GOAL EFFECTS on ACTION and COGNITION 3;L

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

GOAL EFFECTS on ACTION and COGNITION 3;L 13 Goul Effects on Action and Cognition PETER M. GOLLWITZER GORDON B. MOSKOWITZ focus on describing the impact of a-goal on social j"ig- D esearchand theorizing on goals and their effects on (goal conten"t)to explaining the willful control of ll affect, behavior, and cägnition has become very PoP- ments proZesses involved in producing j-udgments ular in social psvchology,as äocumented by the many re- the -(goal- regulation). This has produced a metaphor ot cent edited Uäoks (".e1, Frese & Sabini, 1985; Gollwitzer relatäd asY'flexible strategists" who perhaps have a pre- & Bareh. 1996; Haliich & Kuhl, 1987; Higgins & Sor- humans ';cognitive toward being misers," but are ca- rentinol 1990; Kuhl & Beckmann, 1985rPervin, 1989) and disposition of exerting their riill-aid controlling the nature of review chapters (e.g.,Bargh, 1990; Chaiken,''Liberman,& p^lit" piocessing they expend on a given task' guslu. 198ö; Karnio"l& Rois, 1996; Karoly, 1993; Kruglan- the cognitive iri volitional control of cognitive processing .kil ibgO, Mclntosh & Martin, 1992; Tetlock, 1992) on This inierest has embraced the goal concept' Thus' despite thii theme. The reasons for this are manifold' Some are similarly of the cognitive övolution on social psychol- rooted in the theoretical developments in the psychology ift" i*pÄ"t the attemp-ted neglect of- motivational terms of motivation (see Geen, 1995;-Gollwitzer, t99l; Heck- ogv Jd .t""dt. motiväs), the goal concePt could not be h"or"t, 1991; Kuhl, 1983),others within the impact of the iul.L.. ar""y (see Miller, Gälanter, & Pribram, 1960; cognitive revolution on social psychology (see Fiske, i*äpt rgtiZl. Perhaps the goal concePt was spared be- f9ö3b; Higgins & Bargh, 1987; Smith, 1994; Stevens& ii*ä", cause goals and plari-are h-ighly zuitable to.a.c^ognitivl Fiske, 1995). (Carver^& Scheier, 1981; Kruglanski, 1996) and The psycholoqyof motivation has progressedwithin re- analysil plaväd an important role in cognitive Jcience and artifi- cerrt v"ä.i from"ä focus on describinfi the choice of action (e.g.,Wilensky, 1983). no"Ir'("tt emphasis on goal contenl) to explaining the äi"i int"llig"nce 3.o""rr", inväved in thJwillful control of goal-directed äction (an emphasison goal-relatedbehavioral regulation)' AND REGULÄTION AS This new inteiest in volition led to the embracing ot..tfq CONSTRUCTION willtul BASIC PRINCIPLES Eoalconcept, as goalsare at the starting point of the äontrol ofäction-. Assuming that cognitive activity serves focused on goal content, within the domains of purpose of controlling äcdon (as noted by James,1890, Research the how the type of goal a "ltti al*ays for my doing"), both action and thougit, examines thinking is first anä Process vari- on cognition person selects deterriines some measured outcome moäels have-begun to examine goal -effects i""ft research begins with a basic assumption that that- mediates thä regulation of thä individual's actions' äUf". people buildeis ofwhat is experiencedas reality' g,rt go^lt also affäct cognition for the purPose of aiding 'Btfi" are active it is meant that people bring to their meetings with the peiception of others and deriving meaning trom ob- from the envirönment more than the appropriate ,"r',,,ädsoc^ial events (though one might argue that ultimately stimuli hardware that simply awaits being triggered by someprop- such cognitions are ,rt"dto help äne plan actions-what "Goodtto*, "instru- that enviränment. People have selective interests Bruner, & Austen, 1956, p' 12, called ertv of (reilected by their needs, motives, and goals)'either tran- ;entJ'activity").'In examining this theme within social long t"r*, that help to shape thJconstrual of their psychology, there has been a similar progression from a ti""i.t of this chap- Roman for their helpful comments on an earlier draft thank ute Bayer, Adam Galinsky, Gabriele oettingen, and Robert J. we wish to this chapter was fncili- *o, it th" university of Konstanz, G"rrr,,.^ry The prep:rration of ter. when this chapter w:rs started., Gordon Moskowitz to Peter Gollwitzer and Gordon Moskowitz' tated by a grnnt from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 36I 363 PERSONAL MOTIVATIONAL SYSTEM social u,orld. T]ris makes action and cognition more than prior beliefs, reevaluationsof previously processedinfor_ enr.ironnrentallv ^ determined responses."Weshall refer to mation, and selectiverecall and-attention. this fundarnenial assumption as^the principl e of actiai Oyr objective in this chapter is to review the evidence constntction. ^ for these goal-relatedprinciples as demonstrated through Thus behavior is not triggered simply by features of the researchon goal effects on action and social cognition. Yn environment, but by thJlnteractLin'of those features each of these domains, we focus first on what üe have Ia_ rvith the properties of the individual. peopie choose be_ beled as goal content research and then on what we have tween many possible courses of action withlin particular a labeled as regulated-process research. In each section, situation, with any given individual's chosen reisponse to goal content research is described as concerned with goal identical environmental features subiected to thä review influences on some outcome-how having " god 5f " of that person's prevailing idiosyncrätic goals. Similarly, particular content versus one of a differeit coätent (or teaturesol the environment do not automatically trigger having no goal specified) determines responses. Goal cognrJion. Rather than there being a one_to_one coäe_ content research on both action and cognition contains spo_ndencebetween the physical at-tributes of a stimulus assumptions about the sources and the se'iection of goals. and its mental representätion (in which the processine but each has focused- mor-e on the impact of " gä of system is seen as a recorder or camera, transcrfuine factf specified content rather than on the äanner ln"which perception and inference are viewed as subjectivEty a"_ that content becomes specified. Regulated-process re_ termined (Allport, lg54). people choose between many search is described as concernea irittr thä processes possible interpretations within a particular situation, ,e through which goals help the individual regulaie a goal- with any given individual's judgmänts and inferences g directed response--how people go about negotiäting trom identical environmental features subjected to the their strivings. We begin with goal influences oi behavl review of prevailing goals. The individual contributes "a ior because traditional-ly goals t'arre been analyzed as di_ E somewhat more than healthy pair of eyes and the ap_'& rectors ofaction. f propriate re,sponsemechanismsn (postman, + B.uner, Before turning to the analysisof goal effects McGinnies, 1948, *Wiren on action : p. 142). As Vives (1540) noted: and cognition, we wish to stress ,:. we the following two points: altirm that a thing is or_is ooJ . we judge not ac_ cording to the-things ihemsel.,es, but rathei acäordins to l. The goal-related_principlespreviouslyintroduced are the concept of our mind, becausefor us the . mind is m"ea_ rn tact partly derived from an assumption about human sure of reality, not reality itself.,' pllchologicd functioning that has littlä to do with goals. Researchfocused o,n tfe processes through which goal I his initial assumptionis simply the belief that humarisare effects are exerted also beg'ins with- a basic assumption. bounded in their abilities tdrespond to the social world. Tfrg is that p"oil" have desired .ass.umption states toward Thus, with regard to action, all possible behavioral strate- whichthey aspire and continue striving toward these ends gie,s within Si"9n situatiori cannot be implemented, until the experienced state sufficientlly approximates 1n/ the and thus needs and desiresmust be fulfilled bv specifvins desiredstate (or the desired state is "lt".ed). Attainine this subsets of goal-directed behavior, only some of .,it i"titt E state providesa senseof coherencefor individuals ^s"it al_ individual can carry out. Additionally, certain behaviors lows them to experience the world in a manner that con_ within this subsetmay not be feasibleio enact becausethe torms to their beliefs, wishes, desires,values, and needs. plrson lacks the "opibltlty to do so. It is within this realm Failure to attain it energizes the indiviäual to strive toward of first deliberating on wiihes and desires and then imple_ achievingcoherenc-e._Süch strivings, however, are ofa pro_ menting the processesthrough which these qet translaied ceduralnature, and the procedurei must somehowbe .äg,r_ into action (in the face of-obstructions anä diversions) lated. They need to be protected frorn obrt^"I;, "la;.äi" where goals exert their impact, and the choice as to how to ot changing environments - :ne 1ac,e and needs, and brought regulate behavior begins. to a halt when either deemed no longer attainable or wh"en Similarly, with regärd to cognition, the stimuli presented sufficiently reached. We shall ,uf", ?o ttis ;;sumption re_ in any situation are too numerous and complex for-total rep_ garding processesthrough which people pursue'goals as resentation by the information processin! system.l Thu's, the principle- of regulated coherenci. Th"s, in examinins ac_ only certain^ e]ements from biäoming tron we shall see -the-"great , buzzing that the processesthrough which äoak confusion of the (Lippman, desiräd outer world" lgZZ,7.55) arä fl'9^"^r_lii"i"g loward statesare *fii"täifrrffi selected_for cognitive p.ocessing,-iith other in?ormatlon steps such as deliberating, planning, shielding 'filtered; an ongoins being out (s6e Broadü'ent,Ig58; Bruner, Ig57; course ofaction, steppin{up effortl', and evaTuati;;;;t Deutsch & Deutsch, 1963; Treisman& Geffen, Similarty, 1967).Ad- process models in social colnition dltionally, oescrlbe.peoplei::llf*ar certain information that the individual desires to as striving toward having a senseof"coher_ p.o:::r that passesthrough the attentional fllter may ence with respect to their judgments not be "rrä i.rf"r"rrces. This {-easibleto deliberate on Lecause he desrredstate is or she lacks thä cosni_ met when curräntly acceptedknowledee is tive experienced capacity to do so.
Recommended publications
  • The Situational Character: a Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal , 93 93 Geo L
    Santa Clara Law Santa Clara Law Digital Commons Faculty Publications Faculty Scholarship 11-2004 The ituaS tional Character: A Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal Jon Hanson Santa Clara University School of Law David Yosifon Santa Clara University School of Law Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.law.scu.edu/facpubs Part of the Law and Economics Commons, Law and Society Commons, and the Legal History Commons Automated Citation Jon Hanson and David Yosifon, The Situational Character: A Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal , 93 93 Geo L. J. 1 (2004), Available at: https://digitalcommons.law.scu.edu/facpubs/59 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Faculty Scholarship at Santa Clara Law Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of Santa Clara Law Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Articles The Situational Character: A Critical Realist Perspective on the Human Animal JON HANSON* & DAVID YOSIFON** Th is Article is dedicated to retiring the now-dominant "rational actor" model of human agency, together with its numerous "dispositionist" cohorts, and replacing them with a new conception of human agency that the authors call the "situational character." Th is is a key installment of a larger project recently introduced in an article titled The Situation: An Introduction to the Situational Character, Critical Realism, Power Economics, and Deep Capture. 1 That introduc­ tory article adumbrated, often in broad stroke, the central premises and some basic conclusions of a new app roach to legal theory and policy analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • Working Memory, Cognitive Miserliness and Logic As Predictors of Performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test
    Working Memory, Cognitive Miserliness and Logic as Predictors of Performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test Edward J. N. Stupple ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Maggie Gale ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Christopher R. Richmond ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Abstract Most participants respond that the answer is 10 cents; however, a slower and more analytic approach to the The Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) was devised to measure problem reveals the correct answer to be 5 cents. the inhibition of heuristic responses to favour analytic ones. The CRT has been a spectacular success, attracting more Toplak, West and Stanovich (2011) demonstrated that the than 100 citations in 2012 alone (Scopus). This may be in CRT was a powerful predictor of heuristics and biases task part due to the ease of administration; with only three items performance - proposing it as a metric of the cognitive miserliness central to dual process theories of thinking. This and no requirement for expensive equipment, the practical thesis was examined using reasoning response-times, advantages are considerable. There have, moreover, been normative responses from two reasoning tasks and working numerous correlates of the CRT demonstrated, from a wide memory capacity (WMC) to predict individual differences in range of tasks in the heuristics and biases literature (Toplak performance on the CRT. These data offered limited support et al., 2011) to risk aversion and SAT scores (Frederick, for the view of miserliness as the primary factor in the CRT.
    [Show full text]
  • Mind Perception Daniel R. Ames Malia F. Mason Columbia
    Mind Perception Daniel R. Ames Malia F. Mason Columbia University To appear in The Sage Handbook of Social Cognition, S. Fiske and N. Macrae (Eds.) Please do not cite or circulate without permission Contact: Daniel Ames Columbia Business School 707 Uris Hall 3022 Broadway New York, NY 10027 [email protected] 2 What will they think of next? The contemporary colloquial meaning of this phrase often stems from wonder over some new technological marvel, but we use it here in a wholly literal sense as our starting point. For millions of years, members of our evolving species have gazed at one another and wondered: what are they thinking right now … and what will they think of next? The interest people take in each other’s minds is more than idle curiosity. Two of the defining features of our species are our behavioral flexibility—an enormously wide repertoire of actions with an exquisitely complicated and sometimes non-obvious connection to immediate contexts— and our tendency to live together. As a result, people spend a terrific amount of time in close company with conspecifics doing potentially surprising and bewildering things. Most of us resist giving up on human society and embracing the life of a hermit. Instead, most perceivers proceed quite happily to explain and predict others’ actions by invoking invisible qualities such as beliefs, desires, intentions, and feelings and ascribing them without conclusive proof to others. People cannot read one another’s minds. And yet somehow, many times each day, most people encounter other individuals and “go mental,” as it were, adopting what is sometimes called an intentional stance, treating the individuals around them as if they were guided by unseen and unseeable mental states (Dennett, 1987).
    [Show full text]
  • Nawj Psych Terms
    NAWJ Terms List 1 Psychological terms useful in understanding mechanisms allowing unconscious bias Accentuation Effect: Overestimation of similarities among people within a group and dissimilarities between people from different groups Accentuation principle: States that categorization accentuates perceived similarities within and differences between groups on dimensions that people believe are correlated with the category. The effect is amplified where the categorization/dimension has subjective importance, relevance or value Actor-Observer effect: Tendency to attribute our own behaviors externally and others’ behaviors internally Agentic mode: State of mind thought by Milgram to characterize unquestioning obedience, in which people transfer personal responsibility to the person giving orders Anchoring and adjustment : A cognitive short-cut in which inferences are tied to initial standards or schemas Attitude: A relatively enduring organization of beliefs, feelings and behavioral tendencies towards socially significant objects, groups, events or symbols. Attitude change can occur by inducing someone to perform an act that runs counter to an existing attitude. Attribution : The process of assigning a cause to behaviors and events Availability bias: A cognitive shortcut in which the frequency or likelihood of an event is based on how quickly instances or associations come to mind Bias blind spot: tendency to perceive cognitive and motivational biases much more in others than in oneself Cognition: The knowledge, beliefs, thoughts, and ideas
    [Show full text]
  • 50 Cognitive and Affective Biases in Medicine (Alphabetically)
    50 Cognitive and Affective Biases in Medicine (alphabetically) Pat Croskerry MD, PhD, FRCP(Edin), Critical Thinking Program, Dalhousie University Aggregate bias: when physicians believe that aggregated data, such as those used to develop clinical practice guidelines, do not apply to individual patients (especially their own), they are exhibiting the aggregate fallacy. The belief that their patients are atypical or somehow exceptional, may lead to errors of commission, e.g. ordering x-rays or other tests when guidelines indicate none are required. Ambiguity effect: there is often an irreducible uncertainty in medicine and ambiguity is associated with uncertainty. The ambiguity effect is due to decision makers avoiding options when the probability is unknown. In considering options on a differential diagnosis, for example, this would be illustrated by a tendency to select options for which the probability of a particular outcome is known over an option for which the probability is unknown. The probability may be unknown because of lack of knowledge or because the means to obtain the probability (a specific test, or imaging) is unavailable. The cognitive miser function (choosing an option that requires less cognitive effort) may also be at play here. Anchoring: the tendency to perceptually lock on to salient features in the patient’s initial presentation too early in the diagnostic process, and failure to adjust this initial impression in the light of later information. This bias may be severely compounded by the confirmation bias. Ascertainment bias: when a physician’s thinking is shaped by prior expectation; stereotyping and gender bias are both good examples. Attentional bias: the tendency to believe there is a relationship between two variables when instances are found of both being present.
    [Show full text]
  • Croskerry MD, Phd, FRCP(Edin)
    Clinical Decision Making + Strategies for Cognitive Debiasing Pat Croskerry MD, PhD, FRCP(Edin) International Association of Endodontists Scottsdale, Arizona June 2019 Financial Disclosures or other Conflicts of Interest None It is estimated that an American adult makes 35,000 decisions a day i.e. about 2200 each waking hour Sollisch J: The cure for decision fatigue. Wall Street Journal, 2016 Decision making ‘The most important decision we need to make in Life is how we are going to make decisions’ Professor Gigerenzer Is there a problem with the way we think and make decisions? 3 domains of decision making Patients Healthcare leadership Healthcare providers Patients Leading Medical Causes of Death in the US and their Preventability in 2000 Cause Total Preventability (%) Heart disease 710,760 46 Malignant neoplasms 553,091 66 Cerebrovascular 167,661 43 Chronic respiratory 122,009 76 Accidents 97,900 44 Diabetes mellitus 69,301 33 Acute respiratory 65,313 23 Suicide 29,350 100 Chronic Liver disease 26,552 75 Hypertension/renal 12,228 68 Assault (homicide) 16,765 100 All other 391,904 14 Keeney (2008) Healthcare leadership Campbell et al, 2017 Healthcare providers US deaths in 2013 • 611,105 Heart disease • 584,881 Cancer • 251,454 Medical error Medical error is the 3rd leading cause of death Estimated number of preventable hospital deaths due to diagnostic failure annually in the US 40,000 – 80,000 Leape, Berwick and Bates JAMA 2002 Diagnostic failure is the biggest problem in patient safety Newman-Toker, 2017 Sources of Diagnostic Failure The System 25% The Individual 75% Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N.
    [Show full text]
  • Towards a Balanced Social Psychology: Causes, Consequences, and Cures for the Problem-Seeking Approach to Social Behavior and Cognition
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2004) 27, 000–000 Printed in the United States of America Towards a balanced social psychology: Causes, consequences, and cures for the problem-seeking approach to social behavior and cognition Joachim I. Krueger Department of Psychology, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912. [email protected] http://www.brown.edu/departments/psychology/faculty/krueger.html David C. Funder Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, CA 92506 [email protected] http://www.psych.ucr.edu/faculty/funder/rap/rap.htm Abstract: Mainstream social psychology focuses on how people characteristically violate norms of action through social misbehaviors such as conformity with false majority judgments, destructive obedience, and failures to help those in need. Likewise, they are seen to violate norms of reasoning through cognitive errors such as misuse of social information, self-enhancement, and an over-readiness to at- tribute dispositional characteristics. The causes of this negative research emphasis include the apparent informativeness of norm viola- tion, the status of good behavior and judgment as unconfirmable null hypotheses, and the allure of counter-intuitive findings. The short- comings of this orientation include frequently erroneous imputations of error, findings of mutually contradictory errors, incoherent interpretations of error, an inability to explain the sources of behavioral or cognitive achievement, and the inhibition of generalized the- ory. Possible remedies include increased attention to the complete range of behavior and judgmental accomplishment, analytic reforms emphasizing effect sizes and Bayesian inference, and a theoretical paradigm able to account for both the sources of accomplishment and of error. A more balanced social psychology would yield not only a more positive view of human nature, but also an improved under- standing of the bases of good behavior and accurate judgment, coherent explanations of occasional lapses, and theoretically grounded suggestions for improvement.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Stereotypes and the Predictive Brain: Cognition and Culture in “Biased” Person Perception
    ARTICLE Received 28 Jan 2017 | Accepted 17 Jul 2017 | Published 1 Sep 2017 DOI: 10.1057/palcomms.2017.86 OPEN Implicit stereotypes and the predictive brain: cognition and culture in “biased” person perception Perry Hinton1 ABSTRACT Over the last 30 years there has been growing research into the concept of implicit stereotypes. Particularly using the Implicit Associations Test, it has been demon- strated that experimental participants show a response bias in support of a stereotypical association, such as “young” and “good” (and “old” and “bad”) indicating evidence of an implicit age stereotype. This has been found even for people who consciously reject the use of such stereotypes, and seek to be fair in their judgement of other people. This finding has been interpreted as a “cognitive bias”, implying an implicit prejudice within the individual. This article challenges that view: it is argued that implicit stereotypical associations (like any other implicit associations) have developed through the ordinary working of “the predictive brain”. The predictive brain is assumed to operate through Bayesian principles, developing associations through experience of their prevalence in the social world of the perceiver. If the predictive brain were to sample randomly or comprehensively then stereotypical associations would not be picked up if they did not represent the state of the world. However, people are born into culture, and communicate within social networks. Thus, the implicit stereotypical associations picked up by an individual do not reflect a cognitive bias but the associations prevalent within their culture—evidence of “culture in mind”. Therefore to understand implicit stereotypes, research should examine more closely the way associations are communicated within social networks rather than focusing exclusively on an implied cognitive bias of the individual.
    [Show full text]
  • How Can Geologic Decision Making Under Uncertainty Be Improved? Cristina G
    Solid Earth Discuss., https://doi.org/10.5194/se-2019-70 Manuscript under review for journal Solid Earth Discussion started: 5 April 2019 c Author(s) 2019. CC BY 4.0 License. How can geologic decision making under uncertainty be improved? Cristina G. Wilson1,2, Clare E. Bond3, and Thomas F. Shipley4 1Department of Psychology, College of Arts and Sciences, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA 2Department of Electrical and Systems Engineering, School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA 3Department of Geology and Petroleum Geology, School of Geosciences, University of Aberdeen, Kings College, Aberdeen, UK 4Department of Psychology, College of Arts and Sciences, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA Correspondence: Cristina G. Wilson ([email protected]) Abstract. In the geosciences, recent attention has been paid to the influence of uncertainty on expert decision making. When making decisions under conditions of uncertainty, people tend to employ heuristics (rules of thumb) based on experience, re- lying on their prior knowledge and beliefs to intuitively guide choice. Over 50 years of decision making research in cognitive psychology demonstrates that heuristics can lead to less-than-optimal decisions, collectively referred to as biases. For example, 5 the availability bias occurs when people make judgments based on what is most dominant or accessible in memory; a geosci- entist who has spent the past several months studying strike-slip faults will have this terrain most readily available in her mind when interpreting new seismic data. Given the important social and commercial implications of many geoscience decisions, there is a need to develop effective interventions for removing or mitigating decision bias.
    [Show full text]
  • Taking Behavioralism Too Seriously? the Unwarranted Pessimism of the New Behavioral Analysis of Law
    William & Mary Law Review Volume 43 (2001-2002) Issue 5 Article 4 April 2002 Taking Behavioralism Too Seriously? The Unwarranted Pessimism of the New Behavioral Analysis of Law Gregory Mitchell Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarship.law.wm.edu/wmlr Part of the Jurisprudence Commons Repository Citation Gregory Mitchell, Taking Behavioralism Too Seriously? The Unwarranted Pessimism of the New Behavioral Analysis of Law, 43 Wm. & Mary L. Rev. 1907 (2002), https://scholarship.law.wm.edu/wmlr/vol43/iss5/4 Copyright c 2002 by the authors. This article is brought to you by the William & Mary Law School Scholarship Repository. https://scholarship.law.wm.edu/wmlr TAKING BEHAVIORALISM TOO SERIOUSLY? THE UNWARRANTED PESSIMISM OF THE NEW BEHAVIORAL ANALYSIS OF LAW GREGORY MITCHELL* ABSTRACT Legal scholars increasingly rely on a behavioral analysis of judgment and decision making to explain legalphenomena and argue for legal reforms. The. main argument of this new behavioralanalysis of the law is twofold: (1)All human cognition is beset by systematic flaws in the way that judgments and decisionsare made, andtheseflaws lead topredictable irrational behaviors and (2) these widespreadand systematic nonrational tendencies bring into serious question the assumption of procedural rationality underlying much legal doctrine. This Article examines the psychological research relied on by legal behavioraliststo form this argumentand demonstratesthat this research does not support the bleak and simple portrait of pervasiveirrationality painted by these scholars.Careful scrutiny of the psychological researchreveals greater adherence to norms of rationalitythan that implied by the legal behavioralists,and the methodological and interpretive limitations on this psychological research make extrapolation from experimental settings to real world legal settings often inappropriate.
    [Show full text]
  • Cumulative Subject Index Volumes 1–25 (Contains Links to Entries) ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSTUVWXYZ
    European Review of Social Psychology Cumulative Subject Index Volumes 1–25 (contains links to entries) ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSTUVWXYZ Locators in bold refer to figures/tables goal-based explanations (12) 201–235 – Action control, behavioural intentions Ability, praise and criticism (3) 259 283 (12) 21 Abnormal conditions focus model (2) 59–61 – Action control, emotion regulation (23) Aboriginal Peoples, Canada (24) 15 16 146–156 Absentminded relapses (22) 20 Action initiation, implementation intentions Abstract language use (25) see Linguistic (4) 166–171 abstraction Action readiness (22) 116, 117; (22) 153 Abstract modelling, indirect intergroup Action for social change (24) 244 contact (25) 319, 375 Action-oriented personality factors (24) 146, Abstract reasoning (22) 169 146–147 Abstraction, intragroup variability (7) – Action tendencies, conflict resolution (25) 4, 111 113 9–10, 18 Abu Ghraib, prisoners (24) 2 Activation of attitudes (24) 270–273 Academic personnel selection (25) see Actor effects (22) 367, 369 Personnel selection Actor perspective, positive–negative Academic procrastination (24) 155, 156 asymmetry (1) 38–39 Access consciousness (22) 8–9 – – Actor observer bias Access dissociations (22) 4, 10 17 agent vs. recipient (16) 182–184 Acceptance of death, terror management – linguistic category model (LCM) (2) theory (25) 57, 58 59 17–18 Accessible knowledge prediction (6) 13–16 meta-cognition (22) 185–186 – Actor-Partner Interdependence Model priming (22) 183 185 (APIM) (22) 350; (22) 367 Accessibility of thoughts Acute stress
    [Show full text]
  • ED Cognition: Any Decision by Anyone at Any Time
    ADMINISTRATION SERIES N SE´RIE SUR L’ADMINISTRATION ED cognition: any decision by anyone at any time Pat Croskerry, MD, PhD The ability of an emergency department (ED) to meet threaten patient safety.2,3 These fell into two broad the needs of its patients depends largely on the groups: processes that are largely under the immediate resources available. Relevant resources include nurses, control of the individual and processes that depend physicians, paramedics, specialty backup, care spaces, more on system design and function (Figure 1). Patient inpatient bed base, space, equipment, and supplies. All safety can be compromised by errors in any of these are important to ED function; however, one resource processes, but the greatest impact is likely when that is seldom considered is cognition, the cognitive individual cognition is compromised. The system- capacity of ED staff. Although invisible, it is arguably individual approach described above is a convenient our most critical resource. Everything that is done for way of looking at ED function, but it should be noted patients—their diagnosis, management, treatment, and that the two process types are not independent. disposition—requires decision making that depends on Considerable overlap exists; at the end of the day, all both individual and collective cognition. In the past, aspects of the system are designed by individuals. To we have taken cognition for granted, assuming that if further complicate matters, consider that each process we bring physicians, nurses, paramedics, and patients can be divided into subprocesses. For example, together, the necessary decision making will occur. laboratory error may involve five main subprocesses, However, things may not be quite so simple.
    [Show full text]