Wolf, Wolfdog, Or Dog? Phenotyping Canines!

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Wolf, Wolfdog, Or Dog? Phenotyping Canines! FLA Wolf Identification Pamphlet Www.floridalupine.org Wolf, Wolfdog, or Dog? Phenotyping Canines! FLORIDA LUPINE ASSOC IATION, INC. S P E C I A L P O I N T S O F INTEREST: What is a Wolf ? And How Do You Know? Dogs are domesticated By Kim Miles & Jody Haynes wolves. Well-bred wolfdogs can In his seminal work on animal a wolf?” Some- wolfdog from a dog ex- appear to look like pure wolfdogs, N.A. Iljin (1944) times, the answers are ob- cept through genetic test- wolves to the untrained eye because they are found that “in many cases vious; but quite often they ing, and such tests are often bred for their the wolf cannot be distin- are not. expensive and not readily “wolfy” looks. guished from the dog even FLA created this phe- available to the public. Most wolfdogs are three at a short distance and vice notyping pamphlet in re- It is important to to five generations removed from pure wolf, versa.” He further contend- sponse to the need for a note, however, that tests making them F3 to F5. ed that even though a list clear and concise educa- focused solely on mito- (The F# is the filial number, indicating how of characteristics specific to tional resource that we chondrial DNA far removed from wolf a the wolf had been created hope proves beneficial in (mtDNA) are inherently canine is.) by both Brehm (1922) and addressing these ques- biased. Because mito- Most wolfdogs are the Taenzer (1923), “there are tions. This pamphlet will chondria are inherited offspring of other wolf- none essentially differenti- not magically allow just maternally—with none of dogs, not the product of a pure captive wolf and ating the wolf from the anyone to correctly identi- the crossing over or inde- domestic dog or wolfdog. dog.” However, in the last fy all canines. Instead, it pendent assortment that The most common dog 100 years or so, dog fanci- should be utilized as a occurs in nuclear DNA breeds in wolfdogs are ers have developed strict supplemental tool for (nDNA)—all animals in a Husky, Malamute, and German Shepherd. standards each breed must those who already have given matriarchal line adhere to, allowing one to some knowledge of wolf will have the same more easily distinguish be- morphology (i.e., looks) mtDNA. Thus, mtDNA INSIDE THIS tween traits or characteris- and behavior; taken as a tests can only show ISSUE: tics more consistent with whole, it should aid these whether an animal‟s ma- wolves and those more individuals in making a triarchal line is “wolf” or What is a Wolf? And How 1 Do You Know? commonly found in certain more educated determina- “dog”; they cannot deter- dog breeds (see table on pp. tion regarding the pres- mine if an animal is a Phenotyping Canines: 5 Physical Characteristics 6-7). ence of wolf within a ca- wolfdog with both “wolf” Wolfdog rescuers, var- nine. and “dog” genes. The Chart of Physical Traits: 6 ious animal control person- only sure way to accu- Dog vs. Wolf nel, and officers in the FL DNA TESTING rately determine if an ani- Fish & Wildlife Conserva- mal is a wolf, wolfdog, or Photos: Dog & Wolf 8 tion Commission have of- dog through genetic test- Even with knowledge of Guessing Game: Wolf, ten been called to a loca- ing is to examine both 10 morphology and behavior, Wolfdog or Dog? tion and been faced with mtDNA and nDNA there is still no positively Phenotyping: How Well the questions, “Is this ani- markers—and possibly 11 conclusive way to deter- Did You Do? mal part wolf?” or “Is this even Y chromosomal mine a living wolf from a P a g e 2 Wolf, Wolfdog, or Dog? markers in males, because the Y than most dogs‟ toes. between wolf and dog behavior chromosome is inherited paternally 4. MYTH: The wolf has a precaudal in absolutes because all dog and is consistent along the patriar- scent gland on the top of the tail behavior is derived from the chal line just as mtDNA is to the near the base, identified by a dark wolf. While all wolf behaviors matriarchal line. patch of fur. The dog has no such may not be found in any given gland. FACT: Both wolves AND type of dog, all dogs can dis- MYTH VS. FACT dogs have a precaudal scent gland play various aspects of all wolf (or residual mark) with coarser behaviors to some degree. The In an effort to be as factual and com- hair marking the spot a few inches degree will vary depending up- prehensive as possible, FLA consult- from the base of the tail. Michael on the breed and the individual ed the following wildlife biologists Fox (1971) claims that it is animal. In general, dogs have and organizations to dispel some “demarked by [a] dark patch of “watered down” wolf behav- common myths about wolves and hairs.” An article ap- iors. Dogs are not only wolfdogs: Nick Federoff and Ron pearing in Dog World either friendly or aggres- Nowak, authors of numerous peer- magazine had this to sive; poorly socialized, reviewed wolf articles and creators say: “There will be a abused, and neglected of the current wolf taxonomic sys- precaudal gland, dogs can also be shy tem in use today; Monty Sloan of marked by a black and/or fearful. Accord- spot of fur, on the top ing to an article in Dog Wolf Park; and David Mech, Senior Because dogs are Research Biologist for the Depart- of the tail [in wolves], World magazine, Akita ment of the Interior and board mem- several inches from domesticated Rescue Society of Ameri- ber of the International Wolf Cen- the body, but this is wolves, their ca evaluated 64 Akitas in ter. The following are some of the also present in dogs. rescue and found that all common myths about wolves and Many people errone- behavior is of “[t]he young dogs that their factual corrections: ously identify this “watered down” were born and raised at scent gland as a ma- [a] puppy mill were ex- 1. MYTH: If the animal barks, it is jor difference be- wolf behavior. tremely fearful, shy and not a wolf. FACT: Wolves can tween wolves and needed a great deal of emit a variety of sounds. Alt- dogs” (Marar, 1993). work” (Bouyet, 1992). In addi- hough wolves can learn to bark 5. MYTH: If the ears are rounded and tion, wolves are extremely curi- (Iljin, 1944), it is uncommon; furred, it’s a wolf. FACT: Wolves‟ ous animals (Mech, 1971; Fox, wolves will never emit the ears are rounded and furred, but 1971) and may show no fear of “incessant barking” of a dog; ra- they are not more rounded and/ man through a deliberate so- ther they typically produce a huff- or more furred than all dogs‟ ears. cialization process, through a ing sound. The ears of a Malamute or a process called habituation, or 2. MYTH: Wolves can have curly Chow, for example, are more through selective breeding for tails. FACT: Wolves‟ tails can rounded than those of a wolf. friendliness toward people be held at almost any angle or 6. MYTH: Wolves have a shallow (Ness, 2002). In other words, position, but their tails will nev- chest in relation to a dog of the same “the basics of behavior are the er be curled over their backs like size. FACT: Wolves have deep, same for both animals” (Mech, a Malamute or Chow Chow car- narrow, “keel-shaped” chests— 1991). ries its tail. not shallow chests—which allow 8. MYTH: Wolves howl at the full 3. MYTH: Wolves have retractable them to more easily „plow‟ moon. FACT: Wolves use vocal toes. FACT: Wolves‟ toes are through snow (Mech, 1971). sounds to communicate, and not retractable. However, 7. MYTH: While dogs are either ag- the moon has nothing to do wolves typically have larger gressive or friendly toward human with these vocalizations. Emer- paws and toes than dogs for strangers, wolves, by contrast, are gency sirens, on the other more ease in navigating snow. either very shy or afraid when ap- hand, can trigger howl ses- In addition, wolves‟ toes are proached by strangers. FACT: sions—in wolves, wolfdogs, much more pronounced (raised) There is no way to differentiate and dogs. FLA Wolf Identification Pamphlet P a g e 3 WOLF IDENTIFICATION CRITERIA The easiest way to organize criteria for identifying wolves is to isolate those characteristics specific to wolves from those that never occur in wolves. Because there are so many similarities between wolves and dogs, an evaluator attempting to identify a wolf must apply the identification criteria collectively; in other words, for an animal to be “identified” as a wolf using the following criteria, the animal must exhibit each and every trait in the Always category and it must not exhibit any of the traits in the Never category. (Disclaimer: Although the criteria provided below are as comprehensive as possible, an animal that is NOT a pure wolf might also exhibit every trait listed in the Always category and might not exhibit any of the traits listed in the Never category. FLA assumes no liability for animals misidentified as wolves by anyone adopting the criteria set forth in this document.) ALWAYS 7. TOES: Wolves have five toes on the its “hindlegs between its forelegs front legs (one toe being the dew and leave[s] an undulating sinus- oid-like trail” (Iljin, 1944; Mech 1. HEAD: Wolves have large, wedge claw further up the inside of the 1971); however, Iljin (1944) clar- -shaped heads, averaging 9”-11” foreleg) and four toes on the rear ifies that he has “observed some long and 5”-6” wide (Mech, 1971) (no dew claws are present on the working dogs … with an almost with no pronounced stop (the fore- rear legs—a trait found in some dog typical wolf-gait as a conse- head area that slopes down to the breeds).
Recommended publications
  • Coyote Canis Latrans in 2007 IUCN Red List (Canis Latrans)
    MAMMALS OF MISSISSIPPI 10:1–9 Coyote (Canis latrans) CHRISTOPHER L. MAGEE Department of Wildlife and Fisheries, Mississippi State University, Mississippi State, Mississippi, 39762, USA Abstract—Canis latrans (Say 1823) is a canid commonly called the coyote. It is dog-like in appearance with varied colorations throughout its range. Originally restricted to the western portion of North America, coyotes have expanded across the majority of the continent. Coyotes are omnivorous and extremely adaptable, often populating urban and suburban environments. Preferred habitats include a mixture of forested, open, and brushy areas. Currently, there exist no threats or conservation concerns for the coyote in any part of its range. This species is currently experiencing an increasing population trend. Published 5 December 2008 by the Department of Wildlife and Fisheries, Mississippi State University Coyote location (Jackson 1951; Young 1951; Berg and Canis latrans (Say, 1823) Chesness 1978; Way 2007). The species is sexually dimorphic, with adult females distinctly CONTEXT AND CONTENT. lighter and smaller than adult males (Kennedy Order Carnivora, suborder Caniformia, et al. 2003; Way 2007). Average head and infraorder Cynoidea, family Canidae, subfamily body lengths are about 1.0–1.5 m with a tail Caninae, tribe Canini. Genus Canis consists length of about Young 1951). The skull of the of six species: C. aureus, C. latrans, C. lupus, coyote (Fig. 2) progresses through 6 distinct C. mesomelas, C. simensis, and C. adustus. developmental stages allowing delineation Canis latrans has 19 recognized subspecies between the age classes of juvenile, immature, (Wilson and Reeder 2005). young, young adult, adult, and old adult (Jackson 1951).
    [Show full text]
  • Exploiting Interspecific Olfactory Communication to Monitor Predators
    Ecological Applications, 27(2), 2017, pp. 389–402 © 2016 by the Ecological Society of America Exploiting interspecific olfactory communication to monitor predators PATRICK M. GARVEY,1,2 ALISTAIR S. GLEN,2 MICK N. CLOUT,1 SARAH V. WYSE,1,3 MARGARET NICHOLS,4 AND ROGER P. PECH5 1Centre for Biodiversity and Biosecurity, School of Biological Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand 2Landcare Research, Private Bag 92170, Auckland, 1142 New Zealand 3Royal Botanic Gardens Kew, Wakehurst Place, RH17 6TN United Kingdom 4Centre for Wildlife Management and Conservation, Lincoln University, Canterbury, New Zealand 5Landcare Research, PO Box 69040, Lincoln, 7640 New Zealand Abstract. Olfaction is the primary sense of many mammals and subordinate predators use this sense to detect dominant species, thereby reducing the risk of an encounter and facilitating coexistence. Chemical signals can act as repellents or attractants and may therefore have applications for wildlife management. We devised a field experiment to investigate whether dominant predator (ferret Mustela furo) body odor would alter the behavior of three common mesopredators: stoats (Mustela erminea), hedgehogs (Erinaceus europaeus), and ship rats (Rattus rattus). We predicted that apex predator odor would lead to increased detections, and our results support this hypothesis as predator kairomones (interspecific olfactory messages that benefit the receiver) provoked “eavesdropping” behavior by mesopredators. Stoats exhib- ited the most pronounced responses, with kairomones significantly increasing the number of observations and the time spent at a site, so that their occupancy estimates changed from rare to widespread. Behavioral responses to predator odors can therefore be exploited for conserva- tion and this avenue of research has not yet been extensively explored.
    [Show full text]
  • In the Mood Deer Have a Way of Advertising Their Wares and the Whereabouts– Scent, Sounds, and Body Language
    A buck simulates reproductive response with flehman or lip curling to expose his vomeronasal organ to scent of a female’s urination. (frame from Quality Deer Management Assoc. video) In the Mood Deer have a way of advertising their wares and the whereabouts– scent, sounds, and body language J. Morton Galetto, CU Maurice River Last October I wrote an article about deer that included some aspects of their mating habits. It described mate guarding and the male deer or buck’s propensity to focus on following a female in estrus so as to insure he is the lucky suitor. During rut or the breeding season, when males spar competitively for reproductive rights over females, deer are on the move. The season begins in October and its height in our region is November 10- 20. Females that are not successfully fertilized will come into estrus a second time 28 days later, offering a second chance for them to produce young. All this moving around makes deer and drivers especially prone to road collisions. Bucks are particularly active during sunset and early morning. So slow down and stay alert. In last year’s story we offered lots of advice in this regard. White-tail deer are the local species here in Southern NJ. I thought it might be fun to talk about their indicators of lust – well, fun for me anyway. Deer employ three major methods of communications: scents, vocalizations, and body language. Deer are ungulates, meaning they are a mammal with a cloven hoof on each leg, split in two parts; you could describe them as toes or digits.
    [Show full text]
  • Mammalian Pheromones – New Opportunities for Improved Predator Control in New Zealand
    SCIENCE FOR CONSERVATION 330 Mammalian pheromones – new opportunities for improved predator control in New Zealand B. Kay Clapperton, Elaine C. Murphy and Hussam A. A. Razzaq Cover: Stoat in boulders in the Tasman River bed, Mackenzie Basin. Photo: John Dowding. Science for Conservation is a scientific monograph series presenting research funded by New Zealand Department of Conservation (DOC). Manuscripts are internally and externally peer-reviewed; resulting publications are considered part of the formal international scientific literature. This report is available from the departmental website in pdf form. Titles are listed in our catalogue on the website, refer www.doc.govt.nz under Publications, then Series. © Copyright August 2017, New Zealand Department of Conservation ISSN 1177–9241 (web PDF) ISBN 978–1–98–851436–9 (web PDF) This report was prepared for publication by the Publishing Team; editing by Amanda Todd and layout by Lynette Clelland. Publication was approved by the Director, Threats Unit, Department of Conservation, Wellington, New Zealand. Published by Publishing Team, Department of Conservation, PO Box 10420, The Terrace, Wellington 6143, New Zealand. In the interest of forest conservation, we support paperless electronic publishing. CONTENTS Abstract 1 1. Introduction 2 1.1 The potential roles of pheromones in New Zealand predator control 2 1.2 Aim of this review 4 2. Pheromone identification and function in mammalian predator species 5 2.1 Mice 5 2.2 Rats 7 2.3 Rodent Major Urinary Proteins (MUPs) 9 2.4 Cats 10 2.5 Mustelids 11 2.6 Possums (with reference to other marsupials) 13 3. Odour perception and expression 14 3.1 How do animals perceive odours? 14 3.2 Influence of the MHC 16 4.
    [Show full text]
  • Olfactory Communication in the Ferret (Mustela Furo L.) and Its Application in Wildlife Management
    Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere without the permission of the Author. OLFACTORY COMMUNICATION IN THE FERRET (MUSTELA FURO L.) AND ITS APPLICATION IN WILDLIFE MANAGEMENT A Thesis Prepared in Partial Fulfilment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Zoology at Massey University Barbara Kay Clapperton 1985 �;·.;L��:"·;�� �is Copyrigj;lt Fonn Title:·J.;:E,:£j;esis: O\fgc:..�O("j CoW\I"'h.JA"lC,citQC\ � �� '�'Jd" � (�tJ.o.kgL.) � -\� C\�ic..Q;"er.. \h wA�·�. (1) ta.W-:> I give permission for my thesis to be rrede a���;�d�� .. -; ':: readers in the Massey University Library under conditi¥?� . C • determined by the Librarian. ; �dO n�t�sh my..,):hesi�o be�de�ai�e � _ _ re er/l.thout""my wmtten c6nse� for _ -<-L_ nths. ' ... ' � (2 ) �l.--.::�;. I � t � thej>'is, ov& c0er. rrej0Se s� ¥ I :.. an �r n�t ' ut ¥1h undef'condrtion srdeterdU.n6§. � t e rarl. I do not wish my thesis, or a copy, to be sent to another institution without my written consent for 12 nonths. (3 ) I agree that my thesis rrey be copied for Library use.: . �� ... Signed �,t. .... : B K Clappe n ;" Date (� �v � ; .. \, The cct1m�t of this thesis belongs to the author. Readers must sign t��::�arre in the space below to show that they recognise this. lmy· · are asked to add their perm:ment address.
    [Show full text]
  • Volatile Cues Influence Host-Choice in Arthropod Pests
    animals Review Volatile Cues Influence Host-Choice in Arthropod Pests Jacqueline Poldy Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation, Health & Biosecurity, Black Mountain Laboratory, Canberra, ACT 2601, Australia; [email protected]; Tel.: +61-2-6218-3599 Received: 1 October 2020; Accepted: 22 October 2020; Published: 28 October 2020 Simple Summary: Many significant human and animal diseases are spread by blood feeding insects and other arthropod vectors. Arthropod pests and disease vectors rely heavily on chemical cues to identify and locate important resources such as their preferred animal hosts. Although there are abundant studies on the means by which biting insects—especially mosquitoes—are attracted to humans, this focus overlooks the veterinary and medical importance of other host–pest relationships and the chemical signals that underpin them. This review documents the published data on airborne (volatile) chemicals emitted from non-human animals, highlighting the subset of these emissions that play a role in guiding host choice by arthropod pests. The paper exposes some of the complexities associated with existing methods for collecting relevant chemical features from animal subjects, cautions against extrapolating the ecological significance of volatile emissions, and highlights opportunities to explore research gaps. Although the literature is less comprehensive than human studies, understanding the chemical drivers behind host selection creates opportunities to interrupt pest attack and disease transmission, enabling more efficient pest management. Abstract: Many arthropod pests of humans and other animals select their preferred hosts by recognising volatile odour compounds contained in the hosts’ ‘volatilome’. Although there is prolific literature on chemical emissions from humans, published data on volatiles and vector attraction in other species are more sporadic.
    [Show full text]
  • Microanatomy and Bacterial Flora of the Perineal Glands of the North American Porcupine
    59 Microanatomy and bacterial flora of the perineal glands of the North American porcupine U. Roze, K.T. Leung, E. Nix, G. Burton, and D.M. Chapman Abstract: The perineal glands of the porcupine, Erethizon dorsatum (L., 1758), are sexually dimorphic, paired pockets sprouting osmetrichial hairs. They lie between the anus and urethra, lateral to the midline, amid a sebaceous glandular ex- panse. In their active state, the glandular pockets secrete an amber substance with a terpenoid odor. When inactive, the glands produce no stain or odor. In males, activation of the glands is associated with fully descended testes. The glandular pockets yield a microbiota (‘‘microflora’’) in both their active and inactive states. We hypothesize that the active-state mi- croflora transforms a sebaceous secretion into a pheromonally active product that is disseminated by anal dragging. The glandular microflora was characterized by gas chromatography of bacterial fatty acid methyl esters (GC-FAME) and poly- merase chain reaction – denaturing gradient gel electrophoresis (PCR-DGGE) of 16S ribosomal RNA gene fragments of bacteria. PCR-DGGE results showed the resulting bacteria profiles were the same in both sexes, but differed between the active and inactive states. Active-state microfloras were dominated by members of the Actinobacteria and showed greater coefficients of similarity than inactive-state microfloras. The microflora of individual animals changed with time and with secretory state. We argue for a reproductive role for the activated perineal glands. Re´sume´ : Les glandes pe´rine´ales du porc-e´pic, Erethizon dorsatum (L., 1758), consistent en une paire de poches sexuelle- ment dimorphes d’ou` e´mergent des poils osme´trichiaux.
    [Show full text]
  • Wild Fur Pelt Handling Manual Table of Contents
    WILD FUR PELT HANDLING MANUAL TABLE OF CONTENTS Introduction 2 Raccoon 6 Beaver 12 North American Fur Auctions’ principal business is the sale of An organization of wild fur producers that strive to promote the raw fur pelts. The pelts are received on consignment from culture, heritage, and economic viability of fur harvesting through Beaver Castoreum/Castor 16 producers of ranched-raised furs and harvesters of wild furs. The an ownership position in North American Fur Auctions. Muskrat (Musquash) 18 fur pelts are sold to fur garment manufacturers and fur pelt dealers worldwide. The Company is the largest fur auction house Wild Fur Shippers Council, Respect for our past Commitment to Marten (Sable) 22 in North America, and the third largest fur auction in the world. the future. Fox & Coyote 24 Lynx and Lynx Cat (Bobcat) 28 CONTAC T INFORMATION CREDITS Fisher 30 NAFA Depots in the US NAFA Depots in CANADA Project Manager Content Writing: Andrew Hyde 205 Industrial Circle 65 Skyway Ave. Content Review Editing: Dave Bewick River Otter 32 Stoughton, WI 53589 Toronto ON, M9W 6C7 Squirrels & Weasels: Dave Bewick Tel: 608.205.9200 Tel: 416.675.9320 Content Review: Bill Mackowski Wild Mink 36 Wolves, Bears & Wolverines: Jim Gibb Wolves, Bears & Wolverines 38 Beaver Castor: Ron Lancour Graphic Designer: Luca Di Franco Skunk/Opossum 44 NAFA has large network of agents that collect in both Canada and USA. I would like to thank the NAFA grading staff for all the help they gave me to improve Badger 46 Contact us to find your closest agent. my understanding of the grading process, and how what we do as trappers can affect the grade and price of pelts.
    [Show full text]
  • Wild Mammal Blog About Mammals, for Mammals, by Mammals
    Wild Mammal Blog About Mammals, For Mammals, By Mammals Mammal Blog Downloads Digital Bat House Mammalogy Manual Vertebrate Biology Manual About Links Sitemap Mammalogy Techniques Manual, 2nd Edition The manual consists of 16 chapters covering topics such as radio and GPS tracking, mark and recapture studies, camera trapping studies, optimal foraging, behavioral studies, among others. Each chapter includes background information, student exercises, and sample data sets. 16 Chapters, 167 Figures, 282 pages, 30 Tables $39.95 Learn more here. Now available at Lulu.com, Amazon and Barnes & Noble Hyena Scent Posts Use Symbiotic Microbe Messengers 14 Nov 2013 Filed in: Carnivora | Communication | Behavior Twitter limits human communication to a mere 140 characters. Animals’ scent posts may be equally as short, relatively speaking, yet they convey an encyclopedia of information about the animals that left them. In the current issue of the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, a Michigan State University researcher shows that the detailed scent posts of hyenas (Figure 1) are, in part, products of symbiotic bacteria, microbes that have a mutually beneficial relationship with their hosts. “When hyenas leave paste deposits on grass, the sour­smelling signals relay reams of information for other animals to read,” said Kevin Theis, the paper’s lead author and MSU postdoctoral researcher. “Hyenas can leave a quick, detailed message and go. It’s like a bulletin board of who’s around and how they’re doing.” Figure 1. A spotted hyena scent marking. (courtesy of Michigan State University) Interestingly, it is the bacteria in pastes – more diverse than scientists had imagined – that appear to be doing the yeoman’s job of sending these messages.
    [Show full text]
  • Sliwa 1996 Aardwolf
    A functional analysis of scent marking and mating behaviour in the aardwolf , Proteles cristatus (Sparrman, 1783) by Alexander Sliwa Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Faculty of Biological and Agricultural Sciences University of Pretoria May 1996 ii A functional analysis of scent marking and mating behaviour in the aardwolf, Proteles cristatus (Sparrman 1783) by Alexander Sliwa Supervisor: Dr. Philip R.K. Richardson Department of Zoology and Entomology University of Pretoria ABSTRACT This study attempted to answer how scent marks function in terms of aardwolf Proteles cristatus year- round territory maintenance and mating success. A functional analysis of scent marking in the aardwolf was conducted in a two and a half year field study whilst recording 42 000 paste marks. The anatomy and histology of the anal pouch revealed an efficient organ for producing copious amounts of long- lasting fatty pasting secretion, and the apparatus for applying it onto grass stalks. The histology of the penile pad of aardwolf males was similarly geared to production and application of secretion, though its exact function is still not clear. Aardwolves started practising scent marking motor patterns early in life, but physiological maturation of secretion was complete with eight months and independence from their parents only. Cubs practised paste marking by overmarking scent marks of their parents, adjusting their mark-rate to that of the adult followed. After physical and behavioural maturation of paste marking subadults ceased to mark, coinciding with parental aggression, and some remained in their parent’s territories for another year.
    [Show full text]
  • Buck Odor Production in the Cornual Gland of the Male Goat, Capra Hircus– Validation with Histoarchitecture, Volatile and Proteomic Analysis
    Indian Journal of Biochemistry & Biophysics Vol. 55, June 2018, pp. 183-190 Buck odor production in the cornual gland of the male goat, Capra hircus– Validation with histoarchitecture, volatile and proteomic analysis Devaraj Sankarganesh1,#, Rajamanickam Ramachandran1,2, Radhakrishnan Ashok1, Veluchamy Ramesh Saravanakumar3, Raman Sukirtha1, Govindaraju Archunan4* & Shanmugam Achiraman1,4* 1Department of Environmental Biotechnology, Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli-620 024, Tamil Nadu, India 2Department of Microbial Biotechnology, Bharathiar University, Coimbatore-641 046, Tamil Nadu, India 3Department of Livestock Production and Management, Veterinary College and Research Institute, Namakkal-637 002, Tamil Nadu, India 4Center for Pheromone Technology, Department of Animal Science, Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli- 620 024, Tamil Nadu, India Received 28 January 2018; revised 14 May 2018 In many animals, glandular secretions or pheromones that possess biological moieties contain messages encoded by the intrinsic smell. In male goats, the cornual gland (a sebaceous gland), may synthesize and excrete relevant chemical components that are responsible for the ‘buck effect’. To test this, cornual glands from freshly-slaughtered male goats (N=6) were subjected to histoarchitecture analysis, to infer about the structural alignment, to the GC–MS analysis for volatile compounds and to SDS–PAGE for protein profiling followed by MALDI-TOF to characterize specific protein bands. The gland possesses sebum, vacuoles and hair follicles inferring its capability to synthesize and extrude the scent. We found 14 volatiles in GC–MS analysis, in which 1-octadecanol might be a putative pheromone of buck odor. We identified seven different proteins in SDS-PAGE. Two proteins, 28 and 33 kDa, were highly matched with DNA mismatch repair protein and Abietadiene synthase, respectively, as inferred from MALDI-TOF.
    [Show full text]
  • UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations
    UCLA UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title The Behavioral Significance of Olfactory Scent Cues in the Tasmanian Devil Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1t74c4hd Author Reid-Wainscoat, Elizabeth Ellen Publication Date 2018 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles The Behavioral Significance of Olfactory Scent Cues in the Tasmanian Devil A thesis submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Master of Science in Biology by Elizabeth Ellen Reid-Wainscoat 2018 © Copyright by Elizabeth Ellen Reid-Wainscoat 2018 ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS The Behavioral Significance of Olfactory Scent Cues in the Tasmanian Devil by Elizabeth Ellen Reid-Wainscoat Master of Science in Biology University of California, Los Angeles, 2018 Professor Gregory F. Grether, Chair The Tasmanian devil (Sarcophilus harrisii) is a nocturnal carnivorous marsupial that has suffered precipitous decline in the past 20 years due to a contagious fatal cancer. To provide the best management practices to help re-establish wild populations it is crucial to understand the behavioral ecology of this species. Initial studies proved that, despite their classification as a solitary, non-territorial species, olfactory communication plays a significant role in Tasmanian devil social structure and scent cues deposited at shared latrine sites provide important social cues. To better understand the role of the latrine sites, we tested whether Tasmanian devils communicate sex, season and individual differences through scent cues. Specifically, we analyzed the chemical composition of anal scent gland secretions using gas chromatography-mass spectrometry, which revealed significant differences between seasons (breeding/non-breeding), sexes, and individual males.
    [Show full text]