A Neural Dissociation Within Language: Evidence That the Mental Dictionary Is Part of Declarative Memory, and That Grammatical R

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Neural Dissociation Within Language: Evidence That the Mental Dictionary Is Part of Declarative Memory, and That Grammatical R A Neural Dissociation Within Language: Evidence that the Mental Dictionary is Part of Declarative Memory, and that Grammatical Rules are Processed by the Procedural System The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Ullman, Michael T., Suzanne Corkin, Marie Coppola, Gregory Hickok, John H. Growdon, Walter J. Koroshetz, and Steven Pinker. 1997. A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9(2): 266-276. Published Version doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.266 Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:3600798 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA A Neural Dissociation within Language: Evidence that the Mental Dictionary Is Part of Declarative Memory, and that Grammatical Rules Are Processed by the Procedural System Michael T. Ullman Institute for Cognitive and Computational Science Georgetown University Suzanne Corkin MIT Marie Coppola University of Rochester Gregory Hickok University of California, Irvine John H. Growdon and Walter J. Koroshetz Massachusetts General Hospital Steven Pinker MIT Abstract W Language comprises a lexicon for storing words and a gram- memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease, led to more errors mar for generating rule-governed forms. Evidence is presented with irregular than regular and novel verbs. Grammatical that the lexicon is part of a temporal-parietalhnedial-temporal difficulties in anterior aphasia, and the general impairment of “declarative memory” system and that granlmatical rules are procedures in Parkinson’s disease, led to the opposite pattern. processed by a frontamasal-ganglia “procedural” system. Pa- In contrast to the Parkinson’s patients, who showed sup tients produced past tenses of regular and novel verbs (looked pressed motor activity and rule use, Huntington’s disease pa- and plagged), which require an -ed-suffixation rule, and irregu- tients showed excess motor activity and rule use, underscoring lar verbs (dug), which are retrieved from memory. Word-finding a role for the basal ganglia in grammatical processing. W difficulties in posterior aphasia, and the general declarative INTRODUCTION frontal cortex, including Broca’s area, and links between lexical memory and left temporal and parietal cortex Two capacities give human language its vast expressive (Geschwind, 1965;Goodglass, 1993;Wernicke, 1874).The power. One is a “mental lexicon” containing thousands speech of aphasics with frontal (anterior) lesions is often of words, each a memorized, arbitrary sound-meaning “agrammatic,”with a breakdown of sentence structure pairing. The other is a “mental grammar” of generative and the omission or misuse of grammatical morphemes, rules that combine words into an infinite number of while access to content words such as verbs and nouns larger words, phrases, and sentences (Chomsky, 1965; De is less dramatically impaired (Damasio, 1992;Goodglass, Saussure, 1959;Pinker, 1994). Previous evidence has sug- 1993). Electrophysiological and functional neuroimaging gested links between grammatical processing and left studies have also implicated frontal regions in grammati- 0 1997 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9:2,pp. 266-276 cal processing (Kluender & Kutas, 1993; Neville et al., matical processing (Lieberman et al., 1992), performing 1991; Stromswold et al., 1996). Aphasics with temporal operations comparable to those done for motor pro- or parietal (posterior) lesions often speak in relatively gramming. Damasio and Damasio (1 992), noting the ana- intact sentence structures, but have severely impaired tomical interconnections between cortical language access to content words (Damasio, 1992; Goodglass, areas and the basal ganglia, predict that “the basal ganglia 1993). The dissociation, however, is imperfect and still serve to assemble the components of complex motions controversial (Bates & Wulfeck, 1989; Goodglass, 1993). into a smooth whole, and it seems reasonable that they Most groups of aphasics tested have complex lesion might perform an analogous function in assembling patterns, making symptom-lesion correlations imperfect. word-forms into sentences.” Moreover, testing the dissociation has been problematic We tested this memoryhule dissociation by devising because tasks probing for grammar and for memory have a task based on a simple linguistic system in which differed in ways other than their use of the two capaci- reliance on grammar and lexicon differs, while other ties. In this study we confirm the dissociation using a factors are held constant. Regular (look-looked) and ir- simple language task in which the use of the two Wstic regular (dig-dug) past tense forms of verbs are well- capacities is contrasted while other factors, such as com- matched in complexity (one word), syntax (tensed), and plexity, meaning and task demands, are held constant. meaning (past). But regular verbs are predictable in form We also relate the dissociation to larger principles of (verb stem + e9,and new ones are constantly being neural organization. Cutting across the division of the added (tir?ed, mosbed), whereas irregular verbs are un- brain into systems such as language and visual percep predictable (compare sing-sang, jling$ung, bring- tion, there is an orthogonal division into two major kinds brought), and constitute a fixed list. A simple theory is of memory systems (Cohen & Squire, 1980; Mishkin et that irregular forms are memorized, and regular forms al., 1984; Damasio & Damasio, 1992; Squire et al., 1993). are generated by a rule. The rule comprises two opera- One is a declarative memory system underlying the tions: copying the stem, and adding a suffix. Regulars and learning and storage of information about facts and irregulars interact as follows: Retrieval of an irregular events. It is subserved by a medial temporal circuit con- blocks the rule (dug preempts digged);when an irregu- nected largely with neocortical areas in the temporal lar is not successfully retrieved, the rule may be applied, and parietal lobes, with the medial temporal compo- resulting in “overregularization”errors such as digged. nents consolidating memories that are eventually stored Alternative theories have been proposed. In one, regu- in neocortex (Cohen & Squire, 1980;Mishkin et al., 1984; lars and irregulars are both computed by rules (Chom- Squire et al., 1993; Suzuki & Amaral, 1994). The other is sky & Halle, 1968; Halle & Mohanon, 1985), with aprocedural memory system for the learning and proc- memory compressed to the minimum information nec- essing of motor, perceptual, and cognitive skills. It is essary. In another, regulars and irregulars are both com- subserved by basal ganglia circuits connected largely puted by a connectionist associative memory, with all with frontal cortex (Cohen & Squire, 1980; Gabrieli et rules eliminated (Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986; al., 1993; Heindel et al., 1989; Mishkin et al., 1984; Saint- MacWhinney & Leinbach, 1991). Recent evidence from Cyr et al., 1988; Squire et al., 1993). These parallel basal the structure, processing, and acquisition of several lan- ganglia circuits are functionally segregated;each receives guages has bolstered the memory/rule theory (Marcus projections from particular ipsilateral cortical and sub- et al., 1992; Marcus et al., 1995; Pinker, 1991; Pinker & cortical areas, and projects via the thalamus to a particu- Prince, 1988; Prasada & Pinker, 1993), although contro- lar ipsilateral frontal lobe area. Thus a “motor circuit” versy persists, with some arguing that either an associa- projects to frontal motor areas, while other circuits proj- tive memory, or a set of rules, suffices (Hare & Elman, ect to other frontal areas. The different circuits have 1995; Ling & Marinov, 1993; Plunkett & Marchman, similar synaptic organizations within the basal ganglia 1993). It is therefore important to confirm the mem- (Middleton & Strick, 1994; Alexander et al., 1990;Young ory/rule distinction in the form of a neural dissociation, tk Penney, 1993). and to use it to illuminate the functions of the major Given that word forms are like facts in being arbitrary, brain systems underlying language and their relation to and in possibly having storage sites in temporal and overall brain organization. parietal regions, the temporal-parietavmedial-temporal If indeed (a) irregulars are stored words, (b) the tem- declarative memory system may subserve words as well poral-parietal/medial-temporaldeclarative system under- as facts and events. Given that rules are like skills in lies word memory, (c) regulars are rule-products,and (d) requiring the coordination of procedures in real time, the frontal/basal-ganglia procedural system underlies the and in possibly having neural loci in frontal regions, the processing of rules, then the following double dissocia- frontal/basal-ganglia procedural system may process tions are predicted. Patients with impairments of lexical grammatical rules as well as motor and perceptual skills. memory, or more generally, of declarative memory, from Basal gangha circuitry may project to Broca’s area damage to temporal or parietal
Recommended publications
  • Behavioral Neuroscience Uab Graduate Handbook
    Behavioral Neuroscience Ph.D. Program Policies, Guidelines, & Procedures Student Handbook 2021-2022 University of Alabama at Birmingham Table of Contents Mission Statement __________________________________________ 3 History of the Program _______________________________________ 3 Policies and Procedures ______________________________________ 4 Overview of Student Career ___________________________________ 5 Typical Courses ____________________________________________ 5 Progress Reports ___________________________________________ 6 2nd Year Research Project __________________________________ 7 Qualifying Examination _______________________________________ 8 Dissertation ________________________________________________ 10 Behavioral Neuroscience Student Checklist _______________________ 13 Master’s Degree ____________________________________________ 15 Policies Regarding Adequate Progress __________________________ 16 Policies on Remunerated Activities _____________________________ 16 Vacation, Leave, Holiday Guidelines ____________________________ 17 Degree Requirements and Associated Procedures ________________ 18 2 BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE PROGRAM Mission Statement and History of the Program Mission Statement Behavioral neuroscience is represented by scientists with interests in the physiological and neural substrates of behavior. The mission of the Behavioral Neuroscience Ph.D. program is to produce outstanding young scientists capable of pursuing independent research careers in the field of behavioral neuroscience by providing graduate
    [Show full text]
  • Joan Y. Chiao
    JOAN Y. CHIAO Department of Psychology Northwestern University 2029 Sheridan Rd, Evanston, IL 60208 Email: [email protected] Phone: 847.467.0481 ACADEMIC POSITIONS 2006-present Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University Affiliated Faculty, Neuroscience Institute (NUIN) Affiliated Faculty, Cognitive Science Program Affiliated Faculty, Asian American Studies Program Affiliated Faculty, Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease Center Affiliated Faculty, Multidisciplinary Program in Educational Science Affiliated Faculty, Technology and Social Behavior Program Faculty Associate, Institute for Policy Research EDUCATION Ph.D. Harvard University in Psychology, 2001-2006 B.S. Stanford University in Symbolic Systems with Honors, 1996-2000 RESEARCH INTERESTS • Social affective and cultural neuroscience • Race and its influence on perception, cognition and emotion • Psychological and neural mechanisms underlying social status and dominance • Integration of psychology and neuroscience research to public policy and population health issues HONORS, AWARDS and FELLOWSHIPS 2011 Association for Psychological Science Rising Star Award 2011 NIMH Early Career International Travel Award 2006-07 Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Research Fellow 2003-06 National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship 2005 John Merck Summer Institute: Biology of Developmental Disabilities Allport Travel Funds, Harvard George W. Goethals Teaching Prizes, Harvard Cognitive Neuroscience Society Graduate Student Presenter Award
    [Show full text]
  • Social Neuroscience and Psychopathology: Identifying the Relationship Between Neural Function, Social Cognition, and Social Beha
    Hooker, C.I. (2014). Social Neuroscience and Psychopathology, Chapter to appear in Social Neuroscience: Mind, Brain, and Society Social Neuroscience and Psychopathology: Identifying the relationship between neural function, social cognition, and social behavior Christine I. Hooker, Ph.D. ***************************** Learning Goals: 1. Identify main categories of social and emotional processing and primary neural regions supporting each process. 2. Identify main methodological challenges of research on the neural basis of social behavior in psychopathology and strategies for addressing these challenges. 3. Identify how research in the three social processes discussed in detail – social learning, self-regulation, and theory of mind – inform our understanding of psychopathology. Summary Points: 1. Several psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia and autism, are characterized by social functioning deficits, but there are few interventions that effectively address social problems. 2. Treatment development is hindered by research challenges that limit knowledge about the neural systems that support social behavior, how those neural systems and associated social behaviors are compromised in psychopathology, and how the social environment influences neural function, social behavior, and symptoms of psychopathology. 3. These research challenges can be addressed by tailoring experimental design to optimize sensitivity of both neural and social measures as well as reduce confounds associated with psychopathology. 4. Investigations on the neural mechanisms of social learning, self-regulation, and Theory of Mind provide examples of methodological approaches that can inform our understanding of psychopathology. 5. High-levels of neuroticism, which is a vulnerability for anxiety disorders, is related to hypersensitivity of the amygdala during social fear learning. 6. High-levels of social anhedonia, which is a vulnerability for schizophrenia- spectrum disorders, is related to reduced lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) activity 1 Hooker, C.I.
    [Show full text]
  • Dissociating Hippocampal Versus Basal Ganglia Contributions to Learning and Transfer
    Dissociating Hippocampal versus Basal Ganglia Contributions to Learning and Transfer Catherine E. Myers1, Daphna Shohamy1, Mark A. Gluck1, Steven Grossman1, Alan Kluger2, Steven Ferris3, James Golomb3, 4 5 Geoffrey Schnirman , and Ronald Schwartz Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/15/2/185/1757757/089892903321208123.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract & Based on prior animal and computational models, we Parkinson’s disease, and healthy controls, using an ‘‘acquired propose a double dissociation between the associative learning equivalence’’ associative learning task. As predicted, Parkin- deficits observed in patients with medial temporal (hippo- son’s patients were slower on the initial learning but then campal) damage versus patients with Parkinson’s disease (basal transferred well, while the hippocampal atrophy group showed ganglia dysfunction). Specifically, we expect that basal ganglia the opposite pattern: good initial learning with impaired dysfunction may result in slowed learning, while individuals transfer. To our knowledge, this is the first time that a single with hippocampal damage may learn at normal speed. task has been used to demonstrate a double dissociation However, when challenged with a transfer task where between the associative learning impairments caused by previously learned information is presented in novel recombi- hippocampal versus basal ganglia damage/dysfunction. This nations, we expect that hippocampal damage will impair finding has implications for understanding the distinct
    [Show full text]
  • Social Cognitive Neuroscience: a Review of Core Systems
    C HAPTER 2 2 SOCIAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE: A REVIEW OF CORE SYSTEMS Bruce P. Doré, Noam Zerubavel, and Kevin N. Ochsner Descartes famously argued that the mind is both SOCIAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE everlasting and indivisible (Descartes, 1988). If he APPROACH was right about the first part, he is probably pretty In the past decade, the field of social cognitive neuro- impressed with the advance of human knowledge science (SCN) has attempted to fill this gap, integrat- on the second. Although Descartes’ position on ing the theories and methods of two parent the indivisibility of the mind has been echoed at disciplines: social psychology and cognitive neurosci- times in the history of psychology and neurosci- ence. Stressing the interdependence of brain, mind, ence (Flourens & Meigs, 1846; Lashley, 1929; and social context, SCN seeks to explain psychological Uttal, 2003), the modern field has made steady phenomena at three levels of analysis: the neural level progress in demonstrating that subjective mental of brain systems, the cognitive level of information life can be understood as the product of distinct processing mechanisms, and the social level of the functional systems. Today, largely because of the experiences and actions of social agents (Ochsner & success of cognitive neuroscience models, Lieberman, 2001). In contrast to scientific approaches researchers understand that people’s intellectual that grant near exclusive focus to a single level of anal- faculties emerge from the operation of core ysis (e.g., behaviorism, artificial
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Neuroscience and Psychopharmacology
    Behavioral Neuroscience and Psychopharmacology The Behavioral Neuroscience and Psychopharmacology area of concentration is designed to train students broadly in the general theoretical principles and technical approaches used to investigate the neurobehavioral mechanisms of alcohol and drug abuse. Psychopharmacological approaches to understanding basic principles of learning are also emphasized. Students receive a concentrated lab experience using either animal models (quail, mice or rats) or human subjects. Faculty in the program use different levels of analysis including cell culture models, neurochemical assays, developmental toxicology, classical conditioning of drug effects, operant conditioning, human behavioral pharmacology, and cognitive approaches to behavior. Students are expected to receive in depth training in at least one level of analysis although training that integrates more than one level of analysis is strongly encouraged. PROGRAM REQUIREMENTS COURSE REQUIREMENTS (1) Statistics sequence: PSY 610 - Experimental design PSY 611 - Correlational design (2) PSY 780 - Directed BNP studies PSY 780A - first year students only PSY 780B - first year and beyond (3) Any three proseminars selected from the following areas: Learning Cognitive processes Developmental Psychology Sensation & Perception Physiological Psychology (4) Four electives (a minimum of one of these must be outside of the Psychology Department) (see listing of some possible options on page **). (5) Additional course work as recommended by the advisory committee RESEARCH REQUIREMENTS It is expected that all students in the BNP area will be involved in research thorough out their course of study towards the Ph.D. The area has two formalized requirements that are designed to train the student in conducting research: (1) Master's thesis (2) Dissertation ALLIED AREA REQUIREMENT Each student is expected to develop an allied area to gain expertise in some area outside of the student's main research specialty.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive & Behavioral Neuroscience, B.A
    COGNITIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE, B.A. 2019-2020 MAJOR REQUIREMENTS (68 UNITS) Grades of “C” or better required. FALL WINTER SPRING Required Courses (44 units) NEUR 106 Integrated Foundations of Neuroscience (4), and NEUR 106L Laboratory (1) _______ _______ _______ NEUR 219 Psychology Colloquium (0.5-2 units) 0.5 units must be taken for each year of residence for a maximum of 2 units _______ _______ _______ NEUR 261 Behavioral Neuroscience (4), and NEUR 261L Laboratory (1) Prerequisite BIOL 111, 111L or NEUR 106, 106L; and PSYC 104 _______ _______ _______ NEUR 275 Research Techniques in Neuroscience (4) Prerequisite: NEUR 261 & NEUR 261L _______ _______ _______ PSYC 104 General Psychology (4), and PSYC 104L Laboratory (1) Social Sciences* _____ _____ _____ _____ PSYC 319 Career Colloquium (1) _______ _______ _______ PSYC 321 Methods and Statistics I: Description & Correlation (4), and PSYC 321L Laboratory (0) _______ _______ _______ PSYC 322 Methods and Statistics II: Experiment & Inference (4), and NEUR 322L Laboratory (1) _______ _______ _______ PSYC 323 Methods and Statistics III: Conducting Research (4), and NEUR 323L Laboratory (1) Satisfies Upper Division Rhetoric requirement _______ _______ _______ PSYC 374 Cognitive Psychology (4), and PSYC 374L Laboratory (0) _______ _______ _______ PSYC 435 Learning & Behavior (4), and PSYC 435L Laboratory (0) _______ _______ _______ _______ _______ _______ Plus 16 units from one of the two major emphases: Clinical Emphasis: Cognitive & Computational Emphasis: _______ _______ _______ NEUR
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Systems of the Brain
    Memory systems of the brain Alvaro Pastor Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Computer Science, Multimedia and Telecommunications Department, Barcelona, Spain [email protected] January 1, 2020 Abstract Humans have long been fascinated by how memories are formed, how they can be damaged or lost, or still seem vibrant after many years. Thus the search for the locus and organization of memory has had a long history, in which the notion that is is composed of distinct systems developed during the second half of the 20th century. A fundamental dichotomy between conscious and unconscious memory processes was first drawn based on evidences from the study of amnesiac subjects and the systematic experimental work with animals. The use of behavioral and neural measures together with imaging techniques have progressively led researchers to agree in the existence of a variety of neural architectures that support multiple memory systems. This article presents a historical lens with which to contextualize these idea on memory systems, and provides a current account for the multiple memory systems model. Introduction Memory is a quintessential human ability by means of which the nervous system can encode, store, and retrieve a variety of information [1, 2]. It affords the foundation for the adaptive nature of the organism, allowing to use previous experience and make predictions in order to solve the multitude of environmental situations produced by daily interaction. Not only memory allows to recognize familiarity and return to places, but also richly imagine potential future circumstances and assess the consequences of behavior. Therefore, present experience is inexorably interwoven with memories, and the meaning of people, things, and events in the present depends largely on previous experience.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroeconomics: How Neuroscience Can Inform Economics
    mr05_Article 1 3/28/05 3:25 PM Page 9 Journal of Economic Literature Vol. XLIII (March 2005), pp. 9–64 Neuroeconomics: How Neuroscience Can Inform Economics ∗ COLIN CAMERER, GEORGE LOEWENSTEIN, and DRAZEN PRELEC Who knows what I want to do? Who knows what anyone wants to do? How can you be sure about something like that? Isn’t it all a question of brain chemistry, signals going back and forth, electrical energy in the cortex? How do you know whether something is really what you want to do or just some kind of nerve impulse in the brain. Some minor little activity takes place somewhere in this unimportant place in one of the brain hemispheres and suddenly I want to go to Montana or I don’t want to go to Montana. (White Noise, Don DeLillo) 1. Introduction such as finance, game theory, labor econom- ics, public finance, law, and macroeconomics In the last two decades, following almost a (see Colin Camerer and George Loewenstein century of separation, economics has begun 2004). Behavioral economics has mostly been to import insights from psychology. informed by a branch of psychology called “Behavioral economics” is now a prominent “behavioral decision research,” but other fixture on the intellectual landscape and has cognitive sciences are ripe for harvest. Some spawned applications to topics in economics, important insights will surely come from neu- roscience, either directly or because neuro- ∗ Camerer: California Institute of Technology. science will reshape what is believed about Loewenstein: Carnegie Mellon University. Prelec: psychology which in turn informs economics. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
    [Show full text]
  • The Continuity of Context: a Role for the Hippocampus A.P. Maurer* and L. Nadel^ *University of Florida ^University of Arizona
    The Continuity of Context: A Role for the Hippocampus A.P. Maurer* and L. Nadel^ *University of Florida ^University of Arizona 1 Abstract: Tracking moment-to-moment change in input, and detecting change sufficient to require altering behavior is crucial to survival. We discuss how the brain evaluates change over time, focusing on hippocampus and its role in tracking context. We leverage the anatomy and physiology of the hippocampal longitudinal axis, re-entrant loops, and amorphous networks, to account for stimulus equivalence and the updating of an organism’s sense of it’s context. Place cells play a central role in tracking contextual continuities and discontinuities across multiple scales, a capacity beyond current models of pattern separation and completion. This perspective highlights the critical role of the hippocampus in both spatial cognition and episodic memory: tracking change and detecting boundaries separating one context, or episode, from another. 2 Introduction The problem of how the brain can produce perceptual equivalences across an indefinite number of input combinations has perplexed scholars for decades (1-5). The stimulus equivalence problem led Lashley (2), and subsequently Hebb (3), to argue against linear Stimulus-Response formulations of brain and behavior. An example of equivalence at the perceptual level is the ‘reafference’ from action systems to the visual system that negates the shift in retinal location by ‘predicting’ where an object should fall on the retina given the organism’s current movements (including eye, head and full body movements) (6-9). Retinal location invariance allows us to maintain accurate knowledge of the location of immobile objects in the external world as we move through space and time.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: a Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential
    W&M ScholarWorks Arts & Sciences Book Chapters Arts and Sciences 2007 Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: A Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential. Bruce D. Bartholow Cheryl L. Dickter College of William and Mary, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wm.edu/asbookchapters Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons Recommended Citation Bartholow, B. D., & Dickter, C. L. (2007). Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: A Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential.. E. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Ed.), Social neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior (pp. 376-400). New York, NY: Guilford Press. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/asbookchapters/46 This Book Chapter is brought to you for free and open access by the Arts and Sciences at W&M ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Arts & Sciences Book Chapters by an authorized administrator of W&M ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. V PERSON PERCEPTION, STEREOTYPING, AND PREJUDICE Copyright © ${Date}. ${Publisher}. All rights reserved. © ${Date}. ${Publisher}. Copyright 17 Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception A SELECTIVE REVIEW FOCUSED ON THE EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIAL Bruce D. Bartholow and Cheryl L. Dickter Although social psychologists have long been interested in understanding the cognitive processes underlying social phenomena (e.g., Markus & Zajonc, 1985), their methods for studying them traditionally have been rather limited. Early research in person perception, like most other areas of social-psychological inquiry, relied primarily on verbal reports. Researchers using this approach have devised clever experimental designs to ensure the validity of their conclusions concerning social behavior (see Reis & Judd, 2000).
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae
    CURRICULUM VITAE F. Gregory Ashby May 28, 2018 Office: Psychology East 3847 Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences University of California Santa Barbara, CA 93106 phone: 805-893-7909 fax: 805-893-4303 email: [email protected] lab web page: https://labs.psych.ucsb.edu/ashby/gregory/lab.htm Education: Ph.D., Mathematical Psychology Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, 1980 M.S., Psychology Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, 1976 B.S., Mathematics and Psychology University of Puget Sound, Tacoma, Washington, 1975 Professional Experience: 2013-2015 Chair, Interdepartmental Graduate Program in Dynamical Neuroscience, UC Santa Barbara 2013- Distinguished Professor of Psychological & Brain Sciences (i.e., Professor Above Scale), UC Santa Barbara 2009-2012 Chair, Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, UC Santa Barbara 2004-2006 Director, UCSB Brain Imaging Center 1994-2013 Professor of Psychology, UC Santa Barbara 1988-1994 Associate Professor of Psychology, UC Santa Barbara 1986-1988 Assistant Professor of Psychology, UC Santa Barbara 1980-1986 Assistant Professor of Psychology, Ohio State University 1981-1982 National Science Foundation Postdoctoral Fellow, Sponsored by W. K. Estes, Harvard University 1975-1980 Teaching Assistant to Ben J. Winer, Purdue University Ashby Vitae Page 2 Research Interests: My research combines cognitive neuroscience, cognitive psychology, computational modeling, and empirical data collection to study the basic mechanisms of human learning, from initial acquisition through automaticity. The goals are to understand the underlying perceptual, cognitive, and neural processes, and to build and test neurobiologically detailed computational models. My lab conducts behavioral and neuroimaging (i.e., fMRI) studies of healthy adults. Most experiments study category learning because categorization affords excellent observability of decision processes.
    [Show full text]