The Situational Character: a Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal , 93 93 Geo L

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Situational Character: a Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal , 93 93 Geo L Santa Clara Law Santa Clara Law Digital Commons Faculty Publications Faculty Scholarship 11-2004 The ituaS tional Character: A Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal Jon Hanson Santa Clara University School of Law David Yosifon Santa Clara University School of Law Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.law.scu.edu/facpubs Part of the Law and Economics Commons, Law and Society Commons, and the Legal History Commons Automated Citation Jon Hanson and David Yosifon, The Situational Character: A Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal , 93 93 Geo L. J. 1 (2004), Available at: https://digitalcommons.law.scu.edu/facpubs/59 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Faculty Scholarship at Santa Clara Law Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of Santa Clara Law Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Articles The Situational Character: A Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal JON HANSON* & DAVID YOSIFON** Th is Article is dedicated to retiring the now-dominant "rational actor" model of human agency, together with its numerous "dispositionist" cohorts, and replacing them with a new conception of human agency that the authors call the "situational character." Th is is a key installment of a larger project recently introduced in an article titled The Situation: An Introduction to the Situational Character, Critical Realism, Power Economics, and Deep Capture. 1 That introduc­ tory article adumbrated, often in broad stroke, the central premises and some basic conclusions of a new app roach to legal theory and policy analysis. This Article provides a more complete version of one of those central premises by elucidating a more realistic conception of the human animal than is currently embraced in legal theory. The Article begins with a short introduction to the la rger project, and de­ scribes the central place that a realist conception of the human actor plays in that project. It then explores several bodies of literature within the fields of social, cognitive, behavioral, and neuralpsychology in pursuit of a vision of the human actor that is grounded in social science. Having explicated that conception, the Article then outlines some of the basic implications of it fo r law, legal theory, and social policy. It then analyzes conventional legal scholars', particularly legal economists', arguments fo r ignoring the lessons of social science in their treatment of human agency. As part of that analysis, this Article describes why recent efforts to incorporate some psycho­ logical findings-the sort of work that is often labeled "behavioralist"-have been inadequate. Finally, the authors brieflylook beyond thehuman actor itselfto consider some of the fa irly obvious-but generally ignored-realities of our present social situation, and some of their implications fo r common policypr esumptions. As subsequent work will make clear, this new, situationist conception of the human animal is as important to a realist account of law and legal theory as the dispositionist conception has been to now-dominant accounts. * Professor of Law, Harvard University. ** Visiting Professor of Law, Rutgers Law School-Camden. We are grateful to Tracy Conn, Kemoy Foster, Sam Halabi, Geutam lois, Mariko Miki, Susie Rhees, Matt Thompson, Brandon Weiss, and Andrew Woods for superb research assistance, and to Carol Igoe for outstanding secretarial assistance. Financing from the Harvard Law School Summer Research Fund and Rutgers Law School-Camden School provided valuable research support. We are also grateful for helpful comments to participants at the Yale Legal Theory Workshop, the University of Texas Law School Faculty Workshop, and the 2004 Louise D. Brandeis Lecture Series at Harvard Law School, sponsored by the American Constitution Society. Finally, we would like to express our love and deepest gratitude to Kathleen, Emily, Erin and Ian Hanson, and Paula Noyes-without these characters in our situation, this Article would never have been possible. I. 152 U. PA. L. REV. 129 (2003). 1 2 THE GEORGETOWN LAW JOURNAL [Vol. 93 :1 TABLE OF CONTENTS I. INTRODUCTION ...................................... 6 A. BRINGING THE READER UP TO SPEED . 6 B. AN INTRODUCTION TO DISPOSITIONISM ................... 6 C. THE UBIQUITY OF DISPOSITIONISM ...................... 8 1. Dispositionism in (Law and) Economics . .. .. 8 2. Dispositionism in Classic Liberal Political Theory . .. 10 3. Dispositionism in the Law ...................... 13 a. Contract Law ........... ... .... ..... 13 b. Tort Law . .. .. .. .. .. .. 15 c. Criminal Law .... .............. ... 18 D. THE HIGH STAKES OF DISPOSITIONISM-AND OF OUR ARGUMENT . 20 II. MISSING THE SITUATION AND SEEING DISPOSITION . .. .. .. .. 22 A. TWO SOURCES OF DISPOSITIONISM ...................... 23 1. A Basic Source of Dispositionism . .. .. .. .. 23 2. A Deeper Source of Dispositionism ............... 24 B. THE INTERIOR FUNDAMENTAL ATTRIBUTION ERROR . ........ 25 1. Thinking and Being . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. 28 2. Preferences and Identity . .. .. .. .. .. .. 29 3. Willing, Acting, and Freedom . .. .. .. .. .. .. 30 4. Summary of the Interior Fundamental Attribution Error . 31 5. The Situation of Our Interiors . .. .. .. .. .. .. 32 6. Situation Defined . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. .. 33 III. GETTING TO KNowT HE SITUATIONAL CHARACTER .. .... .... .. 34 A. INSIDE THE SITUATIONAL CHARACTER . .. .. .. .. .. 34 1. Flies, Rats, and Science ............ .......... 34 2. A Closer Look at the Interior Situation ............. 36 B. UNSEEN COGNITION (VS. "THINKING") ................... 37 1. Choice Biases .......... ............ ...... 39 2004] THE SITUATIONAL CHARACTER 3 a. Intratemporal Effe cts . 39 i. Heuristics ........................... 39 ii. Endowment Effe cts . 41 iii. Framing Effe cts . 42 iv. Summary............................ 43 b. Intertemporal Effe cts . 44 2. Process Biases . 49 a. Knowledge Structures ..... 50 i. Th e Stereotypical Knowledge Structure . 52 ii. Cognitive Groupism . 54 iii. Stereotype Threat and Some Exterior Situational Consequences of Knowledge Structures .... ......... .............. 59 b. Attributional Processes . 62 i. Causation, Responsibility, and Blame ... .. .. 62 ii. LayJud ges . ... ................. 65 iii. The Situation of Attributions . 66 c. Counter/actual Thinking ........... ... ... 67 3. Defending our Knowledge Structures .............. 71 a. Where Motive Meets Knowledge Structures . 71 b. Economics: A Preliminary Case Study ... 75 i. Knowledge Structures in the Analysis of Te mporalEffe cts ... .... ...... ......... 76 ii. The Focus on Choice Biases . 82 4. Summary . .................................. 84 C. ATIlTUDES (vs. "PREFERRING") .....•.....••...••••.•• 85 1. Some Evolution of the Concept . 85 a. The Traditional View-Stable and Causal . 85 b. The Modem View-Neither Stable nor Causal . 86 c. The Emerging View- Implicit and Automatic . 87 2. Motivation ................................. 90 4 THE GEORGETOWN LAW JOURNAL [Vol. 93: 1 a. Motive to Understand .. .................... 91 b. Motive to Self-Affirm . .. 94 i. Individual ........................... 95 ii. Group .... .......................... 100 iii. System (and World) .. ............... ... 101 iv. Summary............................ 106 c. Motive to Simplify . .. 106 d. Motive to Cohere . .. .. .. .. 107 i. Individual ........................... 108 ii. Group.............................. 111 3. Affect..................................... 115 D. BEHAVIOR (VS. "CHOOSING") ....... .. ................ 120 1. Visceral Factors . .. 120 2. The Inverted Causal Relationship Between Attitudes and Behavior . .. 122 E. AUTOMATICITY & THE ILLUSION OF CONSCIOUS WILL ("THE WILL") ...... ........ ... ........ .............. 124 1. Automaticity ................................ 126 2. The Illusion of Will ......... .. 128 iv. A FEW POSSIBLE LESSONS FOR LAW AND LEGAL THEORY l33 A. THE LAW . .. .. 134 B. LEGAL THEORY . .. .. .. .. .. .. l36 V. DrsPOSITIONISM IN LAW AND ECONOMICS . .. .. 138 A. THE DISPOSmONIST ACTOR OF LAY AND LEGAL-ECONOMIC THEORIES ... ........................... ........ 139 B. SCRUTINIZING THE MYTH OF THE RATIONAL ACTOR . .. .. 14 4 2004] THE SITUATIONAL CHARACTER 5 C. FIVE TYPES OF INADEQUATE "REALISM" .................. 152 1. Counterfeit Realism . 153 2. Defensive Realism . 159 3. Selective Realism ................ ...... ...... 167 4. Weak-Fonn Realism . ...................... ... 167 5. Strong-Fonn Realism . 169 VI . THE SITUATIONAL CHARACTER'S SITUATION . 17 0 A. SEEING IT ............. .. .................... 17 1 B. MISSING IT . • . 17 2 C. DENYING WHAT WE CANNOT ADMIT . 17 5 VII . CONCLUSION........................................ 177 "What is a human being? Legal theorists must, perforce, answer this question: jurisprudence, aft er all, is about human beings. " - Robin West2 "If you want to know the law and nothing else, you must look at it as a bad man, who cares only fo r the material consequences which such knowledge enables him to predict, not as a good one, who finds his reasons fo r conduct, whether inside the law or outside of it, in the vaguer sanctions of con­ science. " - Oliver Wendell Holmes, Jr. 3 "[Tlhe implicit modern liberalconception of the average person [is that he is] good, but inept, and fo r both reasons not very responsive to incentives, though perhaps rather plastic. The implicit conservative view of the average person,
Recommended publications
  • Minutes of the Meeting of the Board of Trustees of Ohio
    MINUTES OF THE MEETING OF THE BOARD OF TRUSTEES OF OHIO UNIVERSITY Friday, April 17 and Saturday, April 18, 1998 Ohio University, Athens Campus THE OHIO UNIVERSITY BOARD OF TRUSTEES 1110 MINUTES OF April 18, 1998 MEETING TABLE OF CONTENTS Executive Session 174 Roll Call 175 Approval of February 7, 1998 Minutes 175 Communications, Petitions, and Memorials 175 Announcements 175 Reports 176 Unfinished Business 176 New Business 176 Budget, Finance, and Physical Plant Committee 177 Recognition of Student Housing Master Plan 8c Strategic Plan Study Resolution 1998 -- 1565 178 Tax-Deferred Purchase of Additional Service Credit - Resolution 1998 — 1566 180 Alternative Retirement Program — Resolution 1998 — 1567 182 Naming of Lounge in Wren Stadium for Joseph Dean Resolution 1998— 1568 187 Naming of the Basketball Suite in Honor of Vern and Marion Alden Resolution 1998 — 1569 189 Hocking River Bridge — Resolution 1998 — 1570 191 Third Floor Renovation at the Riffe Center, Southern Campus Resolution 1998 — 1571 195 Putnam Hall Childcare Center Improvements — Resolution 1998 — 1572 199 Fire Alarm Replacement at the Convocation Center and Seigfrcd Hall Resolution 1998 — 1573 202 ADA Improvements, Phase Two — Resolution 1998 — 1574 205 Naming of Space in Stocker Center "Loehr Leadership Resource Center" Resolution 1998 — 1575 209 Educational Policies Committee 213 Faculty/Administrative Emeriti Awards — Resolution 1998 — 1576 214 Faculty Fellowship Awards — Resolution 1998 — 1577 448 Name and Mission Change of OUCOM Dept. of Clinical Research to Biomedical
    [Show full text]
  • Working Memory, Cognitive Miserliness and Logic As Predictors of Performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test
    Working Memory, Cognitive Miserliness and Logic as Predictors of Performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test Edward J. N. Stupple ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Maggie Gale ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Christopher R. Richmond ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Abstract Most participants respond that the answer is 10 cents; however, a slower and more analytic approach to the The Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) was devised to measure problem reveals the correct answer to be 5 cents. the inhibition of heuristic responses to favour analytic ones. The CRT has been a spectacular success, attracting more Toplak, West and Stanovich (2011) demonstrated that the than 100 citations in 2012 alone (Scopus). This may be in CRT was a powerful predictor of heuristics and biases task part due to the ease of administration; with only three items performance - proposing it as a metric of the cognitive miserliness central to dual process theories of thinking. This and no requirement for expensive equipment, the practical thesis was examined using reasoning response-times, advantages are considerable. There have, moreover, been normative responses from two reasoning tasks and working numerous correlates of the CRT demonstrated, from a wide memory capacity (WMC) to predict individual differences in range of tasks in the heuristics and biases literature (Toplak performance on the CRT. These data offered limited support et al., 2011) to risk aversion and SAT scores (Frederick, for the view of miserliness as the primary factor in the CRT.
    [Show full text]
  • Mind Perception Daniel R. Ames Malia F. Mason Columbia
    Mind Perception Daniel R. Ames Malia F. Mason Columbia University To appear in The Sage Handbook of Social Cognition, S. Fiske and N. Macrae (Eds.) Please do not cite or circulate without permission Contact: Daniel Ames Columbia Business School 707 Uris Hall 3022 Broadway New York, NY 10027 [email protected] 2 What will they think of next? The contemporary colloquial meaning of this phrase often stems from wonder over some new technological marvel, but we use it here in a wholly literal sense as our starting point. For millions of years, members of our evolving species have gazed at one another and wondered: what are they thinking right now … and what will they think of next? The interest people take in each other’s minds is more than idle curiosity. Two of the defining features of our species are our behavioral flexibility—an enormously wide repertoire of actions with an exquisitely complicated and sometimes non-obvious connection to immediate contexts— and our tendency to live together. As a result, people spend a terrific amount of time in close company with conspecifics doing potentially surprising and bewildering things. Most of us resist giving up on human society and embracing the life of a hermit. Instead, most perceivers proceed quite happily to explain and predict others’ actions by invoking invisible qualities such as beliefs, desires, intentions, and feelings and ascribing them without conclusive proof to others. People cannot read one another’s minds. And yet somehow, many times each day, most people encounter other individuals and “go mental,” as it were, adopting what is sometimes called an intentional stance, treating the individuals around them as if they were guided by unseen and unseeable mental states (Dennett, 1987).
    [Show full text]
  • Nawj Psych Terms
    NAWJ Terms List 1 Psychological terms useful in understanding mechanisms allowing unconscious bias Accentuation Effect: Overestimation of similarities among people within a group and dissimilarities between people from different groups Accentuation principle: States that categorization accentuates perceived similarities within and differences between groups on dimensions that people believe are correlated with the category. The effect is amplified where the categorization/dimension has subjective importance, relevance or value Actor-Observer effect: Tendency to attribute our own behaviors externally and others’ behaviors internally Agentic mode: State of mind thought by Milgram to characterize unquestioning obedience, in which people transfer personal responsibility to the person giving orders Anchoring and adjustment : A cognitive short-cut in which inferences are tied to initial standards or schemas Attitude: A relatively enduring organization of beliefs, feelings and behavioral tendencies towards socially significant objects, groups, events or symbols. Attitude change can occur by inducing someone to perform an act that runs counter to an existing attitude. Attribution : The process of assigning a cause to behaviors and events Availability bias: A cognitive shortcut in which the frequency or likelihood of an event is based on how quickly instances or associations come to mind Bias blind spot: tendency to perceive cognitive and motivational biases much more in others than in oneself Cognition: The knowledge, beliefs, thoughts, and ideas
    [Show full text]
  • 50 Cognitive and Affective Biases in Medicine (Alphabetically)
    50 Cognitive and Affective Biases in Medicine (alphabetically) Pat Croskerry MD, PhD, FRCP(Edin), Critical Thinking Program, Dalhousie University Aggregate bias: when physicians believe that aggregated data, such as those used to develop clinical practice guidelines, do not apply to individual patients (especially their own), they are exhibiting the aggregate fallacy. The belief that their patients are atypical or somehow exceptional, may lead to errors of commission, e.g. ordering x-rays or other tests when guidelines indicate none are required. Ambiguity effect: there is often an irreducible uncertainty in medicine and ambiguity is associated with uncertainty. The ambiguity effect is due to decision makers avoiding options when the probability is unknown. In considering options on a differential diagnosis, for example, this would be illustrated by a tendency to select options for which the probability of a particular outcome is known over an option for which the probability is unknown. The probability may be unknown because of lack of knowledge or because the means to obtain the probability (a specific test, or imaging) is unavailable. The cognitive miser function (choosing an option that requires less cognitive effort) may also be at play here. Anchoring: the tendency to perceptually lock on to salient features in the patient’s initial presentation too early in the diagnostic process, and failure to adjust this initial impression in the light of later information. This bias may be severely compounded by the confirmation bias. Ascertainment bias: when a physician’s thinking is shaped by prior expectation; stereotyping and gender bias are both good examples. Attentional bias: the tendency to believe there is a relationship between two variables when instances are found of both being present.
    [Show full text]
  • Croskerry MD, Phd, FRCP(Edin)
    Clinical Decision Making + Strategies for Cognitive Debiasing Pat Croskerry MD, PhD, FRCP(Edin) International Association of Endodontists Scottsdale, Arizona June 2019 Financial Disclosures or other Conflicts of Interest None It is estimated that an American adult makes 35,000 decisions a day i.e. about 2200 each waking hour Sollisch J: The cure for decision fatigue. Wall Street Journal, 2016 Decision making ‘The most important decision we need to make in Life is how we are going to make decisions’ Professor Gigerenzer Is there a problem with the way we think and make decisions? 3 domains of decision making Patients Healthcare leadership Healthcare providers Patients Leading Medical Causes of Death in the US and their Preventability in 2000 Cause Total Preventability (%) Heart disease 710,760 46 Malignant neoplasms 553,091 66 Cerebrovascular 167,661 43 Chronic respiratory 122,009 76 Accidents 97,900 44 Diabetes mellitus 69,301 33 Acute respiratory 65,313 23 Suicide 29,350 100 Chronic Liver disease 26,552 75 Hypertension/renal 12,228 68 Assault (homicide) 16,765 100 All other 391,904 14 Keeney (2008) Healthcare leadership Campbell et al, 2017 Healthcare providers US deaths in 2013 • 611,105 Heart disease • 584,881 Cancer • 251,454 Medical error Medical error is the 3rd leading cause of death Estimated number of preventable hospital deaths due to diagnostic failure annually in the US 40,000 – 80,000 Leape, Berwick and Bates JAMA 2002 Diagnostic failure is the biggest problem in patient safety Newman-Toker, 2017 Sources of Diagnostic Failure The System 25% The Individual 75% Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N.
    [Show full text]
  • DIALOGUE Page 1
    DIALOGUE Page 1 Volume 18, No. 2 DIALOGUE Dialogue — Fall, 2003 The Official Newsletter of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology Chris Crandall & Monica Biernat, Co-Editors 2004 Convention: Looking Inside the Forward to Austin, Texas Current Issue: meeting in this new property, one of a dozen By Rick Hoyle State of the Society; New 2, 3 we anticipate an enthusiastic preconferences ranging in Arrangements for the 2004 reception by hotel focus from the latest Executive Director chosen convention, to be held management and staff. research findings in specific areas of personality and Student Pub/TIP Winners; 4 January 29-31 in Austin, Carlston on Writing Texas, are nearly complete. The program committee, led social psychology, to career Construction of the new by Mark Leary, has put development, to the art and The first Summer Institute 5, Hilton Austin, convention together the most varied and science of teaching in Social Psychology 16 headquarters, is ahead of extensive SPSP program psychology. schedule. ever (see p. 8 for an article PSPB News; call for Editor 6 from the Program Chair The convention officially Nominations Hotel management projects a about how the decisions opens with a welcome were made). reception at 5:30 on Report from the 7 “soft opening” by November th and a full opening by early Thursday the 29 . As a Publications Committee January. As one of the first If you can make it to Austin reward for our willingness a day early, then you’ll have to commit to holding our SPSP Convention 8 organizations to stage a Programming the option of participating in (Continued on page 3) President’s Column: Jim 9 SPSP Election Results: New Officers Chosen Blascovich on Rumors SPSP Elections were held Blascovich will become Past- term, and represents the Dan Wegner on Discovery 10 and Debate in Science this spring; the Society President.
    [Show full text]
  • Towards a Balanced Social Psychology: Causes, Consequences, and Cures for the Problem-Seeking Approach to Social Behavior and Cognition
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2004) 27, 000–000 Printed in the United States of America Towards a balanced social psychology: Causes, consequences, and cures for the problem-seeking approach to social behavior and cognition Joachim I. Krueger Department of Psychology, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912. [email protected] http://www.brown.edu/departments/psychology/faculty/krueger.html David C. Funder Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, CA 92506 [email protected] http://www.psych.ucr.edu/faculty/funder/rap/rap.htm Abstract: Mainstream social psychology focuses on how people characteristically violate norms of action through social misbehaviors such as conformity with false majority judgments, destructive obedience, and failures to help those in need. Likewise, they are seen to violate norms of reasoning through cognitive errors such as misuse of social information, self-enhancement, and an over-readiness to at- tribute dispositional characteristics. The causes of this negative research emphasis include the apparent informativeness of norm viola- tion, the status of good behavior and judgment as unconfirmable null hypotheses, and the allure of counter-intuitive findings. The short- comings of this orientation include frequently erroneous imputations of error, findings of mutually contradictory errors, incoherent interpretations of error, an inability to explain the sources of behavioral or cognitive achievement, and the inhibition of generalized the- ory. Possible remedies include increased attention to the complete range of behavior and judgmental accomplishment, analytic reforms emphasizing effect sizes and Bayesian inference, and a theoretical paradigm able to account for both the sources of accomplishment and of error. A more balanced social psychology would yield not only a more positive view of human nature, but also an improved under- standing of the bases of good behavior and accurate judgment, coherent explanations of occasional lapses, and theoretically grounded suggestions for improvement.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Stereotypes and the Predictive Brain: Cognition and Culture in “Biased” Person Perception
    ARTICLE Received 28 Jan 2017 | Accepted 17 Jul 2017 | Published 1 Sep 2017 DOI: 10.1057/palcomms.2017.86 OPEN Implicit stereotypes and the predictive brain: cognition and culture in “biased” person perception Perry Hinton1 ABSTRACT Over the last 30 years there has been growing research into the concept of implicit stereotypes. Particularly using the Implicit Associations Test, it has been demon- strated that experimental participants show a response bias in support of a stereotypical association, such as “young” and “good” (and “old” and “bad”) indicating evidence of an implicit age stereotype. This has been found even for people who consciously reject the use of such stereotypes, and seek to be fair in their judgement of other people. This finding has been interpreted as a “cognitive bias”, implying an implicit prejudice within the individual. This article challenges that view: it is argued that implicit stereotypical associations (like any other implicit associations) have developed through the ordinary working of “the predictive brain”. The predictive brain is assumed to operate through Bayesian principles, developing associations through experience of their prevalence in the social world of the perceiver. If the predictive brain were to sample randomly or comprehensively then stereotypical associations would not be picked up if they did not represent the state of the world. However, people are born into culture, and communicate within social networks. Thus, the implicit stereotypical associations picked up by an individual do not reflect a cognitive bias but the associations prevalent within their culture—evidence of “culture in mind”. Therefore to understand implicit stereotypes, research should examine more closely the way associations are communicated within social networks rather than focusing exclusively on an implied cognitive bias of the individual.
    [Show full text]
  • How Can Geologic Decision Making Under Uncertainty Be Improved? Cristina G
    Solid Earth Discuss., https://doi.org/10.5194/se-2019-70 Manuscript under review for journal Solid Earth Discussion started: 5 April 2019 c Author(s) 2019. CC BY 4.0 License. How can geologic decision making under uncertainty be improved? Cristina G. Wilson1,2, Clare E. Bond3, and Thomas F. Shipley4 1Department of Psychology, College of Arts and Sciences, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA 2Department of Electrical and Systems Engineering, School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA 3Department of Geology and Petroleum Geology, School of Geosciences, University of Aberdeen, Kings College, Aberdeen, UK 4Department of Psychology, College of Arts and Sciences, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA Correspondence: Cristina G. Wilson ([email protected]) Abstract. In the geosciences, recent attention has been paid to the influence of uncertainty on expert decision making. When making decisions under conditions of uncertainty, people tend to employ heuristics (rules of thumb) based on experience, re- lying on their prior knowledge and beliefs to intuitively guide choice. Over 50 years of decision making research in cognitive psychology demonstrates that heuristics can lead to less-than-optimal decisions, collectively referred to as biases. For example, 5 the availability bias occurs when people make judgments based on what is most dominant or accessible in memory; a geosci- entist who has spent the past several months studying strike-slip faults will have this terrain most readily available in her mind when interpreting new seismic data. Given the important social and commercial implications of many geoscience decisions, there is a need to develop effective interventions for removing or mitigating decision bias.
    [Show full text]
  • Taking Behavioralism Too Seriously? the Unwarranted Pessimism of the New Behavioral Analysis of Law
    William & Mary Law Review Volume 43 (2001-2002) Issue 5 Article 4 April 2002 Taking Behavioralism Too Seriously? The Unwarranted Pessimism of the New Behavioral Analysis of Law Gregory Mitchell Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarship.law.wm.edu/wmlr Part of the Jurisprudence Commons Repository Citation Gregory Mitchell, Taking Behavioralism Too Seriously? The Unwarranted Pessimism of the New Behavioral Analysis of Law, 43 Wm. & Mary L. Rev. 1907 (2002), https://scholarship.law.wm.edu/wmlr/vol43/iss5/4 Copyright c 2002 by the authors. This article is brought to you by the William & Mary Law School Scholarship Repository. https://scholarship.law.wm.edu/wmlr TAKING BEHAVIORALISM TOO SERIOUSLY? THE UNWARRANTED PESSIMISM OF THE NEW BEHAVIORAL ANALYSIS OF LAW GREGORY MITCHELL* ABSTRACT Legal scholars increasingly rely on a behavioral analysis of judgment and decision making to explain legalphenomena and argue for legal reforms. The. main argument of this new behavioralanalysis of the law is twofold: (1)All human cognition is beset by systematic flaws in the way that judgments and decisionsare made, andtheseflaws lead topredictable irrational behaviors and (2) these widespreadand systematic nonrational tendencies bring into serious question the assumption of procedural rationality underlying much legal doctrine. This Article examines the psychological research relied on by legal behavioraliststo form this argumentand demonstratesthat this research does not support the bleak and simple portrait of pervasiveirrationality painted by these scholars.Careful scrutiny of the psychological researchreveals greater adherence to norms of rationalitythan that implied by the legal behavioralists,and the methodological and interpretive limitations on this psychological research make extrapolation from experimental settings to real world legal settings often inappropriate.
    [Show full text]
  • Cumulative Subject Index Volumes 1–25 (Contains Links to Entries) ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSTUVWXYZ
    European Review of Social Psychology Cumulative Subject Index Volumes 1–25 (contains links to entries) ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSTUVWXYZ Locators in bold refer to figures/tables goal-based explanations (12) 201–235 – Action control, behavioural intentions Ability, praise and criticism (3) 259 283 (12) 21 Abnormal conditions focus model (2) 59–61 – Action control, emotion regulation (23) Aboriginal Peoples, Canada (24) 15 16 146–156 Absentminded relapses (22) 20 Action initiation, implementation intentions Abstract language use (25) see Linguistic (4) 166–171 abstraction Action readiness (22) 116, 117; (22) 153 Abstract modelling, indirect intergroup Action for social change (24) 244 contact (25) 319, 375 Action-oriented personality factors (24) 146, Abstract reasoning (22) 169 146–147 Abstraction, intragroup variability (7) – Action tendencies, conflict resolution (25) 4, 111 113 9–10, 18 Abu Ghraib, prisoners (24) 2 Activation of attitudes (24) 270–273 Academic personnel selection (25) see Actor effects (22) 367, 369 Personnel selection Actor perspective, positive–negative Academic procrastination (24) 155, 156 asymmetry (1) 38–39 Access consciousness (22) 8–9 – – Actor observer bias Access dissociations (22) 4, 10 17 agent vs. recipient (16) 182–184 Acceptance of death, terror management – linguistic category model (LCM) (2) theory (25) 57, 58 59 17–18 Accessible knowledge prediction (6) 13–16 meta-cognition (22) 185–186 – Actor-Partner Interdependence Model priming (22) 183 185 (APIM) (22) 350; (22) 367 Accessibility of thoughts Acute stress
    [Show full text]