to overt narcissism, whereas the m2 (vulnerability) subscale was associated with both overt and covert characteristics. Product term multiple regression analyses were inconclusive in attempting to demonstrate: a) that the low m l / high m2 scoring profile was related to covert narcissism, and b) that narcissistic vulnerability (as manifested by the high m l / high m2 scoring profile) was associated with overt narcissism. However, cumulative evidence suggested a relationship between overt narcissism and narcissistic vulnerability. Overall findings supported the validity of the mirror-hungry scale as a measure of exhibitionistic-vulnerable subclinical narcissism. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. DEDICATION For Yael and Orly, with love. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. VI ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS While working on this dissertation, I often felt as if I had set off on an ocean voyage — while still in the process of constructing the boat. Fortunately, it was anything but a solo trip. To James Marcia, my senior supervisor, I owe a debt of gratitude: his belief in the value of this undertaking, ability to perceive the big picture (especially at those times when I did not), and unstinting support and patience kept this project on course. I would also like to thank Ray Koopman, my statistics advisor, for making himself available on a daily basis during six months of intensive work. I learned more about statistics in those six months than all the years of coursework combined. I am forever grateful to Felicity Estrin, my patient wife, who heard every single idea presented in this dissertation, read and commented on every word of text, and served as response-typist. Her love, support and enthusiasm sustained me. I would also like to thank Elizabeth Michno and Joan Foster, for their (daily) assistance with the execution of the analyses and for their moral support, and to Kate Slaney, who, along with Elizabeth and Joan, doggedly (and good-naturedly) persisted in getting the dreaded LISREL program to work. Thanks also to Joan Wolfe, for extensive help with formatting and de-bugging the dissertation document. To my co-raters and dear friends Alison Bell and Renee Wears, I have but one thing to say: never would I have guessed that I would actually enjoy scoring 4,560 sentence completions. I am grateful to Colleen Wilkie, former co-director of the SFU Clinical Psychology Center, for facilitating that portion of the research done at the CPC, and for her extensive Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. V ll assistance in developing the consent forms. I would also like to thank the therapists and clients who gave freely of their time to take part in this study, and, of course, the student participants, each of whom completed three hours of testing. In their capacity as unofficial statistics advisors, thanks are due to Natalie Thompson of the UBC department of statistics, and Tsur Somerville of the UBC department of Commerce. Special thanks to Suzanne, who understood the true meaning of this voyage, and to Yitzchak and Chana, who always believed in me. Last of all, I would like to thank my parents, Larry and Dana Estrin, whose love and generosity made this all possible. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. TABLE OF CONTENTS APPROVAL.......................................................................................................................II ABSTRACT...................................................... Ill DEDICATION................................................................................................................. V ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS............................................................................................VI
Recommended publications
  • Why Susie Sells Seashells by the Seashore: Implicit Egotism and Major Life Decisions
    ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Why Susie Sells Seashells by the Seashore: Implicit Egotism and Major Life Decisions Brett W. Pelham, Matthew C. Mirenberg, and John T. Jones State University of New York at Buffalo Because most people possess positive associations about themselves, most people prefer things that are connected to the self (e.g., the letters in one’s name). The authors refer to such preferences as implicit egotism. Ten studies assessed the role of implicit egotism in 2 major life decisions: where people choose to live and what people choose to do for a living. Studies 1–5 showed that people are disproportionately likely to live in places whose names resemble their own first or last names (e.g., people named Louis are disproportionately likely to live in St. Louis). Study 6 extended this finding to birthday number preferences. People were disproportionately likely to live in cities whose names began with their birthday numbers (e.g., Two Harbors, MN). Studies 7–10 suggested that people disproportionately choose careers whose labels resemble their names (e.g., people named Dennis or Denise are overrepresented among dentists). Implicit egotism appears to influence major life decisions. This idea stands in sharp contrast to many models of rational choice and attests to the importance of understanding implicit beliefs. What role do people’s thoughts and feelings about themselves important role in major life decisions. For example, only a handful play in their important day-to-day decisions and behaviors? Con- of studies have examined whether self-regulation processes influ- temporary research on the self-concept suggests many answers to ence people’s choice of relationship partners.
    [Show full text]
  • Status Goods: Experimental Evidence from Platinum Credit Cards∗
    Status Goods: Experimental Evidence from Platinum Credit Cards∗ Leonardo Bursztyny Bruno Fermanz Stefano Fiorinx Martin Kanz{ Gautam Raok September 2017 Abstract This paper provides field-experimental evidence on status goods. We work with an Indonesian bank that markets platinum credit cards to high-income customers. In a first experiment, we show that demand for the platinum card exceeds demand for a nondescript control product with identical benefits, suggesting demand for the pure status aspect of the card. Transaction data reveal that platinum cards are more likely to be used in social contexts, implying social image motivations. In a second experiment, we provide evidence of positional externalities from the consumption of these status goods. A final experiment provides suggestive evidence that increasing self-esteem causally reduces demand for status goods, indicating that social image might be a substitute for self image. (JEL: D03, D12, Z13) ∗We would like to thank Nava Ashraf, Abhijit Banerjee, Roland B´enabou, Stefano DellaVigna, Benjamin Enke, Robert Frank, Matthew Gentzkow, Ori Heffetz, David Laibson, Supreet Kaur, David McKenzie, Markus Mobius, Ricardo Perez-Truglia, Christopher Roth, Andrei Shleifer, Kelly Shue, Lise Vesterlund, David Yanagizawa-Drott, numerous seminar participants, four anonymous referees and the Editors for helpful comments and suggestions. The first experiment in this study was pre-registered at the AEA's Social Science Registry (RCT ID AEARCTR- 0000828, available at https://www.socialscienceregistry.org/trials/828). Financial support from the World Bank Strategic Research Program is gratefully acknowledged. The study was approved by the UCLA Institutional Review Board. The opinions expressed in this paper do not necessarily represent the views of the World Bank, its Executive Directors, or the countries they represent.
    [Show full text]
  • Relationships and the Self: Egosystem and Ecosystem
    C HAPTER 4 RELATIONSHIPS AND THE SELF: EGOSYSTEM AND ECOSYSTEM Jennifer Crocker and Amy Canevello Most social scientists assume that people are funda- Crocker & Canevello, 2008, 2012b;ASSOCIATION Crocker, mentally self-interested, that they do what they Olivier, & Nuer, 2009). We first describe this perceive to be advantageous to themselves (Miller, system in general terms that apply to most, if not all, 1999). Unsurprisingly, this view pervades research types of relationships. Then, because romantic rela- on the self, in which people are depicted as self- tionships are the focus of a great deal of interest and enhancing and self-protective, seeking validation research, we describe the principles of romantic and affirmation, and taking credit for successes but relationships driven by egosystem motivation. dodging blame for failures. Perhaps more surprising, We next describe an emerging alternate view, in this view also pervades a great deal of research on which people have the capacity to transcend self- relationships, which presumably involve shared interestPSYCHOLOGICAL and care deeply about people and things bonds between people and feelings such as close- beyond themselves, which we call ecosystem motiva- ness, caring, affection, or love. Many researchers tion (Crocker, 2008; Crocker & Canevello, 2008, assume that people in relationships, as in the rest of 2012b; Crocker et al., 2006). In relationships, their lives, are fundamentally self-centered and people driven by ecosystem motivation seek to pro- self-serving. In this view, people want to AMERICANbe in rela- mote the well-being of the relationship partner not tionships to promote their own ends, ©they use rela- out of selfish motives to obtain benefits in return, tionship partners to satisfy their own needs, and but because they care about the partner or because they sacrifice and compromise in relationships to both people care about the well-being of someone or keep their relationship partners happy so they can something beyond themselves.
    [Show full text]
  • Origins of Narcissism in Children
    Origins of narcissism in children Eddie Brummelmana,b,1, Sander Thomaesb,c, Stefanie A. Nelemansd, Bram Orobio de Castrob, Geertjan Overbeeka, and Brad J. Bushmane,f aResearch Institute of Child Development and Education, Department of Educational Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1001 NG, The Netherlands; bDepartment of Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht 3584 CS, The Netherlands; cCenter for Research on Self and Identity, Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton SO17 1BJ, England; dResearch Centre Adolescent Development, Department of Youth and Family, Utrecht University, Utrecht 3584 CS, The Netherlands; eDepartment of Communication and Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210-1339; and fDepartment of Communication Science, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1081 HV, The Netherlands Edited by Susan T. Fiske, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved February 12, 2015 (received for review November 7, 2014) Narcissism levels have been increasing among Western youth, and (9) and “are under a compulsion to ascribe every perfection to contribute to societal problems such as aggression and violence. the child—which sober observation would find no occasion to The origins of narcissism, however, are not well understood. Here, do” (10). Consequently, children might internalize the belief that we report, to our knowledge, the first prospective longitudinal they are special individuals who are entitled to privileges. In evidence on the origins of narcissism in children. We compared contrast, psychoanalytic theory holds that children are likely to two perspectives: social learning theory (positing that narcissism is grow up to be narcissistic when their parents lack warmth toward cultivated by parental overvaluation) and psychoanalytic theory them (11, 12).
    [Show full text]
  • The Dirty Dozen: a Concise Measure of the Dark Triad
    Psychological Assessment © 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 22, No. 2, 420–432 1040-3590/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019265 The Dirty Dozen: A Concise Measure of the Dark Triad Peter K. Jonason Gregory D. Webster University of West Florida University of Florida There has been an exponential increase of interest in the dark side of human nature during the last decade. To better understand this dark side, the authors developed and validated a concise, 12-item measure of the Dark Triad: narcissism, psychopathy, Machiavellianism. In 4 studies involving 1,085 participants, they examined its structural reliability, convergent and discriminant validity (Studies 1, 2, and 4), and test–retest reliability (Study 3). Their measure retained the flexibility needed to measure these 3 independent-yet-related constructs while improving its efficiency by reducing its item count by 87% (from 91 to 12 items). The measure retained its core of disagreeableness, short-term mating, and aggressiveness. They call this measure the Dirty Dozen, but it cleanly measures the Dark Triad. Keywords: Dark Triad, narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy, measurement The Dark Triad is a term used to describe a constellation of quiring scores on each measure to be standardized (Jonason, Li, three socially undesirable personality traits: narcissism, psychop- Webster, & Schmitt, 2009). athy, and Machiavellianism (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Research Second, assessing the Dark Triad’s 91 items is inefficient, on the Dark Triad has increased exponentially over the last decade. time-consuming, and may cause response fatigue in some partic- An analysis of Google Scholar hit counts for “Dark Triad” in ipants. When studying the Dark Triad and one or more other scientific works reveals an explosive increase from one in 2002 to measures of interest (e.g., self-esteem, Big Five personality traits), at least 38 in 2009.
    [Show full text]
  • An Investigation of Pro-Environmentalism in Communal Narcissists" (2018)
    Fairfield University DigitalCommons@Fairfield Business Faculty Publications Charles F. Dolan School of Business 2018 I'm nice, therefore I go green: An investigation of pro- environmentalism in communal narcissists Iman Naderi Fairfield University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.fairfield.edu/business-facultypubs Copyright 2018 Springer This work is released under a Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial No Derivatives License. Peer Reviewed Repository Citation Naderi, Iman, "I'm nice, therefore I go green: An investigation of pro-environmentalism in communal narcissists" (2018). Business Faculty Publications. 219. https://digitalcommons.fairfield.edu/business-facultypubs/219 Published Citation Naderi, Iman. I'm nice, therefore I go green: An investigation of pro-environmentalism in communal narcissists. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 59 (2018): 54-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.08.010. This item has been accepted for inclusion in DigitalCommons@Fairfield by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@Fairfield. It is brought to you by DigitalCommons@Fairfield with permission from the rights- holder(s) and is protected by copyright and/or related rights. You are free to use this item in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses, you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/or on the work itself. For more information, please contact [email protected]. I’m Nice, Therefore I Go Green: An Investigation of Pro-environmentalism in Communal Narcissists Journal of Environmental Psychology Link: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.08.010 Iman Naderi, Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Impulsivity and the Self-Defeating Behavior of Narcissists
    Personality and Social Psychology Review Copyright © 2006 by 2006, Vol. 10, No. 2, 154–165 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Impulsivity and the Self-Defeating Behavior of Narcissists Simine Vazire Department of Psychology The University of Texas at Austin David C. Funder Department of Psychology University of California, Riverside Currently prominent models of narcissism (e.g., Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001) primarily explain narcissists’self-defeating behaviors in terms of conscious cognitive and affec- tive processes. We propose that the disposition of impulsivity may also play an impor- tant role. We offer 2 forms of evidence. First, we present a meta-analysis demonstrat- ing a strong positive relationship between narcissism and impulsivity. Second, we review and reinterpret the literature on 3 hallmarks of narcissism: self-enhancement, aggression, and negative long-term outcomes. Our reinterpretation argues that impulsivity provides a more parsimonious explanation for at least some of narcissists’ self-defeating behavior than do existing models. These 2 sources of evidence suggest that narcissists’ quest for the status and recognition they so intensely desire is thwarted, in part, by their lack of the self-control necessary to achieve those goals. Narcissists are a puzzle. Their bragging and arro- This article begins by proposing that the widely ac- gance interferes with the attainment of the status and cepted cognitive–affective processing model presented recognition they so poignantly desire. Why do they by Morf and Rhodewalt (2001) be extended to include continually undermine themselves in this way? The re- dispositional impulsivity. We then review the empirical search literature appears to have achieved some con- evidence for our proposal by presenting a sensus about the nature of sub-clinical narcissism1 meta-analysis of the relationship between narcissism with respect to underlying cognitive, social, and affec- and impulsivity, including unpublished results from tive processes (e.g., Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001).
    [Show full text]
  • THE GIFTS of Imperfection
    THE GIFTS OF Imperfection THE GIFTS OF Imperfection Let Go of Who You Think You’re Supposed to Be and Embrace Who You Are by Brené Brown, Ph.D., L.M.S.W. Hazelden Center City, Minnesota 55012 hazelden.org © 2010 by Brené Brown All rights reserved. Published 2010 Printed in the United States of America No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means—electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise—without the express written permission of the publisher. Failure to comply with these terms may expose you to legal action and damages for copyright infringement. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Brown, C. Brené The gifts of imperfection : let go of who you think you’re supposed to be and embrace who you are / by Brené Brown. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references. ISBN 978-1-59285-849-1 (softcover) ISBN 978-1-59285-989-4 (eBook) 1. Self-acceptance. 2. Self-esteem. I. Title. BF575.S37B76 2010 158—dc22 2010016989 Editor’s note The names, details, and circumstances may have been changed to protect the privacy of those mentioned in this publication. This publication is not intended as a substitute for the advice of health care professionals. 13 12 11 10 1 2 3 4 5 6 Cover design by David Spohn Cover illustrations by Nicholas Wilton Interior design and typesetting by Kinne Design To Steve, Ellen, and Charlie. I love you with my whole heart. contents Preface Acknowledgments Introduction: Wholehearted Living Courage, Compassion, and Connection:
    [Show full text]
  • Intellectual Humility and the Curse of Knowledge
    Forthcoming in Arrogance and Polarization. Eds. A. Tanesini and M. Lynch. Routledge. Intellectual Humility and the Curse of Knowledge Michael Hannon University of Nottingham This chapter explores an unappreciated psychological dimension of intellectual humility. In particular, I argue there is a plausible connection between intellectual humility and epistemic egocentrism. Epistemic egocentrism is a well-known cognitive bias – often called ‘the curse of knowledge’ – whereby an agent attributes his or her own mental states to other people. I hypothesize that an individual who exhibits this bias is more likely to possess a variety of traits that are characteristic of intellectual humility. This is surprising because intellectual humility is often regarded as an antidote to cognitive biases, whereas I claim that a particular cognitive bias (namely, the curse of knowledge) may help foster an intellectual virtue. Introduction Intellectual humility is widely considered a virtue.1 Although there is no consensus within philosophy or psychology on a precise definition of intellectual humility, it has been linked to an assortment of desirable traits. For example, intellectual humble people have been said to: not think too highly of themselves; not ascribe to themselves greater excellence than they possesses; show low concern for how special their talents are; take complaints and criticism seriously; generously acknowledge the contributions of others; and not demand special treatment, even 1 For example, see Church and Barrett (2016), Hazlett (2012), Roberts and Wood (2003), Samuelson et al. (2015), Tanesini (2018), and Whitcomb et al. (2017). when deserving. This virtue has been studied by philosophers, psychologists, and theologians, and it seems to involve a mix of cognitive, emotional, behavioural, and motivational components.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem in Relation to the Dark Triad
    © 2014 WUPJ, September 2014, Volume 2 Personality Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem in Relation to the Dark Triad Lauren Stenason* Individuals with characteristics encompassing the three domains of the Dark Triad (psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism) are often thought to possess inflated levels of self-esteem however, there has been controversy as to whether this inflated self-esteem represents their true underlying level of self-esteem. The present study investigated the relationships between the Dark Triad, explicit, and implicit self-esteem. Participants consisted of 231 individuals from The University of Western Ontario with an age range of 17 to 45 years (M = 19.390, SD = 2.908). 81% of participants were female and 19% of participants were male. Participants completed an online survey consisting of the Short Dark Triad, Rosenberg Self- Esteem Scale, and the Name Letter Task. The analysis revealed that Machiavellianism showed a weak negative correlation with last name initial. However, Machiavellianism and explicit self-esteem were not significantly correlated with first name initial. Narcissism showed a moderate positive correlation with explicit self-esteem and a weak positive correlation with last name initial but no significant relationship between narcissism and first name initial. Psychopathy did not reveal any significant correlations with either explicit or implicit self-esteem. Multiple regression analysis revealed that narcissism and psychopathy added significantly to the prediction of explicit self-esteem. Self-esteem is involved in many domains The components of the Dark Triad are regarding personality, psychological disorders, considered maladaptive personality traits that and overall mental health. Different types of include narcissism, psychopathy, and personality characteristics involve varying Machiavellianism.
    [Show full text]
  • 15 Status Symbols, Ecosystems and Sustainability
    15 Status Symbols, Ecosystems and Sustainability Arthur Fishman Abstract Why is it that so many of the consumption goods we value and love are ecologically degrading? From ivory to rice cultivation in semi arid regions, examples abound of goods so highly desired, even when functional substitutes exist, as to generate ecologically unsustainable production levels that lead to extinctions and depletion of precious resources. While the reasons for the high valuation of these goods are complex and manifold, this paper suggests that one factor is a taste for rarity: the rarer an item the more humans will covet it and hence the greater will be its exploitation. Thus these products have become so sought after not in spite of, but precisely because of, their ecologically low supply. The research question This essay addresses the following question: Why is it that so many of the consumption goods we value and love are ecologically degrading? More puzzling, we continue to prize these goods even when adequate substitutes which are less damaging to the environment are available. Recent research suggests that in many cases, tragically, ecologically degrading consumption goods are valued not in spite of their negative ecological impact but precisely because the ecological unsustainability of their consumption. We introduce the concept of rarity value. This refers to an intrinsic value attributed to a good solely on account of its rarity, independently of its 316 Status, Symbols, Ecosystem and Sustainability functionality or lack thereof. Goods which are ecologically limited in supply are thus more highly sought after. Clearly we expect the price of an item to be higher the rarer it is.
    [Show full text]
  • Being in Afro-Brazilian Families by Elizabeth Hordge
    Home is Where the Hurt Is: Racial Socialization, Stigma, and Well- Being in Afro-Brazilian Families By Elizabeth Hordge Freeman Department of Sociology Duke University Date: _____________________ Approved: ________________________________________ Eduardo Bonilla-Silva, Co-Supervisor _________________________________________ Linda George, Co-Supervisor _________________________________________ Lynn Smith-Lovin _________________________________________ Linda Burton __________________________________________ Sherman James Dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Sociology in the Graduate School of Duke University 2012 ! ABSTRACT Home is Where the Hurt Is: Racial Socialization, Stigma, and Well- Being in Afro-Brazilian Families By Elizabeth Hordge Freeman Department of Sociology Duke University Date: _____________________ Approved: ________________________________________ Eduardo Bonilla-Silva, Co-Supervisor _________________________________________ Linda George, Co-Supervisor _________________________________________ Lynn Smith-Lovin _________________________________________ Linda Burton __________________________________________ Sherman James An abstract submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Sociology in the Graduate School of Duke University 2012 ! ! Copyright by Elizabeth Hordge Freeman 2012 ! Abstract This dissertation examines racial socialization in Afro-Brazilian families in
    [Show full text]