A Morpho-Semantic Analysis of the Persistive, Alterative and Inceptive Aspects in Siswati

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Morpho-Semantic Analysis of the Persistive, Alterative and Inceptive Aspects in Siswati Nichols, Peter John (2011) A morpho‐semantic analysis of the persistive, alterative and inceptive aspects in siSwati. PhD thesis, SOAS (School of Oriental and African Studies). http://eprints.soas.ac.uk/12769/ Copyright © and Moral Rights for this thesis are retained by the author and/or other copyright owners. A copy can be downloaded for personal non‐commercial research or study, without prior permission or charge. This thesis cannot be reproduced or quoted extensively from without first obtaining permission in writing from the copyright holder/s. The content must not be changed in any way or sold commercially in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders. When referring to this thesis, full bibliographic details including the author, title, awarding institution and date of the thesis must be given e.g. AUTHOR (year of submission) "Full thesis title", name of the School or Department, PhD Thesis, pagination. 1 UNIVERSITY OF LONDON School of Oriental and African Studies A Morpho-semantic Analysis of the Persistive, Alterative and Inceptive Aspects in siSwati A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics by Peter John Nichols February 22nd 2011 2 I have read and understood regulation 17.9 of the Regulations for students of the School of Oriental and African Studies concerning plagiarism. I undertake that all the material presented for examination is my own work and has not been written for me, in whole or in part by any other person. I also undertake that any quotation or paraphrase from the published or unpublished work of another person has been duly acknowledged in the work which I present for examination. Peter John Nichols 3 Abstract This study develops an analysis of three aspects in siSwati, a southern African Bantu language spoken in Swaziland and South Africa: PERSISTIVE aspect, encoded by the aspect marker -sa-, ALTERATIVE aspect, encoded by -se - and INCEPTIVE aspect encoded by be-. A distinctive characteristic of two of the aspects is that they link two separate time periods and so I have described them as dual-time period aspects. One is the PERSISTIVE which is an aspect already well-documented and studied cross-linguistically in the Bantu languages and other groups and the other I have analysed as the ALTERATIVE which is a new term I have applied to an aspect not formerly recognized in linguistic studies. The PERSISTIVE aspect links two separate time periods in which an activity is persisting in both or not persisting in both, while the ALTERATIVE aspect links two separate time periods with an activity polarity switch (same activity) or an activity switch (different activity). The INCEPTIVE aspect indicates the beginning of (with stative verbs), or entry into (with dynamic verbs) a situation or state. In siSwati temporal cognition, change (ALTERATIVE ) is afforded greater salience than continuity (PERSISTIVE ). The PERSISTIVE has two senses differentiated by whether external forces are surmounted or not, so a PERSISTIVE aspect which describes a situation that continues unopposed is ‘weak’, and one which describes a situation surmounting obstacles is ‘strong’ and -sa- encodes ‘weak’ PERSISTIVE . The ALTERATIVE has a sub-sense which encodes Exclusivity. The analysis is set within the framework of the cognitive linguistic enterprise and draws on Botne and Kershner's cognitive approach to the analysis of Bantu TAM systems (Botne and Kershner, 2008) and their tertiary timeline, dual domain cognitive model which can be used effectively to represent and analyse the dual-time period aspects. Fauconnier’s Mental Spaces Theory supplemented by Cutrer's recent work is also applied in representing my research findings (Cutrer, 1994, Fauconnier, 1985) and the cognitive approach produces fresh insights into the roles of -sa - and -se - as space builders and dual-time period aspect markers and be- as a tense/aspect marker and auxiliary. 4 Contents: Page Acknowledgements 10 Abbreviations 11 1. Introduction 14 2. Overview of siSwati Structure 19 2.1 Definition of central terms and concepts 19 2.2 Morpho-syntactic overview of siSwati 25 2.2.1 Noun classes and agreement 26 2.2.2 The siSwati verb 28 2.2.3 Tense, Aspect and Mood morphology 29 2.2.3.1 TAM systems – a general overview 29 2.2.3.2 TAM systems – a siSwati overview 34 2.2.4 Verbal aspect and aspectual inflection in statives 44 2.3 The moods in siSwati 45 2.3.1 INDICATIVE mood 46 2.3.1.1 INDICATIVE sub-mood: PARTICIPIAL 51 2.3.1.2 INDICATIVE sub-mood: Compound tenses 53 2.3.1.3 INDICATIVE sub-mood: Compound tenses: Stative 61 2.3.1.4 INDICATIVE sub-mood: Compound tenses: FUTURE 63 2.3.1.5 INDICATIVE sub-mood: Compound tenses: REMOTE PAST 65 2.3.2 POTENTIAL mood 67 2.3.3 SUBJUNCTIVE mood 68 2.3.4 IMPERATIVE mood 69 2.3.5 INFINITIVE mood 69 2.4 Copulative predication 70 2.5 Word order 72 2.6 Summary 73 5 3. Methodology 74 3.1 Introduction 74 3.2 Elicitation Tools for siSwati fieldwork 76 3.3 Overview of methodology 86 3.4 The interview (imibutfo ) stage 91 3.5 The examples ( tibonelo ) stage (consultant discussions) 95 3.6 Morphological analysis 97 3.7 Methodological issues 99 3.8 Summary 102 4. The encoding of PERSISTIVE aspect in siSwati 103 4.1 Introduction 103 4.2 The PERSISTIVE aspect in siSwati 104 4.3 The PERSISTIVE as a dual-time period aspect and the intervening period 107 4.4 The PERSISTIVE in ‘weak’ and ‘strong’ situations 110 4.5 The encoding of PERSISTIVE aspect by -sa- > -se- 114 4.6 The PERSISTIVE and the aspect marker -se - 120 4.7 Negation, aspectuality and the counter-expectational in siSwati 126 4.8 The PERSISTIVE and stative verbs 131 4.9 The ‘weak’ and ‘strong’ PERSISTIVE and PERFECTIVE compound tenses 137 4.10 The semantic duality of conjunction and auxiliary solo 141 4.11 Situational analysis of PERSISTIVE and ALTERATIVE aspectuality 151 4.12 Summary 153 5. The encoding of ALTERATIVE aspect in siSwati 155 5.1 Introduction 155 5.2 The ALTERATIVE aspect in siSwati 156 5.2.1 A semantic review of the ALTERATIVE aspect 160 6 5.3 The ALTERATIVE aspect marker -se- with nyalo and manje 164 5.4 Phonological change of the ALTERATIVE -se- 173 5.5 The ALTERATIVE Exclusivity sub-sense 178 5.5.1 Polarity and ALTERATIVE Exclusivity sub-sense 180 5.5.2 Aspectual analysis of ANTERIOR, ALTERATIVE and Exclusivity 183 5.5.3 A semantic analysis of se-nga-ka 184 5.5.4 A morphological analysis of se-nga-ka 186 5.5.5 Re-analysis of the siSwati aspectual taxonomy 187 5.6 Constructions and compounds with the ALTERATIVE auxiliary -se- 190 5.7 Reduplication of aspectual markers 200 5.8 The morpho-semantic complexity of the dual-time period aspect markers 202 5.9 The REMOTE FUTURE tense and the ALTERATIVE 205 5.9.1 The negative FUTURE marker ngeke and the ALTERATIVE 205 5.10 Summary 208 6. Encoding the INCEPTIVE and CONDITIONAL aspects in siSwati 210 6.1 Introduction 210 6.2 The compound tenses with tense/aspect marker be - and auxiliary -be 211 6.3 Be- as IMPERFECTIVE aspect marker in the compound tenses 214 6.4 INCEPTIVE and CONDITIONAL aspectuality 221 6.5 Analytic and contracted compound forms 228 6.6 A morpho-semantic analysis of the auxiliary ngabe 233 6.7 Summary 235 7. The salience of change ( ALTERATIVE ) and continuity (PERSISTIVE ) in siSwati temporal cognition 237 7.1 Introduction 237 7.2 Analysis of the role of adjectives in siSwati 237 7.3 A cognitive linguistics approach to change and continuity 241 7 7.4 Tense selection, change and continuity 242 7.5 Cognitive models of time, dual-time period aspects and change and continuity 244 7.6 Summary 248 8. The analytical application of Mental Spaces Theory 250 8.1 Introduction 250 8.2 Mental Spaces Theory 250 8.2.1 Mental Spaces Theory and tense-aspect in everyday language 268 8.3 Analytical application of Mental Spaces Theory 276 8.4 Summary 297 9. The analytical application of Botne and Kershner’s cognitive model of Bantu TAM systems 299 9.1 Introduction 299 9.2 EGO -centred and time-based cognitive models for time 301 9.2.1 Cognitive model for [MOVING TIME] (English) 301 9.2.2 Cognitive model for [MOVING EGO] (English) 302 9.2.3 [TEMPORAL SEQUENCE] cognitive model 303 9.3 Cognitive models of time and the siSwati TAM System 306 9.3.1 Cognitive models and grammaticalization of FUTURE markers 309 9.4 Overview of Botne & Kershner’s cognitive linguistics approach 311 9.4.1 Aspectuality in the Privileged and Dissociated domains 317 9.4.2 The Organising Principles applied to Nugunu 322 9.5 Botne and Kershner’s cognitive model and the siSwati TAM system 325 9.6 Botne & Kershner’s model as analytical tool 332 9.6.1 The joint role of conjunction/auxiliary solo with PERSISTIVE -sa- 332 9.6.2 ALTERATIVE aspect in greetings and responses 338 9.6.3 PERFECT tense/aspect and ALTERATIVE -se - 341 9.6.4 ALTERATIVE with FUTURE tenses ( SIMPLE and IMPERFECTIVE ) 347 8 9.6.5 Counter-expectational ALTERATIVE and ‘strong’ PERSISTIVE 352 9.7 Summary 355 10. Conclusions 357 10.1 Re-analysis of the siSwati system 361 10.2 Taxonomy of compound tenses in siSwati 363 10.3 Relevance to current understanding and implications for future research 365 References 367 Appendices 370 Appendix 1 Pictogram conventions 370 2 Pictograms Absolute tenses INDICATIVE captioned 374 3 Pictograms Absolute tenses INDICATIVE uncaptioned 381 4 Pictograms Relative tenses INDICATIVE captioned 388 5 Pictograms Stative tenses INDICATIVE captioned 395 6 Pictograms STATIVE PERFECT tenses INDICATIVE captioned 399 7 Time Line Sheets and Time Reference Symbols 403 8 Research interview sheet 411 Text conventions: 1.
Recommended publications
  • Lessons from Research on Ideophones Mark Dingemanse Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6500 AH Nijmegen, NL [email protected]
    a journal of Dingemanse, Mark. 2018. Redrawing the margins of language: Lessons from general linguistics Glossa research on ideophones. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 3(1): 4. 1–30, DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.444 OVERVIEW ARTICLE Redrawing the margins of language: Lessons from research on ideophones Mark Dingemanse Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6500 AH Nijmegen, NL [email protected] Ideophones (also known as expressives or mimetics, and including onomatopoeia) have been systematically studied in linguistics since the 1850s, when they were first described as a lexical class of vivid sensory words in West-African languages. This paper surveys the research history of ideophones, from its roots in African linguistics to its fruits in general linguistics and typology around the globe. It shows that despite a recurrent narrative of marginalisation, work on ideophones has made an impact in many areas of linguistics, from theories of phonological features to typologies of manner and motion, and from sound symbolism to sensory language. Due to their hybrid nature as gradient vocal gestures that grow roots in discrete linguistic systems, ideophones provide opportunities to reframe typological questions, reconsider the role of language ideology in linguistic scholarship, and rethink the margins of language. With ideophones increasingly being brought into the fold of the language sciences, this review synthesises past theoretical insights and empirical findings in order to enable future work to build on them. Keywords: ideophones; iconicity; linguistic theory; language ideology “Words go on living despite the efforts of scholars to manage them.” — Taro Gomi (1989) 1 Introduction Ideophones are marked words that depict sensory scenes like c’onc’on ‘woven tightly’, ulakpulak ‘unbalanced, scary appearance’ and colcol ‘flowing liquid’ in Korean, or muku- muku ‘mumbling mouth movements’, gelegele ‘glittery appearance’ and gbadara-gbadara ‘a drunkard’s wobbling gait’ in Siwu, a Kwa language spoken in Ghana (Dingemanse 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Studies in the Linguistic Sciences
    i u 5i 1 MAR 1 1998 99b Spr- ODV 2 lences VOLUME 25, NUMBER 1 (SPRING 1995) [Published February 1997] DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS JNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS AT URBANA-CHAMPAIGN THELieR/yWOFTHE C9 19S8 Of )Limo\s STUDIES IN THE LINGUISTIC SCIENCES PUBLICATION OF THE DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS IN THE COLLEGE OF LIBERAL ARTS AND SCIENCES OF THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS AT URBANA-CHAMPAIGN General Editor: Elmer H. Antonsen EDITORIAL ASSISTANT: Mark Honegger Editorial board: Elabbas Benmamoun, Eyamba G. Bokamba, Chin-Chuan Cheng, Jennifer S. Cole, Georgia M. Green, Hans Henrich Hock, Braj B. Kachru, Yamuna Kachru, Chin-W. Kim, Charles W. Kisseberth, Peter Lasersohn, Howard Maclay, Jerry L. Morgan, Rajeshwari Pandharipande, James H. Yoon, and Ladislav Zgusta. AIM: SLS is intended as a forum for the presentation of the latest original re- search by the faculty and students of the Department of Linguistics, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign. Scholars outside the Department and from other institutions are also cordially invited to submit original linguistic research for consideration. In all cases, articles submitted for publication will be reviewed by a panel of at least two experts in the appropriate field to determine suitability for publication. Copyright remains with the individual authors. Authors will receive one copy of the particular issue and 10 offprints of their individual contributions. SLS appears twice a year, and one issue is traditionally devoted to restricted, specialized topics. A complete list of available back issues is given inside the back cover. BOOKS FOR REVIEW: Review copies of books may be sent to: Editor, Studies in the Linguistic Sciences Department of Linguistics, 4088 For.
    [Show full text]
  • Part III: the Typology of Motion Events
    Jean-Michel Fortis - Space in Language - Leipzig Summer School 2010 –PART III PART III — THE TYPOLOGY OF MOTION EVENTS 1. TALMY’S TYPOLOGY The initial version of Talmy’s typology of motion events and its evolution are presented in the following sections. One motivation for this lengthy presentation is that Talmy’s typology is sometimes misunderstood and described erroneously (for ex. Croft et al., to appear), with the consequence that some of the objections raised against it miss their target. Another motivation, opposed to the first, is that Talmy’s theory harbors difficulties that are seldom recognized. III.1. TALMY’S FIRST THEORY (1972) Talmy’s first theory aims at comparing the structure of English with a polysynthetic language of California. Perhaps because deep syntactic structures in the generative style were not well-suited for this purpose, Talmy goes to a deeper, semantic, level (like Wallace Chafe, who was facing the same problem with Onondaga, another polysynthetic language ; cf. Chafe 1970). Talmy’s point of departure is the notion of translatory situation. A translatory situation (an event in which a Figure moves along a path) is decomposed into a fixed structure (translatory structure) of 4 components : Figure : “the object which is considered as moving or located with respect to another object.” (F) Ground : “the object with respect to which a 1st is considered as moving or located.” (G) Directional : “the respect with which one object is considered as moving or located to another object.” (D) Motive : “the moving or located state which one object is considered to be in with respect to another object” (M).
    [Show full text]
  • Ideophones, Gestures and Code-Switching Among Young Zulu Speakers in South Africa
    Language Contact and Expressiveness: Ideophones, gestures and code-switching among young Zulu speakers in South Africa by Anna Miri Speyer Mertner 1 Table of Contents Abstract ..................................................................................................................................... 4 Acknowledgements .................................................................................................................. 4 Introduction .............................................................................................................................. 5 1. Ideophones ............................................................................................................................ 6 1.1 Why are ideophones considered expressive? ................................................................... 7 1.2 Iconicity in ideophones .................................................................................................... 8 1.3 A social perspective on ideophones ............................................................................... 10 2. Gestures .............................................................................................................................. 12 2.1 Defining iconic gestures ................................................................................................ 12 2.2 The link between gestures and ideophones .................................................................... 12 3. A descriptive overview of Zulu ........................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • A Demonstration-Based Account of (Pluractional) Ideophones *
    Proceedings of SALT 26: 664–683, 2016 A demonstration-based account of (pluractional) ideophones * Robert Henderson University of Arizona Abstract This paper develops a novel formal semantics of ideophones that can account for their meaning and compositional properties. The proposal extends recent work on iconicity in sign languages in Davidson 2015, whose demonstration- based framework provides a formal foundation for the semantics of ideophones that captures the difference between descriptive meaning and depictive meaning, the kind of meaning ideophones traffic in. After providing a demonstration-based account of the basic ideophone construction in the Mayan language Tseltal, the paper then shows how the demonstration-based account can be used to analyze pluractionality in the ideophone domain. In particular, through case studies on Tseltal and Upper Necaxa Totonanc (Totonacan), I show that there are two previously unrecognized types of ideophonic pluractionality, and that their properties support the demonstration-based account. Keywords: ideophones, pluractionality, iconicity, demonstrations, Mayan, Totonacan 1 Introduction The term ideophone is used to pick out a distinguished class of words in a language that specialize in depicting sensory imagery (Dingemanse 2011: p. 25; 2012). While the expressions considered in this work fit the definition, ideophones are often easier to point at than define. For this reason, consider the following example of the ideophone tsok’ in Tseltal (Mayan). (1) pura ch’il-bil-Ø, tsok’ x-chi-Ø ta mantekat just fried-PERF-B3 IDF:sound.start.to.fry NT-say-B3 P lard ‘it just gets fried, it goes «tsok’» in the lard’(Pérez González 2012: p. 162) The literature on the formal semantics of ideophones is scarce.
    [Show full text]
  • Herent Complement Verbs in Ibibio Viktoria Apel
    Contents Acknowledgments ........................................................................... 1 Schedule ..................................................................................... 2 Abstracts .................................................................................... 10 Conference Venue ........................................................................... 50 Nearby Dining and Attractions ............................................................. 51 Notes ........................................................................................ 54 Invited Speakers Firmin Ahoua,Akin Akinlabi, and Gildas Gondo ....................................12 When a five-way opposition does not represent a five-tone language: The case of Dan Jeff Good .............................................................................10 Stability under contact as a guide to reconstruction: Noun classes, argument alignment, and the Macro-Sudan belt Tom G¨uldemann .....................................................................14 A shared pronominal canon in the Macro-Sudan belt: typological, areal and geneological aspects Claire Halpert .......................................................................11 Surmountable Barriers Sharon Rose .........................................................................13 Areal vowel harmony patterns in the Nuba Mountains and Beyond Papers and Posters Hasiyatu Abubakari ..................................................................28 Contrastive focus particles in Kusaal
    [Show full text]
  • Kejom (Babanki) Ideophones1
    Pius W. Akumbu University of Buea, Cameroon Kejom (Babanki) Ideophones1 This study identifies and describes ideophones used in Babanki (henceforth Kejom as the people prefer to call themselves and their language), a Centre Ring Grassfields Bantu language of Northwest Cameroon. The study of ideophones or marked words that depict sensory imagery (Dingemanse 2011) in Africa has expanded in recent years (Blench 2010) but our knowledge of this class of words in Grassfields Bantu languages is restricted with only a limited reference to ideophones in the literature on these languages (Blench (2010) on Yamba and Limbum, Tamanji (2009) on Bafut and Akumbu & Fogwe (2012) on Babanki). The properties of Kejom ideophones are examined in this paper and it is concluded that they are marked due to their divergent phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics and pragmatics. It is realized that the ideophones vividly depict one or more sensory events, ranging from sight to emotion. Keywords: Ideophone, Kejom, Reduplication, Sensory, Vowel Lengthening 1. Identification of Kejom Kejom is spoken in Kejom Keku (Big Babanki) and Kejom Ketinguh (Babanki Tungo) in Tubah Subdivision, Mezam Division, Northwest Region of Cameroon. The two villages have a total population of 39.000 people (Ethnologue 2015). Víʔə́ kə̀ jɔ̀ m ‘the people of Kejom’ speak gáʔə́ kə̀ jɔ̀ m ‘the language of Kejom’. Others call them “Babanki”, a designation the people themselves recognize and accept. When speaking with English speakers, they refer to themselves as “Babanki” which is actually the administrative and linguistic name given to them. Ethnologue (2015) lists Babanki with the code ISO 693-3 [bbk] and classifies it as a central Ring language of the wide Grassfields subgroup of Bantoid within Benue-Congo.
    [Show full text]
  • 'To Give an Imagination to the Listeners': the Neglected
    ‘To give an imagination to the listeners’: The neglected poetics of Navajo ideophony* ANTHONY K. WEBSTER Abstract Ideophony is a neglected aspect of investigations of world poetic traditions. This article looks at the use of ideophony in a variety of Navajo poetic genres. Examples are given from Navajo place-names, narratives, and songs. A final example involves the use of ideophony in contemporary written Navajo poetry. Using the work of Woodbury, Friedrich, and Becker it is argued that ideophones are an example of form-dependent expression, poetic indeterminacy, and the inherent exuberances and deficiencies of translation and thus strongly resists translation. This fact becomes more relevant when understood in light of the current language shift from Navajo to English. Keywords: Navajo; ideophones; translation; poetics; language shift; poetry. 1. Introduction: On ideophones In this article, I wish to describe an underappreciated poetic feature found in both Navajo oral tradition and the emerging genre of written Navajo poetry (see Webster 2004). That feature is the use of sound symbolism, onomatopoeia, and ideophony. In particular, I am concerned with the use of ‘sound imitative’ expressions (Hinton et al. 1994: 3). This class of sound symbolism attempts to ‘simulate’ some non-linguistic activity or image in a linguistic form (Nuckolls 2000: 235). These are, following the terminology of Clement Doke (1935) and Janis Nuckolls (2006), ‘ideo- phones’ (see also Tedlock 1999). Doke defines ideophones as follows: A vivid representation of an idea in sound. A word, often onomatopoeic, which describes a predicate, qualificative or adverb in respect to manner, color, sound, smell, action, state or intensity.
    [Show full text]
  • Where Do Ideophones Come From?
    Portland State University PDXScholar Applied Linguistics Faculty Publications and Presentations Applied Linguistics Fall 1989 Where Do Ideophones Come From? George Tucker Childs Portland State University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/ling_fac Part of the African Languages and Societies Commons, and the Linguistics Commons Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Citation Details Childs, George Tucker (1989). Where do ideophones come from. Studies in the linguistic sciences, 19(2), 55-76. This Article is brought to you for free and open access. It has been accepted for inclusion in Applied Linguistics Faculty Publications and Presentations by an authorized administrator of PDXScholar. Please contact us if we can make this document more accessible: [email protected]. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences Volume 19, Number 2, Fall 1989 WHERE DO IDEOPHONES COME FROM?* G. Tucker Childs Temple University This paper begins with an analysis of ideophones in Kisi (West Atlantic, Guinea). This examination leads to consideration of the diachrony of ideophones, but because of the lack of data with any real time depth, we are limited to finding clues in the synchronic data. This study looks first at verbs, the class of words which Kisi ideophones resemble most closely. Such is also the case in other African languages, where ideophones can often be analyzed as verbs, as has been done for several Southern Bantu languages. I then demonstrate how Kisi ideophones can be derived from verbs, illustrating several possible scenarios for such a process. I then look at possible derivational relationships with other word categories, finding that with other word categories the direction of derivation is the opposite.
    [Show full text]
  • Contrastive Linguistics
    Volume 2, Issue 5, May 2015, PP 170-181 ISSN 2349-0373 (Print) & ISSN 2349-0381 (Online) www.arcjournals.org International Journal of Humanities Social Sciences and Education (IJHSSE) Contrastive Linguistics: An Exploration of Ideophones in Yoruba and Edo Speech Communities Ayoola Oluwafunmiso Moses Department of English, University of Zululand, Private Bag X 1001, KwaDlangezwa, 3886, Republic of South Africa [email protected] Abstract Crystal (1997: 189) defines “Ideophones” as “a term used in linguistics and phonetics for any vivid representation of an idea in sound, such as occurs through onomatopoeia”. Ideophones tend to be longer in terms of the combination of sounds than lexical classes. Thus, it enables the users to pack meaning into single morphemes thereby making the words semantically multidimensional. (Woodbury1987:715). Vowel repetition or lengthening is also a characteristic of ideophones. Ideophones are often phonologically anomalous in terms of sounds and sound sequences, tonal structure and phonological behaviour. (Welmer1973:27). In any case, these features have an income relation between sound and meaning. Just like any natural language, ideophones represent a robust word category in African language. To this end, this work is designed to arrive at an applicable analysis and classification of Edo and Yoruba ideophones using a contrastive approach inspired by the idea of canonical typology. The theory of Autosegmental morphology as propounded by Welmer (1981), Marrantz (1982) and Anderson (1992) is employed in this study. This theory proposes that reduplication is essential affixation, but what is affixed is a prosodic template, that is, a syllable foot or even a phonological word is the affixation of a consonant- vowel (CV) skeleton which is itself a morpheme to a stem.
    [Show full text]
  • Pluractional Demonstrations∗
    Pluractional Demonstrations∗ Robert Henderson | The University of Arizona Abstract This paper develops a novel formal semantics for ideophones that can account for their meaning and compositional properties. The proposal ex- tends recent work on iconicity in sign languages by Davidson (2015), whose demonstration-based framework provides a formal foundation for the seman- tics of ideophones that captures the difference between descriptive meaning and depictive meaning, the kind of meaning ideophones traffic in. After pro- viding a demonstration-based account of the basic ideophone construction in the Mayan language Tseltal, the paper then shows how the demonstration- based account can be used to analyze pluractionality in the ideophone do- main. In particular, through case studies on Tseltal and Upper Necaxa Toto- nanc (Totonacan), I show that there are two previously unrecognized types of ideophonic pluractionality, and that their properties support the demonstration- based account. The first, which I call “demonstration-external pluraction- ality”, involves a speaker using an ideophone to do a plurality of demon- strations that characterize a plurality of events. The second kind of ideo- phonic pluractionality, which I call a “demonstration-internal pluractional- ity”, is much more similar to pluractionality in the verbal domain, and in- volves special morphology that derives ideophone stems that can only be used to demonstrate plural events. Finally, I use the contrast between these two types of pluractionality in the ideophone domain to clarify the line be- tween the iconic and non-iconic aspects of the semantics of ideophones. Keywords: ideophones, pluractionality, iconicity, demonstrations, Mayan, Totonacan ∗I would like to thank Scott AnderBois, Diana Archangeli, Nicolas Asher, Rebekah Baglini, Dy- lan Bumford, Simon Charlow, Lucas Champollion, Kathryn Davidson, Jeremy Kuhn, Heidi Harley, Serge Minor, Jaime Pérez González, and Philippe Schlenker for extremely helpful discussions about ideophones and iconicity.
    [Show full text]
  • Survey of Bantu Ideophones.Pdf
    AFRICAN LANGUAGE STUDIES XII J971 SCHOOL OF ORIENTAL AND AFRICAN STUDIES UNIVERSITY OF LONDON Sole Agents: Luzac & Company Ltd., 46 Great Russell Street, London, W.C.i. School of Oriental and African Studies University of London SBN : 901877 17 4 / SURVEY OF BANTU IDEOPHONES By William J. Samarin Introduction This survey of Bantu ideophones is meant to report on what in general is known about them and to stimulate further investigation. Its aim is a modest one. To achieve the first goal I have had to depend on what I have read. Therefore the picture that emerges here has its basis largely in the literature that was available to me. All the African grammars in the libraries of the Hartford Seminary Foundation (Hartford, Connecticut), the University of Leiden (The Netherlands), and the University of Toronto (Canada) were personally examined. Of these 250 grammars (give or take a few), there were about 150 that dealt in some way with ideophones. Among these were about 90 grammars of Bantu languages. These are listed in the Bibliography, including ten titles added just before this paper went to press ; I was therefore unable to utilize them at all. Among them is Hulstaert's very important work of 1966. To achieve the second goal I have had to review the literature somewhat critically. I use the term critical in the academic sense. I disparage no writer for his observations. Nevertheless, I have pointed out such things as errors of interpretation, generalizations based on insufficient data, or the absence of sub- stantiation by empirical research. In a few cases I suggest specific kinds of research that might be undertaken or topics that need further study.
    [Show full text]