How Does Grammatical Gender Affect Noun Representations in Gender-Marking Languages?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

How Does Grammatical Gender Affect Noun Representations in Gender-Marking Languages? How does Grammatical Gender Affect Noun Representations in Gender-Marking Languages? Hila Gonen1 Yova Kementchedjhieva2 Yoav Goldberg1;3 1Department of Computer Science, Bar-Ilan University 2University of Copenhagen 3Allen Institute for Artificial Intelligence [email protected],[email protected],[email protected] Abstract to inanimate nouns (e.g. dream, book). This gram- matical gender assignment is mostly arbitrary: the Many natural languages assign grammatical same inanimate concept can have different gender gender also to inanimate nouns in the lan- in different languages. For example, a flower is guage. In such languages, words that relate masculine in Italian (fiore) and feminine in Ger- to the gender-marked nouns are inflected to man (Blume). agree with the noun’s gender. We show that this affects the word representations of inani- Languages often maintain an agreement system mate nouns, resulting in nouns with the same in which certain words agree on different morpho- gender being closer to each other than nouns logical features with other words they relate to. with different gender. While “embedding de- For example, English present-tense verbs are in- biasing” methods fail to remove the effect, we flected to agree with their nominal subject on the demonstrate that a careful application of meth- number feature. In other languages the agreement ods that neutralize grammatical gender signals system is more elaborate, and in particular verbs, from the words’ context when training word embeddings is effective in removing it. Fix- adjectives, determiners and other functions agree ing the grammatical gender bias yields a posi- with nouns on many features, including gender tive effect on the quality of the resulting word (Corbett, 2006).1 embeddings, both in monolingual and cross- Such grammatical agreement affects the distri- lingual settings. We note that successfully re- butional environment of nouns, as nouns of differ- moving gender signals, while achievable, is ent gender become surrounded by different word not trivial to do and that a language-specific forms: feminine nouns co-occur more with the morphological analyzer, together with careful usage of it, are essential for achieving good re- feminine forms of words, while masculine nouns sults. with the masculine forms. For example, the Italian word viaggio (“journey”-masc) will co-occur with 1 Introduction durato (“last”-masc) and lungo (“long”-masc), while the word gita (“trip”-fem) will co-occur Work on distributional word embeddings focuses with durata (“last”-fem) and lunga (“long”-fem). almost exclusively on English, or on cross-lingual Such changes in the distributional environment and language-agnostic techniques. However, lan- may bias the learned distributional representations guages are diverse and different languages ex- of inanimate nouns. Indeed, we see that the major- hibit different linguistic phenomena, which may ity of the top-10 nearest neighbors of the word gita interact with the English-centric embedding learn- in Italian (“trip”-fem) are feminine words. Also, ing algorithms. In this work we look into one we notice that the word viaggio (“journey”-masc) such phenomenon—grammatical gender—and ex- is not on the list, while in English, for comparison, amine its effect on the learned representation. we can find journey in the top-10 nearest neigh- Many languages have rich grammatical sys- bors of trip. tems, that often include a complex gender system In this work, we are interested in investigating, as well (Corbett, 1991). Languages with grammat- demonstrating and quantifying this effect beyond ical gender assign and morphologically mark gen- 1As the gender of nouns is fixed, the other elements are in- der not only to animate nouns (which have biologi- flected to accommodate the agreement constraint. The nouns cal sex, e.g. man, woman, mother, father), but also are said to assign gender to the other words. 463 Proceedings of the 23rd Conference on Computational Natural Language Learning, pages 463–471 Hong Kong, China, November 3-4, 2019. c 2019 Association for Computational Linguistics the anecdotal level. We also explore methods for stream tasks. These models are based on the dis- removing such unwanted biases. tributional hypothesis according to which words We demonstrate that both in Italian and in Ger- that occur in the same contexts tend to have sim- man, the grammatical gender affects similarities ilar meanings (Harris, 1954). Indeed, they aim to between word representations (using words from create word representations that are derived from SimLex-999 (Hill et al., 2015; Leviant and Re- their shared contexts, where the context of a word ichart, 2015)): pairs of nouns with similar gender is essentially the words in its proximity (be it ac- are closer to each other while pairs of nouns with cording to linear order in the sentence or according different gender are farther apart. to syntactic relations) (Mikolov et al., 2013; Pen- After quantifying the effect, we explore several nington et al., 2014; Levy and Goldberg, 2014). methods of reducing it. A popular choice would be to simply lemmatize all the words prior to feed- Gender Biases in Word Embeddings Social ing them to the embedding learning algorithm. gender bias was demonstrated to be consistent However, full lemmatization can be destructive, in and pervasive across different word embeddings the sense that it will also remove morphological (Caliskan et al., 2017). Bolukbasi et al. (2016) distinction that we may want to keep. We thus show that using word embeddings for simple seek more surgical approaches. Interestingly, re- analogies surfaces many gender stereotypes. In cent embedding debiasing approaches (Bolukbasi addition, they define the gender bias of a word w by its projection on the “gender direction”: et al., 2016) do not work well. We instead look −! −! −! for methods that attempt to neutralize the gender w · (he − she), assuming all vectors are normal- signals from the training data. We find that such ized. Positive bias stands for male-bias. For ex- methods are effective in reducing the effect, but ample, the bias of manager is 0:06, while the bias 2 are also language specific and tricky to get right: of nurse is −0:10 . we rely on language specific morphological ana- Recently, some work has been done to reduce lyzers while carefully accounting for their pecu- social gender bias in word embeddings, both as liarities and adjusting our use for each language. a post-processing step (Bolukbasi et al., 2016) We take this work as a reminder that (a) linguistic and as part of the training procedure (Zhao et al., resources such as lexicons and morphological an- 2018). Bolukbasi et al. (2016) use a post- alyzers are still relevant and useful (cf. (Zalmout processing debiasing method. Given a word em- and Habash, 2017)); (b) languages are diverse and bedding matrix, they make changes to the word different languages require different treatments; vectors in order to reduce the gender bias for all and (c) small details may matter a lot. In partic- words that are not inherently gendered. They do ular, existing tools and resources, either learned or that by zeroing the gender projection of each word 3 human curated, should not be trusted blindly, but on a predefined gender direction. be carefuly adapted for the problem. In Zmigrod et al. (2019), the authors mitigate Finally, we show that reducing the effect of social gender bias in gender marking languages grammatical agreement also has a positive ef- using counterfactual data augmentation. Gender- fect on the quality of the resulting word repre- marking languages add several interesting dimen- sentations, both in monolingual and cross-lingual sions to the story: words relating to animate con- settings. We conclude that grammatical gen- cepts such as “nurse” or “cat” may have both mas- der indeed has its imprints on the representa- culine and feminine versions; the distributional tions of inanimate nouns, and that this should be environment of a word contains many more ex- taken into account when working with gender- plicit gender cues; and inanimate concepts are marking languages. Our code and debiased em- also assigned gender. All of these factors inter- beddings are available at https://github. act in complicated ways. In this work we focus on com/gonenhila/grammatical_gender. purely grammatical gender—the gender that is as- signed to inanimate nouns—and its effect on their 2 Background and Related Work resulting representations. 2 Word Embeddings Word embeddings have be- in English word2vec embeddings (Mikolov et al., 2013) trained on Wikipedia. come an important component in many NLP mod- 3The gender direction is chosen to be the top principal els and are widely used for a vast range of down- component (PC) of ten gender pair difference vectors. 464 Grammatical Gender Bias in Word Embed- 3.1 Inanimate Noun Pairs from SimLex-999 dings Grammatical gender is manifested in a We take the inanimate noun portion of the similar way to social bias. For example, when −! −! SimLex-999 dataset (Hill et al., 2015), a gold stan- lui − lei projected on the Italian gender direction dard resource for evaluating distributional seman- (Italian equivalents of “he” and “she”), the word tic models. This dataset has an English version, century “secolo” ( , masculine) has positive bias of and also German and Italian versions (Leviant and 0.073, while the word “zuppa” (soup, feminine) 4 Reichart, 2015), and includes both similar and dis- has negative bias of -0.079. similar word pairs, with human-assigned similar- We attribute this behavior to grammatical agree- ity judgments for each pair. This gives us 529 pairs ment. Since the context of different-gender nouns of English words, along with high quality trans- is expected to be very different because of the lations to Italian and German. We manually as- agreement of the surrounding words, and since sociate the Italian and German words with their the resulting representations are based on the con- grammatical gender.
Recommended publications
  • Representation of Inflected Nouns in the Internal Lexicon
    Memory & Cognition 1980, Vol. 8 (5), 415423 Represeritation of inflected nouns in the internal lexicon G. LUKATELA, B. GLIGORIJEVIC, and A. KOSTIC University ofBelgrade, Belgrade, Yugoslavia and M.T.TURVEY University ofConnecticut, Storrs, Connecticut 06268 and Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut 06510 The lexical representation of Serbo-Croatian nouns was investigated in a lexical decision task. Because Serbo-Croatian nouns are declined, a noun may appear in one of several gram­ matical cases distinguished by the inflectional morpheme affixed to the base form. The gram­ matical cases occur with different frequencies, although some are visually and phonetically identical. When the frequencies of identical forms are compounded, the ordering of frequencies is not the same for masculine and feminine genders. These two genders are distinguished further by the fact that the base form for masculine nouns is an actual grammatical case, the nominative singular, whereas the base form for feminine nouns is an abstraction in that it cannot stand alone as an independent word. Exploiting these characteristics of the Serbo­ Croatian language, we contrasted three views of how a noun is represented: (1) the independent­ entries hypothesis, which assumes an independent representation for each grammatical case, reflecting its frequency of occurrence; (2) the derivational hypothesis, which assumes that only the base morpheme is stored, with the individual cases derived from separately stored inflec­ tional morphemes and rules for combination; and (3) the satellite-entries hypothesis, which assumes that all cases are individually represented, with the nominative singular functioning as the nucleus and the embodiment of the noun's frequency and around which the other cases cluster uniformly.
    [Show full text]
  • Verbal Case and the Nature of Polysynthetic Inflection
    Verbal case and the nature of polysynthetic inflection Colin Phillips MIT Abstract This paper tries to resolve a conflict in the literature on the connection between ‘rich’ agreement and argument-drop. Jelinek (1984) claims that inflectional affixes in polysynthetic languages are theta-role bearing arguments; Baker (1991) argues that such affixes are agreement, bearing Case but no theta-role. Evidence from Yimas shows that both of these views can be correct, within a single language. Explanation of what kind of inflection is used where also provides us with an account of the unusual split ergative agreement system of Yimas, and suggests a novel explanation for the ban on subject incorporation, and some exceptions to the ban. 1. Two types of inflection My main aim in this paper is to demonstrate that inflectional affixes can be very different kinds of syntactic objects, even within a single language. I illustrate this point with evidence from Yimas, a Papuan language of New Guinea (Foley 1991). Understanding of the nature of the different inflectional affixes of Yimas provides an explanation for its remarkably elaborate agreement system, which follows a basic split-ergative scheme, but with a number of added complications. Anticipating my conclusions, the structure in (1c) shows what I assume the four principal case affixes on a Yimas verb to be. What I refer to as Nominative and Accusative affixes are pronominal arguments: these inflections, which are restricted to 1st and 2nd person arguments in Yimas, begin as specifiers and complements of the verb, and incorporate into the verb by S- structure. On the other hand, what I refer to as Ergative and Absolutive inflection are genuine agreement - they are the spell-out of functional heads, above VP, which agree with an argument in their specifier.
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: a Case Study from Koro
    Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro By Jessica Cleary-Kemp A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair Assistant Professor Peter S. Jenks Professor William F. Hanks Summer 2015 © Copyright by Jessica Cleary-Kemp All Rights Reserved Abstract Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro by Jessica Cleary-Kemp Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair In this dissertation a methodology for identifying and analyzing serial verb constructions (SVCs) is developed, and its application is exemplified through an analysis of SVCs in Koro, an Oceanic language of Papua New Guinea. SVCs involve two main verbs that form a single predicate and share at least one of their arguments. In addition, they have shared values for tense, aspect, and mood, and they denote a single event. The unique syntactic and semantic properties of SVCs present a number of theoretical challenges, and thus they have invited great interest from syntacticians and typologists alike. But characterizing the nature of SVCs and making generalizations about the typology of serializing languages has proven difficult. There is still debate about both the surface properties of SVCs and their underlying syntactic structure. The current work addresses some of these issues by approaching serialization from two angles: the typological and the language-specific. On the typological front, it refines the definition of ‘SVC’ and develops a principled set of cross-linguistically applicable diagnostics.
    [Show full text]
  • The “Person” Category in the Zamuco Languages. a Diachronic Perspective
    On rare typological features of the Zamucoan languages, in the framework of the Chaco linguistic area Pier Marco Bertinetto Luca Ciucci Scuola Normale Superiore di Pisa The Zamucoan family Ayoreo ca. 4500 speakers Old Zamuco (a.k.a. Ancient Zamuco) spoken in the XVIII century, extinct Chamacoco (Ɨbɨtoso, Tomarâho) ca. 1800 speakers The Zamucoan family The first stable contact with Zamucoan populations took place in the early 18th century in the reduction of San Ignacio de Samuco. The Jesuit Ignace Chomé wrote a grammar of Old Zamuco (Arte de la lengua zamuca). The Chamacoco established friendly relationships by the end of the 19th century. The Ayoreos surrended rather late (towards the middle of the last century); there are still a few nomadic small bands in Northern Paraguay. The Zamucoan family Main typological features -Fusional structure -Word order features: - SVO - Genitive+Noun - Noun + Adjective Zamucoan typologically rare features Nominal tripartition Radical tenselessness Nominal aspect Affix order in Chamacoco 3 plural Gender + classifiers 1 person ø-marking in Ayoreo realis Traces of conjunct / disjunct system in Old Zamuco Greater plural and clusivity Para-hypotaxis Nominal tripartition Radical tenselessness Nominal aspect Affix order in Chamacoco 3 plural Gender + classifiers 1 person ø-marking in Ayoreo realis Traces of conjunct / disjunct system in Old Zamuco Greater plural and clusivity Para-hypotaxis Nominal tripartition All Zamucoan languages present a morphological tripartition in their nominals. The base-form (BF) is typically used for predication. The singular-BF is (Ayoreo & Old Zamuco) or used to be (Cham.) the basis for any morphological operation. The full-form (FF) occurs in argumental position.
    [Show full text]
  • 4.1 Inflection
    4.1 Inflection Within a lexeme-based theory of morphology, the difference between derivation and inflection is very simple. Derivation gives you new lexemes, and inflection gives you the forms of a lexeme that are determined by syntactic environment (cf. 2.1.2). But what exactly does this mean? Is there really a need for such a distinction? This section explores the answers to these questions, and in the process, goes deeper into the relation between morphology and syntax. 4.1.1 Inflection vs. derivation The first question we can ask about the distinction between inflection and derivation is whether there is any formal basis for distinguishing the two: can we tell them apart because they do different things to words? One generalization that has been made is that derivational affixes tend to occur closer to the root or stem than inflectional affixes. For example, (1) shows that the English third person singular present inflectional suffix -s occurs outside of derivational suffixes like the deadjectival -ize, and the plural ending -s follows derivational affixes including the deverbal -al: (1) a. popular-ize-s commercial-ize-s b. upheav-al-s arriv-al-s Similarly, Japanese derivational suffixes like passive -rare or causative -sase precede inflectional suffixes marking tense and aspect:1 (2) a. tabe-ru tabe-ta eat- IMP eat- PERF INFLECTION 113 ‘eats’ ‘ate’ b. tabe-rare- ru tabe-rare- ta eat - PASS-IMP eat- PASS-PERF ‘is eaten’ ‘was eaten’ c. tabe-sase- ru tabe-sase- ta eat- CAUS-IMP eat- CAUS-PERF ‘makes eat’ ‘made eat’ It is also the case that inflectional morphology does not change the meaning or grammatical category of the word that it applies to.
    [Show full text]
  • The Syntax of Answers to Negative Yes/No-Questions in English Anders Holmberg Newcastle University
    The syntax of answers to negative yes/no-questions in English Anders Holmberg Newcastle University 1. Introduction This paper will argue that answers to polar questions or yes/no-questions (YNQs) in English are elliptical expressions with basically the structure (1), where IP is identical to the LF of the IP of the question, containing a polarity variable with two possible values, affirmative or negative, which is assigned a value by the focused polarity expression. (1) yes/no Foc [IP ...x... ] The crucial data come from answers to negative questions. English turns out to have a fairly complicated system, with variation depending on which negation is used. The meaning of the answer yes in (2) is straightforward, affirming that John is coming. (2) Q(uestion): Isn’t John coming, too? A(nswer): Yes. (‘John is coming.’) In (3) (for speakers who accept this question as well formed), 1 the meaning of yes alone is indeterminate, and it is therefore not a felicitous answer in this context. The longer version is fine, affirming that John is coming. (3) Q: Isn’t John coming, either? A: a. #Yes. b. Yes, he is. In (4), there is variation regarding the interpretation of yes. Depending on the context it can be a confirmation of the negation in the question, meaning ‘John is not coming’. In other contexts it will be an infelicitous answer, as in (3). (4) Q: Is John not coming? A: a. Yes. (‘John is not coming.’) b. #Yes. In all three cases the (bare) answer no is unambiguous, meaning that John is not coming.
    [Show full text]
  • Corpus Study of Tense, Aspect, and Modality in Diglossic Speech in Cairene Arabic
    CORPUS STUDY OF TENSE, ASPECT, AND MODALITY IN DIGLOSSIC SPEECH IN CAIRENE ARABIC BY OLA AHMED MOSHREF DISSERTATION Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2012 Urbana, Illinois Doctoral Committee: Professor Elabbas Benmamoun, Chair Professor Eyamba Bokamba Professor Rakesh M. Bhatt Assistant Professor Marina Terkourafi ABSTRACT Morpho-syntactic features of Modern Standard Arabic mix intricately with those of Egyptian Colloquial Arabic in ordinary speech. I study the lexical, phonological and syntactic features of verb phrase morphemes and constituents in different tenses, aspects, moods. A corpus of over 3000 phrases was collected from religious, political/economic and sports interviews on four Egyptian satellite TV channels. The computational analysis of the data shows that systematic and content morphemes from both varieties of Arabic combine in principled ways. Syntactic considerations play a critical role with regard to the frequency and direction of code-switching between the negative marker, subject, or complement on one hand and the verb on the other. Morph-syntactic constraints regulate different types of discourse but more formal topics may exhibit more mixing between Colloquial aspect or future markers and Standard verbs. ii To the One Arab Dream that will come true inshaa’ Allah! عربية أنا.. أميت دمها خري الدماء.. كما يقول أيب الشاعر العراقي: بدر شاكر السياب Arab I am.. My nation’s blood is the finest.. As my father says Iraqi Poet: Badr Shaker Elsayyab iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I’m sincerely thankful to my advisor Prof. Elabbas Benmamoun, who during the six years of my study at UIUC was always kind, caring and supportive on the personal and academic levels.
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Verb Constructions: Argument Structural Uniformity and Event Structural Diversity
    SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTIONS: ARGUMENT STRUCTURAL UNIFORMITY AND EVENT STRUCTURAL DIVERSITY A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS AND THE COMMITTEE ON GRADUATE STUDIES OF STANFORD UNIVERSITY IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Melanie Owens November 2011 © 2011 by Melanie Rachel Owens. All Rights Reserved. Re-distributed by Stanford University under license with the author. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution- Noncommercial 3.0 United States License. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/us/ This dissertation is online at: http://purl.stanford.edu/db406jt2949 ii I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Beth Levin, Primary Adviser I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Joan Bresnan I certify that I have read this dissertation and that, in my opinion, it is fully adequate in scope and quality as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Vera Gribanov Approved for the Stanford University Committee on Graduate Studies. Patricia J. Gumport, Vice Provost Graduate Education This signature page was generated electronically upon submission of this dissertation in electronic format. An original signed hard copy of the signature page is on file in University Archives. iii Abstract Serial Verb Constructions (SVCs) are constructions which contain two or more verbs yet behave in every grammatical respect as if they contain only one.
    [Show full text]
  • From Grammatical Number to Exact Numbers: Early Meanings of 'One', 'Two', and 'Three' in English, Russian, and Japanese
    UC Irvine UC Irvine Previously Published Works Title From grammatical number to exact numbers: Early meanings of 'one', 'two', and 'three' in English, Russian, and Japanese Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9hg912bn Journal Cognitive Psychology, 55 ISSN 0010-0285 Authors Sarnecka, Barbara W. Kamenskaya, Valentina G. Yamana, Yuko et al. Publication Date 2007-09-01 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Manuscript of Sarnecka et al From Grammatical Number 1 Running head: FROM GRAMMATICAL NUMBER TO EXACT NUMBERS From Grammatical Number to Exact Numbers: Early Meanings of ‘One,’ ‘Two,’ and ‘Three’ in English, Russian, and Japanese Barbara W. Sarnecka University of California, Irvine Valentina G. Kamenskaya Herzen State Pedagogical University of Russia Yuko Yamana Kobe Gakuin University, Japan Tamiko Ogura Kobe University, Japan Yulia. B. Yudovina University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands From Grammatical Number 2 Abstract This study examines whether singular/plural marking in a language helps children learn the cardinal meanings of the words ‘one,’ ‘two,’ and ‘three.’ First, CHILDES data in English, Russian (which marks singular/plural), and Japanese (which does not) were compared for frequency, variability, and contexts of number-word use. Then, groups of monolingual children (ages 2-10 to 3-6) in the USA, Russia, and Japan were tested on Counting and Give-N tasks. More English and Russian learners knew the meaning of each word than Japanese learners, regardless of whether singular/plural cues appeared in the task itself (e.g., “Give two apples” vs. “Give two”). Authors conclude that the earliest number-word meanings come from the conceptual framework of grammatical number, rather than of positive integers.
    [Show full text]
  • Iconicity and Tense, Aspect, and Mood Morphology in Yamitp PT
    Concentric: Studies in Linguistics 31.1 (June 2005): 65-94 1 Iconicity and Tense, Aspect, and Mood Morphology in YamiTP PT D. Victoria Rau Providence University This study aims to investigate the relationships between the forms and functions of the Tense-Aspect-Modality categories. Bybee’s hypothesis (1985) was tested against Yami data to discover how the categories of tense, aspect, and mood interact to support the iconicity hypothesis. The morphemic order and the degree of fusion are not arbitrary, in that the categories that most affect the meanings of the stems occur closest to the stem, whereas those that have least impact on the lexical meaning of the stem occur in the periphery. Greater semantic cohesion of concepts is reflected in a higher degree of fusion in their corresponding expression units. It was found that although the order of the functional categories follows Bybee’s iconicity principle, in that the valence affix is closest to the stem, while the aspect and modality markers are in the periphery, Bybee’s definition of terms is not entirely true of Yami and Philippine-type languages in general because there is a mixture of derivation and inflection in the Yami verbal system. The indicative focus affixes are ‘derivational’, but the non-indicative affixes are ‘inflectional’. Key words: tense, aspect, modality, Yami, iconicity 1. Introduction This study investigated the tense-aspect-modality (TAM) system in Yami, a Batanic language spoken by approximately 3000 speakers on Orchid Island. Previous research of TAM has been approached from three major perspectives: semantics (Comrie 1976), discourse (Schiffrin 1981, Hopper 1982, Myhill 1992), and cross-linguistic comparison (Bybee 1985).
    [Show full text]
  • Evidentiality and Mood: Grammatical Expressions of Epistemic Modality in Bulgarian
    Evidentiality and mood: Grammatical expressions of epistemic modality in Bulgarian DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements o the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Anastasia Smirnova, M.A. Graduate Program in Linguistics The Ohio State University 2011 Dissertation Committee: Brian Joseph, co-advisor Judith Tonhauser, co-advisor Craige Roberts Copyright by Anastasia Smirnova 2011 ABSTRACT This dissertation is a case study of two grammatical categories, evidentiality and mood. I argue that evidentiality and mood are grammatical expressions of epistemic modality and have an epistemic modal component as part of their meanings. While the empirical foundation for this work is data from Bulgarian, my analysis has a number of empirical and theoretical consequences for the previous work on evidentiality and mood in the formal semantics literature. Evidentiality is traditionally analyzed as a grammatical category that encodes information sources (Aikhenvald 2004). I show that the Bulgarian evidential has richer meaning: not only does it express information source, but also it has a temporal and a modal component. With respect to the information source, the Bulgarian evidential is compatible with a variety of evidential meanings, i.e. direct, inferential, and reportative, as long as the speaker has concrete perceivable evidence (as opposed to evidence based on a mental activity). With respect to epistemic commitment, the construction has different felicity conditions depending on the context: the speaker must be committed to the truth of the proposition in the scope of the evidential in a direct/inferential evidential context, but not in a reportative context.
    [Show full text]
  • Affirmation and Denial in Aristotle's De Interpretatione
    Topoi https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-019-09669-y Afrmation and Denial in Aristotle’s De interpretatione Mika Perälä1 © The Author(s) 2019 Abstract Modern logicians have complained that Aristotelian logic lacks a distinction between predication (including negation) and assertion, and that predication, according to the Aristotelians, implies assertion. The present paper addresses the question of whether this criticism can be levelled against Aristotle’s logic. Based on a careful study of the De interpretatione, the paper shows that even if Aristotle defnes what he calls simple assertion in terms of predication, he does not confound predication and assertion. That is because, frst, he does not understand compound assertion in terms of predication, and secondly, he acknowledges non-assertive predicative thoughts that are truth-evaluable. Therefore, the implications of Aristotle’s ‘predica- tion theory of assertion’ are not as devastating as the critics believe. Keywords Aristotle · Predication · Assertion · Afrmation · Denial · Negation 1 Introduction is white. It is not clear why he should make this assumption. Indeed, the assumption appears to be a confusion. Therefore, In the De interpretatione (Int.), Aristotle holds that afrma- it is not surprising that some modern logicians such as Gottlob tion (κατάφασις) and denial (ἀπόφασις) constitute two dif- Frege have complained that Aristotelian logic lacks an ade- ferent kinds of assertion (ἀπόφανσις) or assertive sentence quate distinction between predication (including negation) and (λόγος ἀποφαντικός). He conceptualizes the distinction in assertion.1 Even if this criticism is not directly raised against terms of combination (σύνθεσις) and separation, or division Aristotle’s logic, there are good grounds to raise doubts about (διαίρεσις).
    [Show full text]