Essay Deutsch D

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Essay Deutsch D 1 Essay Deutsch D National Hearing Conservation Association Annual Conference , 2003, February, Dallas. Invited lecture. Invited Lay language paper presented at the 156th meeting of the Acoustical Society of America. Absolute pitch is associated with a large auditory digit span A clue to its genesis. Mozart Festival, Kennedy Center for the Performing Arts , 1998, Washington, D. 1983, November. Schiffman, Sensation and Perception. Heller and W. , and Dooley, K. Auditory Perception, Cognition and Action Meeting , 2002, November, Kansas City. Stereo TV, Invited presentation. Marks, The Unity of the Senses. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1996, 99, 2482,. The pitch levels of female speech in two Chinese villages. , Sensation and Perception. Physics Today , 2010, February, 40-45,. Ear Club , 2004, May, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley. Invited Lay language paper presented at the 148th meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, San Diego, November,. New York Oxford University Press. Mozart Festival, Kennedy Center for the Performing Arts , 1998, Washington, D. Basic Structure and Function in the Central Nervous System , 1974, New York MacMillan Co. Representation of pitch and pitch combinations. Invited Lay Language paper presented at the 149th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America. and Deutsch, D. Musical space. Two issues at the interface between speech and music. Paper presented at the. Winter Conference on Brain Research , 1986, Keystone. International conference on Psychology and the Arts , 1983, Cardiff, Wales. Psychonomic Society Meeting , 1984, El Paso. Encyclopedia of Perception , 2009, 1, 160-164, Sage. Invited Presentation, Annual meeting of the. , Psychological Foundations of Musical Behavior. , and Dooley, K. 2004, November, San Diego. Generalizzione dell ottava e ritenzione dell informazione melodica. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1974, 56, s25,. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1974, 26, 229-234,. Royal Society Discussion Meeting on Auditory Processing of Complex Sounds , 1991, Royal Society, London. McFadden Ed. Some new pitch paradoxes and their implications. Illusions in sound and music. , and Deutsch, D. In- ternational Library of Critical Writings in Psychology , 1993, Aldershot Edward Elgar Publishing. The Tritone Paradox and the Pitch Range of the Speaking Voice Reply to Repp. Hearing music in ensembles. Auditory illusions, handedness, and the spatial environment. Frontiers of Bioscience special issue , 2007, 12, 4473-4482,. Presented to the Society of Experimental Psychologists , 2003. The processing of pitch combinations. , 2002, 47, 410-412. Colloquium presentation. The Oxford Companion to the Mind , 1987, 61-63. Kubovy and J. Paradoxien der Tonhohenwahrnehmung. Bridgeman Eds. The tritone paradox Its presence and form of distribution in a general population. Absolute pitch among students in an American music conservatory Association with tone language fluency. A new pitch paradox. Auditory pattern recognition. Keynote panelist. JASA , 2021, April, 149, 2829,. Heller and W. Handbook of Perception and Action, Vol 1 , 1996, 1, 253-296, Academic Press. Knudsen Distinguished Lecture presented at the 161th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1979, 65, s92,. Foreward to R. International Association of Empirical Aesthetics , 2008, August, Chicago. , 2005, 1, 307-341, Gottingen Hogrefe. A new pitch circularity illusion. Musical Intelligence. Tone language and absolute pitch Prevalence among American and Chinese conservatory students. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 2009, April, 125, 2683, Portland. Invited Address. Music and the Cognitive Sciences, Contemporary Music Review , 1989, Harwood Academic Publishers, London. Perception Psychophysics , 1972, 11, 411-412,. MIT Learning Workshop. The paradox of pitch circularity. 0 now from the Chrome Web Store. Reuben Van Fleet Theater Society for Amateur Scientists , 2001, November, San Diego. Workshop on Physical and Neuropsychological Foundations of Music , 1983, Ossiach, Austria. Royal Swedish Academy of Music , 1981, Stockholm, Sweden. International Conference on Psychology and the Arts , 1983, Cardiff, Wales. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1984, 76, s77,. Grouping mechanisms in music. Dichotic listening to musical sequences Relationship to hemispheric specialization of function. Music perception. , and Sereno, M. Deutsch, D, Jiang, C. Deliege Eds. Audio Magazine , 1987, 36-48. Underwood Ed. Special session organizer. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1983,. Nature , 1978, 274, 721,. Contemporary Psychology APA REVIEW OF BOOKS. Absolute pitch among students at the Shanghai Conservatory of Music A large-scale direct-test study. , The Psychology of Attention. , The Psychology of Attention. Science , 1970, 168, 1604-1605,. Attention Some theoretical considerations. The phenomenology of musical hallucinations. Selected Invited Presentations. Mind, Life, Universe Conversations with great scientists of our time , 2007, 241-248, Chelsea Green Publishing Company, White River Junction, VT. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1983, 73, 1865-1867,. On the opposite end of the spectrum are those papers written in German from my early undergraduate years -- please recall that I d only had three years of highschool German when I started writing those, so forgive any horrendous errors. Influence of relational context on pitch recognition as a function of retention interval. Psychology and Music. Journal of Shenghai Conservatory of Music , 2016, 4, 133-145,. Perception and Psychophysics , 1978, 24, 7-10,. Ear dominance depends on sequential, frequency-specific interactions between tones at the two ears. Music Recognition. Vietnamese translation of Speaking in tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1978, 64, s146,. Short Term Memory , 1975, New York Academic Press. Invited Presentation. 2012, October, Kansas City. Response to Auditory Perception in Music Teaching and Learning by J. Grouping mechanisms in music. Completing the CAPTCHA proves you are a human and gives you temporary access to the web property. Absolute pitch, speech, and tone language Some experiments and a proposed framework. Review of Music and Memory An Introduction , by Bob Snyder. In Science and the Arts. Deutsch and J. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1978, 63, s31,. Workshop on Drugs and Cognition. Music, Mind and Brain , 1982, 119-136, Plenum, New York. Gathering for Gardner 6 g4g6 , 2004, March, Atlanta. Pitch proximity in the grouping of simultaneous tones. Handedness and memory for tonal pitch. The psychology of music, 3rd Edition , 2013, 183-248, San Diego Elsevier. AAAS Annual Meeting , 1997, Seattle. The Psychology of Music, Ist Edition , Volume I, Japanese translation. International Library of Critical Writings in Psychology , 1993, Aldershot Edward Elgar Publishing. Octave equivalence in the processing of tonal sequences. The paradox of pitch circularity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience , 2008, 1,. Special Issue of the Journal of the Audio Engineering Society , 1983,. Sensory Perception Laboratory Manual. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 2002, 11, 200-204,. Invited Presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance , 2004, 30, 355-364,. Language experience influences nonlinguistic pitch perception. Processi cognitivi in musica , 1986, 133-163, Milano, Franco Angeli Libri. Invited tutorial presentation. Some new pitch paradoxes and their implications. Journal of Acoustical Society of America , 2004, 116, 2580,. 1987, Nishimura Co. Psychonomic Society Meeting , 1983, San Diego. Reisberg Ed. Journal of the Acoustical Society of 2 America , 1989, 86, S17,. 72nd Audio Engineering Society Convention , 1982, Anaheim. Campbell and C. On-line perception of Mandarin Tones 2 and 3 Evidence from eye movements. Sound and Science Symposium , 2009, March, University of Califor- nia, Los Angeles. Computer evaluation of musical performance from the acoustic signal An exploratory study on performance anxiety. Narmour and R. Cognitive Processes in the Perception of Art , 1984, North Holland. Invited speaker. Bornstein Ed. La perception des structures musicales. Memory for auditory information. Mapping of Interactions in the Pitch Memory Store. Invited Presentation, Acoustical Society of America. A new pitch circularity illusion. , and Henthorn, T. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 2008, November, 124, 2471,. Perception Psychophysics , 1983, 34, 331-337,. Invited Presentation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 2010, 128, 890-893,. If you are on a personal connection, like at home, you can run an anti-virus scan on your device to make sure it is not infected with malware. Review of Coren, S. Octave generalization and melody identification. , Griffiths, T. , Los Angeles. Scientific American Mind, 2010, July August, 36-43. Selected Conference Presentations. Encyclopedia of Psychology, Vol 1. Proceedings of the First International Conference on Music Perception and Cognit , 1989, 107-112, Kyoto, Japan. and Dolson, M. , Lenguaje musical y humano. 72nd Audio Engineering Society Convention , 1982, Anaheim. Auditory illusions and the brain. Memory and attention to music. This week s citation classic. Third Workshop on the Physical and Neuropsychological Foundations of Music , 1980, Ossiach, Austria. Recognition of durations embedded in temporal patterns.
Recommended publications
  • Absolute Pitch (AP)
    Absolute Pitch (AP) • A.k.a. ‘perfect pitch’ • The ability to name or produce a tone without a reference tone • Very rare: 1 in 10,000 Vs. Relative pitch (RP) • Most people use relative pitch: • Recognizing tones relative to other tones • Remember and produce intervals abstracted from specific pitch, or given a reference pitch AP: how it works • Thought to be a labeling process: – AP possessors associate names/ meaning with pitches or pitch classes – Retain this association over time • AP is not ‘perfect’; i.e., auditory perception/ pitch discrimination not more accurate than RP Imaging evidence • When making judgments using AP: • possessors compared to non- possessors show more activation in frontal naming/labeling areas • Anatomically, AP possessors show greater planum temporale asymmetry – Apparently due to reduced RH PT size AP ‘flavors’ • AP not purely ‘have’ or ‘have-not; ability level varies along continuum • Some possessors make more accurate judgments with certain instruments – e.g. piano vs. pure sine wave tones – Sometimes called ‘absolute piano’ AP ‘flavors’ cont’d • Other possessors may perform more accurately with white-key notes than black-key notes – E.g. C,D,E vs. C#, D# • May be due to early learning influence – Early musical training on keyboard usually starts with white-key notes only • So, is AP learned? Learnable? Nature vs. Nurture, of course • The debate continues: – Some researchers ascribe genetic origins to AP, suspecting that early musical training is neither sufficient nor necessary – Others find most possessors
    [Show full text]
  • Classical Net Review
    The Internet's Premier Classical Music Source BOOK REVIEW The Psychology of Music Diana Deutsch, editor Academic Press, Third Edition, 2013, pp xvii + 765 ISBN-10: 012381460X ISBN-13: 978-0123814609 The psychology of music was first explored in detail in modern times in a book of that name by Carl E. Seashore… Psychology Of Music was published in 1919. Dover's paperback edition of almost 450 pages (ISBN- 10: 0486218511; ISBN-13: 978-0486218519) is still in print from half a century later (1967) and remains a good starting point for those wishing to understand the relationship between our minds and music, chiefly as a series of physical processes. From the last quarter of the twentieth century onwards much research and many theories have changed the models we have of the mind when listening to or playing music. Changes in music itself, of course, have dictated that the nature of human interaction with it has grown. Unsurprisingly, books covering the subject have proliferated too. These range from examinations of how memory affects our experience of music through various forms of mental disabilities, therapies and deviations from "standard" auditory reception, to attempts to explain music appreciation psychologically. Donald Hodges' and David Conrad Sebald's Music in the Human Experience: An Introduction to Music Psychology (ISBN-10: 0415881862; ISBN-13: 978- 0415881869) makes a good introduction to the subject; while Aniruddh Patel's Music, Language, and the Brain (ISBN-10: 0199755302; ISBN-13: 978-0199755301) is a good (and now classic/reference) overview. Oliver Sacks' Musicophilia: Tales of Music and the Brain (ISBN-10: 1400033535; ISBN-13: 978-1400033539) examines specific areas from a clinical perspective.
    [Show full text]
  • Convergent Evolution in a Large Cross-Cultural Database of Musical Scales
    Convergent evolution in a large cross-cultural database of musical scales John M. McBride1,* and Tsvi Tlusty1,2,* 1Center for Soft and Living Matter, Institute for Basic Science, Ulsan 44919, South Korea 2Departments of Physics and Chemistry, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Ulsan 44919, South Korea *[email protected], [email protected] August 3, 2021 Abstract We begin by clarifying some key terms and ideas. We first define a scale as a sequence of notes (Figure 1A). Scales, sets of discrete pitches used to generate Notes are pitch categories described by a single pitch, melodies, are thought to be one of the most uni- although in practice pitch is variable so a better descrip- versal features of music. Despite this, we know tion is that notes are regions of semi-stable pitch centered relatively little about how cross-cultural diversity, around a representative (e.g., mean, meadian) frequency or how scales have evolved. We remedy this, in [10]. Thus, a scale can also be thought of as a sequence of part, we assemble a cross-cultural database of em- mean frequencies of pitch categories. However, humans pirical scale data, collected over the past century process relative frequency much better than absolute fre- by various ethnomusicologists. We provide sta- quency, such that a scale is better described by the fre- tistical analyses to highlight that certain intervals quency of notes relative to some standard; this is typically (e.g., the octave) are used frequently across cul- taken to be the first note of the scale, which is called the tures.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians Sarah E
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 2013 Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians Sarah E. Weber University of Massachusetts Amherst Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/theses Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons, Fine Arts Commons, Music Education Commons, and the Music Theory Commons Weber, Sarah E., "Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians" (2013). Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014. 1162. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.umass.edu/theses/1162 This thesis is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians A Thesis Presented by SARAH WEBER Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts Amherst in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF MUSIC September 2013 Music Theory © Copyright by Sarah E. Weber 2013 All Rights Reserved Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians A Thesis Presented by SARAH WEBER _____________________________ Gary S. Karpinski, Chair _____________________________ Andrew Cohen, Member _____________________________ Brent Auerbach, Member _____________________________ Jeff Cox, Department Head Department of Music and Dance DEDICATION For my parents and Grandma. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Kristen Wallentinsen for her help with experimental logistics, Renée Morgan for giving me her speakers, and Nathaniel Liberty for his unwavering support, problem-solving skills, and voice-over help.
    [Show full text]
  • Major Heading
    THE APPLICATION OF ILLUSIONS AND PSYCHOACOUSTICS TO SMALL LOUDSPEAKER CONFIGURATIONS RONALD M. AARTS Philips Research Europe, HTC 36 (WO 02) Eindhoven, The Netherlands An overview of some auditory illusions is given, two of which will be considered in more detail for the application of small loudspeaker configurations. The requirements for a good sound reproduction system generally conflict with those of consumer products regarding both size and price. A possible solution lies in enhancing listener perception and reproduction of sound by exploiting a combination of psychoacoustics, loudspeaker configurations and digital signal processing. The first example is based on the missing fundamental concept, the second on the combination of frequency mapping and a special driver. INTRODUCTION applications of even smaller size this lower limit can A brief overview of some auditory illusions is given easily be as high as several hundred hertz. The bass which serves merely as a ‘catalogue’, rather than a portion of an audio signal contributes significantly to lengthy discussion. A related topic to auditory illusions the sound ‘impact’, and depending on the bass quality, is the interaction between different sensory modalities, the overall sound quality will shift up or down. e.g. sound and vision, a famous example is the Therefore a good low-frequency reproduction is McGurk effect (‘Hearing lips and seeing voices’) [1]. essential. An auditory-visual overview is given in [2], a more general multisensory product perception in [3], and on ILLUSIONS spatial orientation in [4]. The influence of video quality An illusion is a distortion of a sensory perception, on perceived audio quality is discussed in [5].
    [Show full text]
  • Speaking in Tones Music and Language Are Partners in the Brain
    Speaking in Tones Music and language are partners in the brain. Our sense of song helps us learn to talk, read and even make friends By Diana Deutsch 36 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MIND July/August 2010 © 2010 Scientific American ne afternoon in the summer of opera resembling sung ordinary speech), the cries 1995, a curious incident occurred. of street vendors and some rap music. I was fi ne-tuning my spoken com- And yet for decades the experience of musicians mentary on a CD I was preparing and the casual observer has clashed with scientifi c ) about music and the brain. To de- opinion, which has held that separate areas of the music tect glitches in the recording, I was looping phrases brain govern speech and music. Psychologists, lin- O so that I could hear them over and over. At one point, guists and neuroscientists have recently changed their sheet ( when I was alone in the room, I put one of the phras- tune, however, as sophisticated neuroimaging tech- es, “sometimes behave so strangely,” on a loop, be- niques have helped amass evidence that the brain ar- gan working on something else and forgot about it. eas governing music and language overlap. The latest iStockphoto Suddenly it seemed to me that a strange woman was data show that the two are in fact so intertwined that singing! After glancing around and fi nding nobody an awareness of music is critical to a baby’s language there, I realized that I was hearing my own voice re- development and even helps to cement the bond be- petitively producing this phrase—but now, instead tween infant and mother.
    [Show full text]
  • The Future of Theory
    The Future of Theory Editor's Introduction. Graduate students ask questions. They seek a place in a field with which they are coming to terms. Graduate students connect their familiar extra-theoretical worlds with their newly-chosen discipline. They develop a collegial unity not only from common interests but also through shared situations and the transitional aspect of their circumstance. And graduate students are willing to take risks, to try out new territory without knowing what lies out there. The following article came about from the ideas of graduate students in music theory at Indiana University. Current topics at our noon-time forums (brown-bag lunches in the shared office) in January 1990 included popular predictions, not only for the new year but also for the new decade and impending new century. It was a logical step to wonder about the forecast for music theory. We decided to take our musings beyond our graduate student life and ask our more experienced colleagues in the field for their forecasts. Our list was compiled of prominent theorists known for their publications and reflecting a range of ages and interests. (Our own excellent theory faculty was excluded from the list to increase the available space. Those faculty members consulted during the planning of this project approved of our decision.) The Editorial Board decided to ask more rather than fewer theorists as we did not know how many would have time or inclination to respond. The letter sent to these theorists requested their opinions of the "future and course of music theory." Respondents were told not to be limited to that subject if other topics were on their minds.
    [Show full text]
  • City Research Online
    City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Cross, I. (1989). The cognitive organisation of musical pitch. (Unpublished Doctoral thesis, City University London) This is the accepted version of the paper. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link: https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/eprint/7663/ Link to published version: Copyright: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. Reuse: Copies of full items can be used for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge. Provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ [email protected] The Cognitive Organisation of Musical Pitch A dissertation submitted in fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in The City University, London by Ian Cross September1989 ABSTRACT This thesis takes as its initial Premise the idea that the rationales for the forms of pitch organisation employed within tonal music which have been adopted by music theorists have strongly affected those theorists` conceptions of music, and that it is of critical importance to music theory to investigate the potential origination of such rationales within the human sciences.
    [Show full text]
  • The Octave Illusion and Auditory Perceptual Integration
    The Octave Illusion and Auditory Perceptual Integration DIANA DEUTSCH University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California I. Introduction . 1 II. The Octave Illusion . 2 A. The Basic Effect . 2 B. Handedness Correlates . 4 C. Further Complexities: Ears or Auditory Space? . 6 D. Dependence of the Illusion on Sequential Interactions . 6 III. Parametric Studies of Ear Dominance . 7 A. Apparatus . 7 B. Experiment 1 . 7 C. Experiment 2 . 9 D. Experiment 3 . 10 E. Experiment 4 . .11 F. Hypothesized Basis for Ear Dominance . 13 G. Discussion . 13 IV. Parametric Studies of Lateralization by Frequency . .15 A. Experiment 1 . 15 B. Experiment 2. 16 C. Experiment 3 . 16 D. Experiment 4 . 16 E. Discussion . 18 V. The What–Where Connection . 18 Discussion . 19 VI. Conclusion . 19 References . 20 I. INTRODUCTION hemisphere damage to Brodmann’s areas 39 and 40 has been found to produce deficits in visual perceptual clas- A philosophical doctrine stemming from the empiri- sification (Warrington and Taylor, 1973). Further, vari- cists of the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries is that ous studies on human and subhuman species point to objects manifest themselves simply as bundles of attrib- an anatomical separation between the pathways medi- ute values. This doctrine has had a profound influence ating pattern discrimination on the one hand and local- on the thinking of sensory psychologists and neurophys- ization on the other (Ingle et al., 1967-1968). For exam- iologists. For example, it is assumed that when we see an ple, Schneider found that ablation of visual cortex in object, we separately appreciate its color, its shape, its hamsters led to an inability to discriminate visual pat- location in the visual field, and so on.
    [Show full text]
  • Music Perception 13
    Music Perception 13 LEARNING OBJECTIVES ISLE EXERCISES 13.1 Kurdish Music Example 13.1 Explain how frequency is related to pitch, chroma, and the octave. 13.2 Javanese Gamelan Music Example 13.2 Summarize the basic neuroscience of music, including how training and experience can affect the representation of music in the brain. 13.3 Ancient Greek Music 13.4 35,000-Year-Old Flute 13.3 Discuss how learning and culture affect music perception. distribute 13.5 Is This Music? 13.6 The Octave and or Tone Similarity INTRODUCTION 13.7 Pentatonic Music 13.8 Meter and Beat Wherever you travel, you will find music. It may sound very different from the music you are accustomed to hearing, but you will recognize it instantly as music. In Kurdistan, we 13.9 Bolero Clip find a unique culture of music featuring such instrumentspost, as the tanbur (a fretted string 13.10 Attack and Decay instrument), the qernête (a double-reed wind instrument), and the şimşal (a flutelike 13.11 Examples of Melody instrument) (Figure 13.1). Although most of you may never have heard of these instru- ments and may never have heard Kurdish music before, you would instantly recognize 13.12 Types of Scales them as musical instruments, and you might even like Kurdish music (see ISLE 13.1 for 13.13 Gestalt Principles an example of Kurdish music). Review copy, © Aurora Photos/Alamy 13.14 Gestalt Principle: Proximity: Bach’s Partita No. 3 in E major not 13.15 A Shave and a Haircut 13.16 Cross-Modal Matchings as a Simulation of Synesthesia Do 13.17.
    [Show full text]
  • 1353 Syllabus Final
    Music and Language (Psychology 1353), Fall 2010 Mondays & Wednesdays, 4:00 – 5:30 pm Location: WJH 4 Website: http://my.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k72181 (please check often for updates and changes) Instructor: L. Robert (Bob) Slevc, PhD. Email: [email protected] Office hours by appointment Overview Music and language are among the skills and activities that most uniquely define us as human. As far as we know, every human culture uses some form of music and of language and has done so for a very long time: bone flutes have been discovered dating from about 40,000 years ago and singing probably emerged even earlier, along with spoken language, sometime in the Paleolithic era. In contrast, there seem to be no non-human animals that make use of music and language to anywhere near the extent that we do. This means that the processing of music and language are unlike many other impressive aspects of perception and cognition (e.g., vision or memory) in that we can learn only a limited amount from studies of other species. But processing music and language are, in a sense, similar feats–both involve the conversion of complex auditory sequences into meaningful units and structures–and so much can be learned from comparative work on these two complex abilities. Importantly, we can learn both from aspects that are shared in the processing of music and language and from aspects that are distinct. This idea that there might be a relationship between music and language is not a new one, but goes back at least as far as Plato.
    [Show full text]
  • Music Notation, but Not Action on a Keyboard, Influences
    No Effect of Action on the Tritone Paradox 315 MUSIC NOTATION , BUT NOT ACTION ON A KEYBOARD , INFLUENCES PIANISTS’ JUDGMENTS OF AMBIGUOUS MELODIES BRUNO H. REPP Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001; Proffitt, Haskins Laboratories 2006; Schütz-Bosbach & Prinz, 2007; Wilson & Knoblich, 2005). Almost all of these studies have been concerned ROBERT M. GOEHR K E with visual perception. However, Repp and Knoblich Yale University (2007, 2009) recently claimed to have found effects of action on auditory perception. PITCH INCREASES FROM LEFT TO RIGHT ON PIANO Their research was based on the fact that on a piano key- keyboards. When pianists press keys on a keyboard to board, the pitch of produced tones increases from left to hear two successive octave-ambiguous tones spanning a right. Pianists are continuously exposed to this relationship, tritone (half-octave interval), they tend to report hear- whereas other musicians and some nonmusicians merely ing the tritone go in the direction consistent with their know it but do not have sensorimotor experience with it. key presses (Repp & Knoblich, 2009). This finding has On each trial of their experiment, Repp and Knoblich been interpreted as an effect of action on perceptual (2009) showed participants a pair of two-digit numbers judgment. Using a modified design, the present study that corresponded to two keys on a labeled piano keyboard separated the effect of the action itself from that of the (a silent MIDI controller). The keys always spanned the visual stimuli that prompt the action. Twelve expert pia- musical interval of a tritone (half an octave), with the sec- nists reported their perception of octave-ambiguous ond key being either higher (i.e., to the right of) or lower three-note melodies ending with tritones in two condi- than (to the left of) the first key.
    [Show full text]