Studies in the Linguistic Sciences

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Studies in the Linguistic Sciences i u 5i 1 MAR 1 1998 99b Spr- ODV 2 lences VOLUME 25, NUMBER 1 (SPRING 1995) [Published February 1997] DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS JNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS AT URBANA-CHAMPAIGN THELieR/yWOFTHE C9 19S8 Of )Limo\s STUDIES IN THE LINGUISTIC SCIENCES PUBLICATION OF THE DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS IN THE COLLEGE OF LIBERAL ARTS AND SCIENCES OF THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS AT URBANA-CHAMPAIGN General Editor: Elmer H. Antonsen EDITORIAL ASSISTANT: Mark Honegger Editorial board: Elabbas Benmamoun, Eyamba G. Bokamba, Chin-Chuan Cheng, Jennifer S. Cole, Georgia M. Green, Hans Henrich Hock, Braj B. Kachru, Yamuna Kachru, Chin-W. Kim, Charles W. Kisseberth, Peter Lasersohn, Howard Maclay, Jerry L. Morgan, Rajeshwari Pandharipande, James H. Yoon, and Ladislav Zgusta. AIM: SLS is intended as a forum for the presentation of the latest original re- search by the faculty and students of the Department of Linguistics, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign. Scholars outside the Department and from other institutions are also cordially invited to submit original linguistic research for consideration. In all cases, articles submitted for publication will be reviewed by a panel of at least two experts in the appropriate field to determine suitability for publication. Copyright remains with the individual authors. Authors will receive one copy of the particular issue and 10 offprints of their individual contributions. SLS appears twice a year, and one issue is traditionally devoted to restricted, specialized topics. A complete list of available back issues is given inside the back cover. BOOKS FOR REVIEW: Review copies of books may be sent to: Editor, Studies in the Linguistic Sciences Department of Linguistics, 4088 For. Lang. Bldg., University of Illinois 707 S. Mathews, Urbana, IL 61801, USA SUBSCRIPTION: Requests for subscriptions should be addressed to SLS Sub- scriptions, Department of Linguistics, 4088 Foreign Languages Building, Univer- sity of Illinois, 707 South Mathews, Urbana, Illinois 61801. Price: $10.00 per single issue. e-mail address: [email protected] Telephone: (217) 333-3563 Fax: (217) 333-3466 STUDIES IN THE LINGUISTIC SCIENCES Papers in General Linguistics EDITOR Elmer H. Antonsen EDITORIAL ASSISTANT Mark Honegger VOLUME 25, NUMBER 1 (SPRING 1995) [Published February 1997] DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS, UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS URBAN A, ILLINOIS 61801 EDITOR'S NOTE Although this issue bears the volume and issue designation 25:1 Spring 1995, thereby filling a gap in the journal's twice-a-year scheduled appearance, because of unavoidable circumstances it could not be published until February 1997. We have therefore indicated the actual publication date in square brackets where appropriate. ' 7 CONTENTS Editor's Note ii ELMER H. ANTONSEN: On phonological reconstruction: "Weil die Schrift . immer strebt . 1 LAURA J. DOWNING: Correspondence effects in Siswati reduplication 1 ANNA M. KISHE: The modernization of Tanzanian KiSwahili and language change 37 ANDREW TILIMBE KULEMEKA: On the meaning of Chichewa ideophones 5 1 CAROL KUTRYB: The effect of complementizer that on extraction from embedded clauses 67 ANITA PANDEY: The pragmatics of code alteration in Nigerian English 75 YURIKO SUZUKI KOSE: Sentence-final particles in Japanese — an alterna- tive to scalar analyses 119 FRANS VAN COETSEM: Variation in the rate of language change due to societal influence: Examples from the Germanic languages 137 MARY A. WU: Meaning and form: Computing definite and uniqueness readings of complex noun phrases in Mandarin Chinese 145 REVIEWS Alaa Elgibali (ed.) (1996). Understanding Arabic: Essays in Con temporary Arabic Linguistics in Honor of El-Said Badawi. (Elabbas Benmamoun) 159 Christoph Gutknecht & Lutz J. Rolle (1996). Translating by Factors. (Ladislav Zgusta) 163 Alexander M. Schenker (1996). The Dawn of Slavic: An Introduction to Slavic Philology. (Frank Y. Gladney) 167 Studies in the Linguistic Sciences Volume 25, Number 1 (Spring 1995) [publ. February 1997] ON PHONOLOGICAL RECONSTRUCTION: 'WEIL DIE SCHRIFT IMMER STREET ...' Elmer H. Antonsen University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign [email protected] For James W. Marchand on his 70th birthday, 11 November 1996 Recent treatments of umlaut in the Germanic languages by adher- ents of the generative school deny the validity of the American struc- turalist explanation of this phenomenon, according to which original allophonic variants developed into phonemic contrasts. The generativ- ists claim to 'take the orthography [of medieval manuscripts] seriously', whereby a lack of written designation is taken to indicate the absence of phonological differentiation. I attempt to show that such studies cannot claim to deal with phonology, but are strictly orthographic analyses. 0. Introduction The early years of the 19th century were certainly exciting ones for the pioneers in the newly developing science of historical linguistics, and partic- ularly in historical Germanic linguistics.* Some of us are familiar with the rela- tionship between Jacob Grimm and Rasmus Rask in this fledgling science, al- though the profound influence that Rask had on Grimm is sometimes obscured by later investigators (see Antonsen 1962, and Robins 1990:189). One thing they both had in common, something that was simply a sign of the age in which they worked: they took the view that they were dealing with Buchstaben or Bogstaver 'letters', and they wrote about the relationship among letters and of the change of one letter to another. It was not an easy thing to overcome, and those of us who have taught beginning linguistics courses are familiar with the difficulty naive students (and sometimes even more sophisticated ones) have in distinguishing between the written symbol, on the one hand, and the phonological reality behind it, on the other. Through the combined efforts of the 19th-century phoneticians and the Neogrammarians the fundamental distinction between sound and letter was clarified to a large extent (cf. Pedersen 1962:303-310), but even so, the con- fusion between the two has remained a stumbling block in historical Germanic research right up to the present day, since scholars in our field still are not agreed on the nature of the relationship between writing and phonology. In more recent discussions of the phenomenon of umlaut in the Germanic languages, some scholars have declared their firm intention to 'take the orthog- raphy seriously' (e.g., Voyles 1976:1). Voyles (1976:2) assumes 'the orthography of these texts — as indeed of the OHG literary monuments in general — accurately reflects the phonology of the language', which is to be taken to mean that the lack 2 Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 25: 1 (Spring 1995) of an explicit designation in thie writing system is to be equated with the absence of a given phonological feature. It is basically this problem that I would like to address, along with some related questions that have interested me over the years and deserve more attention than they have received in the past. 1. The discovery of the 'cause' of umlaut It is an established fact that the one fundamental discovery concerning Ger- manic umlaut was made by Rasmus Rask in his Vejledning til det Islandske eller gamle Nordiske Sprog [Guide to the Icelandic or Old Nordic Language] (1811: 44-45) when he proposed that the varying vowels in the root syllable of a para- digm like Icelandic hond 'hand' (see Table 1) were dependent on the nature of the VOWEL THAT FOLLOWED, OR THAT HAD ONCE FOLLOWED, the root syllable, so that the o of the nominative and accusative forms was dependent on a formerly present -u in the following syllable (as could be confirmed by the presence of such a vowel in Go. handus and handu), while the a of the genitive was dependent on the -a of its suffix, and the e of the dative on the following -/. Sg. nom. hond (cf Gothic handus) gen. handar dat. hendi ace. hond (cf Gothic handu) Table 1. It took considerable time for Jacob Grimm to become convinced of the cor- rectness of this analysis by Rask, but eventually he came to embrace it as his own in the second edition of the first volume of his Deutsche Grammatik [German(ic) Grammar] (1822:12.169), which he had thoroughly revised after reading Rask's works and before publishing the further volumes. This discovery by Rask was, of course, merely the beginning of a long and arduous search to unveil the secrets of that vocalic phenomenon so characteristic of Germanic. In a number of instances, this search for an understanding of umlaut was actually impeded by theories that were advanced by Jacob Grimm and subsequently accepted by the scholarly community for years to come. I do not mean to rave against Jacob Grimm, who is one of my true heroes (I am reminded of the remark attributed to Niels Bohr: T do not mean to be critical, but this is sheer nonsense'). It is necessary to put things into perspective, however, and although we must understand the limitations imposed by his times, in some respects Grimm failed to see clearly where others nevertheless did. An example of this failing can be found by comparing the work of Georg Friedrich Benecke (1813:169), Grimm's close collaborator, who saw a definite relationship between the umlauted vowels and the ending -e of Middle High German forms like those in Table 2. At first, Jacob Grimm vigorously denied that there could be any connection between the umlauted vowel and the suffix. nom. 4 Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 25: 1 (Spring 1995) work for describing diachronic phonological developments in terms of the rise of predictable conditioned variations, which through some type of further develop- ment eventually produced distinctive phonological units that previously had not been present or that had displayed a different distribution from that formerly present. This theory was applied to the study of umlaut in the Germanic languages in a pioneering article before the Second World War by W.
Recommended publications
  • Lessons from Research on Ideophones Mark Dingemanse Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6500 AH Nijmegen, NL [email protected]
    a journal of Dingemanse, Mark. 2018. Redrawing the margins of language: Lessons from general linguistics Glossa research on ideophones. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 3(1): 4. 1–30, DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.444 OVERVIEW ARTICLE Redrawing the margins of language: Lessons from research on ideophones Mark Dingemanse Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6500 AH Nijmegen, NL [email protected] Ideophones (also known as expressives or mimetics, and including onomatopoeia) have been systematically studied in linguistics since the 1850s, when they were first described as a lexical class of vivid sensory words in West-African languages. This paper surveys the research history of ideophones, from its roots in African linguistics to its fruits in general linguistics and typology around the globe. It shows that despite a recurrent narrative of marginalisation, work on ideophones has made an impact in many areas of linguistics, from theories of phonological features to typologies of manner and motion, and from sound symbolism to sensory language. Due to their hybrid nature as gradient vocal gestures that grow roots in discrete linguistic systems, ideophones provide opportunities to reframe typological questions, reconsider the role of language ideology in linguistic scholarship, and rethink the margins of language. With ideophones increasingly being brought into the fold of the language sciences, this review synthesises past theoretical insights and empirical findings in order to enable future work to build on them. Keywords: ideophones; iconicity; linguistic theory; language ideology “Words go on living despite the efforts of scholars to manage them.” — Taro Gomi (1989) 1 Introduction Ideophones are marked words that depict sensory scenes like c’onc’on ‘woven tightly’, ulakpulak ‘unbalanced, scary appearance’ and colcol ‘flowing liquid’ in Korean, or muku- muku ‘mumbling mouth movements’, gelegele ‘glittery appearance’ and gbadara-gbadara ‘a drunkard’s wobbling gait’ in Siwu, a Kwa language spoken in Ghana (Dingemanse 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Part III: the Typology of Motion Events
    Jean-Michel Fortis - Space in Language - Leipzig Summer School 2010 –PART III PART III — THE TYPOLOGY OF MOTION EVENTS 1. TALMY’S TYPOLOGY The initial version of Talmy’s typology of motion events and its evolution are presented in the following sections. One motivation for this lengthy presentation is that Talmy’s typology is sometimes misunderstood and described erroneously (for ex. Croft et al., to appear), with the consequence that some of the objections raised against it miss their target. Another motivation, opposed to the first, is that Talmy’s theory harbors difficulties that are seldom recognized. III.1. TALMY’S FIRST THEORY (1972) Talmy’s first theory aims at comparing the structure of English with a polysynthetic language of California. Perhaps because deep syntactic structures in the generative style were not well-suited for this purpose, Talmy goes to a deeper, semantic, level (like Wallace Chafe, who was facing the same problem with Onondaga, another polysynthetic language ; cf. Chafe 1970). Talmy’s point of departure is the notion of translatory situation. A translatory situation (an event in which a Figure moves along a path) is decomposed into a fixed structure (translatory structure) of 4 components : Figure : “the object which is considered as moving or located with respect to another object.” (F) Ground : “the object with respect to which a 1st is considered as moving or located.” (G) Directional : “the respect with which one object is considered as moving or located to another object.” (D) Motive : “the moving or located state which one object is considered to be in with respect to another object” (M).
    [Show full text]
  • Ideophones, Gestures and Code-Switching Among Young Zulu Speakers in South Africa
    Language Contact and Expressiveness: Ideophones, gestures and code-switching among young Zulu speakers in South Africa by Anna Miri Speyer Mertner 1 Table of Contents Abstract ..................................................................................................................................... 4 Acknowledgements .................................................................................................................. 4 Introduction .............................................................................................................................. 5 1. Ideophones ............................................................................................................................ 6 1.1 Why are ideophones considered expressive? ................................................................... 7 1.2 Iconicity in ideophones .................................................................................................... 8 1.3 A social perspective on ideophones ............................................................................... 10 2. Gestures .............................................................................................................................. 12 2.1 Defining iconic gestures ................................................................................................ 12 2.2 The link between gestures and ideophones .................................................................... 12 3. A descriptive overview of Zulu ........................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • A Demonstration-Based Account of (Pluractional) Ideophones *
    Proceedings of SALT 26: 664–683, 2016 A demonstration-based account of (pluractional) ideophones * Robert Henderson University of Arizona Abstract This paper develops a novel formal semantics of ideophones that can account for their meaning and compositional properties. The proposal extends recent work on iconicity in sign languages in Davidson 2015, whose demonstration- based framework provides a formal foundation for the semantics of ideophones that captures the difference between descriptive meaning and depictive meaning, the kind of meaning ideophones traffic in. After providing a demonstration-based account of the basic ideophone construction in the Mayan language Tseltal, the paper then shows how the demonstration-based account can be used to analyze pluractionality in the ideophone domain. In particular, through case studies on Tseltal and Upper Necaxa Totonanc (Totonacan), I show that there are two previously unrecognized types of ideophonic pluractionality, and that their properties support the demonstration-based account. Keywords: ideophones, pluractionality, iconicity, demonstrations, Mayan, Totonacan 1 Introduction The term ideophone is used to pick out a distinguished class of words in a language that specialize in depicting sensory imagery (Dingemanse 2011: p. 25; 2012). While the expressions considered in this work fit the definition, ideophones are often easier to point at than define. For this reason, consider the following example of the ideophone tsok’ in Tseltal (Mayan). (1) pura ch’il-bil-Ø, tsok’ x-chi-Ø ta mantekat just fried-PERF-B3 IDF:sound.start.to.fry NT-say-B3 P lard ‘it just gets fried, it goes «tsok’» in the lard’(Pérez González 2012: p. 162) The literature on the formal semantics of ideophones is scarce.
    [Show full text]
  • Herent Complement Verbs in Ibibio Viktoria Apel
    Contents Acknowledgments ........................................................................... 1 Schedule ..................................................................................... 2 Abstracts .................................................................................... 10 Conference Venue ........................................................................... 50 Nearby Dining and Attractions ............................................................. 51 Notes ........................................................................................ 54 Invited Speakers Firmin Ahoua,Akin Akinlabi, and Gildas Gondo ....................................12 When a five-way opposition does not represent a five-tone language: The case of Dan Jeff Good .............................................................................10 Stability under contact as a guide to reconstruction: Noun classes, argument alignment, and the Macro-Sudan belt Tom G¨uldemann .....................................................................14 A shared pronominal canon in the Macro-Sudan belt: typological, areal and geneological aspects Claire Halpert .......................................................................11 Surmountable Barriers Sharon Rose .........................................................................13 Areal vowel harmony patterns in the Nuba Mountains and Beyond Papers and Posters Hasiyatu Abubakari ..................................................................28 Contrastive focus particles in Kusaal
    [Show full text]
  • Kejom (Babanki) Ideophones1
    Pius W. Akumbu University of Buea, Cameroon Kejom (Babanki) Ideophones1 This study identifies and describes ideophones used in Babanki (henceforth Kejom as the people prefer to call themselves and their language), a Centre Ring Grassfields Bantu language of Northwest Cameroon. The study of ideophones or marked words that depict sensory imagery (Dingemanse 2011) in Africa has expanded in recent years (Blench 2010) but our knowledge of this class of words in Grassfields Bantu languages is restricted with only a limited reference to ideophones in the literature on these languages (Blench (2010) on Yamba and Limbum, Tamanji (2009) on Bafut and Akumbu & Fogwe (2012) on Babanki). The properties of Kejom ideophones are examined in this paper and it is concluded that they are marked due to their divergent phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics and pragmatics. It is realized that the ideophones vividly depict one or more sensory events, ranging from sight to emotion. Keywords: Ideophone, Kejom, Reduplication, Sensory, Vowel Lengthening 1. Identification of Kejom Kejom is spoken in Kejom Keku (Big Babanki) and Kejom Ketinguh (Babanki Tungo) in Tubah Subdivision, Mezam Division, Northwest Region of Cameroon. The two villages have a total population of 39.000 people (Ethnologue 2015). Víʔə́ kə̀ jɔ̀ m ‘the people of Kejom’ speak gáʔə́ kə̀ jɔ̀ m ‘the language of Kejom’. Others call them “Babanki”, a designation the people themselves recognize and accept. When speaking with English speakers, they refer to themselves as “Babanki” which is actually the administrative and linguistic name given to them. Ethnologue (2015) lists Babanki with the code ISO 693-3 [bbk] and classifies it as a central Ring language of the wide Grassfields subgroup of Bantoid within Benue-Congo.
    [Show full text]
  • 'To Give an Imagination to the Listeners': the Neglected
    ‘To give an imagination to the listeners’: The neglected poetics of Navajo ideophony* ANTHONY K. WEBSTER Abstract Ideophony is a neglected aspect of investigations of world poetic traditions. This article looks at the use of ideophony in a variety of Navajo poetic genres. Examples are given from Navajo place-names, narratives, and songs. A final example involves the use of ideophony in contemporary written Navajo poetry. Using the work of Woodbury, Friedrich, and Becker it is argued that ideophones are an example of form-dependent expression, poetic indeterminacy, and the inherent exuberances and deficiencies of translation and thus strongly resists translation. This fact becomes more relevant when understood in light of the current language shift from Navajo to English. Keywords: Navajo; ideophones; translation; poetics; language shift; poetry. 1. Introduction: On ideophones In this article, I wish to describe an underappreciated poetic feature found in both Navajo oral tradition and the emerging genre of written Navajo poetry (see Webster 2004). That feature is the use of sound symbolism, onomatopoeia, and ideophony. In particular, I am concerned with the use of ‘sound imitative’ expressions (Hinton et al. 1994: 3). This class of sound symbolism attempts to ‘simulate’ some non-linguistic activity or image in a linguistic form (Nuckolls 2000: 235). These are, following the terminology of Clement Doke (1935) and Janis Nuckolls (2006), ‘ideo- phones’ (see also Tedlock 1999). Doke defines ideophones as follows: A vivid representation of an idea in sound. A word, often onomatopoeic, which describes a predicate, qualificative or adverb in respect to manner, color, sound, smell, action, state or intensity.
    [Show full text]
  • Where Do Ideophones Come From?
    Portland State University PDXScholar Applied Linguistics Faculty Publications and Presentations Applied Linguistics Fall 1989 Where Do Ideophones Come From? George Tucker Childs Portland State University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/ling_fac Part of the African Languages and Societies Commons, and the Linguistics Commons Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Citation Details Childs, George Tucker (1989). Where do ideophones come from. Studies in the linguistic sciences, 19(2), 55-76. This Article is brought to you for free and open access. It has been accepted for inclusion in Applied Linguistics Faculty Publications and Presentations by an authorized administrator of PDXScholar. Please contact us if we can make this document more accessible: [email protected]. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences Volume 19, Number 2, Fall 1989 WHERE DO IDEOPHONES COME FROM?* G. Tucker Childs Temple University This paper begins with an analysis of ideophones in Kisi (West Atlantic, Guinea). This examination leads to consideration of the diachrony of ideophones, but because of the lack of data with any real time depth, we are limited to finding clues in the synchronic data. This study looks first at verbs, the class of words which Kisi ideophones resemble most closely. Such is also the case in other African languages, where ideophones can often be analyzed as verbs, as has been done for several Southern Bantu languages. I then demonstrate how Kisi ideophones can be derived from verbs, illustrating several possible scenarios for such a process. I then look at possible derivational relationships with other word categories, finding that with other word categories the direction of derivation is the opposite.
    [Show full text]
  • Contrastive Linguistics
    Volume 2, Issue 5, May 2015, PP 170-181 ISSN 2349-0373 (Print) & ISSN 2349-0381 (Online) www.arcjournals.org International Journal of Humanities Social Sciences and Education (IJHSSE) Contrastive Linguistics: An Exploration of Ideophones in Yoruba and Edo Speech Communities Ayoola Oluwafunmiso Moses Department of English, University of Zululand, Private Bag X 1001, KwaDlangezwa, 3886, Republic of South Africa [email protected] Abstract Crystal (1997: 189) defines “Ideophones” as “a term used in linguistics and phonetics for any vivid representation of an idea in sound, such as occurs through onomatopoeia”. Ideophones tend to be longer in terms of the combination of sounds than lexical classes. Thus, it enables the users to pack meaning into single morphemes thereby making the words semantically multidimensional. (Woodbury1987:715). Vowel repetition or lengthening is also a characteristic of ideophones. Ideophones are often phonologically anomalous in terms of sounds and sound sequences, tonal structure and phonological behaviour. (Welmer1973:27). In any case, these features have an income relation between sound and meaning. Just like any natural language, ideophones represent a robust word category in African language. To this end, this work is designed to arrive at an applicable analysis and classification of Edo and Yoruba ideophones using a contrastive approach inspired by the idea of canonical typology. The theory of Autosegmental morphology as propounded by Welmer (1981), Marrantz (1982) and Anderson (1992) is employed in this study. This theory proposes that reduplication is essential affixation, but what is affixed is a prosodic template, that is, a syllable foot or even a phonological word is the affixation of a consonant- vowel (CV) skeleton which is itself a morpheme to a stem.
    [Show full text]
  • Pluractional Demonstrations∗
    Pluractional Demonstrations∗ Robert Henderson | The University of Arizona Abstract This paper develops a novel formal semantics for ideophones that can account for their meaning and compositional properties. The proposal ex- tends recent work on iconicity in sign languages by Davidson (2015), whose demonstration-based framework provides a formal foundation for the seman- tics of ideophones that captures the difference between descriptive meaning and depictive meaning, the kind of meaning ideophones traffic in. After pro- viding a demonstration-based account of the basic ideophone construction in the Mayan language Tseltal, the paper then shows how the demonstration- based account can be used to analyze pluractionality in the ideophone do- main. In particular, through case studies on Tseltal and Upper Necaxa Toto- nanc (Totonacan), I show that there are two previously unrecognized types of ideophonic pluractionality, and that their properties support the demonstration- based account. The first, which I call “demonstration-external pluraction- ality”, involves a speaker using an ideophone to do a plurality of demon- strations that characterize a plurality of events. The second kind of ideo- phonic pluractionality, which I call a “demonstration-internal pluractional- ity”, is much more similar to pluractionality in the verbal domain, and in- volves special morphology that derives ideophone stems that can only be used to demonstrate plural events. Finally, I use the contrast between these two types of pluractionality in the ideophone domain to clarify the line be- tween the iconic and non-iconic aspects of the semantics of ideophones. Keywords: ideophones, pluractionality, iconicity, demonstrations, Mayan, Totonacan ∗I would like to thank Scott AnderBois, Diana Archangeli, Nicolas Asher, Rebekah Baglini, Dy- lan Bumford, Simon Charlow, Lucas Champollion, Kathryn Davidson, Jeremy Kuhn, Heidi Harley, Serge Minor, Jaime Pérez González, and Philippe Schlenker for extremely helpful discussions about ideophones and iconicity.
    [Show full text]
  • Survey of Bantu Ideophones.Pdf
    AFRICAN LANGUAGE STUDIES XII J971 SCHOOL OF ORIENTAL AND AFRICAN STUDIES UNIVERSITY OF LONDON Sole Agents: Luzac & Company Ltd., 46 Great Russell Street, London, W.C.i. School of Oriental and African Studies University of London SBN : 901877 17 4 / SURVEY OF BANTU IDEOPHONES By William J. Samarin Introduction This survey of Bantu ideophones is meant to report on what in general is known about them and to stimulate further investigation. Its aim is a modest one. To achieve the first goal I have had to depend on what I have read. Therefore the picture that emerges here has its basis largely in the literature that was available to me. All the African grammars in the libraries of the Hartford Seminary Foundation (Hartford, Connecticut), the University of Leiden (The Netherlands), and the University of Toronto (Canada) were personally examined. Of these 250 grammars (give or take a few), there were about 150 that dealt in some way with ideophones. Among these were about 90 grammars of Bantu languages. These are listed in the Bibliography, including ten titles added just before this paper went to press ; I was therefore unable to utilize them at all. Among them is Hulstaert's very important work of 1966. To achieve the second goal I have had to review the literature somewhat critically. I use the term critical in the academic sense. I disparage no writer for his observations. Nevertheless, I have pointed out such things as errors of interpretation, generalizations based on insufficient data, or the absence of sub- stantiation by empirical research. In a few cases I suggest specific kinds of research that might be undertaken or topics that need further study.
    [Show full text]
  • The Onomatopoeic Ideophone-Gesture Relationship in Pastaza Quichua Sarah Ann Hatton Brigham Young University
    Brigham Young University BYU ScholarsArchive All Theses and Dissertations 2016-12-01 The Onomatopoeic Ideophone-Gesture Relationship in Pastaza Quichua Sarah Ann Hatton Brigham Young University Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd Part of the Linguistics Commons BYU ScholarsArchive Citation Hatton, Sarah Ann, "The Onomatopoeic Ideophone-Gesture Relationship in Pastaza Quichua" (2016). All Theses and Dissertations. 6123. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/6123 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in All Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. The Onomatopoeic Ideophone-Gesture Relationship in Pastaza Quichua Sarah Ann Hatton A thesis submitted to the faculty of Brigham Young University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Janis B. Nuckolls, Chair Cynthia L. Hallen William G. Eggington Department of Linguistics and English Language Brigham Young University Copyright © 2016 Sarah Ann Hatton All Rights Reserved ABSTRACT The Onomatopoeic Ideophone-Gesture Relationship in Pastaza Quichua Sarah Ann Hatton Department of Linguistics and English Language, BYU Master of Arts The relationship between ideophones and gestures has only recently been studied and is not yet completely understood. The topic has been specifically addressed by Kita (1993), Klassen (1998), Dingemanse (2013), Mihas (2013), and Reiter (2013). Yet there has been little focus on onomatopoeic ideophones. Onomatopoeic ideophones have been set aside as different by many previous researchers (Klassen, 1998, pp. 28-31; Kilian-Hatz, 2001, pp. 161-163; Dingemanse, 2011, pp.
    [Show full text]