Bioplausible Multiscale Filtering in Retino-Cortical Processing As A

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Bioplausible Multiscale Filtering in Retino-Cortical Processing As A Brain Informatics (2017) 4:271–293 DOI 10.1007/s40708-017-0072-8 Bioplausible multiscale filtering in retino-cortical processing as a mechanism in perceptual grouping Nasim Nematzadeh . David M. W. Powers . Trent W. Lewis Received: 25 January 2017 / Accepted: 23 August 2017 / Published online: 8 September 2017 Ó The Author(s) 2017. This article is an open access publication Abstract Why does our visual system fail to reconstruct Differences of Gaussians and the perceptual interaction of reality, when we look at certain patterns? Where do Geo- foreground and background elements. The model is a metrical illusions start to emerge in the visual pathway? variation of classical receptive field implementation for How far should we take computational models of vision simple cells in early stages of vision with the scales tuned with the same visual ability to detect illusions as we do? to the object/texture sizes in the pattern. Our results suggest This study addresses these questions, by focusing on a that this model has a high potential in revealing the specific underlying neural mechanism involved in our underlying mechanism connecting low-level filtering visual experiences that affects our final perception. Among approaches to mid- and high-level explanations such as many types of visual illusion, ‘Geometrical’ and, in par- ‘Anchoring theory’ and ‘Perceptual grouping’. ticular, ‘Tilt Illusions’ are rather important, being charac- terized by misperception of geometric patterns involving Keywords Visual perception Á Cognitive systems Á Pattern lines and tiles in combination with contrasting orientation, recognition Á Biological neural network Á Self-organizing size or position. Over the last decade, many new neuro- systems Á Classical receptive field (CRF) models Á physiological experiments have led to new insights as to Geometrical illusions Á Tilt effects Á Difference of how, when and where retinal processing takes place, and Gaussians Á Perceptual grouping Á Gestalt grouping the encoding nature of the retinal representation that is sent principles to the cortex for further processing. Based on these neu- robiological discoveries, we provide computer simulation evidence from modelling retinal ganglion cells responses to some complex Tilt Illusions, suggesting that the emergence 1 Introduction of tilt in these illusions is partially related to the interaction of multiscale visual processing performed in the retina. The We investigate here whether computational modelling of output of our low-level filtering model is presented for vision can provide similar interpretation of visual data to several types of Tilt Illusion, predicting that the final tilt our own experiences, based on simple bioplausible mod- percept arises from multiple-scale processing of the elling of multiscale retinal cell responses to the visual scene. Our visual perception of the world is the result of N. Nematzadeh (&) Á D. M. W. Powers Á T. W. Lewis multiple levels of visual processing. This starts with mul- College of Science and Engineering, Flinders University, tiple levels of visual filtering within the retina and ends in GPO Box 2100, Adelaide, SA 5001, Australia multiple levels of processing in the visual cortex. The e-mail: nasim.nematzadeh@flinders.edu.au bottom-up visual processing gives rise to simple percept or URL: http://www.flinders.edu.au/people/nasim.nematzadeh features, but multimodal and top-down information flow D. M. W. Powers leads to more complex concepts, as well as influencing e-mail: david.powers@flinders.edu.au basic perception. In our visual system, fast and accurate T. W. Lewis visual processing functions as a parsimonious system with e-mail: trent.lewis@flinders.edu.au 123 272 N. Nematzadeh et al. minimal redundancy, with the processing in the retina an intermediate brightness between the brightness of tiles, serving different purposes and operating in different ways giving rise to appearance of mortar lines as divergent and from the later processing levels of the cortex. We regard convergent instead of parallel lines. On the other hand, in this assumption as fundamental to a biologically plausible the Bulge patterns, superimposed dots on top of a simple vision model, and human-competitive computer vision, checkerboard give rise to the impression of a bulge or tilt. reflecting our understanding of human vision. This is highly affected by the precise position of dots. The Even given the increasingly detailed biological charac- illusory perception of these patterns is connected to the terization of both retinal and cortical cells over the last half figure and ground perception, and in particular, in the a century (1960s–2010s), there remains considerable Bulge patterns, grouping of dots together creates an illu- uncertainty, and even some controversy, as to the nature sory figure shape, on top of a textured background (here a and extent of the encoding of visual information by the checkerboard can be also a grid). This produces apparent retina, and conversely of the subsequent processing and border shifts of the checkerboard edges and increases or decoding in the cortex (see e.g. the review of physiological reduces the impression of the bulges or bows in these retinal findings by Field and Chichilnisky [1] and Golish patterns. and Meister [2]). 1.2 Simultaneous Brightness/Lightness Illusions 1.1 History of Geometrical illusions To be able to differentiate between competing explanations The visual distortion experiences we encounter in visual for Geometrical illusions, consider the existing techniques illusions give clues as to some of the biological charac- for explaining the simultaneous Brightness/Lightness Illu- teristics of our visual processing that result in some erro- sions such as variations of White’s effect [15–17]. When neous perception. The explanations of optical illusions rely investigating Brightness/Lightness Illusions, high-level on our interpretation of the world, and the ambiguities explanatory models are sometimes seen as a result of against our visual experiences result in the illusory percept. lightness shift of the same luminance (thus brightness), There are various types of optical illusions, and in which decodes as different lightness for example, in [18]. ‘‘Appendix’’ (Table 1) we illustrate important representa- High-level explanations need higher cortical processing of tives of the various families including impossible 3D visual clues such as lightness/transparency as well as past arrangements such as Penrose Triangle and Penrose stair- experiences and inferences [19–21]. At the same time, to case [3–5], stimuli with multistable perception, flipping address their final percept, they might involve ideas of back and forth between different perception such as Necker interpolation (1D) or filling-in (2D) [22–25] as well. More cube [6]; also Herring’s and Orbison’s illusions [7–9] recently, the ‘Anchoring theory’ of Gilchrist et al. [26]is consist of horizontal and vertical lines located on a part of a based on ‘grouping factors’ that signal depth information, radial display, inducing tilt/bow/bulge as a result of the without any consideration of the spatial frequency of the three-dimensional percept and the perspective clues. Her- pattern. Further explanations for these illusions rely on mann Grid and Mach Bands have commonly accepted ‘Junction analysis’ like T-junctions [27] and ‘Scission explanations involving the low-level visual retinal/cortical theory’ [28, 29] which triggers the parsing of targets into processing by simple cells [10, 11] and the Lateral Inhi- multiple layers of reflectance, transparency and illumina- bition (LI) mechanism, which some of them need high- tion in which erroneous decomposition leads to Brightness/ level explanations. Table 1 in ‘‘Appendix’’ reflects simi- Lightness Illusions. larity of illusions, but due to the shortage of space and Modern ‘low-level theories’ on Brightness/Lightness table arrangement, it may not exactly match other illusion Illusions, for example Kingdom and Moulden [30], and classifications [12] based on other explanations. A com- Blakeslee and McCourt [17, 31], suggest that a set of plete reference list to the source and original illusory pat- spatial frequency filters at early stages of visual processing, terns given in Table 1 is also provided in ‘‘Appendix’’ mainly preprocessing in the retina are responsible for some (Table 2). A neurobiological explanation for a variety of Brightness/Lightness Illusions. Recent investigations on Geometrical illusion can be found in [13]. diverse range of lightness/brightness/transparency (LBT) The patterns explored in this paper are ‘second-order illusions by Kingdom [32] have shown different origins for Tilt’ Illusions [14] (Tile Illusions) involving the enhance- some of these effects due to whether illusion arises from ment of contrast between textural elements of a back- encoding of brightness or the lightness of the pattern. He ground such as a checkerboard, for example Cafe´ Wall and concluded that the most promising developments in LBT is Bulging checkerboard illusions. In the Cafe´ Wall illusion, a model of brightness coding based on ‘multiscale filtering’ the illusory tilt percept is the result of mortar lines between (models such as [17]) in conjunction with ‘contrast shifted rows of black and white tiles. The mortar lines have normalization’. 123 Bioplausible multiscale filtering in retino-cortical processing as a mechanism in perceptual… 273 1.3 Tilt illusions vision, is able to highlight the emergence of tilt in these patterns
Recommended publications
  • Ibn Al-‐Haytham the Man Who Discovered How We
    IBN AL-HAYTHAM THE MAN WHO DISCOVERED HOW WE SEE IBN AL-HAYTHAM EDUCATIONAL WORKSHOPS 1 Ibn al-Haytham was a pioneering scientific thinker who made important contributions to the understanding of vision, optics and light. His methodology of investigation, in particular using experiment to verify theory, shows certain similarities to what later became known as the modern scientific method. 2 Themes and Learning Objectives The educational initiative "1001 Inventions and the World of Ibn Al-Haytham" celebrates the legacy of Ibn al-Haytham. The global initiative was launched by 1001 Inventions in partnership with UNESCO in 2015 in celebration of the United Nations International Year of Light. The initiative engaged audiences around the world with events at the UNESCO headquarters in Paris, United Nations in New York, the China Science Festival in Beijing, the Royal Society in London and in more than ten other cities around the world. Inspired by Ibn al-Haytham, exhibits, hands-on workshops, science demonstrations, films and learning materials take children on a fascinating journey into the past, sparking their interest in science while promoting integration and intercultural appreciation. This document includes a range of hands-on workshops and science demonstrations, with links to fantastic resources, for understanding the fundamental principles of light, optics and vision. The activities help engage young people to make, design and tinker while better understanding the significant contributions of Ibn al-Haytham to our understanding of both vision and light. Learning Objectives ü Inspire young people to study science, technology, engineering and maths (STEM) and pursue careers in science. ü Improve awareness of light, optics and vision through Ibn al-Haytham’s discoveries.
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling and Rendering of Impossible Figures
    Modeling and Rendering of Impossible Figures TAI-PANG WU The Chinese University of Hong Kong CHI-WING FU Nanyang Technological University SAI-KIT YEUNG The Hong Kong University of Science and Technology JIAYA JIA The Chinese University of Hong Kong and CHI-KEUNG TANG The Hong Kong University of Science and Technology 13 This article introduces an optimization approach for modeling and rendering impossible figures. Our solution is inspired by how modeling artists construct physical 3D models to produce a valid 2D view of an impossible figure. Given a set of 3D locally possible parts of the figure, our algorithm automatically optimizes a view-dependent 3D model, subject to the necessary 3D constraints for rendering the impossible figure at the desired novel viewpoint. A linear and constrained least-squares solution to the optimization problem is derived, thereby allowing an efficient computation and rendering new views of impossible figures at interactive rates. Once the optimized model is available, a variety of compelling rendering effects can be applied to the impossible figure. Categories and Subject Descriptors: I.3.3 [Computer Graphics]: Picture/Image Generation; I.4.8 [Image Processing and Computer Vision]: Scene Analysis; J.5 [Computer Applications]: Arts and Humanities General Terms: Algorithms, Human Factors Additional Key Words and Phrases: Modeling and rendering, human perception, impossible figure, nonphotorealistic rendering ACM Reference Format: Wu, T.-P., Fu, C.-W., Yeung, S.-K., Jia, J., and Tang, C.-K. 2010. Modeling and rendering of impossible figures. ACM Trans. Graph. 29, 2, Article 13 (March 2010), 15 pages. DOI = 10.1145/1731047.1731051 http://doi.acm.org/10.1145/1731047.1731051 1.
    [Show full text]
  • BYU Museum of Art M. C. Escher: Other Worlds Education Resources
    BYU Museum of Art M. C. Escher: Other Worlds Education Resources Escher Terminology Crystallography: A branch of science that examines the structures and properties of crystals; greatly influenced the development of Escher’s tessellations de Mesquita, S. J.: A master printmaker at the School for Architecture and Decorative Arts, where Escher was studying, who encouraged Escher to pursue art rather than architecture H. S. M. Coxeter: A mathematician whose geometric “Coxeter Groups” became known as tessellations. Like Escher, Coxeter also loved music and its mathematical properties. The initial ideas for Circle Limit came from Coxeter. Infinity/Droste Effect: An image appearing within itself in smaller and smaller versions on to infinity; for example, when one mirror placed in front and one behind, creating an in infinite tunnel of the image Lithograph: A printing process in which the image to be printed is drawn on a flat stone surface and treated to hold ink while the negative space is treated to repel ink. Mezzotint: A method of engraving a copper or steel plate by scraping and burnishing areas to produce effects of light and shadow. Möbius strips: A surface that has only one side and one edge, making it impossible to orient; often used to symbolize eternity; used to how red ants is Escher’s Möbius Strip II Necker cube: An optical illusion proposed by Swiss crystallographer Louis Albert Necker, where the drawing of a cube has no visual cues as to its orientation; in Escher’s Belvedere, the Necker cube becomes an “impossible” cube. 1
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Drawing Conclusions
    Drawing Conclusions: An Exploration of the Cognitive and Neuroscientific Foundations of Representational Drawing Rebecca Susan Chamberlain Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology Thesis submitted to UCL for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy July 2013 1 Declaration ‘I, Rebecca Susan Chamberlain confirm that the work presented in this thesis is my own. Where information has been derived from other sources, I confirm that this has been indicated in the thesis.’ 2 Publications The work in this thesis gave rise to the following publications: Chamberlain, R., McManus, I. C., Riley. H., Rankin, Q., & Brunswick, N. (2013) Local processing enhancements in superior observational drawing are due to enhanced perceptual functioning, not weak central coherence. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66 (7), 1448-66. Chamberlain, R. (2012) Attitudes and Approaches to Observational Drawing in Contemporary Artistic Practice. Drawing Knowledge. TRACEY Drawing and Visualisation Research. Chamberlain, R., McManus, I. C., Riley. H., Rankin, Q., & Brunswick, N. (In Press) Cain’s House Revisited and Revived: Extending Theory and Methodology for Quantifying Drawing Accuracy. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Art. 3 Acknowledgements My first thanks extend to my supervisor and mentor, Professor Chris McManus, for his infectious enthusiasm for research and for the countless words of wisdom (academic and non-academic) he has given me over the last six years. I would like to thank my collaborators at The Royal College of Art and Swansea Metropolitan University, Howard Riley and Qona Rankin, who have been so generous with their time, resources and thoughts on the artistic mind. This research also wouldn’t have been possible without the hundreds of art and design students who were willing to take time out of their hectic lives to participate in my studies and discuss their experiences and reflections with me.
    [Show full text]
  • Seeing and Recognizing
    Information sheet Seeing and recognizing Our society relies heavily on visual perception. People orient themselves in the world primarily us- ing their sense of sight. That’s why our entire culture and civilization is heavily based on visual communication, from cave painting long ago to the written language and modern information and data technology, such as sending videos using smartphones. A brief summary of the most important facts related to the seeing process is provided below. Formation of an image on the retina The eye works like a camera lens: Ambient light passes through the cornea, the pupil, the lens, and the transparent vitreous body, after which it impinges on the light-sensitive layer, the retina. The sensory cells in the retina con- vert the light signals to electrical impulses that are transmitted to the brain via the optic nerve. Accommodation The term “accommodation” is used to refer to the adaptation of the eye for near vision or long-distance vision. The focal length of the lens is changed in order to adapt to the dis- tance of objects: If you look at close objects, the ciliary muscle contracts and the lens be- comes more rounded. If you look into the dis- tance, the ciliary muscle relaxes and the lens becomes flatter. This is how you focus on near and far objects. © Siemens Stiftung 2017. Content licensed under CC BY-SA 4.0 international Page 1 of 4 Information sheet Seeing colors and brightness levels The retina is the light-sensitive layer of the eye containing the rod and cone cells.
    [Show full text]
  • Illusion -- from Mathworld an Illusion Object Or Drawing Which Appears to Have Properties Which Are Physically Impossible, Deceptive, Or Counterintuitive
    Illusion -- From MathWorld An illusion object or drawing which appears to have properties which are physically impossible, deceptive, or counterintuitive. Kitaoka maintains a web page of beautiful optical illusions which appear to show rotation despite actually being http://mathworld.wolfram.com/Illusion.html - 21k - 2003-10-05 Young Girl-Old Woman Illusion -- From MathWorld A famous perceptual illusion in which the brain switches between seeing a young girl and an old woman (or "wife" and "mother in law"). An anonymous German postcard from 1888 (left figure) depicts the image in its earliest known form, and a rendition on an advertisement for the Anchor Buggy Company from 1 http://mathworld.wolfram.com/YoungGirl-OldWomanIllusion.html - 19k - 2002-08-18 Impossible Fork -- From MathWorld ImpossibleFork.gifThe impossible fork (Seckel 2002, p. 151), also known as the devil's pitchfork (Singmaster) or blivet, is a classic impossible figure originally due to Schuster (1964). While each prong of the fork (or, in the original work, "clevis") appears normal, attempting to determine their manner http://mathworld.wolfram.com/ImpossibleFork.html - 19k - 2004-10- 01 Penrose Stairway -- From MathWorld An impossible figure in which a stairway in the shape of a square appears to circulate indefinitely while still possessing normal steps (Penrose and Penrose 1958). The Dutch artist M. C. Escher included a Penrose stairway in his mind-bending illustration "Ascending and Descending" (Bool et al. 1982, p. 3 http://mathworld.wolfram.com/PenroseStairway.html - 20k - 2004- 02-01 Hering Illusion -- From MathWorld An optical illusion due to the physiologist Ewald Hering (1861). The two horizontal lines are both straight, but they look as if they were bowed outwards.
    [Show full text]
  • Constructing Drawings of Impossible Figures with Axonometric Blocks and Pseudo-3D Manipulations
    Proceedings of Bridges 2014: Mathematics, Music, Art, Architecture, Culture Constructing Drawings of Impossible Figures with Axonometric Blocks and Pseudo-3D Manipulations Tiffany C. Inglis David R. Cheriton School of Computer Science University of Waterloo [email protected] Abstract Impossible figures are found in graphical designs, mathematical art, and puzzle games. There are various techniques for constructing these figures both in 2D and 3D, but most involve tricks that are not easily generalizable. We describe a simple framework that uses axonometric blocks for construction and permits pseudo-3D manipulations even though the figure may not have a real 3D counterpart. We use this framework to create impossible figures with complex structures and decorative patterns. Introduction Impossible figures [1, 6] are optical illusions involving objects that can be drawn in 2D but are infeasible in 3D (i.e., cannot be physically constructed). Figure 1a shows the impossible cube and the Penrose triangle, both of which have geometric requirements that are impossible to satisfy in 3D, and the blivet, which relies on negative space to create an illusion. Impossible figures are also featured in many of M. C. Escher’s lithographs [7], such as the Penrose stairs—an ever-ascending staircase—that is featured in both Ascending and Descending and Waterfall. Many of these impossible figures resemble 3D objects that can be constructed out of blocks under parallel projec- tion. It seems natural that one should be able to apply the same block manipulations used to construct real 3D objects to construct these impossible figures. However, since impossible figures have no 3D counterparts, defining a set of pseudo-3D manipulations is non-trivial.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Science
    Agenda Pomona College Upcoming talks LCS 11: Cognitive Science Lorraine Tyler Thursday at 4:15PM, Edmunds 101 Vision William Marslen-Wilson Friday at noon, Edmunds 101 The eye and the visual system HoUman’s visual rules Jesse A. Harris GQ 5.1 due Sunday, 9PM April 17, 2013 Signup for group meetings Jesse A. Harris: LCS 11: Cognitive Science, Vision 1 Jesse A. Harris: LCS 11: Cognitive Science, Vision 2 The problem But this problem is nothing compared to . Vision feels easy, but it’s not at all. The miracle of vision! The visual system transforms a fragmented 2D image into a Constraints on vision coherent 3D construct. 1. Limited perceptual Veld 2. Physical blindspots 3. Subject to all kinds of illusions and perceptual error Miracle starts with the eye itself . Jesse A. Harris: LCS 11: Cognitive Science, Vision 3 Jesse A. Harris: LCS 11: Cognitive Science, Vision 4 Visible eye Cornea Curved transparent lens through which light enters. Focuses light onto the retina. Light bent and slowed by cornea. Iris Provides an adjustable aperture (colored part of eye). Pupil Hole in middle of iris, adjusts for amount of light Smaller when more light, limiting the amount let through to another lens. Jesse A. Harris: LCS 11: Cognitive Science, Vision 5 Jesse A. Harris: LCS 11: Cognitive Science, Vision 6 Inner eye Lens Contracts to give give more refractive power, needed for focusing on close objects. Power to accommodate by contraction decreases with age. Retina Light sensitive layer lining back of eye, containing multiple types of photoreceptors (rods and cones) and a chain of cells that provide the Vrst, rough processing, ending in the retinal ganglion cells.
    [Show full text]
  • Optical Illusions and Their Causes Examining Differing Explanations
    Optical Illusions and their Causes Examining Differing Explanations Sarah Oliver AHS Capstone 9 May, 2006 Illusion is the first of all pleasures. Oscar Wilde Introduction Optical illusions stimulate us by challenging us to see things in a new way. They are interesting within scientific disciplines because they lie on the border of what we are able to see. According to Al Seckel (2000), they “…are very useful tools for vision scientists, because they can reveal the hidden constraints of the perceptual system in a way that normal perception does not.” Different kinds of optical illusions may be better explained by one discipline or another. Some optical illusions trick us because of the properties of light and the way our eyes work, and are addressed by biology and perhaps optics, while other illusions depend on a “higher” level of processing which is better addressed by psychology. Some illusions can only be fully explained using knowledge of more than one discipline. When there are both biological and psychological explanations, they may both be right. There is already a substantial body of work that specifically addresses optical illusions, however, there is still enough disagreement within disciplines and between disciplines to make this an interesting project. One of the challenges of this project is the question of how to choose a satisfactory definition of “optical illusion.” Every source has a different definition that includes or excludes phenomena as it suits the author’s purpose. Gregory, one of the most well-known researchers of optical illusions, notes that illusion 1 … may be the departure from reality, or from truth; but how are these to be defined? As science’s accounts of reality get ever more different from appearances, to say that this separation is ‘illusion’ would have the absurd consequence of implying that almost all perceptions are illusory.
    [Show full text]
  • Systematic Structural Analysis of Optical Illusion Art and Application in Graphic Design with Autism Spectrum Disorder
    SYSTEMATIC STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS OF OPTICAL ILLUSION ART AND APPLICATION IN GRAPHIC DESIGN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER RELATORE :PROF. ARCH. PH.D ANNA MAROTTA CORRELATORE : ARCH. PH.D ROSSANA NETTI STUDENT:LI HAORAN SYSTEMATIC STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS OF OPTICAL ILLUSION ARTAND APPLICATION IN GRAPHIC DESIGN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER Abstract To explore the application of optical illusion in different fields, taking M.C. Escher's painting as an example, systematic analysis of optical illusion, Gestalt psychology, and Geometry are performed. Then, from the geometric point of view, to analyze the optical illusion formation model. Taking Fraser Spiral Illusion and Herring Illusion as examples, through the dismantling and combination of Fraser Spiral Illusion and the analysis of the spiral angle, and Herring Illusion tilt angle analysis, the structure of the graph is further explored. This results in a geometric drawing method that optimizes the optical illusion effect and applies this method to graphic design combined with the autism spectrum. Therefore, this project will remind the ordinary people and designers to pay attention to the universal design while also paying attention to extreme people through the combination of graphic design and optical illusion. That is, focus on autism spectrum disorder (ASD), provide more inclusive design and try their best to provide autism spectrum with the deserving autonomy and independence in public space. Meanwhile, making the autism spectrum “integrate” into the general public’s environment so that their abilities match the environment and improve self-care capabilities. Keywords Autism Spectrum Disorder, Inclusive Design, Graphic Design, Gestalt Psychology, Optical Illusion, Systemic Perspective Introduction 1. Systematic analysis of the content of the thesis 1.1 The purpose and the significance of study 1.1.1 Methodology with system diagram framework 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Uncertainty in Visual Processes Predicts Geometrical Optical Illusions
    Uncertainty in Visual Processes Predicts Geometrical Optical Illusions Cornelia Ferm¨uller1 and Henrik Malm2 1Computer Vision Laboratory Center for Automation Research Department of Computer Science Institute for Advanced Computer Studies University of Maryland College Park, MD 20742-3275 2Mathematical Imaging Group (MIG) Department of Mathematics (LTH) Lund Institute of Technology/ Lund University P.O. Box 118 S-221 00 Lund, Sweden Abstract It is proposed in this paper that many geometrical optical illusions, as well as illusory patterns due to motion signals in line drawings, are due to the statistics of visual computations. The interpretation of image patterns is preceded by a step where image features such as lines, intersections of lines, or local image movement must be derived. However, there are many sources of noise or uncertainty in the formation and processing of images, and they cause problems in the estimation of these features; in particular, they cause bias. As a result, the locations of features are perceived erroneously and the appearance of the patterns is altered. The bias occurs with any visual processing of line features; under average conditions it is not large enough to be noticeable, but illusory patterns are such that the bias is highly pronounced. Thus, the broader message of this paper is that there is a general uncertainty principle which governs the workings of vision systems, and optical illusions are an artifact of this principle. Key words: optical illusions, motion perception, bias, estimation processes, noise The support of this research by the National Science Foundation under grant IIS-00-8-1365 is gratefully acknowledged.
    [Show full text]
  • Image Parsing Mechanisms of the Visual Cortex
    To appear in: The Visual Neurosciences (L.M. Chalupa and J.S. Werner, eds.), Cambridge: MIT Press. Image Parsing Mechanisms of the Visual Cortex Rüdiger von der Heydt Krieger Mind/Brain Institute and Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218 Short title: same as above Correspondence: Rüdiger von der Heydt Krieger Mind/Brain Institute Johns Hopkins University 3400 North Charles Street Baltimore, MD 21218 Phone: 410 516-6416 Fax: 410 516-8648 E-mail: [email protected] R. von der Heydt: Image Parsing Mechanisms of the Visual Cortex 1. Introduction In this chapter I will discuss visual processes that are often labeled as “intermediate level vision”. As a useful framework we consider vision as a sequence of processes each of which is a mapping from one representation to another. Understanding vision then means analyzing how information is represented at each stage and how it is transformed between stages (Marr, 1982). It is clear that the first stage, the level of the photoreceptors, is an image representation. This is a 2-dimensional array of color values, resembling the bitmap format of digital computers. Retinal processes transform this representation into a format that is suitable for transmission through the optic nerve to central brain structures. A radical transformation then takes place in the primary visual cortex. At the output of area V1 we find visual information encoded as a “feature map”, a representation of local features. The two dimensions of retinal position are encoded in the locations of the receptive fields of cortical neurons, but each neuron now represents not a pixel, but a small patch of the image, and several new dimensions are added to the three color dimensions, such as orientation, spatial frequency, direction of motion, and binocular disparity.
    [Show full text]