The Telicity Parameter Revisited Hana Filip SRI International – NLP Group Discern & Stanford University
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Greek Verb Aspect
Greek Verb Aspect Paul Bell & William S. Annis Scholiastae.org∗ February 21, 2012 The technical literature concerning aspect is vast and difficult. The goal of this tutorial is to present, as gently as possible, a few more or less commonly held opinions about aspect. Although these opinions may be championed by one academic quarter and denied by another, at the very least they should shed some light on an abstruse matter. Introduction The word “aspect” has its roots in the Latin verb specere meaning “to look at.” Aspect is concerned with how we view a particular situation. Hence aspect is subjective – different people will view the same situation differently; the same person can view a situation differently at different times. There is little doubt that how we see things depends on our psychological state at the mo- ment of seeing. The ‘choice’ to bring some parts of a situation into close, foreground relief while relegating others to an almost non-descript background happens unconsciously. But for one who must describe a situation to others, this choice may indeed operate consciously and deliberately. Hence aspect concerns not only how one views a situation, but how he chooses to relate, to re-present, a situation. A Definition of Aspect But we still haven’t really said what aspect is. So here’s a working definition – aspect is the dis- closure of a situation from the perspective of internal temporal structure. To put it another way, when an author makes an aspectual choice in relating a situation, he is choosing to reveal or conceal the situation’s internal temporal structure. -
Perfect-Traj.Pdf
Aspect shifts in Indo-Aryan and trajectories of semantic change1 Cleo Condoravdi and Ashwini Deo Abstract: The grammaticalization literature notes the cross-linguistic robustness of a diachronic pat- tern involving the aspectual categories resultative, perfect, and perfective. Resultative aspect markers often develop into perfect markers, which then end up as perfect plus perfective markers. We intro- duce supporting data from the history of Old and Middle Indo-Aryan languages, whose instantiation of this pattern has not been previously noted. We provide a semantic analysis of the resultative, the perfect, and the aspectual category that combines perfect and perfective. Our analysis reveals the change to be a two-step generalization (semantic weakening) from the original resultative meaning. 1 Introduction The emergence of new functional expressions and the changes in their distribution and interpretation over time have been shown to be systematic across languages, as well as across a variety of semantic domains. These observations have led scholars to construe such changes in terms of “clines”, or predetermined trajectories, along which expressions move in time. Specifically, the typological literature, based on large-scale grammaticaliza- tion studies, has discovered several such trajectorial shifts in the domain of tense, aspect, and modality. Three properties characterize such shifts: (a) the categories involved are sta- ble across cross-linguistic instantiations; (b) the paths of change are unidirectional; (c) the shifts are uniformly generalizing (Heine, Claudi, and H¨unnemeyer 1991; Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca 1994; Haspelmath 1999; Dahl 2000; Traugott and Dasher 2002; Hopper and Traugott 2003; Kiparsky 2012). A well-known trajectory is the one in (1). -
Nominal and Verbal Parameters in the Diachrony of Differential Object Marking in Spanish Marco García García University of Cologne
Chapter 8 Nominal and verbal parameters in the diachrony of differential object marking in Spanish Marco García García University of Cologne This paper deals with the influence that nominal and verbal parameters have on DOMinthe diachrony of Spanish. Comparing selected corpus studies, I will focus first on the different nominal parameters that build up the animacy and referentiality scales, in particular on an- imacy and definiteness. In order to clarify how far DOM has diachronically evolved, special attention will be paid to inanimate objects, which can be viewed as the alleged endpointin the development of DOM in Spanish. Secondly, I will provide a systematic overview of the relevant verbal parameters, which include aspect, affectedness and agentivity. The study will show a complex interaction of nominal and verbal parameters, revealing some unex- pected correlations: Obligatory object marking is not only found with human and strongly affected objects involved in a telic event, but also with inanimate, non-affected andagen- tive objects embedded in a stative event. In other words, in Spanish DOM patterns with both extremely high and extremely low transitivity. These findings sharply contrast with traditional accounts concerning the development as well as the explanation of DOM. 1 Introduction Differential object marking (DOM) is a well-attested phenomenon within Romance lan- guages (for an overview see Bossong 1998: 218–230). While in some Romance languages, such as Catalan or Modern Portuguese, DOM is confined to a reduced number of con- texts, in others, such as Sardinian or Spanish, it is found in many more contexts. This paper will focus on Spanish, where DOM seems to have reached a greater stage of de- velopment than in any other Romance language. -
The Perfect Dilemma: Aspect in the Koine Greek Verbal System, Accepted and Debated Areas of Research
Perfect Dilemma 1 Running head: PERFECT DILEMMA The Perfect Dilemma: Aspect in the Koine Greek Verbal System, Accepted and Debated Areas of Research Christopher H. Johnson A Senior Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation in the Honors Program Liberty University Spring 2009 Perfect Dilemma 2 Acceptance of Senior Honors Thesis This Senior Honors Thesis is accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation from the Honors Program of Liberty University. ______________________________ Wayne Brindle, Th.D. Thesis Chair ______________________________ David Croteau, Ph.D. Committee Member ______________________________ Paul Muller, Ph.D. Committee Member ______________________________ Brenda Ayres, Ph.D. Honors Director ______________________________ Date Perfect Dilemma 3 Abstract Verbal aspect is a recent but very promising field of study in Koine Greek, which seeks to describe the semantic meaning of the verbal forms. This study surveys the works of the leading contributors in this field and offers critiques of their major points. The subject matter is divided into three sections: methods, areas of agreement, and areas of dispute, with a focus on the latter. Overall, scholarship has provided a more accurate description of the Greek verbal system through the theory of verbal aspect, though there are topics that need further research. This study’s suggestions may aid in developing verbal aspect and substantiating certain features of its theory. Perfect Dilemma 4 Introduction “There is a prevalent but false assumption that everything in NT Greek scholarship has been done already,”1 so argues Lars Rydbeck as he urges scholars to continue to work in studying the Koine language. His appeal has been answered by numerous studies in Koine Greek and in the developments in its grammar. -
30. Tense Aspect Mood 615
30. Tense Aspect Mood 615 Richards, Ivor Armstrong 1936 The Philosophy of Rhetoric. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Rockwell, Patricia 2007 Vocal features of conversational sarcasm: A comparison of methods. Journal of Psycho- linguistic Research 36: 361−369. Rosenblum, Doron 5. March 2004 Smart he is not. http://www.haaretz.com/print-edition/opinion/smart-he-is-not- 1.115908. Searle, John 1979 Expression and Meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Seddiq, Mirriam N. A. Why I don’t want to talk to you. http://notguiltynoway.com/2004/09/why-i-dont-want- to-talk-to-you.html. Singh, Onkar 17. December 2002 Parliament attack convicts fight in court. http://www.rediff.com/news/ 2002/dec/17parl2.htm [Accessed 24 July 2013]. Sperber, Dan and Deirdre Wilson 1986/1995 Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell. Voegele, Jason N. A. http://www.jvoegele.com/literarysf/cyberpunk.html Voyer, Daniel and Cheryl Techentin 2010 Subjective acoustic features of sarcasm: Lower, slower, and more. Metaphor and Symbol 25: 1−16. Ward, Gregory 1983 A pragmatic analysis of epitomization. Papers in Linguistics 17: 145−161. Ward, Gregory and Betty J. Birner 2006 Information structure. In: B. Aarts and A. McMahon (eds.), Handbook of English Lin- guistics, 291−317. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Rachel Giora, Tel Aviv, (Israel) 30. Tense Aspect Mood 1. Introduction 2. Metaphor: EVENTS ARE (PHYSICAL) OBJECTS 3. Polysemy, construal, profiling, and coercion 4. Interactions of tense, aspect, and mood 5. Conclusion 6. References 1. Introduction In the framework of cognitive linguistics we approach the grammatical categories of tense, aspect, and mood from the perspective of general cognitive strategies. -
Hundred Sentences and XFST Assignments
Hundred Sentences and XFST Assignments 2-19-14 Your assignment • Make an elicitation checklist of at least 100 sentences covering these things: – Transitive and intransitive verbs. – Semantic verb classes: stativity, dynamicity, and telicity. – Tense/mood/aspect – Special sentence types: • Existential • Copula • Possession • Questions – Speech acts: • Statement • Command/prohibition • Question – Negation Your assignment continued • Noun types – Pronoun – Common – Proper – Count/Mass – Concrete/Abstract • Noun phrases – Definiteness – Possession – Proximity – Diminutive/Augmentative – Quantity • Cardinal numbers • Quantifiers: all, each, both, some – Ordinal (first, second) – Partitive • The top of the tree • The bottom of the tree Your assignment continued • Adjectives – Comparative – Superlative – Intensive (very) – Unintensive (a little) • Adpositions and/or case marking • Typical modiers of nouns and verbs. – Location, time, manner, etc. Example: Belele (Bwele) (Inspiration from Warlpiri and Iñupiaq) Bele-le word-generic Intransitive sentences. SV word order. “Wordkind” Fwe-le bar -la -to Verb morphology: root-aspect-tense Bird-gener sing-hab-pres “Birds sing” Aspects: habitual -la Fwe-n bar -la -to Punctual Ø Bird-def sing-hab-pres Inceptive -go “The bird sings” Fwe-n bar-na Tenses: Bird-def sing-past Present -to “The bird sang” Past -na Fwe-n bar- go- na Bird-def sing-incep-past “The bird started to sing” Example continued • Compound noun: First element not marked for number and definiteness Nar -bi ndo –ki -n There are adjective stems, but they happy -one day –pl –def have to be turned into nouns in order “the happy days” to use them. -bi turns an adjective into a noun. • Two noun phrases (either order, or can be discontinuous): Nar -bi -ki -n ndo –ki -n happy -one –pl-def day –pl –def Nouns and their modifiers can be in either order of can be “the happy ones the days” separated if the modifier is marked for number and ndo –ki -n nar -bi -ki -n definiteness. -
Early Sensitivity to Telicity: the Role of the Count/Mass Distinction in Event Individuation
W O R K I N G P A P E R S I N L I N G U I S T I C S The notes and articles in this series are progress reports on work being carried on by students and faculty in the Department. Because these papers are not finished products, readers are asked not to cite from them without noting their preliminary nature. The authors welcome any comments and suggestions that readers might offer. Volume 41(4) 2010 (April) DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI‘I AT MĀNOA HONOLULU 96822 An Equal Opportunity/Affirmative Action Institution WORKING PAPERS IN LINGUISTICS: UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI‘I AT MĀNOA, VOL. 41(4) DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS FACULTY 2010 Victoria B. Anderson Byron W. Bender (Emeritus) Benjamin Bergen Derek Bickerton (Emeritus) Robert A. Blust Robert L. Cheng (Adjunct) Kenneth W. Cook (Adjunct) Kamil Deen Patricia J. Donegan (Co-Graduate Chair) Katie K. Drager Emanuel J. Drechsel (Adjunct) Michael L. Forman (Emeritus) George W. Grace (Emeritus) John H. Haig (Adjunct) Roderick A. Jacobs (Emeritus) Paul Lassettre P. Gregory Lee Patricia A. Lee Howard P. McKaughan (Emeritus) William O’Grady (Chair) Yuko Otsuka Ann Marie Peters (Emeritus, Co-Graduate Chair) Kenneth L. Rehg Lawrence A. Reid (Emeritus) Amy J. Schafer Albert J. Schütz, (Emeritus, Editor) Ho Min Sohn (Adjunct) Nicholas Thieberger Laurence C. Thompson (Emeritus) ii EARLY SENSITIVITY TO TELICITY: THE ROLE OF THE COUNT/MASS DISTINCTION IN EVENT INDIVIDUATION YUKIE HARA1 This paper presents evidence that English-speaking children are sensitive to telicity based on the count/mass distinction of the object noun in verb phrases such as eat an apple (telic) vs. -
Corpus Study of Tense, Aspect, and Modality in Diglossic Speech in Cairene Arabic
CORPUS STUDY OF TENSE, ASPECT, AND MODALITY IN DIGLOSSIC SPEECH IN CAIRENE ARABIC BY OLA AHMED MOSHREF DISSERTATION Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2012 Urbana, Illinois Doctoral Committee: Professor Elabbas Benmamoun, Chair Professor Eyamba Bokamba Professor Rakesh M. Bhatt Assistant Professor Marina Terkourafi ABSTRACT Morpho-syntactic features of Modern Standard Arabic mix intricately with those of Egyptian Colloquial Arabic in ordinary speech. I study the lexical, phonological and syntactic features of verb phrase morphemes and constituents in different tenses, aspects, moods. A corpus of over 3000 phrases was collected from religious, political/economic and sports interviews on four Egyptian satellite TV channels. The computational analysis of the data shows that systematic and content morphemes from both varieties of Arabic combine in principled ways. Syntactic considerations play a critical role with regard to the frequency and direction of code-switching between the negative marker, subject, or complement on one hand and the verb on the other. Morph-syntactic constraints regulate different types of discourse but more formal topics may exhibit more mixing between Colloquial aspect or future markers and Standard verbs. ii To the One Arab Dream that will come true inshaa’ Allah! عربية أنا.. أميت دمها خري الدماء.. كما يقول أيب الشاعر العراقي: بدر شاكر السياب Arab I am.. My nation’s blood is the finest.. As my father says Iraqi Poet: Badr Shaker Elsayyab iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I’m sincerely thankful to my advisor Prof. Elabbas Benmamoun, who during the six years of my study at UIUC was always kind, caring and supportive on the personal and academic levels. -
The Interaction Between Telicity and Agentivity: Experimental Evidence from Intransitive Verbs in German and Chinese
Originally published in: Lingua vol. 200 (2017), pp. 84-106. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2017.08.006 POSTPRINT a,b,* a a a,b Tim Graf , Markus Philipp , Xiaonan Xu , Franziska Kretzschmar , a Beatrice Primus a Institute of German Language and Literature I, University of Cologne, Albertus-Magnus-Platz, 50923 Cologne, Germany b CRC 1252 Prominence in Language, University of Cologne, Germany The interaction between telicity and agentivity: Experimental evidence from intransitive verbs in German and Chinese Abstract: Telicity and agentivity are semantic factors that split intransitive verbs into (at least two) different classes. Clear-cut unergative verbs, which select the auxiliary HAVE, are assumed to be atelic and agent-selecting; unequivocally unaccusative verbs, which select the auxiliary BE, are analyzed as telic and patient-selecting. Thus, agentivity and telicity are assumed to be inversely correlated in split intransitivity. We will present semantic and experimental evidence from German and Mandarin Chinese that casts doubts on this widely held assumption. The focus of our experimental investigation lies on variation with respect to agentivity (specifically motion control, manipulated via animacy), telicity (tested via a locative vs. goal adverbial), and BE/HAVE-selection with semantically flexible intransitive verbs of motion. Our experimental methods are acceptability ratings for German and Chinese (Experiments 1 and 2) and event-related potential (ERP) measures for German (Experiment 3). Our findings contradict the above- mentioned assumption that agentivity and telicity are generally inversely correlated and suggest that for the verbs under study, agentivity and telicity harmonize with each other. Furthermore, the ERP measures reveal that the impact of the interaction under discussion is more pronounced on the verb lexeme than on the auxiliary. -
Tense in Time: the Greek Perfect 1
Gerš/Stechow Draft 8. January 2002: EVA-CARIN GER… AND ARNIM VON STECHOW TENSE IN TIME: THE GREEK PERFECT 1. Survey .................................................................................................................................1 2. On the Tense/Aspect/Aktionsart-architecture .......................................................................4 3. Chronology of Tense/Aspect Systems................................................................................10 3.1. Archaic Greek (700-500 BC) .....................................................................................11 3.2. Classical (500 Ð 300 BC)............................................................................................18 3.3. Postclassical and Greek-Roman (300 BC Ð 450 AD)..................................................29 3.4. Transitional and Byzantine/Mediaeval Period (300 Ð 1450 AD) .................................31 3.5. Modern Greek (From 1450 AD).................................................................................33 4. Conclusion.........................................................................................................................35 * 1. SURVEY The paper will deal with the diachronic development of the meaning and form of the Greek Perfect. The reason for focusing on this language is twofold: first, it has often been neglected in the modern linguistic literature about tense; secondly, in Greek, it is possible to observe (even without taking into account the meaning and form of the Perfect in Modern -
Reminder Tense Vs Aspect in English
Reminder Research questions + references should be handed in next week! Thursday, March 7 , by 5.30 p.m. No handwritten proposals will be accepted! If you still have not found a suitable topic, come to see me! Please note: I will deduct points if references are not presented in the proper format (see class notes) or if they are missing. Tense vs aspect in English Tense and aspect use deictic systems: Past Present Future (anchor) I will sleep. I sleep. I slept. 1 Past Present Future (anchor) past moment I had slept. Past Present Future (anchor) future moment I will have slept. 2 Aspect interacts with the meaning of tense, often sharing markers/endings etc. It requires (sometimes additional) anchors but those anchors serve evaluative purposes. Two aspect types across languages: • Perfective (completed action) Perfect/ parfait - per (thoroughly)-facere (to make) ensures that the event is completed before the anchoring point and this is of relevance to the speaker. • non-perfective 2.1.single non- completed action 2.1. Several actions (habitual action) English 2.Progressive aspect as an example of non-perfective aspect It describes action as on-going from the point of view of a past/present/future anchor contained in the when- sentence. I was building a ship, when you came. Past progressive I am building a ship. Present progressive I will be buidling a ship, when …. Future progressive Additional tense meaning for present progressive : immediate future I am coming.(=I will come; I am on my way) 3 Perfect Tenses in English as perfective aspect: In addition to the present anchor, which locates the time of the action, Perfect tenses in English use an (additional ) aspect anchor. -
The Telicity Parameter Revisited Hana Filip SRI in Ternational- NL P Group Discern & Stanfo Rd University
The Telicity Parameter Revisited Hana Filip SRI In ternational- NL P Group Discern & Stanfo rd University 1. Introduction The goals of this paper are threefold: First, to review some recent syntactic accounts of cross-linguistic differences in the expression of telicity in Slavic vs. Germanic languages. Second, I will argue that the parametric variation in the encoding of telicity cannot be based on a unidirectional specifier head agreement between the verbal functional head linked to the telicity of the VP and the DO-DP in its specifier position, with languages exhibiting two clearly distinct modes of assigning telicity to the functional head. In the simplest terms, in Germanic languages, it is assIgned by the DO-DP and in Slavic by the perfective/imperfective aspect of the lexical VP head. Rather, in a given telicity structure in both Slavic and Germanic languages, we actually observe mutual constraints and interactions between the head verb and one of its semantic arguments, namely, the incremental argument. Third, the variation in the encoding of telicity cannot be limited just to syntactic factors. Instead, it is semantic (and also pragmatic) factors that ultimately motivate (i) the phenomena that the syntactic parametric approach tries to capture, and also (ii) telicity phenomena that are a priori precluded by it, leftout or unnoticed. In this connection, I will defend the familiar (though often forgotten) insights of Krifka's (1986 and elsewhere) and Dowty's (1991) mereological theory oftelicity. 2. The syntactic telicity parameter One dominant strand of research locates all variation in the encoding of telicity in syntax, with much of the work being motivated by assumptions of Minimalism and/or Distributed Morphology.