The English Resultative

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The English Resultative The English resultative by Elizabeth Christie A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate and Postdoctoral Affairs in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Cognitive Science Carleton University Ottawa, Ontario ©2015 Elizabeth Christie Abstract The purpose of this thesis is to augment our empirical knowledge of the English resultative and to provide a theoretical treatment of the syntax and semantics of the resultative using current linguistic tools. This involves using both theoretical and experimental methods to ensure that the model reflects human usage of natural language and follows the principles required for formal language modelling. The main questions addressed are 1) Is the result phrase an argument, an ad- junct, or something else (an added/derived argument, sometimes called an argument- adjunct)? 2) How do we best capture the properties of the resultative in a formal model? In order to address these questions, it must be determined what falls into the overall category of the resultative and how the resultative can be sub-classified into further sub-categories to best reflect the potentially distinctive properties of related resultative constructions. These divisions are then tested for fit with the theoretical categories of argument, adjunct and added/derived argument. Lastly, an analysis is provided using Lexical-Functional Grammar and Glue Semantics. ii iii Acknowledgements The work of writing a thesis is never a singular effort. Many people become important partners in the project over the time it takes to complete, and my journey was no different. Without the tireless efforts of those around me, the work that I did would have just been one person sitting at a desk typing, and would never have become a complete project in the end. To begin with, I would like to thank the scholarships and funding bodies which made my PhD possible, including the 2010 President's Doctoral Fellowship, the Kevin Sampson Scholarship, and the Wargaret Wade Labarge Fund. Without their generous support, this project would have never begun, let alone come to fruition in the form that it is now. Next, I would like to thank the administrative team within my department: Colleen Fulton, May Hyde, Liane Dubreiul, Georgina Henderson, and, more recently, John Tracey. Your helpful knowledge and friendly smiles have made being in the department for the last five years much easier and more enjoyable. To the members of the Logic, Language and Information Lab: thank you for all of your helpful comments and feedback, as well as the opportunity to share my ideas in a small group environment. To my committee members, Kumiko Murasugi and John Logan, thank you for your support in designing and interpreting my emperiment. Without the many hours Kumiko dedicated to my study, I know I would still be sitting at my desk trying to decide how to get the type of information I needed from speakers. Of course, this thesis would not have happened without the support of my family and friends. Bryan, my loving husband, you picked me up when things got difficult, and helped me concentrate when my mind would wander. I do not know how I would iv have faced the sheer volume of effort this took if I had not had your unwavering support. To my mother, father and sister, you endlessly fielded questions about whether or not a given sentence was still considered \Englsih" or not, and you did so without thinking (or at least without saying) how weird my questions were becoming. You kept me grounded and never let me forget what the world was like outside of of academia. And, Deirdre, my best friend, you reminded me that my struggles were normal, that I could handle whatever life threw at me, and, most importantly, you always gave me the boost I needed to feel confident about myself and my ideas. Finally, and most important in the process of writing a thesis, my supervisors: Ash Asudeh and Ida Toivonen. Ash, your constant honesty and keen eye for the theoretical implications of my work kept me balanced and on track (no matter what speed I may, or may not, have been going). I could always count on your advice when things weren't at their best. Ida, although we didn't always see eye-to-eye on theory in our meetings, I always knew that you were there to support me and to make sure that I also enjoyed the social side of grad school. I know I could not have succeeded in completing my thesis without the two of you there to guide and support me. Contents Abstract . ii Acknowledgements . iii 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Defining the resultative . 2 1.2 Arguments and adjuncts . 5 1.3 A note about terminology . 7 1.4 Structure of the thesis . 8 2 Background 9 2.1 Semantics . 10 2.1.1 Pieces of the resultative . 10 2.1.2 Telicity and the resultative . 23 2.2 Syntax . 31 2.3 Conclusion . 37 3 The resultative and the argument/adjunct distinction 38 3.1 Introduction . 38 3.2 Tests for argumenthood . 42 v CONTENTS vi 3.2.1 Core participants (semantic selection) . 43 3.2.2 Syntactic obligatoriness . 44 3.2.3 VP preposing . 46 3.2.4 VP anaphora (do so)......................... 47 3.2.5 Pseudocleft . 48 3.2.6 Verb specificity . 50 3.2.7 Fixed preposition . 51 3.2.8 Prepositional content . 52 3.2.9 Uniqueness/iterativity . 54 3.3 Testing the result phrase . 55 3.3.1 Semantic distinctions . 56 3.3.2 Argument structure distinctions . 64 3.3.3 Lexical category distinctions . 72 3.4 Conclusion . 80 4 Experimental results 81 4.1 Introduction . 81 4.2 Experiment 1: Transitive resultatives . 83 4.2.1 Methods . 83 4.2.2 Results . 86 4.2.3 Discussion of experiment 1 . 88 4.3 Experiment 2: Unaccusative resultatives . 89 4.3.1 Methods . 89 4.3.2 Results . 91 4.3.3 Discussion of experiment 2 . 92 CONTENTS vii 4.4 Experiment 3: Unergative resultatives . 93 4.4.1 Methods . 93 4.4.2 Results . 95 4.4.3 Discussion of experiment 3 . 97 4.5 General discussion . 97 5 Analysis 100 5.1 Introduction . 100 5.2 Proposed resultative construction . 100 5.3 Resultative templates . 104 5.3.1 Resultative c- and f-Structures . 105 5.3.2 Target semantics for the resultative . 113 5.3.3 Proposed templates . 115 5.4 Walking through the proofs . 124 5.4.1 Transitive property resultative . 124 5.4.2 Unaccusative path resultative . 134 5.4.3 Fake reflexive property resultative . 141 5.5 Conclusions . 150 6 Conclusions and future work 152 6.1 Conclusions . 152 6.2 Future work . 154 Appendices 156 A Stimuli for experiment 1: Transitive resultatives 157 CONTENTS viii B Stimuli for experiment 2: Unaccusative resultatives 165 C Stimuli for experiment 3: Unergative resultatives 169 D Participant Means and Standard Deviations 172 List of Tables 2.1 Event Classes . 25 3.1 Argument/adjunct test results by semantic type . 65 3.2 Argument/adjunct test results by argument structure type . 72 3.3 Argument/adjunct test results by lexical category of result phrase . 80 4.1 Single stimulus item for experiment 1 . 85 4.2 Single stimulus item for experiment 2 . 90 4.3 Single stimulus item for experiment 3 . 94 D.1 Participant means and standard deviations per condition for experi- ment 1 . 173 D.2 Participant means and standard deviations per condition for experi- ment 2 . 174 D.3 Participant means and standard deviations per condition for experi- ment 3 . 175 ix List of Figures 4.1 Mean response by result phrase type (Expt 1) . 86 4.2 Mean response by target phrase location (Expt 1) . 88 4.3 Mean responses by result phrase type (Expt 2) . 91 4.4 Mean response by target phrase location (Expt 2) . 93 4.5 Mean response by result phrase type (Expt 3) . 96 5.1 LFG templates for the resultative . 116 5.2 Logical proof for the sentence Kim hammered the metal flat . 133 5.3 Logical proof for the sentence The syrup flowed down the tubes . 140 5.4 Logical proof for the sentence Kim laughed herself silly . 148 x Chapter 1 Introduction Sentences like (1) are referred to as resultatives: (1) Kim hammered the metal flat. In (1), Kim hammers the metal, and as a result, the metal becomes flat. This inter- pretation is what gives the construction its name: the final phrase of the sentence describes the result of the action of the sentence. Resultatives have many interesting characteristics and have thus received consid- erable attention in the literature for nearly fifty years of syntactic research. However, there is still disagreement about the exact specifications a resultative sentence must meet to be considered part of the construction. This includes which phrase types are possible in the final phrase (the result phrase); what the formal semantic interpreta- tion of a resultative sentence should be; and what kinds of result to consider. The purpose of this thesis is to augment our empirical knowledge of the English resultative and to address the question of how to treat the resultative in a modern grammatical framework. This will include both theoretical and experimental methods 1 1.1. DEFINING THE RESULTATIVE 2 to ensure that my model reflects human usage of natural language and follows the principles required for formal language modelling. The questions to be addressed are 1) Is the result phrase an argument, an adjunct, or something else? 2) How do we accurately capture the properties of the resultative in a formal model? In order to address these questions, I will first try to determine what falls into the category of the resultative and how it can be divided to best reflect the potentially distinctive properties.
Recommended publications
  • Sign Language Typology Series
    SIGN LANGUAGE TYPOLOGY SERIES The Sign Language Typology Series is dedicated to the comparative study of sign languages around the world. Individual or collective works that systematically explore typological variation across sign languages are the focus of this series, with particular emphasis on undocumented, underdescribed and endangered sign languages. The scope of the series primarily includes cross-linguistic studies of grammatical domains across a larger or smaller sample of sign languages, but also encompasses the study of individual sign languages from a typological perspective and comparison between signed and spoken languages in terms of language modality, as well as theoretical and methodological contributions to sign language typology. Interrogative and Negative Constructions in Sign Languages Edited by Ulrike Zeshan Sign Language Typology Series No. 1 / Interrogative and negative constructions in sign languages / Ulrike Zeshan (ed.) / Nijmegen: Ishara Press 2006. ISBN-10: 90-8656-001-6 ISBN-13: 978-90-8656-001-1 © Ishara Press Stichting DEF Wundtlaan 1 6525XD Nijmegen The Netherlands Fax: +31-24-3521213 email: [email protected] http://ishara.def-intl.org Cover design: Sibaji Panda Printed in the Netherlands First published 2006 Catalogue copy of this book available at Depot van Nederlandse Publicaties, Koninklijke Bibliotheek, Den Haag (www.kb.nl/depot) To the deaf pioneers in developing countries who have inspired all my work Contents Preface........................................................................................................10
    [Show full text]
  • Perfect-Traj.Pdf
    Aspect shifts in Indo-Aryan and trajectories of semantic change1 Cleo Condoravdi and Ashwini Deo Abstract: The grammaticalization literature notes the cross-linguistic robustness of a diachronic pat- tern involving the aspectual categories resultative, perfect, and perfective. Resultative aspect markers often develop into perfect markers, which then end up as perfect plus perfective markers. We intro- duce supporting data from the history of Old and Middle Indo-Aryan languages, whose instantiation of this pattern has not been previously noted. We provide a semantic analysis of the resultative, the perfect, and the aspectual category that combines perfect and perfective. Our analysis reveals the change to be a two-step generalization (semantic weakening) from the original resultative meaning. 1 Introduction The emergence of new functional expressions and the changes in their distribution and interpretation over time have been shown to be systematic across languages, as well as across a variety of semantic domains. These observations have led scholars to construe such changes in terms of “clines”, or predetermined trajectories, along which expressions move in time. Specifically, the typological literature, based on large-scale grammaticaliza- tion studies, has discovered several such trajectorial shifts in the domain of tense, aspect, and modality. Three properties characterize such shifts: (a) the categories involved are sta- ble across cross-linguistic instantiations; (b) the paths of change are unidirectional; (c) the shifts are uniformly generalizing (Heine, Claudi, and H¨unnemeyer 1991; Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca 1994; Haspelmath 1999; Dahl 2000; Traugott and Dasher 2002; Hopper and Traugott 2003; Kiparsky 2012). A well-known trajectory is the one in (1).
    [Show full text]
  • The Aorist in Modern Armenian: Core Value and Contextual Meanings Anaid Donabedian-Demopoulos
    The aorist in Modern Armenian: core value and contextual meanings Anaid Donabedian-Demopoulos To cite this version: Anaid Donabedian-Demopoulos. The aorist in Modern Armenian: core value and contextual mean- ings. Zlatka Guentcheva. Aspectuality and Temporality. Descriptive and theoretical issues, John Benjamins, pp.375 - 412, 2016, 9789027267610. 10.1075/slcs.172.12don. halshs-01424956 HAL Id: halshs-01424956 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-01424956 Submitted on 6 Jan 2017 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. The Aorist in Modern Armenian: core value and contextual meanings, in Guentchéva, Zlatka (ed.), Aspectuality and Temporality. Descriptive and theoretical issues, John Benjamins, 2016, p. 375-411 (the published paper miss examples written in Armenian) The aorist in Modern Armenian: core values and contextual meanings Anaïd Donabédian (SeDyL, INALCO/USPC, CNRS UMR8202, IRD UMR135) Introduction Comparison between particular markers in different languages is always controversial, nevertheless linguists can identify in numerous languages a verb tense that can be described as aorist. Cross-linguistic differences exist, due to the diachrony of the markers in question and their position within the verbal system of a given language, but there are clearly a certain number of shared morphological, syntactic, semantic and/or pragmatic features.
    [Show full text]
  • Mathias Jenny: the Verb System of Mon
    Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2005 The verb system of Mon Jenny, Mathias Posted at the Zurich Open Repository and Archive, University of Zurich ZORA URL: https://doi.org/10.5167/uzh-110202 Dissertation Originally published at: Jenny, Mathias. The verb system of Mon. 2005, University of Zurich, Faculty of Arts. Mathias Jenny: The Verb System of Mon Contents Preface v Abbreviations viii 1. Introduction 1 1.1 The Mon people and language 1 1.2 Sources and methodology 8 1.3 Sentence structure 13 1.4 Sentence types 15 1.4.1 Interrogative sentences 15 1.4.2 Imperative sentences 18 1.4.3 Conditional clauses 20 1.5 Phonology of Mon 23 1.5.1 Historical overview 23 1.5.2 From Middle Mon to Modern Mon 26 1.5.3 Modern dialects 30 1.5.4 The phonology of SM 33 1.5.5 Phonology and the writing system 37 2. Verbs in Mon 42 2.1 What are verbs? 42 2.2 The predicate in Mon 43 2.2.1 Previous studies of the verb in Mon 44 2.2.2 Verbs as word class in Mon 47 2.2.3 Nominal predicates and topicalised verbs 49 2.3 Negation 51 2.3.1 Negated statements and questions 51 2.3.2 Prohibitive 58 2.3.3 Summary 59 2.4 Verbal morphology 60 2.4.1 Historical development 60 2.4.2 Modern Mon 65 2.4.3 Summary 67 2.5 Verb classes and types of verbs 67 2.5.1 Transitive and intransitive verbs 68 2.5.2 Aktionsarten 71 2.5.3 Resultative verb compounds (RVC) 75 2.5.4 Existential verbs (Copulas) 77 2.5.4.1 ‹dah› th ‘be something’ 78 2.5.4.2 ‹måï› mŋ ‘be at, stay’ 80 2.5.4.3 ‹nwaÿ› nùm ‘be at, exist, have’ 82 2.5.4.4 ‹(hwa’) seï› (hùʔ) siəŋ ‘(not to) be so’ 86 i Mathias Jenny: The Verb System of Mon 2.5.5 Directionals 88 2.6 Complements of verbs 89 2.6.1 Direct objects 90 2.6.2 Oblique objects 91 2.6.3 Complement clauses 95 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Ter + Past Participle Or Estar + Gerund? Aspect and Syntactic Variation in Brazilian Portuguese
    Ter + past participle or Estar + gerund? Aspect and Syntactic Variation in Brazilian Portuguese Ronald Beline Mendes University of São Paulo The verbal aspect in Brazilian Portuguese (BP) has been described by a number of authors. Wachowicz 2003, based on Verkuyl 1993 and 1999, has accounted for the iterative and progressive interpretations of estar (be) + gerund. Ilari 2000 shows why ter (have) + participle is recognized as the iterative periphrasis in that language, even though it can possibly express progressive, durative meanings. Castilho 2000 proposes an aspectual typology for the aspect and shows how periphrastic constructions are productive in BP aspectual composition. None of those authors have, however, approached the use of those periphrases from a variationist, quantitative perspective. In contemporary BP, estar + gerund and ter + participle actually appear in the same context when the aspectual interpretation is iterative, and are commonly found in the data provided by a same speaker. That is a synchronic evidence of their variable use, as shown below: (1) Eu estou estudando muito ultimamente. I am studying much lately "I have been studying a lot lately" (iterative) (2) Eu tenho estudado muito ultimamente. I have studied much lately "I have been studying a lot lately" (iterative) Diachronic data show that their uses changed. In the 16th century, neither of the periphrases were used to express iterative - estar + gerund could only express durative, progressive meanings, whereas ter + participle would convey a resultative, punctual meaning, as in (3) and (4) respectively. In the 17th and 18th centuries, some tokens of ter + participle are found to be ambiguous, the iterative and resultative being possible.
    [Show full text]
  • Mandarin Resultative Constructions and Aspect Incorporation Chienjer Lin University of Arizona
    Aspect is Result: Mandarin Resultative Constructions and Aspect Incorporation Chienjer Lin University of Arizona 0 Introduction Aspect and resultative constructions have been treated as distinct phenomena in the linguistic literature. Viewpoint aspect encodes ways of viewing the “internal temporal constituency of a situation;” in particular, it provides information about completion and boundedness that is superimposed on verb phrases (Comrie 1976:3). It takes place at a functional position above the verb phrase. A sentence with simple past tense like (1) describes an action that occurred prior to the speech time. The perfective aspect phrase have V in (2) emphasizes the completion and experience of this action. Aspect is therefore seen as additional information that modifies the telicity of a verb phrase. (1) I saw him in the park. (2) I have seen him in the park twice. Resultatives on the other hand are taken as information within the verb phrase. They typically involve at least two events—a causative event followed by a resultative state, which is denoted by a small clause embedded under the main verb (Hoekstra 1989). For instance, the verb phrase wipe the slate clean involves the action of wiping and the final state of the slate’s being clean. The resultative state is embedded under the verb phrase, governed by the causative verb head. This paper explores the possibility of an alternative account for viewpoint aspect. Instead of viewing aspect as a functional head above verb phrases, I attempt to associate aspect with resultative predicates and claim that (at least part of) aspect should be base generated below the verb phrases like resultatives.
    [Show full text]
  • Building Resultatives Angelika Kratzer, University of Massachusetts at Amherst August 20041
    1 Building Resultatives Angelika Kratzer, University of Massachusetts at Amherst August 20041 1. The construction: Adjectival resultatives Some natural languages allow their speakers to put together a verb and an adjective to create complex predicates that are commonly referred to as “resultatives”. Here are two run-of-the- mill examples from German2: (1) Die Teekanne leer trinken The teapot empty drink ‘To drink the teapot empty’ (2) Die Tulpen platt giessen The tulips flat water Water the tulips flat Resultatives raise important questions for the syntax-semantics interface, and this is why they have occupied a prominent place in recent linguistic theorizing. What is it that makes this construction so interesting? Resultatives are submitted to a cluster of not obviously related constraints, and this fact calls out for explanation. There are tough constraints for the verb, for example: 1 . To appear in Claudia Maienborn und Angelika Wöllstein-Leisten (eds.): Event Arguments in Syntax, Semantics, and Discourse. Tübingen, Niemeyer. Thank you to Claudia Maienborn and Angelika Wöllstein-Leisten for organizing the stimulating DGfS workshop where this paper was presented, and for waiting so patiently for its final write-up. Claudia Maienborn also sent comments on an earlier version of the paper, which were highly appreciated. 2 . (2) is modeled after a famous example in Carrier & Randall (1992). 2 (3) a. Er hat seine Familie magenkrank gekocht. He has his family stomach sick cooked. ‘He cooked his family stomach sick.’ b. * Er hat seine Familie magenkrank bekocht. He has his family stomach sick cooked-for And there are also well-known restrictions for the participating adjectives (Simpson 1983, Smith 1983, Fabb 1984, Carrier and Randall 1992): (4) a.
    [Show full text]
  • Types of Predicate
    chapter 9 Types of Predicate This chapter deals with the types of predicates. The predicate of a sentence can be expressed by almost any word classes, not only through a verb. How- ever, verbal predicates are most common, about 87% of all predicates are verbs. They can be simple, containing only one verb or complex with more than one verbal element. The following figure shows the occurrence of predicates in an annotated sub-corpus. In the following sections, first the verbal predicates are discussed. 1 Verbal Predication The most common distinction between verbal predicates is their simplicity. The verbal predicate usually is simple. Complex predicates consist of an aux- iliary and a lexical verb. There are only few auxiliary verbs, one of these aux- iliaries is the negation auxiliary, which serves to form standard negation and appears most often in complex verbal predicates. Other auxiliaries are seman- tically not empty, but they have an additional, mostly modal meaning. In this section, only examples for the usage of auxiliary konɨďi ‘go away’ are given. For a detailed description of auxiliaries see Section 3.3 in Chapter 3, for negation see Chapter 12. The auxiliary verb konɨďi ‘go away’ appears only in essive or resultative- translative constructions. In these constructions an NP is followed by the infini- tive form (converb1) of the BE-verb (isʲa) and the conjugated form of the aux- iliary. Word order variations are not allowed. Contrary to Enets and Nenets, the infinitive/converb construction wasn’t grammaticalized as the essive case. This form has word stress of its own, and it never undergoes cliticization.
    [Show full text]
  • Resultative and Depictive Constructions in English*
    Resultative and Depictive Constructions in English* Chang-Su Lee Department ofEnglish Language Education I. Introduction The following illustrates two types of secondary predication, the resultative and the depictive construction: (1) a. John hammered the metal flat. (Resultative Construction) b. John ate the meat raw. (Depietive Construction) In (la), the adjective flat, called a resultative, a result phrase, or a resultative predicate, describes the final state of the object NP which results from the action or process denoted by the verb. Thus (la) means "John caused the metal to become flat by hammering it." On the other hand, in (lb) the adjective raw, called a depictive, a depictive phrase, or a depictive predicate, characterizes the state of an NP at the time of the initiation of the main predicate's action." So the sentence (lb) has the meaning "John ate the meat, and at the time he ate it, it was raw." Many ltngutsts have assumed that the resultative and the depictive construction are not syntactically different despite their difference in semantic interpretation (cf. Rapoport, 1990). In this paper, however, we propose that the resultative and the depietive construction involve two different types of syntactic configuration. *This paper is the summarized version of Lee's Ph.D. Thesis. 1) "Resultative" and "depletive" may be used to indicate a resultative and a depictive sentence. respectively. 56 THE SNU JOURNAL OF EDUCATION RESEARCH II. Syntactic Status of Resultatives and Depictives In the literature, it is generally assumed that resultatives and depictives are predicates (Hoekstra, 1984, 1988; Rothstein, 1985; Rapoport, 1990; Napoli, 1992). Let's consider the following examples: (2) a.
    [Show full text]
  • Auxiliary Verbs in the Left Periphery: Spanish Ir and Venir As Focus Markers
    Auxiliary Verbs in the Left Periphery: Spanish ir and venir as Focus Markers Ana Bravo ∗ 1. Spanish standard and non-standard constructions with ir ‘to go’ + infinitive∗∗ Apart from the well known and fully acknowledged temporal-aspectual meaning of either futurity or prospectivity, the construction1 with the auxiliary verb ir ‘to go’ [ir a + infinitive] may be used to convey the two less standard meanings of resultativity and focus, as exemplified in (1) and (2). It must be observed that they are common to all varieties of Spanish, although it seems that in some of these varieties venir subtitutes ir as a focus marker (2c):2 (1) RESULTATIVE IR a. El hombre […] cerró los ojos, trastabilló y fue a golpearse con [… ] la campana. [CREA, Vicente Molina Foix, La quincena soviética, Spain] 3 ‘The man closed his eyes, stumbled and ended up hitting himself against the bell.’ b. El vehículo se salió de la cinta asfáltica y fue a chocar con el puente Camella. [CREA, La Hora, 30/04/1997, Guatemala] ‘The vehicle went off of the road and, at the end, it crashed into Camella Bridge.’ (2) FOCUS IR ~ VENIR a. Fíjese que el güey del Aristóteles […] le fue a poner a su hijo mayor Cristo Rey. ¡Pobre cuate! [CREA, Arturo Azuela, La casa de las mil vírgenes, México] ‘That guy of Aristoteles had to give his older son the name of Cristo Rey, as if there were no more names. ¡Poor guy!’ b. Fue a aparecer en el peor momento. [RAE-ASALE, § 28.8j] ‘He had to appear at the worst moment.’ c.
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparative Study on Mandarin and Cantonese Resultative Verb Compounds
    PACLIC 29 A Comparative Study on Mandarin and Cantonese Resultative Verb Compounds Helena Yan Ping Lau Sophia Yat Mei Lee Department of Chinese and Bilingual Studies Department of Chinese and Bilingual Studies The Hong Kong Polytechnic University The Hong Kong Polytechnic University Hong Kong Hong Kong [email protected] [email protected] Shi 2002)1. As one of the main varieties of Chinese, Abstract Cantonese seems to be closely-related to Mandarin. There are, however, some remarkable differences This paper explores the conditions where Mandarin RVCs can be preserved in their between them in terms of the usage. An example of Cantonese counterparts. Six types of Mandarin Mandarin RVC construction is shown in (1), with a RVCs – ergatives, unergatives, accusatives, syntactically parallel yet ill-formed sentence in causatives, pseudo-passives and object- Cantonese illustrated in (2). fronting – have been examined. Modifications have been made for certain kinds of RVCs that (1) 我 跑丟-了 車票 are usually misclassified. The analysis has been 1.SG run lost-ASP ticket done at both the lexical and syntactic levels. At the lexical level, the concept of ‘strong ‘I lost the ticket as I ran.’ resultative’ and ‘weak resultative’ has been adduced to support the idea that indirect (2) *我 跑跌-咗 張 車飛 causation cannot be expressed by RVC in 1.SG run lost-ASP CL ticket Cantonese. At the syntactic level, the presentations of the same RVCs falling into Since RVCs in Cantonese are found to be less different sentence types are illustrated. Since productive than they are in Mandarin, most of the the structure of Mandarin RVCs are often previous works have been dedicated to the study of restricted in Cantonese, three substitutive constructions have been introduced for Mandarin Chinese, neglecting numerous concerns presenting the same resultatives in Cantonese.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Tsezian Languages Bernard Comrie, Maria Polinsky, and Ramazan
    Tsezian Languages Bernard Comrie, Maria Polinsky, and Ramazan Rajabov Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany University of California, San Diego, USA Makhachkala, Russia 1. Sociolinguistic Situation1 The Tsezian (Tsezic, Didoic) languages form part of the Daghestanian branch of the Nakh-Daghestanian (East Caucasian) language family. They form one branch of an Avar-Andi-Tsez grouping within the family, the other branch of this grouping being Avar-Andi. Five Tsezian languages are conventionally recognized: Khwarshi (Avar x∑arßi, Khwarshi a¥’ilqo), Tsez (Avar, Tsez cez, also known by the Georgian name Dido), Hinuq (Avar, Hinuq hinuq), Bezhta (Avar beΩt’a, Bezhta beΩ¥’a. also known by the Georgian name Kapuch(i)), and Hunzib (Avar, Hunzib hunzib), although the Inkhokwari (Avar inxoq’∑ari, Khwarshi iqqo) dialect of Khwarshi and the Sagada (Avar sahada, Tsez so¥’o) dialect of Tsez are highly divergent. Tsez, Hinuq, Bezhta, and Hunzib are spoken primarily in the Tsunta district of western Daghestan, while Khwarshi is spoken primarily to the north in the adjacent Tsumada district, separated from the other Tsezian languages by high mountains. (See map 1.) In addition, speakers of Tsezian languages are also to be found as migrants to lowland Daghestan, occasionally in other parts of Russia and in Georgia. Estimates of the number of speakers are given by van den Berg (1995) as follows, for 1992: Tsez 14,000 (including 6,500 in the lowlands); Bezhta 7,000 (including 2,500 in the lowlands); Hunzib 2,000 (including 1,300 in the lowlands); Hinuq 500; Khwarshi 1,500 (including 600 in the lowlands).
    [Show full text]