Demonstratives and Their Reflexes: a Study in Grammaticalization from Eyak

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Demonstratives and Their Reflexes: a Study in Grammaticalization from Eyak Chris Donlay LING 236: Advanced Language Change Final Paper November 30, 2009 Demonstratives and Their Reflexes: A Study in Grammaticalization from Eyak The demonstrative has long been cited as prime material for grammaticalization. Its very nature makes it prone to further evolution. Because it is deictic, its meaning is general and use frequent – two common preconditions for grammaticalization (Hopper and Traugott 2003). This flexibility allows it to appear in a variety of constructions, which then leads to the development of polysemy and perhaps morphological and phonological change. And indeed demonstratives often follow this path. In a cross-linguistic study of 85 languages, Diessel (1999) shows that they may grammaticalize into a number of other functions, from personal pronouns and complementizers to determiners, copulas and focus markers. No language will develop all of these uses, but most will develop some. And given the importance of the independent demonstrative, its original use remains even as it evolves other functions. This phenomenon, in which older and newer uses coexist within a language, is called layering in grammaticalization (Hopper and Traugott 2003:124-126). While Diessel instantiates these many functions with examples from a variety of languages, he does not focus on the resultant layering that exists in each. To that end, in this paper I analyze the grammaticalization of demonstratives in a single Native American language: Eyak, a relative of the Athabaskan family. Despite the fact that there are no historical records for the language, Eyak provides an ideal laboratory for this kind of study. Its demonstratives have clearly evolved in function but not to the point where morphological and phonological change have rendered them unrecognizable. As a result, it is possible to identify the demonstrative origins of certain pronouns, articles, copulas, focus markers and discourse markers in the language. This transparency also highlights two findings not discussed by Diessel: a dual demonstrative that simultaneously refers to two referents, and discourse markers that do not originate from adverbial demonstratives. Donlay, Demonstratives in Eyak, p. 2 Eyak Eyak is an extinct language formerly spoken by communities who lived on the Gulf of Alaska, roughly between the towns of Yakutat and Cordova (Krauss 1982). The language descends from the Athabaskan-Eyak family, which is believed to have existed approximately three millennia ago (Krauss 1982). Eyak split from Proto-Athabaskan at some point before the latter came into being about two thousand years ago. It is therefore a distant relative of Slave, the lone Athabaskan language included in Diessel’s study. Syntactically, Eyak is similar to its Athabaskan relatives. It is a verb-final language with complex verbal morphology. The verb structure consists of a stem and a series of templatic affix positions, both prefixes, suffixes and enclitics. All other elements in a clause occur before the verb and word order among them is flexible. Third person subjects are canonically not marked on the verb (Krauss 1965). The demonstrative system in Eyak is a simple one, containing four words: al nonhuman proximal aw nonhuman distal/unmarked anh human singular ahnu∙ human plural They are further described in the next section. The previously-mentioned dual demonstrative, unhaw, only exists in the focus construction and is discussed in that section. Eyak has not yet been fully documented. There is no comprehensive grammar, but a dictionary (Krauss 1970a) and annotated folktales (Krauss 1970b, Krauss 1982) have been published. The data used in this paper are gathered from the later work. And although the structure of narratives favor third-person constructions, there is enough reported speech in the tales to see how personal deictics function in the language. The spelling system used in the narratives is shown in Table 1 alongside their IPA equivalents, though only the former is used in this paper. The vowels in Eyak are [i, e, ǝ, a, u]; the shwa is transcribed as a. Long vowels are indicated with a raised dot (e.g. i∙ [i:]). Vowels may also be nasalized, which is shown in the transcription by n following the vowel (Krauss Donlay, Demonstratives in Eyak, p. 3 1970b, Krauss 1982). Interlinear glosses and literal translations are provided by me unless otherwise noted1. Table 1: Consonant inventory of Eyak Plain d [t] dl [tɬ] dz [ʦ] j [ʧ] g [g] gw [gʷ] g [q] Aspirated t [tʰ] tl [tɬʰ] ts [ʦʰ] ch [ʧʰ] k [kʰ] q [qʰ] Glottalized t’ [t’] tl’ [tɬ’] ts’ [ʦ’] ch’ [ʧ’] k’ [k’] q’ [q’] Fricative ɫ [ɬ] s [s] sh [ʃ] x [x] xw [xʷ] x [χ] Sonorant w [w] m [m] l [l] n [n] y [j] Glottal h [h] ’ [Ɂ] (adapted from Krauss 1970b, Krauss 1982) Defining a Demonstrative The origin of demonstratives is a controversial subject. The process of grammaticalization rests on the idea that all grammatical items evolve from lexical expressions. Yet demonstratives, which clearly count as grammatical items, were until recently believed to be linguistic primitives. Diessel (1999) takes this view, suggesting that demonstratives may exist in language prior to the beginning of grammaticalization and, therefore, prior to any other grammatical form. More recently, Heine and Kuteva have shown that demonstratives may be derived from locative adverbs and even verbs such as ‘go’ or ‘see’ (2007:84). However, this does not completely disprove the earlier view. The fact that a language evolves new demonstratives does not mean that there were none previously, nor that all demonstratives originate in this way. Indeed, the literature is full of examples of new constructions supplanting older synonymous ones (e.g. Hopper and Traugott 2003). Their fundamental usefulness to communication suggests that ‘demonstratives might form a class of deictic expressions that belong to the basic vocabulary of every language’ (Diessel 1999:8). The data from Eyak does not resolve this issue; the origin of its demonstratives remains unclear. 11 Abbreviations: CONT contrastive, COP copula, CUST customary, DEM demonstrative, DIST distal, DU dual, FILL filler, FOC focus, HUM human, NEG negative, OBJ object, PFV perfective, PL plural, POSS possessive, PROG progressive, PROX proximal, REP repetitive, SG singular, SUBJ subject. Donlay, Demonstratives in Eyak, p. 4 Despite these questions, Diessel’s study of demonstratives is instructive. For example, he takes a broad view in defining the demonstrative, highlighting both its syntactic and pragmatic uses: They are primarily used to focus the hearer’s attention on objects or locations in the speech situation (often in combination with a pointing gesture), but they may also function to organize the information flow in the ongoing discourse. More specifically, demonstratives are often used to keep track of prior discourse participants and to activate specific shared knowledge. The most basic function of demonstratives is, however, to orient the hearer outside of discourse in the surrounding situation. (Diessel 1999:2) In the study, Diessel examines demonstratives in terms of four parameters: morphology, semantics, syntax and pragmatics. Each is discussed in turn, covering both Diessel’s generalizations and how they relate to the specifics of Eyak. Morphology Cross-linguistically, the morphology of demonstratives varies considerably (Diessel 1999:22). In some, demonstratives are invariable, in others they are highly inflected. Typically, they remain unbound, though cliticization is common. In Eyak, the four demonstratives are invariable and are largely unbound. They do take postpositions as suffixes, however. Unbound and bound demonstratives are shown in (1) and (2) respectively. (1) Aw qit dasa’iɫinu∙, DEM.DIST downhill dump-PFV-3PL They [literally ‘those’, i.e. ravens] dumped them down, (Krauss 1982:51) (2) Awq’ ya∙naht ate∙k’. DEM.DIST-on down.towards lie.down-CUST It would lie down on top of them [literally ‘on those’, i.e. ducks]. (Krauss 1982:49) Semantics Semantically, demonstratives typically contain two types of information: distance and reference (Diessel 1999:35-36). The former registers the relationship between the target item Donlay, Demonstratives in Eyak, p. 5 and the speaker’s deictic center, differentiating between, for example, proximal and distal. The latter encodes characteristics about the referent, which may include animacy, gender and humanness. In Eyak, the demonstratives are semantically asymmetric. The two nonhuman demonstratives encode both distance and reference, but number is not differentiated. Al is proximal, meaning ‘this one’/‘these’ and aw is distal meaning ‘that one’/‘those’. These forms also encode basic parameters about the referent, which may be animate but nonhuman, inanimate, or propositional. Furthermore, the distal has also developed an unmarked meaning (Krauss, personal communication), a common cross-linguistic occurrence (Diessel 1999). These three uses are instantiated in the following examples. (3) animate but nonhuman aw daɫaxwe∙k DEM.DIST groundhog those groundhogs (Krauss 1982:112) (4) inanimate aw t’ik’ɫ sixa’ DEM.DIST bow-and-arrow 1SG-POSS my bow-and-arrow [literally, ‘that bow-and-arrow of me’] (Krauss 1982:85) (5) propositional Awleht q'uhnu∙ aw shaxwa∙shɫ. DEM.DIST-because.of FOC-3PL DEM.DIST butcher-PFV That's why [literally, ‘because of that’, i.e. previous events] they butchered it. (Krauss 1982:52) The human demonstratives, on the other hand, only encode humanness and number, but not distance or gender. Anh refers to a singular human and ahnu∙ plural humans. They are also invariable and typically unbound, although they too may take postpositions as suffixes. Examples are shown below. Donlay, Demonstratives in Eyak, p. 6 (6) Anh utl’ yi∙nhinh awa∙ uxaht q’e’ sdiyahɫ. DEM.HUM.SG 3P-with be-3SG however 3P-without back go-PFV His companion [literally, ‘that one (who) is with him’], however, had gone back without him. (Krauss 1982:110:7) (7) Anhch’ k’ushiyah ula’x di∙’yahɫ. DEM.HUM.SG-with angry 3P-over become.angry-PFV She is infuriated with him. (Krauss 1982:91) Syntax In terms of syntax, Diessel (1999:57-58) distinguishes between the syntactic context and categorial status of demonstratives.
Recommended publications
  • I Just Think. . . : the Meaning and Discourse Role of Just 1 Just As A
    I just think. : The meaning and discourse role of just Stephen Emet March 1, 2017 The following are some examples from the Switchboard corpus (Godfrey and Holliman, 1993), which give a sense of just's frequency and flexibility in conversation: (1) we enjoyed watching the country and the Grammies and stuff but i just i just don't care for him i just never have (2) i just i just i thought that it might be convenient for me sometime to just like i said just pull over and fish for a while (3) oh ye[ah]- yeah well i- you know i don't i don't know all that much myself i just i just know when i hear it that i like it" (4) well it's i just i just think the more you you know you just each person has to consciously think about doing something little or something big whatever you can do 1 Just as a hedge? Just can be used to attenuate the force of a directive. (5) (a) Just sit down in that chair. (b) Just close your eyes. (c) If you'd just sign there for us. (Lee, 1987) The just seems to make the directive more polite than the bare imperative. Just can also reduce the force of an assertion, by indicating uncertainty. (6) um-hum that's true i i like i say i don't have any problem with people using firearms you know for sporting purposes or hunting purposes i just think it's just maybe a little too easy you know to acquire one I just think acts as a \plausibility shield" (Prince et al., 1982), indicating the speaker's level of commitment.
    [Show full text]
  • Discourse Marker Sequencing and Grammaticalization*
    Berkeley Linguistics Society. 2013. 108-122. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/bls.v39i1.3873 39 Published by the Linguistic Society of America Discourse Marker Sequencing and Grammaticalization* CHRISTIAN KOOPS AND ARNE LOHMANN University of New Mexico and University of Vienna 1 Introduction This paper deals with the grammatical properties of discourse markers (DMs), specifically their ordering preferences relative to one another. While the data presented here are synchronic, we approach the topic of DM sequencing from the perspective of grammaticalization. From this perspective, DMs can be understood as the result of a process in which elements serving other functions, for example grammatical functions at the level of sentential syntax, come to be conventionally used as markers of discourse-level relations, or what Schiffrin (1987: 31) operationally defined as “sequentially dependent elements which bracket units of talk.” Here we are concerned with the final outcome of this process. We ask: to what degree do fully formed DMs retain or lose the grammatical properties associated with their previous role, specifically their syntactic co-occurrence constraints? In other words, what degree of syntactic decategorialization (in the sense of Hopper 1991) do DMs display? This raises the question of how DMs grammaticalize. As they constitute a broad and diverse class of elements with different developmental trajectories, we draw on Auer’s (1996) taxonomy of relevant grammaticalization processes, which covers a wide range of diverse types. Auer’s analysis deals specifically with grammaticalization in the syntactic position known as the “pre-front field” (Vor- Vorfeld) of spoken German. In drawing on his model, we assume that this * We thank the audiences at BLS 39 in Berkeley and HLDS 10 in Albuquerque for their comments and suggestions on our analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of Demonstrative Pronoun and Demonstrative Determiner This in Upper-Level Student Writing: a Case Study
    English Language Teaching; Vol. 8, No. 5; 2015 ISSN 1916-4742 E-ISSN 1916-4750 Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education The Use of Demonstrative Pronoun and Demonstrative Determiner this in Upper-Level Student Writing: A Case Study Katharina Rustipa1 1 Faculty of Language and Cultural Studies, Stikubank University (UNISBANK) Semarang, Indonesia Correspondence: Katharina Rustipa, UNISBANK Semarang, Jl. Tri Lomba Juang No.1 Semarang 50241, Indonesia. Tel: 622-4831-1668. E-mail: [email protected] Received: January 20, 2015 Accepted: February 26, 2015 Online Published: April 23, 2015 doi:10.5539/elt.v8n5p158 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/elt.v8n5p158 Abstract Demonstrative this is worthy to investigate because of the role of this as a common cohesive device in academic writing. This study attempted to find out the variables underlying the realization of demonstrative this in graduate-student writing of Semarang State University, Indonesia. The data of the study were collected by asking three groups of students (first semester, second semester, third semester students) to write an essay. The collected data were analyzed by identifying, classifying, calculating, and interpreting. Interviewing to several students was also done to find out the reasons underlying the use of attended and unattended this. Comparing the research results to those of the Michigan Corpus of Upper-level Student Paper (MICUSP) as proficient graduate-student writing was done in order to know the position of graduate-student writing of Semarang State University in reference to MICUSP. The conclusion of the research results is that most occurrences of demonstrative this are attended and these occurrences are stable across levels, similar to those in MICUSP.
    [Show full text]
  • The Definite Determiner in Early Middle English
    43 The defi nite determiner in Early Middle English: What happened with þe? Cynthia L. Allen Australian National University Abstract This paper offers new data bearing on the question of when English developed a defi nite article, distinct from the distal demonstrative. It focuses primarily on one criterion that has been used in dating this development, namely the inability of þe (Modern English the, the refl ex of the demonstrative se) to be used as a pronoun. I argue that this criterion is not a satisfactory one and propose a treatment of þe as a form which could occupy either the head D of DP or the specifi er of DP. This is an approach consistent with Crisma’s (2011) position that a defi nite article emerged within the Old English (OE) period. I offer a new piece of evidence supporting Crisma’s demonstration of a difference between OE poetry and the prose of the ninth century and later. 1. Introduction: dating the defi nite article A long-standing problem in historical English syntax is dating the emergence of the defi nite article. A major diffi culty here, noted by Johanna Wood (2003) and others, is defi ning exactly what we mean by an ‘article.’ Millar (2000:304, note 11) comments that we might say that Modern English does not have a ‘true’ article, but only a ‘weakly demonstrative defi nite determiner’ on Himmelmann’s (1997) proposed path of development for defi nite articles. However, no one can doubt that English has moved along this path from a demonstrative determiner to something that has become more purely grammatical, with loss of deictic properties.
    [Show full text]
  • Types of Pronouns in English with Examples
    Types Of Pronouns In English With Examples If unpainted or hedgy Lucian usually convoke his bisexuals frazzling spotlessly or chain-stitch revilingly and damans.unamusingly, Unpreparedly how thinned legal, is Eddie?Durand Bomb tussled and hesitator culinary and Ossie pare always hemp. sanitized unbecomingly and fiddle his Learn with pronouns might be correct when they need are two personal pronouns refer to reflexive Such as a type of mutual respect that are used are like modern english grammar might be a noun. What i know how to their requested pronouns in english with pronouns examples of. Pronouns exemplify such a word class, or rather several smaller classes united by an important semantic distinction between them and all the major parts of speech. This is the meaning of english pronouns in with examples of mine, and a legal barriers often make. Common pronouns include I, me, mine, she, he, it, we, and us. New York Times best selling author, Tim Ferriss. This information was once helpful! Asused here in its demonstrative meaning, to introduce a parenthetical clause. The Health Sciences Library is open to Health Sciences affiliates. That and those refer to something less near to the speaker. Notice that occur relative clauses are within commas, and if removed they do these change the meaning being expressed in a one way, believe the sentences still a sense. Well, I managed to while speaking Italian after about month. The meal is receiving feedback will be delivered a speech. The examples of in english with pronouns specify a pronoun usage in the action is.
    [Show full text]
  • Signaling Coherence Relations in Text Generation: a Case Study of German Temporal Discourse Markers
    . Signaling coherence relations in text generation: A case study of German temporal discourse markers Dissertation zur Erlangung der Doktorw¨urde durch den Promotionsausschuss Dr. phil der Universit¨at Bremen vorgelegt von Brigitte Grote Berlin, den 29. Januar 2003 Contents 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Motivation.................................... 1 1.1.1 Discoursemarkersinwrittentext . .. 2 1.1.2 Discourse marker choice in text generation . ..... 6 1.2 Scopeofthestudy ............................... 8 1.3 Goalsofthisresearch............................. 9 1.4 Organizationofthethesis . .. 11 I State of the art and methodology 15 2 Earlier research on discourse markers 17 2.1 Definitionsinresearchliterature . ...... 17 2.1.1 Terminologyanddefinitions . 18 2.1.2 Identifying discourse markers in text . .... 21 2.2 Discourse markers in multilingual generation . ......... 23 2.2.1 Marker occurrence and placement . 25 2.2.2 Markerselection ............................ 26 2.2.3 Discourse marker choice in the overall generation process ...... 33 2.3 Discourse markers in descriptive linguistics . .......... 36 2.3.1 In-depth studies of small sets of discourse markers . ........ 37 2.3.2 Accounts covering a large range of discourse markers . ....... 38 2.4 Conclusions ................................... 41 i ii CONTENTS 3 Discourse representation in TG 43 3.1 Introduction: Coherence relations in text generation . ............ 43 3.2 RhetoricalStructureTheory . ... 46 3.2.1 RSTintextanalysis .......................... 46 3.2.2 RSTinautomatictextgeneration . 49 3.3 EnhancingRST................................. 53 3.3.1 Setofrelations ............................. 53 3.3.2 Minimalunits.............................. 56 3.3.3 Language specificity and relation to the linguistic surface ...... 57 3.3.4 Types of relations and levels of discourse representation....... 59 3.3.5 Decomposing coherence relations . .. 63 3.4 Conclusions ................................... 66 4 Framework and methodology 69 4.1 Framework...................................
    [Show full text]
  • The Definite Article in Recent Grammatical Theory Rhonda Lee Schuller Iowa State University
    Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Retrospective Theses and Dissertations Dissertations 1976 The definite article in recent grammatical theory Rhonda Lee Schuller Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd Part of the English Language and Literature Commons, and the Rhetoric and Composition Commons Recommended Citation Schuller, Rhonda Lee, "The definite article in recent grammatical theory" (1976). Retrospective Theses and Dissertations. 15. https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/15 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The definite article in recent grammatical theory by Rhonda Lee Schuller A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Partial Fulfillment of The Requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF ARTS Major: English Approved: Iowa State University Ames, Iowa 1976 ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page INTRODUCTION 1 A TRADITIONAL LOOK AT THE 3 Philosophy and The 7 The Definite Article Conspiracy 10 TRANSFORMATIONAL GRAMMAR: A VERY DEEP THE 16 AN ARTICLE IS AN ARTICLE IS AN ARTICLE? 30 THE CONCLUSION 37 FOOTNOTES 40 A LIST OF WORKS CONSULTED 41 1 INTRODUCTION The definite article is more difficult to define than the native speaker of English might realize. I propose to survey various treatments of the definite article, noting their strengths and weaknesses, in an attempt to reach an under­ standing of the function and meaning of the definite article in English.
    [Show full text]
  • Day 17: Possessive and Demonstrative Adjectives LESSON 17: Possessive and Demonstrative Adjectives
    Day 17: Possessive and Demonstrative Adjectives LESSON 17: Possessive and Demonstrative Adjectives We all know what adjectives can do (right??) These are the words that describe a noun. But their purpose is not limited to descriptions such as cool or kind or pretty. They have a host of other uses like providing more information about the noun they’re appearing with or even pointing out something. In this lesson, we’ll be talking about (or rather, breezing through) possessive adjectives and demonstrative adjectives. These are relatively easy topics that won’t be needing a lot of brain cell activity. So sit back and try to enjoy today’s topic. First, possessive adjectives. When you need to express that a noun belongs to another person or thing, you use possessive adjectives. We know it in English as the words: my, your, his, her, its, our, and their. In French, the possessive adjectives (like all other kinds of adjectives) need to agree to the noun they’re describing. Here’s a nifty little table to cover all that. Track 45 When used with When used with When used with plural What it means masculine singular feminine singular noun noun whether feminine noun or masculine mon ma (*mon) mes my ton ta (*ton) tes your son sa (*son) ses his/her/its/one’s notre notre nos our votre votre vos your leur leur leurs their Note that *mon, ton and son are used in the feminine form with nouns that begin with a vowel or the letter h. Here are some more reminders in using possessive adjectives: • Possessive adjectives always come BEFORE the noun.
    [Show full text]
  • Demonstratives in Discourse Åshild Næss University of Oslo Anna Margetts Monash University Yvonne Treis LLACAN (CNRS, INALCO)
    Chapter 1 Introduction: Demonstratives in discourse Åshild Næss University of Oslo Anna Margetts Monash University Yvonne Treis LLACAN (CNRS, INALCO) 1 Discourse markers and their functions Over the last decades, there has been extensive discussion in the typological lit- erature of the functions and uses of demonstratives. It is well established that demonstratives are not restricted to referring to items in situational use based on concrete spatial parameters, but that discourse deictic, anaphoric/tracking, and recognitional uses are also common, if not universal, functions of demonstra- tives (see Himmelmann 1996; 1997; and Diessel 1999 for systematic overviews). Studies have shown that many parameters beyond location and configuration of referents and speech-act participants play a role in demonstrative choice. In particular, directing the addressee’s attention towards a target entity and prior knowledge of a referent either through the previous discourse or from the real world have been identified as relevant (see e.g. Burenhult 2003; Dawuda 2009; Diessel 2006; Enfield 2003; Hanks 1990; 1992; 2005; 2009; Küntay & Özyürek 2006; Özyürek 1998). The diachronic development from demonstratives to other types of markers with grammatical and discourse functions has also been extensively discussed (see again Himmelmann 1996; 1997; Diessel 1999). Åshild Næss, Anna Margetts & Yvonne Treis. 2020. Introduction: Demonstratives in discourse. In Åshild Næss, Anna Margetts & Yvonne Treis (eds.), Demonstratives in discourse, 1–20. Berlin: Language Science Press. DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.4055812 Åshild Næss, Anna Margetts & Yvonne Treis This volume investigates discourse functions of demonstratives, that is, the type of functions demonstratives perform when they develop into discourse markers.1 The notion of discourse marker is not very clearly defined, and the question thus arises which functions they comprise, how they can be described, and to what ex- tent demonstratives and their functions match this description.
    [Show full text]
  • Demonstratives and Definite Articles in Plains Cree
    Demonstratives and Definite Articles in Plains Cree DANIELLE E. CYR York University 1. Introduction It is the usage, in the grammatical description of Algonquian languages, to classify demonstratives within a single formal class, whether their func­ tional role may be that of a demonstrative pronoun, a demonstrative noun- determiner, or a definite article. This is the case with two Eastern Cree lan­ guages spoken in Quebec, Montagnais and James Bay Cree.1 For example for Montagnais Ford and Bacon say (1978:30): "Pour indiquer la possession d'un defini, il suffit de juxtaposer le demonstratif au nom possessif". And for James Bay Cree Vaillancourt (1978:31) says: "La ou le frangais fait us­ age d'un article defini, le cris fait usage d'un demonstratif". The situation seems to be the same for Plains Cree as we find in Wolfart and Carroll (1981:84): "For Cree it has been conventional to group words according to their form rather than their function. We call awa a pronoun even where it is used much like an English article". In other studies (Cyr 1993, and Cyr and Axelsson 1988), I have shown that, in Montagnais, the typology of textual functions and textual frequencies of the demonstratives corresponds more closely to that of definite articles in other article languages than to that of demonstrative noun-determiners. A study of textual data comprising 12,000 words of Montagnais narratives shows in fact that demonstratives are :The research for this paper was partly supported by Research Grant 410- 90-1056 from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, and by the Faculty of Arts of York University.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 3 Basic Concepts
    Monica Macaulay DRAFT last updated 3/8/13 1 Chapter 3 Basic Concepts 1. Introduction This chapter introduces some basic concepts and facts about Menominee that will be needed to understand the material in subsequent chapters. Here we look at parts of speech, person, animacy, obviation, and verb types. 2. Parts of speech Different languages have different inventories of parts of speech. It’s safe to say that virtually all languages have nouns and verbs, but beyond that we find a lot of differences. According to Bloomfield, Menominee has the following parts of speech: (1) Menominee Parts of Speech (Bloomfield 1962:25) • Nouns • Verbs • Pronouns • Particles • Negator There are also prefixes (which come before words) and lots of suffixes (which come after words). The prefixes are discussed briefly below, and both the prefixes and suffixes are discussed further in Chapters 4, 5, and 6. Brief discussion of each of the parts of speech appears in this chapter, and more detail is given in later chapters. 2.1. Nouns We all learn in school that nouns describe a “person, place, or thing.” That works for many nouns, but not necessarily all of them. For example, it leaves out abstract concepts like joy or independence. In Menominee, beyond looking at the meaning, you can tell if something is a noun by the following factors (among other things): (a) whether it can be pluralized (by -ak or -an), (b) whether it can have the locative ending -eh added, (c) whether it can be possessed (using the three person-marking prefixes discussed below) (d) whether it can have a demonstrative (like eneh ‘that (inan.)’ or enoh ‘that (an.)’) before it.
    [Show full text]
  • Influence of Animacy and Grammatical Role on Production and Comprehension of Intersentential Pronouns in German L1-Acquisition1
    Influence of animacy and grammatical role on production and comprehension of intersentential pronouns in German L1-acquisition1 Dagmar Bittner Centre for General Linguistics, Typology and Universals Research (ZAS) Berlin, Germany In anaphora resolution theory, it has been assumed that anaphora resolution is based on a reversed mapping of antecedent salience and anaphora complexity: minimal complex anaphora refer to maximal salient antecedents. In order to ex- amine whether and by which developmental steps German children gain com- mand of this mapping maxim we conducted an experiment on production and comprehension of intersentential pronouns including the three pronoun types zero, personal, and demonstrative pronoun. With respect to antecedent salience, the ex- periment varied syntactic role (subject/object) and in/animacy. Six age groups of children (age range from 2;0 to 6;0) and an adult control group has been tested. The hypothesis arising from the mapping maxim is that zero pronoun correlates with more salient antecedents than personal and demonstrative pronoun, the latter correlating with the least salient antecedents. The results are: In production, chil- dren first establish the opposition of zero pronoun with animate antecedents vs. demonstrative pronoun with inanimate antecedents. In a next step, syntactic role comes into play and a more complex system opposing the three presented pronoun types is established. In comprehension, however, the effect of pronoun type re- mains weak and antecedent features remain a strong factor in reference choice. However, also adults employ pronoun type and antecedent features. The oldest children and the adults show variation in personal pronoun resolution according to the animacy pattern of the potential antecedents.
    [Show full text]