How Might Pragmatic Language Skills Affect the Written Expression of Students with Language Learning Disabilities?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

How Might Pragmatic Language Skills Affect the Written Expression of Students with Language Learning Disabilities? LWW/TLD TLD200065 January 27, 2011 15:40 Char Count= 0 Top Lang Disorders Vol. 31, No. 1, pp. 40–53 Copyright c 2011 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins How Might Pragmatic Language Skills Affect the Written Expression of Students with Language Learning Disabilities? Gary A. Troia, PhD This article describes ways in which pragmatic language abilities may play a role in the writing per- formance of children and adolescents with language learning disabilities. First, a brief overview is presented of how pragmatic language difficulties might negatively influence writing performance. Next, research on the writing performance of students with language and learning problems is reviewed. Key components based on these reviews are incorporated into a sociocognitive model of writing in which pragmatic language abilities are highlighted and illustrated as they apply to writing poetry. Finally, some suggestions are offered for scholars and practitioners to encourage attention to pragmatic issues in the writing of children and adolescents. Key words: children and adolescents, language impairment, learning disabilities, pragmatics, writing RITTEN EXPRESSION can be a de- Research over the past three decades has Wmanding form of communication for provided substantial insight into the writ- children and youth with speech/language im- ing problems experienced by children and pairments and learning disabilities. These two adolescents with disabilities, both in terms categories represent nearly 68% of all indi- of the nature of their difficulties as well viduals between the ages of 6 and 21 years as the underlying causes (see Silliman & served under the Individuals with Disabili- Berninger, 2011). There have been many ef- ties Education Act (U.S. Department of Educa- forts to identify the cognitive and linguistic tion, 2003). For a variety of reasons, writing underpinnings of literacy problems in these is an area of difficulty for many children with populations, or more broadly, children and disabilities, and problems with pragmatic as- adolescents with language learning disabil- pects of written language, which are the focus ities (LLD), but these have largely focused of this article, deserve attention but often are on the domains of memory, executive con- overlooked. trol, phonology, morphosyntax, and seman- tics (e.g., Bishop, North, & Donlan, 1996; Bourke & Adams, 2010; Catts, Fey, Zhang, & Tomblin, 1999, 2001; Dockrell, Lindsay, Connelly, & Mackie, 2007; Roth, Speece, & Author Affiliation: Department of Counseling, Cooper, 2002; Stothard, Snowling, Bishop, Educational Psychology and Special Education, Chipchase, & Kaplan, 1998). Many individ- Michigan State University, East Lansing uals with LLD also experience difficulties Corresponding Author: Gary A. Troia, PhD, Depart- in the realm of pragmatics (e.g., Bishop ment of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Spe- cial Education, Michigan State University, East Lansing, & Adams, 1991, 1992; Bishop & Norbury, MI 48824 ([email protected].) 2002; Leonard, 1998), possibly due, in part, DOI: 10.1097/TLD.0b013e31820a0b71 to deficits in right hemisphere processing 40 Copyright © 2011 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited. LWW/TLD TLD200065 January 27, 2011 15:40 Char Count= 0 Pragmatic, Written Expression, and Students with LLD 41 (Shields, 1991). Thus, it should prove fruit- teraction and adjustment are not well under- ful to explore how pragmatics, the use of lan- stood. It is not known to what extent deficient guage in social contexts to fulfill communica- social skills related to pragmatic language dif- tive purposes and regulate social interactions ficulties, as well as the pragmatic difficulties (Bates, 1976; Prutting, 1982), and difficulties themselves, influence written expression. in this domain, may be manifest in written ex- pression as well. PRAGMATICS, WRITTEN LANGUAGE, Pragmatic difficulties can be expected to AND STUDENTS WITH DISABILITIES have a negative impact on students’ social and emotional well-being (Schalock, 1996). Written expression, in most circumstances, For instance, individuals with LLD tend to par- may be viewed as a form of communication ticipate in fewer peer interactions and are for social purposes aimed at achieving social- less preferred communication partners (e.g., interaction goals. Although currently it is un- Cartledge, Frew, & Zaharias, 1985; Fujiki, Brin- clear how the social adjustment problems of ton, Robinson, & Watson, 1997; Gertner, Rice, children with LLD interact with writing de- & Hadley, 1994; Hadley & Rice, 1991; Juvo- mands in the classroom, pragmatic difficulties nen & Bear, 1992; Pearl et al., 1998; Rice, can be expected to limit the development of Sell, & Hadley, 1991; Weiner & Schneider, written expression for these children through 2002). Consequently, they have difficulty es- their disruption on participation in social lit- tablishing and maintaining friendships. More- eracy practices as well as their direct impact over, these individuals exhibit a greater in- on the use of language forms and functions. cidence of problem behaviors, most often Aspects of at least three fundamental compo- internalizing behaviors (e.g., Cohen, 1986; nents of pragmatics may be involved: presup- Fujiki, Brinton, Morgan, & Hart, 1999; Fu- position abilities, discourse regulation, and jiki, Brinton, & Todd, 1996; Redmond & nonliteral (figurative) language usage. Rice, 1998). Children and adolescents with Pragmatic difficulties likely have a pro- LLD also perceive themselves as socially less nounced impact on writing performance adept than their peers (e.g., Jerome, Fujiki, when perspective taking is at a premium, Brinton, & James, 2002). Overall, these stu- such as when tailoring a written text for a dents display fewer sociable behaviors, such specific audience. Perspective-taking relies on as sharing, cooperation, and offering com- at least one particular aspect of pragmatic fort, which are strongly related to positive functioning—presupposition—which is di- peer relationships (e.g., Bender & Wall, 1994; rectly related to social cognition and theory Brinton & Fujiki, 2004; Hart, Robinson, Mc- of mind, both of which involve making infer- Neilly, Nelson, & Olsen, 1995; Kavale & For- ences regarding the actions, beliefs, and inten- ness, 1996; Ladd & Price, 1987; Pearl et tions of others and to adapt based on those in- al., 1998; Vaughn, Hogan, Kouzekanani, & ferences (Carruthers & Smith, 1996; McTear Shapiro, 1990). These attributes are thought & Conti-Ramsden, 1991). In other words, to be the result of difficulties perceiving and how much a student presupposes about a interpreting social cues, which are frequently reader’s prior knowledge in written text is linguistically coded (Pearl, Donahue, & Bryan, predicated on his or her inferences about 1986; Weiner, 1980, 2004). However, the what the reader believes, knows, and wants. severity of language difficulty does not nec- Children with LLD tend either to presuppose essarily predict social competence, and when too much shared knowledge between them- the linguistic demands of social contexts are selves and their readers, rendering communi- minimized, children with LLD may continue cation ineffective because the reader is left to exhibit less sociable behaviors (Brinton & to ponder what has been unstated, or to Fujiki, 2004). Thus, the causal mechanisms be- presuppose too little shared knowledge, ren- tween specific pragmatic skills and social in- dering a text cumbersome and laden with Copyright © 2011 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited. LWW/TLD TLD200065 January 27, 2011 15:40 Char Count= 0 42 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JANUARY–MARCH 2011 unnecessary information. Likewise, children tion. Consequently, and because the reader’s with LLD may be unskilled at employing the response to written text is often not available Gricean conversational maxims of truthful- during the production of that text, students ness, relevance, informativeness, and direct- with LLD are likely to produce short pieces of ness (Grice, 1975), as required by written writing bereft of detail and lacking polished task demands, audience needs, and personal organization that fail to adequately attend to rhetorical goals (e.g., Bliss, 1992; Noel, 1980; genre conventions and reader needs. Spekman, 1981). For instance, a student with A final aspect of pragmatics with which LLD might erroneously present personal opin- children with LLD may struggle is figurative ions as facts to support an argument in an language. Figurative language comprehension opinion essay or fail to use polite forms when and use is important for communication suc- making a request in writing. A student with cess because nearly two thirds of spoken En- LLD also may have difficulty correctly using glish is figurative in nature (Arnold & Hornett, deictic terms that mark noun relationships 1990). In addition, approximately a third of (e.g., I versus you, a vs. the), spatial relation- teachers’ utterances contain multiple mean- ships (e.g., here vs. there, this vs. that), and ing words (the foundation of figurative ex- temporal relationships (e.g., before vs. after, pressions) or idiomatic expressions and about now vs. then) because of poor presupposition 7% of reading materials used in elementary (e.g., Bishop, 1997). schools contains idioms (Lazar, Warr-Leeper, Children and adolescents with LLD fre- Beel-Nicholson, & Johnson, 1989). Figurative quently exhibit
Recommended publications
  • The Empirical Base of Linguistics: Grammaticality Judgments and Linguistic Methodology
    UCLA UCLA Previously Published Works Title The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/05b2s4wg ISBN 978-3946234043 Author Schütze, Carson T Publication Date 2016-02-01 DOI 10.17169/langsci.b89.101 Data Availability The data associated with this publication are managed by: Language Science Press, Berlin Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California The empirical base of linguistics Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology Carson T. Schütze language Classics in Linguistics 2 science press Classics in Linguistics Chief Editors: Martin Haspelmath, Stefan Müller In this series: 1. Lehmann, Christian. Thoughts on grammaticalization 2. Schütze, Carson T. The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology 3. Bickerton, Derek. Roots of language ISSN: 2366-374X The empirical base of linguistics Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology Carson T. Schütze language science press Carson T. Schütze. 2019. The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology (Classics in Linguistics 2). Berlin: Language Science Press. This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/89 © 2019, Carson T. Schütze Published under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Licence (CC BY 4.0): http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ISBN: 978-3-946234-02-9 (Digital) 978-3-946234-03-6 (Hardcover) 978-3-946234-04-3 (Softcover) 978-1-523743-32-2
    [Show full text]
  • CAS LX 522 Syntax I
    It is likely… CAS LX 522 IP This satisfies the EPP in Syntax I both clauses. The main DPj I′ clause has Mary in SpecIP. Mary The embedded clause has Vi+I VP is the trace in SpecIP. V AP Week 14b. PRO and control ti This specific instance of A- A IP movement, where we move a likely subject from an embedded DP I′ clause to a higher clause is tj generally called subject raising. I VP to leave Reluctance to leave Reluctance to leave Now, consider: Reluctant has two θ-roles to assign. Mary is reluctant to leave. One to the one feeling the reluctance (Experiencer) One to the proposition about which the reluctance holds (Proposition) This looks very similar to Mary is likely to leave. Can we draw the same kind of tree for it? Leave has one θ-role to assign. To the one doing the leaving (Agent). How many θ-roles does reluctant assign? In Mary is reluctant to leave, what θ-role does Mary get? IP Reluctance to leave Reluctance… DPi I′ Mary Vj+I VP In Mary is reluctant to leave, is V AP Mary is doing the leaving, gets Agent t Mary is reluctant to leave. j t from leave. i A′ Reluctant assigns its θ- Mary is showing the reluctance, gets θ roles within AP as A θ IP Experiencer from reluctant. required, Mary moves reluctant up to SpecIP in the main I′ clause by Spellout. ? And we have a problem: I vP But what gets the θ-role to Mary appears to be getting two θ-roles, from leave, and what v′ in violation of the θ-criterion.
    [Show full text]
  • Greek and Latin Roots, Prefixes, and Suffixes
    GREEK AND LATIN ROOTS, PREFIXES, AND SUFFIXES This is a resource pack that I put together for myself to teach roots, prefixes, and suffixes as part of a separate vocabulary class (short weekly sessions). It is a combination of helpful resources that I have found on the web as well as some tips of my own (such as the simple lesson plan). Lesson Plan Ideas ........................................................................................................... 3 Simple Lesson Plan for Word Study: ........................................................................... 3 Lesson Plan Idea 2 ...................................................................................................... 3 Background Information .................................................................................................. 5 Why Study Word Roots, Prefixes, and Suffixes? ......................................................... 6 Latin and Greek Word Elements .............................................................................. 6 Latin Roots, Prefixes, and Suffixes .......................................................................... 6 Root, Prefix, and Suffix Lists ........................................................................................... 8 List 1: MEGA root list ................................................................................................... 9 List 2: Roots, Prefixes, and Suffixes .......................................................................... 32 List 3: Prefix List ......................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Language Structure: Phrases “Productivity” a Property of Language • Definition – Language Is an Open System
    Language Structure: Phrases “Productivity” a property of Language • Definition – Language is an open system. We can produce potentially an infinite number of different messages by combining elements differently. • Example – Words into phrases. An Example of Productivity • Human language is a communication system that bears some similarities to other animal communication systems, but is also characterized by certain unique features. (24 words) • I think that human language is a communication system that bears some similarities to other animal communication systems, but is also characterized by certain unique features, which are fascinating in and of themselves. (33 words) • I have always thought, and I have spent many years verifying, that human language is a communication system that bears some similarities to other animal communication systems, but is also characterized by certain unique features, which are fascinating in and of themselves. (42 words) • Although mainstream some people might not agree with me, I have always thought… Creating Infinite Messages • Discrete elements – Words, Phrases • Selection – Ease, Meaning, Identity • Combination – Rules of organization Models of Word reCombination 1. Word chains (Markov model) Phrase-level meaning is derived from understanding each word as it is presented in the context of immediately adjacent words. 2. Hierarchical model There are long-distant dependencies between words in a phrase, and these inform the meaning of the entire phrase. Markov Model Rule: Select and concatenate (according to meaning and what types of words should occur next to each other). bites bites bites Man over over over jumps jumps jumps house house house Markov Model • Assumption −Only adjacent words are meaningfully (and lawfully) related.
    [Show full text]
  • 24.902F15 Class 13 Pro Versus
    Unpronounced subjects 1 We have seen PRO, the subject of infinitives and gerunds: 1. I want [PRO to dance in the park] 2. [PRO dancing in the park] was a good idea 2 There are languages that have unpronounced subjects in tensed clauses. Obviously, English is not among those languages: 3. irθe (Greek) came.3sg ‘he came’ or ‘she came’ 4. *came Languages like Greek, which permit this phenomenon are called “pro-drop” languages. 3 What is the status of this unpronounced subject in pro-drop languages? Is it PRO or something else? Something with the same or different properties from PRO? Let’s follow the convention of calling the unpronounced subject in tensed clauses “little pro”, as opposed to “big PRO”. What are the differences between pro and PRO? 4 -A Nominative NP can appear instead of pro. Not so for PRO: 5. i Katerina irθe the Katerina came.3sg ‘Katerina came’ 6. *I hope he/I to come 7. *he to read this book would be great 5 -pro can refer to any individual as long as Binding Condition B is respected. That is, pro is not “controlled”. Not so for OC PRO. 8. i Katerinak nomizi oti prok/m irθe stin ora tis the K thinks that came on-the time her ‘Katerinak thinks that shek/m /hem came on time’ 9. Katerinak wants [PROk/*m to leave] 6 -pro can yield sloppy or strict readings under ellipsis, exactly like pronouns. Not so OC PRO. 10. i Katerinak nomizi oti prok/m irθe stin ora tis the K thinks that came on-the time her ke i Maria episis and the Maria also ‘Katerinak thinks that shek came on time and Maria does too’ =…Maria thinks that Katerina came on time strict …Maria thinks that Maria came on time sloppy 7 11.
    [Show full text]
  • Final Review: Syntax Fall 2007
    Final Review: Syntax Fall 2007 Jean Mark Gawron San Diego State University December 12, 2007 1 Control and Raising Key: S Subject O Object C Control R Raising For example, SOR = Subject(to)-Object Raising. Example answers: 1.1 seem a. Identify the control type [subject/object]. What NP is understood as controller of the infinitive (does or is expected to do or tries to do or ... the action described by the verb in infinitival form) John tries to go Subject SSR, SC John seems to go Subject SSR, SC John is likely to go Subject SSR, SC John is eager to go Subject SSR, SC Mary persuaded John to go Object SOR, OC Mary expected John to go Object SOR, OC Mary promised John to go Subject SSR, SC The control type of seem is subject! b. Produce relevant examples: (1) a. Itseemstoberaining b. There seems to be a problem c. The chips seems to be down. d. It seems to be obvious that John is a fool. e. The police seem to have caught the burglar. f. The burglar seems to have been caught by the police. c. Example construction i. Construct embedded clause: (a) itrains. Simpleexample;dummysubject * John rains Testing dummy subjecthood ittorain Putintoinfinitivalform ii. Embed (a) ∆ seem [CP it to rain] Embed under seem [ctd.] it seem [CP t torain] Moveit— it seems [CP t to rain] Add tense, agreement (to main verb) d. Other examples (b) itisraining. Alternativeexample;dummysubject *Johnisraining Testingdummysubjecthood ittoberaining Putintoinfinitivalform ∆ seem [CP it to be raining] Embed under seem it seem [CP t toberaining] Moveit— it seems [CP t to be raining] Add tense, agreement (to main verb) (c) Thechipsaredown.
    [Show full text]
  • Experimenting with Pro-Drop in Spanish1
    Andrea Pešková Experimenting with Pro-drop in Spanish1 Abstract This paper investigates the omission and expression of pronominal subjects (PS) in Buenos Aires Spanish based on data from a production experiment. The use of PS is a linguistic phenomenon demonstrating that the grammar of a language needs to be considered independently of its usage. Despite the fact that the observed Spanish variety is a consistent pro-drop language with rich verbal agreement, the data from the present study provide evidence for a quite frequent use of overt PS, even in non-focal, non- contrastive and non-ambiguous contexts. This result thus supports previous corpus- based empirical research and contradicts the traditional explanation given by grammarians that overtly realized PS in Spanish are used to avoid possible ambiguities or to mark contrast and emphasis. Moreover, the elicited semi-spontaneous data indicate that the expression of PS is optional; however, this optionality is associated with different linguistic factors. The statistical analysis of the data shows the following ranking of the effects of these factors: grammatical persons > verb semantics > (syntactic) clause type > (semantic) sentence type. 1. Introduction It is well known that Spanish is a pro-drop (“pronoun-dropping”) or null- subject language whose grammar permits the omission of pronominal 1 A preliminary version of this paper was presented at the international conference Variation and typology: New trends in syntactic research in Helsinki (August 2011). I am grateful to the audience for their fruitful discussions and useful commentaries. I would also like to thank the editors, Susann Fischer, Ingo Feldhausen, Christoph Gabriel and the anonymous reviewers for their detailed and helpful comments on an earlier version of this article.
    [Show full text]
  • The Position of Subjects*
    Lingua 85 (1991) 21 l-258. North-Holland 211 The position of subjects* Hilda Koopman and Dominique Sportiche Department of Linguistics, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA 90024, USA Grammatical theories all use in one form or another the concept of canonical position of a phrase. If this notion is used in the syntax, when comparing the two sentences: (la) John will see Bill. (1 b) Bill John will see. we say that Bill occupies its canonical position in (la) but not in (lb). Adopting the terminology of the Extended Standard Theory, we can think of the canonical position of a phrase as its D-structure position. Since the concept of canonical position is available, it becomes legitimate to ask of each syntactic unit in a given sentence what its canonical position is, relative to the other units of the sentence. The central question we address in this article is: what is the canonical position of subjects1 Starting with English, we propose that the structure of an English clause is as in (2): * The first section of this article has circulated as part of Koopman and Sportiche (1988) and is a written version of talks given in various places. It was given in March 1985 at the GLOW conference in Brussels as Koopman and Sportiche (1985), at the June 1985 CLA meeting in Montreal, at MIT and Umass Amherst in the winter of 1986, and presented at UCLA and USC since. The input of these audiences is gratefully acknowledged. The second section is almost completely new. 1 For related ideas on what we call the canonical postion of subjects, see Contreras (1987), Kitagawa (1986) Kuroda (1988), Speas (1986) Zagona (1982).
    [Show full text]
  • Word Recognition in Two Languages and Orthographies: English and Greek
    Memory & Cognition 1994, 22 (3), 313-325 Word recognition in two languages and orthographies: English and Greek HELENA-FIVI CHITIRI St. Francis Xavier University, Antigonish, Nova Scotia, Canada and DALE M. WILLOWS Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Word recognition processes of monolingual readers of English and of Greek were examined with respect to the orthographic and syntactic characteristics of each language. Because of Greek's direct letter-to-sound correspondence, which is unlike the indirect representation of English, the possibility was raised of a greater influence of the phonological code in Greek word recognition. Because Greek is an inflected language, whereas English is a word order language, it was also possible that syntax might influence word recognition patterns in the two languages differen­ tially. These cross-linguistic research questions were investigated within the context of a letter cancellation paradigm. The results provide evidence that readers are sensitive to both the ortho­ graphic and the linguistic idiosyncracies of their language. The results are discussed in terms of the orthographic depth hypothesis and the competition model. In this research, we examined word recognition in En­ ber (singular, plural), and the case (nominative, genitive, glish and Greek, two languages with different orthogra­ accusative, vocative) of the noun. Verb endings provide phies and syntactic characteristics. We examined the rela­ information about person and number as well as about tionship of orthography to the role of the phonological the voice and the tense of a verb. code in word recognition as it is exemplified in the pro­ Greek words are typically polysyllabic (Mirambel, cessing of syllabic and stress information.
    [Show full text]
  • Pronominal Vs. Anaphoric Pro in Kannada
    Pronominal vs. anaphoric pro in Kannada Anuradha Sudharsan∗ The English and Foreign Languages University Hyderabad, Telangana, India Kannada licenses a pronominal pro and an anaphor pro in root and subordinate clauses. In the subordi- nate clauses, pro’s person feature largely determines its pronominal/anaphoric status. Accordingly, there are four types of pro, each with a distinct referential property. The 1st and 2nd person pros allow pronomi- nal and anaphoric interpretations. In their pronominal reference, they refer to the speaker and the listener, respectively. In their anaphoric reference, they refer to the matrix subject and the object, respectively, irre- spective of the latter’s person feature, which results in a feature mismatch between pro and its antecedent. The 3rd person pro allows only pronominal interpretation. Kannada quotative verbs report direct speech. The null subjects in the embedded clauses of reported speech are basically pronominal because they are ‘copies’ of the pronominal subjects in direct speech. Accordingly, the embedded verbal inflection corre- sponds to inflection in direct speech, which results in a feature clash between pro and its antecedent. The fourth type, a null anaphor, occurs in non-argument endu-clauses. It is bound by an NP in the matrix with which it shares its F-features. It is the semantic relation between the non-argument and main clauses that explains the presence of an anaphoric pro here. Keywords: pro, pronominal, feature clash, anaphor, Kannada 1. Introduction In most studies on the pro-drop parameter, pro was characterized as a pronominal empty category based on evidence from European null subject languages.
    [Show full text]
  • 24.910 Topics in Linguistic Theory: Propositional Attitudes Spring 2009
    MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu 24.910 Topics in Linguistic Theory: Propositional Attitudes Spring 2009 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms. Mar. 31 24.910, Spring 2009 (Stephenson) Raising and Control Ingredients of analysis (as presented in Carnie text): ¾ Theta-roles / Theta Criterion: Requirement that predicates have exactly the right number of semantic arguments in the same clause (at D-structure) ¾ Abstract Case: Requirement that NPs be in (or move to) one of a few specified positions: Specifier of finite T [for nominative case] Complement to V [for accusative case] [Other possibilities are not relevant for control / raising] ¾ EPP (“Extended Projection Principle”): Requirement that sentences have a subject Four cases: ¾ Subject-to-subject raising ¾ Subject-to-object raising ¾ Subject control ¾ Object control 1.1. Subject-to-subject raising (1) John is likely to leave. ¾ theta-grid for (be) likely: proposition j ¾ theta-grid for leave: Agent i ¾ D-structure: (2) [ __spec is likely [John to leave]TP2 ]TP1 Theta-role of is likely is assigned to John to leave is in TP1 Theta-role of leave is assigned to John Æ Theta Criterion is satisfied BUT: John is in the specifier of the non-finite TP2 Æ can’t get case AND: the EPP is not satisfied (the sentence has no subject) 1 Mar. 31 24.910, Spring 2009 (Stephenson) ¾ John moves to Spec TP1 at S-structure: (3) [ Johni is likely [ _ti _ to leave]TP2 ]TP1 John gets nominative case from the finite T in TP1 EPP is satisfied (since John is the subject) 1.2.
    [Show full text]
  • The Minimalist Program 1 1 the Minimalist Program
    The Minimalist Program 1 1 The Minimalist Program Introduction It is my opinion that the implications of the Minimalist Program (MP) are more radical than generally supposed. I do not believe that the main thrust of MP is technical; whether to move features or categories for example. MP suggests that UG has a very different look from the standard picture offered by GB-based theories. This book tries to make good on this claim by outlining an approach to grammar based on one version of MP. I stress at the outset the qualifier “version.” Minimalism is not a theory but a program animated by certain kinds of methodological and substantive regulative ideals. These ideals are reflected in more concrete principles which are in turn used in minimalist models to analyze specific empirical phenomena. What follows is but one way of articulating the MP credo. I hope to convince you that this version spawns grammatical accounts that have a theoretically interesting structure and a fair degree of empirical support. The task, however, is doubly difficult. First, it is unclear what the content of these precepts is. Second, there is a non-negligible distance between the content of such precepts and its formal realization in specific grammatical principles and analyses. The immediate task is to approach the first hurdle and report what I take the precepts and principles of MP to be.1 1 Principles-Parameters and Minimalism MP is many things to many researchers. To my mind it grows out of the per- ceived success of the principles and parameters (P&P) approach to grammatical competence.
    [Show full text]