Weight-By-Position Adjunction and Syllable Structure1
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Saami and Scandinavians in the Viking
Jurij K. Kusmenko Sámi and Scandinavians in the Viking Age Introduction Though we do not know exactly when Scandinavians and Sámi contact started, it is clear that in the time of the formation of the Scandinavian heathen culture and of the Scandinavian languages the Scandinavians and the Sámi were neighbors. Archeologists and historians continue to argue about the place of the original southern boarder of the Sámi on the Scandinavian peninsula and about the place of the most narrow cultural contact, but nobody doubts that the cultural contact between the Sámi and the Scandinavians before and during the Viking Age was very close. Such close contact could not but have left traces in the Sámi culture and in the Sámi languages. This influence concerned not only material culture but even folklore and religion, especially in the area of the Southern Sámi. We find here even names of gods borrowed from the Scandinavian tradition. Swedish and Norwegian missionaries mentioned such Southern Sámi gods such as Radien (cf. norw., sw. rå, rådare) , Veralden Olmai (<Veraldar goð, Frey), Ruona (Rana) (< Rán), Horagalles (< Þórkarl), Ruotta (Rota). In Lule Sámi we find no Scandinavian gods but Scandinavian names of gods such as Storjunkare (big ruler) and Lilljunkare (small ruler). In the Sámi languages we find about three thousand loan words from the Scandinavian languages and many of them were borrowed in the common Scandinavian period (550-1050), that is before and during the Viking Age (Qvigstad 1893; Sammallahti 1998, 128-129). The known Swedish Lapponist Wiklund said in 1898 »[...] Lapska innehåller nämligen en mycket stor mängd låneord från de nordiska språken, av vilka låneord de äldsta ovillkorligen måste vara lånade redan i urnordisk tid, dvs under tiden före ca 700 år efter Kristus. -
Cebuano Grammar Notes Jessie Grace U. Rubrico
Cebuano Grammar Notes Jessie Grace U. Rubrico 1. Cebuano phonemes Sixteen consonants and three vowels constitute the segmental phonemes of the Cebuano language, while stress and length constitute its suprasegmentals. 1.1 Consonants according to their points of articulation Bilabial Labio-velar Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal / p / / t / / k / / ? / / b / / d / / g / / m / / n / / ? / / s / / h / / w / / l / / r / / y / 1.2 Vowels according to tongue advancement (1) and position (2), lip rounding (3). (1) Front Central Back / i / /u/ (2) High [ i ] [u] (3) mid [ I ] [o] (3) low / a / Phoneme /i/ has two phonetic representations, [i] and [I] which freely alternate. The phoneme /u/ has also two allophones which may be considered to be in complementary distribution, to wit: / u / [ u ] / ___ C# Example: ug, uy! [ o ] / (V) C___# Example: ko, mo, ako, imo 1.3 Suprasegmentals. Stress and Vowel length are phonemic in Cebuano. 1.3.1 Stress. Cebuano lexical items may be accented: a) on the penultimate vowel as in dayon , unya, ligo; b) on the final vowel as in dayón, walá, sukád; c) on both vowels in a two-syllable word, if these vowels are glottal as in sab-?a; ?ak-?ak; d) the position of the stress on the stem (i.e., final or penult) is retained when suffixation is applied as in palit > palitan e) in word reduplication, the stress of the base is carried over as primary stress on the second component while the initial component gets the secondary stress. Example, gamaygamay, hinayhinay f) the glottal stress - whether in the initial, medial, or final position- is retained whenever affixation is applied. -
Hokkaido Ainu Dialects: Variation from the Perspective of the Geographical Distribution of Vocabulary
Hokkaido Ainu Dialects: Variation from the Perspective of the Geographical Distribution of Vocabulary Mika FUKAZAWA Preparatory Office for National Ainu Museum (The foundation for Ainu culture) 1 INTRODUCTION • The aim of this study is to generate geographical maps and to describe the variations in Hokkaido Ainu dialects. • The Ainu language has three dialect categories: Hokkaido, Sakhalin, and Northern Kurils. • It is well known that “for Hokkaido, there are some differences in vocabulary, phonology, word formation, and the like between northeastern and southwestern dialects” (Tamura 2000: 3) 2 INTRODUCTION The Kamchatka Peninsula Sakhalin The Kuril Islands Hokkaido Honshu 3 INTRODUCTION • Regarding the Ainu dialects, the most important work on them was conducted by Hattori, Chiri, and their collaborators from 1955–1956. • Currently, few native speakers of Ainu exist, and collecting sufficient words and sentences through fieldwork is difficult. • My studies adopted geolinguistics for the Ainu language, referring to the audio and written materials that Hattori, Chiri, and other researchers have collected. 4 THE ATLAS OF AINU DIALECTS 5 AINU DIALECTS Hokkaido: 1. Yakumo/ 2. Oshamambe/ 3. Horobetsu/ 4. Biratori (Fukumitsu)/ 5. Nukibetsu/ 6. Niikappu/ 7. Samani/ 8. Obihiro/ 9. Kushiro/ 10. Bihoro/ 11. Asahikawa/ 12. Nayoro/ 13. Sōya/ 14. Chitose/ 15. Shizunai/ 16. Hombetsu/ 17. Mukawa/ 18. Nemuro Sakhalin: 19. Ochiho/ 20. Tarantomari/ 21. Maoka/ 22. Shiraura/ 23. Raichishka/ 24. Nairo Kurils: 25. Shumushu 6 INTRODUCTION • Nakagawa (1996) first suggested the geographical distribution and several patterns of Ainu. • Here, I consider the following major three types within Hokkaido Ainu dialects. • The Eastern-Western type • The Saru-Chitose (and Sakhalin) type • The ABA type 7 1. -
Forms and Functions of Negation in Huaraz Quechua (Ancash, Peru): Analyzing the Interplay of Common Knowledge and Sociocultural Settings
Forms and Functions of Negation in Huaraz Quechua (Ancash, Peru): Analyzing the Interplay of Common Knowledge and Sociocultural Settings Dissertation zur Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors der Philosophie am Fachbereich Geschichts- und Kulturwissenschaften der Freien Universität Berlin vorgelegt von Cristina Villari aus Verona (Italien) Berlin 2017 1. Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Michael Dürr 2. Gutachterin: Prof. Dr. Ingrid Kummels Tag der Disputation: 18.07.2017 To Ani and Leonel III Acknowledgements I wish to thank my teachers, colleagues and friends who have provided guidance, comments and encouragement through this process. I gratefully acknowledge the support received for this project from the Stiftung Lateinamerikanische Literatur. Many thanks go to my first supervisor Prof. Michael Dürr for his constructive comments and suggestions at every stage of this work. Many of his questions led to findings presented here. I am indebted to him for his precious counsel and detailed review of my drafts. Many thanks also go to my second supervisor Prof. Ingrid Kummels. She introduced me to the world of cultural anthropology during the doctoral colloquium at the Latin American Institute at the Free University of Berlin. The feedback she and my colleagues provided was instrumental in composing the sociolinguistic part of this work. I owe enormous gratitude to Leonel Menacho López and Anita Julca de Menacho. In fact, this project would not have been possible without their invaluable advice. During these years of research they have been more than consultants; Quechua teachers, comrades, guides and friends. With Leonel I have discussed most of the examples presented in this dissertation. It is only thanks to his contributions that I was able to explain nuances of meanings and the cultural background of the different expressions presented. -
Locality, Neutrality, and Contrast: a New Resolution to the Votic Paradox
Locality, neutrality, and contrast: A new resolution to the Votic paradox Daniel Currie Hall, Saint Mary’s University nels 48 • háskóli íslands • október 2017 Outline 1 Introduction 2 The paradox illustrated 3 Theoretical questions 4 The contrastive status of /i/ 5 Proposal: A new resolution to the paradox 6 Consequences 1 Introduction 1.1 The language Votic (also Vod, Votian; endonym Vaďďa tšeeli or Vađđa ceeli; iso-639 code vot): Finland • Uralic ▶ Finno-Ugric ▶ Finnic ▶ Votic Helsinki • Spoken in western Russia (four villages in Leningrad ࢫ Oblast; Kuznetsova et al. 2015: 135) Gulf of Finland ࢝ · St. Petersburg ·· ࢫ Villages with Votic speakers • “The total number of Votic speakers now could be6to Tallinn 10” (Heinsoo & Kuusk 2011: 172). • “At present, Votic is almost never used as a means of Estonia Russia communication” (Kuznetsova et al. 2015: 137). Figure 1: Location of Votic 1.2 The paradox The Votic paradox, identified by Blumenfeld & Toivonen (2016): • /i/ is transparent to vowel place harmony (as in Finnish). ☛ This suggests that /i/ is not specified for place. • But /i/ conditions a front allophone of /l/. ☛ This suggests that /i/ is specified for place. Blumenfeld & Toivonen’s solution: • Place is specified on /i/, but it is not contrastive. • Non-contrastive features are ‘weak,’ and sometimes ignored (Calabrese 1995; Nevins 2010; Rhodes 2010). • Harmony applies only to contrastive specifications. • /l/ allophony is sensitive to all specifications. My proposal: • Place is contrastive on /i/ in Votic… • …but it is marked by a different feature from the one that participates in vowel harmony. d. c. hall locality, neutrality, and contrast nels 48 2 The paradox illustrated 2.1 Harmony Front–back vowel pairs participate in place harmony, which propagates from left to right: (1) Front stem + elative /-ssA/ (Ahlqvist 1856; Ariste 1968) a. -
Agentive and Patientive Verb Bases in North Alaskan Inupiaq
AGENTTVE AND PATIENTIVE VERB BASES IN NORTH ALASKAN INUPIAQ A DISSERTATION Presented to the Faculty of the University of Alaska Fairbanks in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY By TadatakaNagai, B.Litt, M.Litt. Fairbanks, Alaska May 2006 © 2006 Tadataka Nagai Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. UMI Number: 3229741 INFORMATION TO USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. ® UMI UMI Microform 3229741 Copyright 2006 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company. All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest Information and Learning Company 300 North Zeeb Road P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. AGENTIVE AND PATIENTIYE VERB BASES IN NORTH ALASKAN INUPIAQ By TadatakaNagai ^ /Z / / RECOMMENDED: -4-/—/£ £ ■ / A l y f l A £ y f 1- -A ;cy/TrlHX ,-v /| /> ?AL C l *- Advisory Committee Chair Chair, Linguistics Program APPROVED: A a r// '7, 7-ooG Date Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. iii Abstract This dissertation is concerned with North Alaskan Inupiaq Eskimo. -
Vowel Devoicing of Ainu -How It Differs and Not Differs from Vowel Devoicing of Japanese-*
Vowel Devoicing of Ainu -How it differs and not differs from Vowel Devoicing of Japanese-* Hidetoshi Shiraishi University of Groningen 1 Introduction The oldest description of Vowel Devoicing (henceforth VD) of Ainu dates back to the earliest stages of linguistic research of the language (Kindaichi 1931, Chiri 1936, 1942). Nevertheless, our knowledge of the phenomenon has not been deepened since then. For instance, we still know little about the exact phonological environment for VD to occur, or its distribution in actual speech. This situation contrasts sharply with that of Japanese, the geographic neighbor of Ainu, where VD has always been one of the most appealing phonological topics among linguists. Numerous investigations were conducted from various linguistic fields for more than a century long. The present work is an attempt to fill this gap. It is still preliminary in nature. As a primary source, I have used sound recordings of only two speakers of a single dialect. My first task was to check VD in these recordings in order to verify whether previous works were adequate in their description. Except for a couple of recent descriptions (Tamura 1997, 1998), there were no studies that tackled this issue using sound materials in the first place. I will therefore compare the results of my investigation with those previous descriptions. Another interesting issue is whether VD of Ainu shares characteristics with VD of Japanese. Considering those facts that 1) these two languages have * Earlier versions of this paper were presented at the meetings of the Tsukuba Circle of Phonologists (July 2000), Chiba University Eurasian Society (March 2001) and De Dag van Fonetiek (December 2002). -
Inuit=People for Thousands of Years, Inuit Thought They Were Alone in The
Inuit=People For thousands of years, Inuit thought they were alone in the world. They called themselves Inuit or the people. There are different variations of “Inuit” across the circumpolar world. There are the Yupik of Siberia; Inuppiat/ Yupik of Alaska; Inuvialuit in the western Arctic of Canada; Inuinnaqtun in the Kitikmeot Region of Nunavut; Inuit who cover all of the regions from Kivalliq, Qikiqtaaluk, Nunavik and Labrador; and then the Kalaalliit of Greenland. Inuit were given the name Eskimo by the early Europeans—as we are widely known around the world today. We have always called ourselves Inuit and most regard Eskimo as a foreign word that it is. In fact, many Inuit today find the word Eskimo to be offensive and derogatory. We prefer the name Inuit as that is what we call ourselves. The word Eskimo comes from Cree and means, “eater of raw meat”. Inuit are known to eat cooked meat once in a while! In Inuktitut (our language), we have grammatical singular, dual and plural forms as opposed to the English singular and plural. That is: one person = inuk; two persons = inuuk; and, for three or more, it is inuit. When referring to the Inuit as a collective, some English-speakers may say “the Inuit people” which is, to Inuktitut-speakers, just repeating the word people twice. “The Inuit” by itself is sufficient, and that is what we prefer. Inuit believe that they have one language that they share with the rest of the other Inuit groups around the world. Even though Inuit are scattered all over the globe, they share one language-the Inuit Language1. -
Only and Focus in Imbabura Quichua 1
Only and focus in Imbabura Quichua Jos Tellings University of California, Los Angeles⇤ 1 Introduction This paper investigates the interaction of focus and the exclusive particle -lla ‘only’ in Im- babura Quichua. Imbabura Quichua (henceforth Quichua)1 is a Quechuan language spoken in Imbabura Province in Northern Ecuador. Quichua is a highly agglutinative, suffixing language with a predominantly verb-final word order. A 2008 estimate of the number of speakers is 150,000 (G´omez-Rend´on 2008:182fn.). Existing literature on this language in- cludes one descriptive grammar (Cole 1982), whereas most theoretical work on this language is directed towards (morpho)syntax (Cole and Hermon 1981; Hermon 2001; Willgohs and Farrell 2009), and its evidential system (to be discussed in section 2.2 below) (see S´anchez 2010:236↵. for a more exhaustive bibliography on the Quechuan languages). The study of focus in Quichua is worthwhile for a number of reasons. First, as I will dis- cuss in a little more detail in section 2.1 below, Quichua is a relatively uncommon language from a point of view of focus typology, because it realizes focus non-phonologically, and it has a bound morpheme exclusive particle -lla. The semantic study of focus is still domi- nated by English and other languages that realize focus by phonological means. Studying a typologically marked language will be insightful in testing our theory for cross-linguistic validity. Second, this work contributes to an existing body of research on the suffix -mi which appears in several Quechuan languages, and which belongs to perhaps the best studied parts of the Quechuan language family. -
Masterarbeit / Master's Thesis
MASTERARBEIT / MASTER’S THESIS Titel der Masterarbeit / Title of the Master’s Thesis “Dating the split of the Japonic language family. The Pre-Old Japanese corpus” verfasst von / submitted by Patrick Elmer, BA MA angestrebter akademischer Grad / in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts (MA) Wien, 2019 / Vienna 2019 Studienkennzahl lt. Studienblatt / A 066 599 degree programme code as it appears on the student record sheet: Studienrichtung lt. Studienblatt / Masterstudium Indogermanistik und historische degree programme as it appears on Sprachwissenschaft UG2002 the student record sheet: Betreut von / Supervisor: Univ.-Prof. Mag. Dr. Melanie Malzahn, Privatdoz. Table of contents Part 1: Introduction ..................................................................................................... 8 1.1 The Japonic language family .............................................................................................. 9 1.2 Previous research: When did Japonic split into Japanese and Ryūkyūan .......................... 11 1.3 Research question and scope of study .............................................................................. 15 1.4 Methodology ................................................................................................................... 16 Part 2: Language data ................................................................................................ 19 2.1 Old Japanese ................................................................................................................... -
Appositive Possession in Ainu and Around the Pacific
Appositive possession in Ainu and around the Pacific Anna Bugaeva1,2, Johanna Nichols3,4,5, and Balthasar Bickel6 1 Tokyo University of Science, 2 National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics, Tokyo, 3 University of California, Berkeley, 4 University of Helsinki, 5 Higher School of Economics, Moscow, 6 University of Zü rich Abstract: Some languages around the Pacific have multiple possessive classes of alienable constructions using appositive nouns or classifiers. This pattern differs from the most common kind of alienable/inalienable distinction, which involves marking, usually affixal, on the possessum and has only one class of alienables. The language isolate Ainu has possessive marking that is reminiscent of the Circum-Pacific pattern. It is distinctive, however, in that the possessor is coded not as a dependent in an NP but as an argument in a finite clause, and the appositive word is a verb. This paper gives a first comprehensive, typologically grounded description of Ainu possession and reconstructs the pattern that must have been standard when Ainu was still the daily language of a large speech community; Ainu then had multiple alienable class constructions. We report a cross-linguistic survey expanding previous coverage of the appositive type and show how Ainu fits in. We split alienable/inalienable into two different phenomena: argument structure (with types based on possessibility: optionally possessible, obligatorily possessed, and non-possessible) and valence (alienable, inalienable classes). Valence-changing operations are derived alienability and derived inalienability. Our survey classifies the possessive systems of languages in these terms. Keywords: Pacific Rim, Circum-Pacific, Ainu, possessive, appositive, classifier Correspondence: [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] 2 1. -
Chapter 1: Introduction and Background
CONSTRAINTS ON PATTERNS OF PRIMARY AND SECONDARY STRESS Laura W. McGarrity Submitted to the faculty of the University Graduate School in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Linguistics Indiana University August 2003 Accepted by the Graduate Faculty, Indiana University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy ____________________________________ Daniel A. Dinnsen, Ph.D., Chair ____________________________________ Stuart Davis, Ph.D. ____________________________________ Kenneth de Jong, Ph.D. August 27, 2003 ____________________________________ Judith A. Gierut, Ph.D. ii © 2003 Laura Wilbur McGarrity ALL RIGHTS RESERVED iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I owe a great deal of thanks to the members of my committee, without whose guidance and support this dissertation could never have been written: Dan Dinnsen, Stuart Davis, Ken de Jong, and Judith Gierut. I am particularly indebted to my chair, advisor, and mentor, Dan Dinnsen. He has always been a constant source of support, encouragement, and inspiration to me; it is solely because of him that I became a phonologist. His willingness to listen and discuss every idea that went into this dissertation has been absolutely invaluable to me. I am truly fortunate to have had the honor of working with him. I would also like to thank the other Linguistics faculty members at Indiana University, particularly Bob Botne. Though not a member of my committee (or even a phonologist!), he has always shown an interest in my scholarship, which I greatly appreciate. Thanks are also due to my friends and colleagues at Indiana University: Masa Deguchi, Caitlin Dillon, Marilyn Estep, Ashley Farris, B.J.