A Problem of Cosmic Proportions: Floyd Henry Allport and the Concept of Collectivity in American Social Psychology

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Problem of Cosmic Proportions: Floyd Henry Allport and the Concept of Collectivity in American Social Psychology A PROBLEM OF COSMIC PROPORTIONS: FLOYD HENRY ALLPORT AND THE CONCEPT OF COLLECTIVITY IN AMERICAN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CATHY FAYE A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY GRADUATE PROGRAM IN PSYCHOLOGY YORK UNIVERSITY TORONTO, ONTARIO October 2013 © Cathy Faye, 2013 ii Abstract Floyd Henry Allport (1890-1978) is widely regarded as a significant figure in the establishment of experimental social psychology in the United States in the early twentieth century. His famous 1924 textbook and his early experimental work helped set the stage for a social psychology characterized by individualism, behaviorism, and experiment. Allport is particularly well-known for his banishment of the group concept from social psychology and his argument that the individual is the only viable, scientific object of study for the serious social psychologist. This early part of Allport’s career and the role it played in establishing American social psychology is relatively well documented. However, there is little scholarship regarding Allport’s work after the 1920s. An examination of this time period demonstrates that Allport’s earliest individualism was in fact rather short-lived, as he subjected it to serious revision in the early decades of the twentieth century. The increasing complexity of the bureaucratic structure of American society in the early 1900s, the economic collapse of the 1930s, and the onset of the Second World War were significant events in the development of Allport’s ideas regarding the individual. While his early work is marked by a concerted effort to create an ideal science for understanding the individual and the social, his later work was much more concerned with the social implications of individualism and collectivism. As the social world around him grew more complex, so too did his own social psychology, culminating in a significant change in Allport’s philosophy of science. These findings contribute to our understanding of social psychology and its history by: providing a novel view of one of social psychology’s central historical figures; demonstrating the difficult, persistent, and context-dependent nature of the individualism-collectivism divide in American social psychology; and providing a platform for thinking about the ways in which historians remember and write the stories of important figures in the field. iii Acknowledgements I would like to thank my advisor Christopher Green for all of his assistance and patience with this project and every project through my graduate years. I must also thank him for that first trip to Syracuse and for introducing me to the many joys (and frustrations!) of archival work. I would also like to thank my dissertation committee, Ward Struthers, Ian Nicholson, Kenton Kroker, and especially Alexandra Rutherford and Thomas Teo, who had helped me in so many ways over the years. I was also fortunate to learn much about the history of psychology from Raymond Fancher and Michael Pettit, both of whom contributed to this work indirectly. I would also like to thank Sam Parkovnick, who has always been more than generous with his time, his ideas, and his own work on the history of social psychology and on the life and work of Allport. It was always nice to know that someone other than me thought Floyd was at least as interesting as Gordon! Thanks to my sister who was always the only one forward enough to tell me to stop complaining and just write. Thanks are also due to Zdravko Marjanovic and Lisa Fiksenbaum for keeping me somewhat sane and mostly laughing. I benefited greatly from the collective wisdom and friendship of the whole York University history/theory contingent, but special thanks are due to Dan Denis, Kate Harper, and David Clark. Also, I am very grateful for the continued friendship and invaluable mentorship of my undergraduate advisor, Donald Sharpe. I have been fortunate to receive much financial support for this work. The central funding was provided by a doctoral fellowship from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council and from two Ontario Graduate Scholarships provided by the Government of Ontario. Much of the archival travel funding was supported by the Pierre Elliott Trudeau Fellowship at iv York University, as well as the Faculty of Graduate Studies Fieldwork Costs Fund and the Research Costs Fund. I am also very indebted to the staff at the archives I visited in the process of researching this project. This includes the staff at the Byrd Library at Syracuse University, the Pusey Library at Harvard University, and the Bentley Historical Library at the University of Michigan. I am also indebted to the staff at the Resource Sharing Department at the York University Library (particularly Sandra Snell and Mary Lehane), who were almost always successful at digging up all sorts of materials for me. I am also grateful to Joseph James Ahern, the Library Hall Stacks Manager at the American Philosophical Society, for kindly seeking out and sending along some of Allport’s correspondence held in the Society’s collection. v Table of Contents Abstract……………………………………………………………………………………………ii Acknowledgments………………………………………………… …………………...………..iii Table of Contents…………………………………………………………………………….…....v Preface…………………………………………………………………………….……………….1 Introduction……………………………………………………………………….……………….3 The Hero and the Villain………...………………………………………………………...5 Reconstructing Allport……………………………………………..……...…….……….11 Methods and Sources……………………………………………..……...……...……….15 Chapter One: Sociology, Psychology and the Emergence of Social Psychology……….……….18 Debating the Social in Nineteenth-Century Europe…………...…..……………….…….19 Debating the Social in Early Twentieth-Century America…………………………...….27 Chapter Two: Allport at Harvard, 1915-1922….………………………………….………….….42 Allport’s Early Life………………….……………………………………………..…….44 The Beginnings of Allportian Individualism…………………………………………….46 The Beginnings of Social Facilitation Research……………………………………...….57 Chapter Three: The Struggle for Supremacy…………………………………………………….68 Textbook Social Psychology………………….………………………………………….72 A Scientific Social Psychology……………………………………………………….….81 The Group Fallacy and the Response from Social Science…………………………..….89 Chapter Four: Social Psychology, Social Institutions, and Social Control.…………..………...100 Social Science and Social Institutions in 1930s America………………….………..….103 Allport and the Study of Social Institutions……………………………………....…….109 vi Chapter Five: A Tragic Confusion of Values……..……………………………………...…….129 Social Psychology and the Second World War………………….……...………..…….130 Social Science, Society, and Democracy…………………………………………...…..133 Redefining the Role of the Individual in a World at War………………………..……..140 Democracy in the Laboratory……………………………………..…….………….…..144 Beyond Nations: Allportian Individualism in the Postwar World………..……...……..149 Chapter Six: The Master Problem of Social Psychology…………………..……………….…..154 Social Psychology in the Post-War World………………….………………….……….155 The Turn Towards Systems Theories………………………………………………......159 Beyond Individualism………………………………………………………………......163 Event-Structure Theory…………………………..……………………………..……....166 From Event-Structure to Enestruence……………………………………………..…....173 A Problem of Cosmic Proportions…………………………………………….…..…....175 Conclusion…………………………………………………………………………………..….181 Allport’s Influence on American Social Psychology………………….……………….182 After 1924………………………………………………………………...…………….186 Founders and Unfinished Projects……………………………………………………...191 The Hero and the Villain…………..…………………………………………………....196 A New View of Allport’s Role in the History of Social Psychology……..……………202 References…………………………………………………………………………………...….209 1 Preface As an undergraduate student in my first social psychology class, I recall learning about Floyd Henry Allport and the important role he played in establishing social psychology in North America. I do not recall the exact textbook that introduced me to him, but I do recall its tone and content. Allport was described as the father of experimental social psychology, the man who ingeniously demonstrated that the subject matter of social psychology could be studied using experimental methods. Prior to Allport’s groundbreaking work, the author noted, the field simply did not exist; the kinds of topics we now explore in social psychology were explored by armchair philosophers. For a young student enamored with the impressive and seemingly highly relevant experimental findings of the field—the Hawthorne effect, cognitive dissonance, Milgram’s obedience studies—Floyd Allport seemed an excellent and most worthy ancestor. As I moved through my education, encountering social psychology again and again—first in advanced undergraduate seminars, and later in the context of graduate coursework and thesis writing—this view of Allport became increasingly complicated. It seemed that many contemporary social psychologists had little to say about Allport, nominating Kurt Lewin or Leon Festinger instead as the progenitor of their practice. I recall browsing The Heart of Social Psychology, a book that—according to its subtitle—offered a “backstage view of a passionate science” (Aron & Aron, 1986). I was surprised to find that Allport played but a bit part in that narrative and, in fact, the authors did not seem to look very kindly on his contributions. As I began to study the history of social psychology more intensely, the situation became even more convoluted. These narratives
Recommended publications
  • Introduction to Social Psychology Question Pool
    Introduction to Social psychology question pool Edited by Paszkál Kiss 1 CONTENTS Titles, Topics ...................................................................................................................................................................... 3 Authors .............................................................................................................................................................................. 3 Social Psychology 1. Readings ........................................................................................................................................... 4 Social Psychology 2. Readings ........................................................................................................................................... 6 Test Questions .................................................................................................................................................................. 9 Methods in social psychology ........................................................................................................................................... 9 Measuring attitudes, attitude scales ............................................................................................................................ 9 Experiments in social psychology ............................................................................................................................... 12 Qualitative research methods....................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Psychological Bulletin
    Vol. 19, No. 8 August, 1922 THE PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN CONTRIBUTIONS TO THE HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY— 1916-1921 By COLEMAN R. GRIFFITH University of Illinois Contributions to the history of psychology since 1915 fall nat- urally into two groups. There are, on the one hand, the systematic and the experimental studies which have made the science six years older. There are, on the other hand, the historical and biographical notes and the large and searching retrospections which relate con- temporary psychology to earlier stages in the development of the science. It is to an enumeration of these contributions that the present paper is devoted. We cannot, of course, comment in detail upon the character of contemporary psychology, for an historian, speaking of his own times is like the Hawaiian surf-rider who seeks to judge the incoming tide from his experiences while riding a single wave; but it is possible to get a certain amount of information about the current trend of a science by considering events of various kinds which reflect or which have, presumably, influenced its general course. These events taken in conjunction with what appear, at the present moment, to be outstanding experimental and systematic studies may give a suggestion of the history of the last few years. In psychology, as well as in many other sciences, the most important event, during the period under survey, was the World War. The science of psychology was, as all know, well on the way toward complete mobilization for military purposes when the war ended. Since many of the consequences of this reorganization have not yet appeared, a detailed account of this aspect of the period must fall to a future historian.
    [Show full text]
  • Classical Crowd Psychology, Ideology and the Social Identity Approach
    Contemporary understanding of riots: classical crowd psychology, ideology and the social identity approach Article (Accepted Version) Stott, Clifford and Drury, John (2016) Contemporary understanding of riots: classical crowd psychology, ideology and the social identity approach. Public Understanding of Science, 26 (1). pp. 2-14. ISSN 0963-6625 This version is available from Sussex Research Online: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/60239/ This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version. Copyright and reuse: Sussex Research Online is a digital repository of the research output of the University. Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. To the extent reasonable and practicable, the material made available in SRO has been checked for eligibility before being made available. Copies of full text items generally can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any format or medium for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge, provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. http://sro.sussex.ac.uk Contemporary understanding of riots: classical crowd psychology, ideology and the social identity approach.
    [Show full text]
  • It's Contagious: Rethinking a Metaphor Dialogically
    Article Culture & Psychology 2015, Vol. 21(3) 359–379 It’s contagious: ! The Author(s) 2015 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav Rethinking a metaphor DOI: 10.1177/1354067X15601190 dialogically cap.sagepub.com Zachary J Warren Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA Se´amus A Power University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA Abstract ‘‘Contagion’’ appears frequently in peer-reviewed articles and in popular media to explain the spread of ideas, feelings, and behaviors. In the context of social science, however, we argue that this metaphor leads to magical thinking and should be described as a simile, rather than a metaphor. We review literature on ‘‘social contagion’’ using the dialogical paradigm and conclude that peer-reviewed claims tend to correspond with imagined realities from epidemiology rather than social science, including assumptions of passive and linear microbial spread, as well as pathology. We explore case studies on the spread of laughter, riot behavior, and ‘‘mass psychogenic illness,’’ and find that social contagion involves social meanings negotiated at the level of persons and groups that are uncharacteristic to the spread of diseases. Dialogism is presented as a correction to the epidemiological paradigm. Keywords Dialogical theory, contagion theory, psychogenic illness, spread, metaphor, riot behavior, laughter, Bakhtin, Afghanistan, dialogism Introduction In February 2012, the Le Roy School District in Le Roy, New York, reported a ‘‘twitching disease’’ that affected over a dozen teen girls and at least one adult. The New York Department of Health launched an investigation in search of envir- onmental causes, and environmental activist Erin Brochovich sent a team Corresponding author: Se´amus A Power, Department of Comparative Human Development, Social Sciences Research Building, Office 103, 1126 East 59th St, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637, USA.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Implicit Motive Pattern Recognition As Psychometric Signs of Severe Crises Dirk Johannßen Chris Biemann MIN Faculty MIN Faculty Dept
    Social Media Unrest Prediction during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Neural Implicit Motive Pattern Recognition as Psychometric Signs of Severe Crises Dirk Johannßen Chris Biemann MIN Faculty MIN Faculty Dept. of Informatics Dept. of Informatics Universitat¨ Hamburg Universitat¨ Hamburg & Nordakademie 22527 Hamburg, Germany http://lt.informatik.uni-hamburg.de/ fbiemann, [email protected] Abstract The COVID-19 pandemic has caused international social tension and unrest. Besides the crisis itself, there are growing signs of rising conflict potential of societies around the world. Indi- cators of global mood changes are hard to detect and direct questionnaires suffer from social desirability biases. However, so-called implicit methods can reveal humans intrinsic desires from e.g. social media texts. We present psychologically validated social unrest predictors and replicate scalable and automated predictions, setting a new state of the art on a recent German shared task dataset. We employ this model to investigate a change of language towards social unrest during the COVID-19 pandemic by comparing established psychological predictors on samples of tweets from spring 2019 with spring 2020. The results show a significant increase of the conflict-indicating psychometrics. With this work, we demonstrate the applicability of automated NLP-based approaches to quantitative psychological research. 1 Introduction The COVID-19 pandemic and the reactions to it have led to growing social tensions. Guiterrez-´ Romero (2020) studied the effects of social distancing and lockdowns on riots, violence against civilians, and food-related conflicts in 24 African countries. The author found that the risk of riots and violence have increased due to lockdowns.
    [Show full text]
  • History of Psychology
    The Psych 101 Series James C. Kaufman, PhD, Series Editor Department of Educational Psychology University of Connecticut David C. Devonis, PhD, received his doctorate in the history of psychology from the University of New Hampshire’s erstwhile pro- gram in that subject in 1989 with a thesis on the history of conscious pleasure in modern American psychology. Since then he has taught vir- tually every course in the psychology curriculum in his academic odys- sey from the University of Redlands in Redlands, California, and the now-closed Teikyo Marycrest University (formerly Marycrest College in Davenport, Iowa) to—for the past 17 years—Graceland University in Lamoni, Iowa, alma mater of Bruce Jenner and, more famously for the history of psychology, of Noble H. Kelly (1901–1997), eminent con- tributor to psychology’s infrastructure through his many years of ser- vice to the American Board of Examiners in Professional Psychology. Dr. Devonis has been a member of Cheiron: The International Society for the History of Behavioral and Social Sciences since 1990, a con- tributor to many of its activities, and its treasurer for the past 10 years. Currently he is on the editorial board of the American Psychological Association journal History of Psychology and is, with Wade Pickren, coeditor and compiler of the online bibliography History of Psychology in the Oxford Bibliographies Online series. History of Psychology 101 David C. Devonis, PhD Copyright © 2014 Springer Publishing Company, LLC All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or trans- mitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Springer Publishing Company, LLC, or authorization through payment of the appropriate fees to the Copyright Clearance Cen- ter, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400, fax 978-646-8600, [email protected] or on the Web at www.copyright.com.
    [Show full text]
  • Crowd Psychology and American Culture, 1890-1940
    "Mental Epidemics": Crowd Psychology and American Culture, 1890-1940 Eugene E. Leach In 1900, disillusioned with high-powered newspaper work and weary of cities, progressive journalist Ray Stannard Baker quit New York and fled to Arizona. Going west to find himself was a gesture of affiliation sanctified by both national myth and his family folklore of pioneer stock ancestry and his father's move west to start over after failing in business. But the Arizona deserts had no power to heal him. In his memoirs he recounted a moment of reckoning with the omnipotence of crowds: he could not forget the congestion that lay just beyond the horizon. For better or worse, to him America was epitomized by suffocating New York: What a different world I knew from that of my ancestors! They had the wilderness, I had crowds. I found teeming, josding, restless cities; I found immense smoking, roaring industries; I found a labyrinth of tangled communication. I found hugeness and evil.1 Baker decided that learning to navigate this world of crowds would be "the prime test" of the modern citizen. E. A. Ross had a grimmer and more intellectualized encounter with crowds. In 1894, he jotted down "thirty-three distinct means by which society controls its members" in a list that became twenty American Journal of Sociology articles and the popular book Social Control (1901).2 Ross' work grew from his assumption, shared with Frederick Jackson Turner, that the closing of the frontier would 0026-3079/92/3301 -005$ 1.50/0 5 inaugurate a difficult new epoch for America.
    [Show full text]
  • Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations Author(S): J
    Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations Author(s): J. G. Morawski Source: Isis, Vol. 77, No. 2 (Jun., 1986), pp. 219-242 Published by: University of Chicago Press on behalf of History of Science Society Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/232650 Accessed: 22-12-2015 00:42 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. History of Science Society and University of Chicago Press are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Isis. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 129.133.6.95 on Tue, 22 Dec 2015 00:42:52 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations By J. G. Morawski* IN 1929 JAMES ANGELL, president of Yale, announced plans for a unique teaching and research center for those fields "directly concerned with the problems of man's individual and group conduct. The purpose is to correlate knowledge and coordinate technique in related fields that greater progress may be made in the understanding of human life. The time has certainly come once again to attempt a fruitful synthesis of knowledge." The New York Times described the experiment as dismantling the disciplinary "Great Wall of China" and compared it with the Renaissance transformation of knowledge.1 The Insti- tute of Human Relations (IHR), as the center was named, received over $4.5 million from the Rockefeller Foundation for its first decade of operation.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychologists and Physicians in the Borderlands of Science, 1900-1942
    PSYCHOLOGISTS AND PHYSICIANS IN THE BORDERLANDS OF SCIENCE, 1900-1942 By WADE EDWARD PICKREN A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 1995 For my mother: WILLIE MERLE PICKREN, and in memoriam, BILL PICKREN, You taught me to love and work. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to express my deepest gratitude to the chairman of my dissertation committee, Donald A. Dewsbury. Dr. Dewsbury has, from the beginning of this long project, been a model of encouragement, kindness, and unfailing generosity. He has shared his time, his great breadth of learning, his editorial ability, and his materials with me. My understanding of the history of psychology has been greatly deepened by our conversations. I also wish to acknowledge that Dr. Dewsbury has helped me to understand that data is a plural! Dr. Wilse B. Webb has also stimulated much thought in me about what I was doing and where I was going with my ideas. Although I did not avail myself of his wisdom as oft as I would have liked, his voice and his sharp eye were always with me. I hope that, in the future, time will allow me a greater opportunity to benefit from his great knowledge and experience. Both near at hand and from afar, Dr. Toby Appel has blessed me with the keenness of her insight . Her acceptance and friendly corrections of my halting efforts to write history have been much appreciated. One of my most pleasant memories of this experience is that of sitting at a table at iii Cafe Gardens talking about the history of biology or psychology, while hoping to hear some Van Morrison on the house music system.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology of a Revolution by GUSTAVE LE
    THE PSYCHOLOGY OF REVOLUTION BY GUSTAVE LE BON CONTENTS INTRODUCTION. THE REVISION OF HISTORY PART I THE PSYCHOLOGICAL ELEMENTS OF REVOLUTIONARY MOVEMENTS BOOK I GENERAL CHARACTERISTICS OF REVOLUTIONS CHAPTER I. SCIENTIFIC AND POLITICAL REVOLUTIONS 1. Classification of Revolutions 2. Scientific Revolutions 3. Political Revolutions 4. The results of Political Revolutions CHAPTER II. RELIGIOUS REVOLUTIONS 1. The importance of the study of Religious Revolutions in respect of the comprehension of the great Political Revolutions 2. The beginnings of the Reformation and its first disciples 3. Rational value of the doctrines of the Reformation 4. Propagation of the Reformation 5. Conflict between different religious beliefs. The impossibility of tolerance 6. The results of Religious Revolutions CHAPTER III. THE ACTION OF GOVERNMENTS IN REVOLUTIONS 1. The feeble resistance of Governments in time of Revolution 2. How the resistance of Governments may overcome Revolution 3. Revolutions effected by Governments. Examples: China, Turkey, &c 4. Social elements which survive the changes of Government after Revolution CHAPTER IV. THE PART PLAYED BY THE PEOPLE IN REVOLUTIONS 1. The stability and malleability Of the national mind 2. How the People regards Revolution 3. The supposed part of the People during Revolution 4. The popular entity and its constituent elements BOOK II THE FORMS OF MENTALITY PREVALENT DURING REVOLUTION CHAPTER I. INDIVIDUAL VARIATIONS OF CHARACTER IN TIME OF REVOLUTION 1. Transformations of Personality 2. Elements of character predominant in time of Revolution CHAPTER II. THE MYSTIC MENTALITY AND THE JACOBIN MENTALITY 1. Classification of mentalities predominant in time of Revolution 2. The Mystic Mentality 3. The Jacobin Mentality CHAPTER III.
    [Show full text]
  • Points of View in the Modern History of Psychology
    Points of View in the Modern History of Psychology Edited by Claude E. Buxton Department of Psychology Yale University New Haven, Connecticut 1985 ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Publishers) Orlando San Diego New York London Toronto Montreal Sydney Tokyo Passages from the following are reprinted by permission of the publishers: Newell, Α., Duncker on Thinking, in S. Koch & D. Leary (Eds.), A Century of Psychology as Science. Copyright 1985 by McGraw-Hill. Neisser, U., Cognitive Psychology. © 1967 by Prentice-Hall. COPYRIGHT © 1985 BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Orlando, Florida 32887 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24-28 Oval Road, London NW1 7DX LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOGING IN PUBLICATION DATA Main entry under title: Points of view in the modern history of psychology. Includes indexes. 1. Psychology— History. I. Buxton, Claude E. BF81.P57 1985 150\9 85-4010 ISBN 0-12-148510-2 (alk. paper) PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA 85 86 87 88 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Contributors Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' contributions begin. Mitchell G. Ash (295), Department of History, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242 William Bevan (259), John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, Chicago, Illinois 60603 Arthur L. Blumenthal (19, 51), Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts at Boston, Boston, Massachusetts 02125 Claude E.
    [Show full text]
  • Distinguished Contributions to Engineering Psychology
    Who Made Distinguished Contributions to Engineering Psychology EDITED BY Henry L. Taylor Contents Introduction 1 HENRY L TAYLOR APA Division 21: Roots and Rooters 4 EARL A. ALLUISI Paul Morris Fitts, 1912-1965 23 RICHARD W. PEW Jack W. Dunlap, 1902-1977 45 JESSE ORLANSKY Alexander Coxe Williams, Jr., 1914-1962 68 STANLEY N. ROSCOE Ross A. McFarland, 1901-1976 94 MALCOLM L. RITCHIE George Edward Briggs, 1926-1974 108 W. C. HOWELL Jerome Hirsch Ely, 1924-1963 117 MARTIN A. TOLCOTT Introduction HENRY L. TAYLOR Director, Institute of Aviation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign John J. O'Hare proposed in 1987 establishing an ad hoc Committee on Monographs of Distinguished Division 21 members. The Executive Committee, at its August 1987 meeting, approved the proposal and requested that he chair the committee. The criteria for the selection of candidates for a monograph were that a person be a past member of Division 21, deceased, and distinguished for his or her contribution to engineering psychology. The criteria for selection of authors to write about the candidates were familiarity with the candidate, willingness to allow the APA to hold copyright of materials, agreement that all royalties for sale of publication would be assigned to Division 21, and agreement that each essay would be completed in time to make a Division 21 contribution to the celebration of the APA's centenary. The following announcement was published in the fall1988 issue of the Division 21 Newsletter,16(1): Monographs of Distinguished Members As a contribution to the celebration of APA's centenary in 1992, Division 21 would like to prepare monographs on deceased members of our Division who have made distinguished contributions to the field of applied experimental psychology or engineering psychology.
    [Show full text]