<<

University of Nebraska Medical Center DigitalCommons@UNMC

Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies

Summer 8-18-2017

Bedtime Fading with Response Cost for Treatment of Disturbances in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Sela Ann Sanberg University of Nebraska Medical Center

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unmc.edu/etd

Part of the Applied Behavior Analysis Commons, Behavior and Behavior Mechanisms Commons, Psychology Commons, Clinical Psychology Commons, Commons, and the Commons

Recommended Citation Sanberg, Sela Ann, "Bedtime Fading with Response Cost for Treatment of Sleep Disturbances in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder" (2017). Theses & Dissertations. 224. https://digitalcommons.unmc.edu/etd/224

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate Studies at DigitalCommons@UNMC. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses & Dissertations by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@UNMC. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Running Head: BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 1

Bedtime Fading with Response Cost for Treatment of Sleep Disturbances in

Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

by

Sela Ann Sanberg, Ph.D., BCBA

A DISSERTATION

Presented to the Faculty of

the University of Nebraska Medical Center Graduate College

in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Medical Sciences Interdepartmental Area Graduate Program

Psychology Department

(Applied Behavior Analysis)

Under the Supervision of Doctor Brett R. Kuhn

University of Nebraska Medical Center Omaha, Nebraska

July, 2017

Supervisory Committee:

Keith Allen, Ph.D. Holly Roberts, Ph.D. William Warzak, Ph.D. BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 2 2

Bedtime Fading with Response Cost for Treatment of Sleep Disturbances in

Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Sela A. Sanberg, Ph.D., BCBA

University of Nebraska Medical Center, 2017

Advisor: Brett R. Kuhn, Ph.D., C.BSM

As many as 82% of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) experience numerous chronic sleep-related problems and at a much higher frequency and severity than their typically developing peers. Behavioral treatments are considered best practice and first-line treatment to address sleep problems. These treatments tend to address one specific sleep-related behavior at a time. Bedtime Fading with Response Cost (BFRC) is a promising intervention that targets a multitude of sleep problems concurrently and has yet to be replicated by more than one investigative team in the home setting with children on the autism spectrum. This study evaluated the effectiveness of BFRC in decreasing sleep disturbances in children with ASD using as change agents by implementing treatment in the home environment. A non-concurrent multiple baseline design across three participants was used. Results indicate that BFRC was effective in eliminating unwanted co-sleeping, frequent night awakenings, and dependent and reducing sleep onset latency, bedtime resistance, and disruptive sleep-related behaviors.

Follow-up data demonstrate that gains were maintained. Parents reported high satisfaction with BFRC and sleep outcomes for their children. This study extends both the

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 3 3 practice and science of -implemented behavioral interventions as treatment options for children with ASD and co-occurring sleep problems.

Keywords: autism, sleep, children, parents, behavioral intervention

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 4 4

Bedtime Fading with Response Cost for Treatment of Sleep Disturbances in Children

with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Introduction

Background

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder affecting one out of 68 children in the United States (Center for Disease Control and Prevention

[CDC], 2016). ASD is characterized by core deficits in social communication and social interactions and atypical repetitive behaviors and restricted interests (American

Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013; CDC, 2016; Reynolds & Malow, 2011). Sleep disturbance is one of the most common and challenging behaviors experienced by children with ASD (Dominick, Ornstein Davis, Lainhart, Tager-Flusberg, & Folstein,

2007; Levin & Scher, 2016; Reynolds & Malow, 2011; Wiggs & Stores, 2004) and is frequently associated with co-occurring behavioral difficulties (Adams, Matson, & Jang,

2014; Mazurek & Sohl, 2016). Prevalence rates of chronic sleep-related problems in children with ASD range from approximately 52% to 82% compared with 32% to 50% in children with typical development (Hodge, Carollo, Lewin, Hoffman, & Sweeney, 2014;

Krakowiak, Goodlin-Jones, Hertz-Picciotto, Croen, & Hansen, 2008).

Sleep disturbance is an umbrella term for a range of sleep-related problems presumably lacking an underlying medical basis, as opposed to sleep disorders. In the pediatric sleep literature, the sleep disturbance and sleep problems terms are often used interchangeably (Turner & Johnson, 2012) to refer to common parent-reported sleep concerns such as bedtime refusal, delayed sleep onset, frequent night awakenings, and undesired co-sleeping (Blampied & France, 1993). Parents of children with ASD report a

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 5 5 variety of disturbed sleep behaviors, such as reduced sleep duration, delayed sleep onset, early rising, excessive daytime sleep, lack of independent sleep initiation skills, frequent night waking, difficulty following bedtime routines and undesired co-sleeping

(Christodulu & Durand, 2004; Durand, 2002; Knight & Johnson, 2014; Moon, Corkum,

& Smith, 2010; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997; Weiskop, Richdale, & Matthews, 2005).

Notably, these particular sleep problems are not uniquely different than those reported by parents of typically developing children. It is clear, however, that children with ASD are affected by disrupted sleep at a much higher frequency and with a much greater severity than their typically developing peers (Anders, Iosif, Schwichtenberg, Tang & Goodlin-

Jones, 2011; Hodge, Carollo, Lewin, Hoffman, & Sweeney, 2014; Krakowiak, Goodlin-

Jones, Hertz-Picciotto, Croen, & Hansen, 2008; Owens & Whitmans, 2004).

Parents’ report that their child’s sleep problems exacerbate difficulties with social functioning, mood, and fatigue (Lopez-Wagner, Hoffman, Sweeney, Hodge, & Gilliam,

2008), which may, in turn, decrease the child’s quality of life and limit independent functioning. Sleep problems in children with ASD can also have detrimental effects on the entire household (Patzold, Richdale, & Tonge, 1998; Tilford, et al., 2015). When children do not sleep, it often means that their siblings and parents do not sleep, either.

Parents of children with ASD who sleep poorly during the previous night are likely to have an increased negative affect/decreased positive affect the following day (Mihaila &

Hartley, 2016). Additionally, mothers who experience chronic poor sleep perceive their child’s behavior problems as occurring at a higher frequency and severity and/or their own skills as less effective (Mihaila & Hartley, 2016).

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 6 6

Parents of children with ASD experience higher levels of stress and depression and lower levels of marital intimacy than parents of children with and without other disabilities (Fisman, Wolf, & Noh, 1989; Levin & Scher, 2016; Tilford, et al., 2015).

Parents of children with ASD report a lack of time for activities, recreation, and for each other, which can negatively impact the quality of (Fisman, Wolf, &

Noh, 1989). This may explain why sleep disturbances experienced by children with ASD often result in increased family stress, marital discord, and decreased parental intimacy

(Goodlin-Jones, Tang, Liu, & Anders, 2008; Fisman, Wolf, & Noh, 1989; Hoffman, et. al., 2008; Mihaila & Hartley, 2016; Meltzer, 2011; Patzold, Richdale, & Tonge, 1998;

Piazza & Fisher, 1991b; Tilford, et al., 2015).

Treatment

Pharmacological agents in conjunction with behavioral intervention are often recommended for the treatment of sleep disturbance in children with ASD (Johnson &

Malow, 2008). Medications prescribed to children with ASD to treat sleep disturbance include synthetic melatonin, benzodiazepines, clonidine, beta-blockers, as well as antipsychotics as a means to counter undesired motor effects (Murray, et. al., 2014).

Melatonin is a naturally occurring hormone released by the pineal gland to help promote and maintain the circadian rhythm. Synthetically derived melatonin is widely available in the United States as an over-the-counter supplement (Bramble & Feehan, 2005) and is used as a first-line treatment for promoting sleep in children on the autism spectrum

(Malow & McGrew, 2008).

To date, the use of behavioral principles (Jin, Hanley, & Beauliue, 2013; Knight

& Johnson, 2014; Turner & Johnson, 2012; Vriend, Corkum, Moon, & Smith, 2011) or

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 7 7 melatonin (Malow, et al., 2012; Rossignol & Frye, 2011) to address behavioral sleep disturbances in children with ASD has been explored in 16 and 10 studies, respectively.

Of the melatonin studies, two represent retrospective reviews of records (Andersen,

Kaczmarska, McGrew, & Malow, 2008; Gupta & Hutchins, 2005) and six combine the implementation of behavioral principles with the delivery of melatonin (Garstang &

Wallis, 2006; Giannotti, Cortesi, Cerquiglini, & Bernabei, 2006; Malow, et al., 2012;

Paavonen, Nieminen-von Wendt, Vanhala, Aronen, & von Wendt, 2003; Wasdell, et. al.,

2008; Wright, et. al., 2010). Only two studies have evaluated melatonin administration alone without concomitant use of behavioral methods, and one of them is a brief case study (Jan, Freeman, Wasdell, & Bomben, 2004; Wirojanan, et. al., 2009).

Melatonin has been shown to improve sleep outcomes in children with ASD by reducing sleep onset latency (Dodge & Wilson, 2001), and increasing total sleep time

(Wirojanan, et. al., 2009). The sustainability of such benefits, once the medication is withdrawn, has yet to be evaluated. Research results indicate that melatonin does not have an effect on the parental concerns of frequency of night awakenings (Dodge &

Wilson, 2001; Wirojanan, et al., 2009). Moreover, lacking is the evaluation of melatonin as a treatment option for other disruptive sleep behaviors that parents have identified as their primary concern (e.g., bedtime resistance and undesired co-sleeping) (Christodulu &

Durand, 2004; Weiskop, Matthews, & Richdale, 2001; Weiskop, Richdale, & Matthews

2005).

At this time, there is no established dosage or recommended length of use of melatonin in addressing sleep problems in children with ASD (Kennaway, 2015; Malow, et al., 2012; Rossignol & Frye, 2011). Some children with ASD may experience poor

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 8 8 metabolism and disappearing effects of melatonin, leading to potential dose escalation

(Braam, et al., 2012). Similar to other pharmacological agents, melatonin falls short for many parents because taking a medication does not teach healthy sleep behaviors to ensure lasting change (Malow & McGrew, 2008). Therefore, research on the exclusive use of melatonin to improve sleep disturbances in children, with and without ASD, remains limited and needs to be more rigorously evaluated (Kennaway, 2015).

In the field of behavioral sleep medicine, interventions are based on the principles of learning that employ methods of reinforcement, contingency management, shaping, and extinction to teach healthy and appropriate sleep behaviors that last a lifetime.

Interventions with a validated body of research to support their utility, referred to as empirically supported treatments (ESTs), are considered first-line treatments in behavioral sleep medicine. The ‘gold standard’ criteria for determining the validity of such interventions were first established by members of the American Psychological

Association Division 12 task force (Chambless & Ollendick, 2001). These criteria include definitions of “well established”, “probably efficacious”, and “promising” interventions, which function as guidelines for determining and choosing the best available ESTs (Chambless, Baker, Baucom, Beutler, Calhoun, Crits-Christoph, et al.

1998; Chambless & Hollon, 1998; Powers, 1999).

Behavior-based treatments are considered best practice in addressing sleep problems in typically developing children due to significant improvements experienced by 80% of children treated (Mindell, Kuhn, Lewin, Meltzer, & Sadeh, 2006). At this time, research indicates that behavioral interventions for the treatment of sleep problems in children with ASD have yet to be classified as well established or probably efficacious

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 9 9

(Schreck, 2001; Vriend, Corkum, Moon, & Smith, 2011). This may be due to insufficient methodological rigor, lack of significant or equivalent outcomes, and/or the absence of duplicated studies by more than one investigator or research group that is needed to meet classification requirements.

Behavioral interventions addressing sleep problems in children with ASD include bedtime routine (Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997; Weiskop, Richdale, & Matthews, 2005); extinction (Durand, Gernert-Dott, & Mapstone, 1996; Weiskop, Matthews, & Richdale,

2001; Wolf, Risley, & Mees, 1964); scheduled awakenings (Durand, 2002); sleep restriction (Christodulu & Durand, 2004; Durand & Christodulu, 2004); stimulus fading

(Christodulu & Durand, 2004; Howlin, 1984); chronotherapy (Piazza, Hagopian, Hughes,

& Fisher, 1998); parent education (Malow, et al., 2014; Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010;

Weiskop, Matthews, & Richdale, 2001); bedtime fading with (Moon, Corkum, & Smith,

2010; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997) and without response cost (DeLeon, Fisher, &

Marhefka, 2004); as well as multi-component treatment packages (Jin, Hanley, &

Beauliue, 2013; Knight & Johnson, 2014; Montgomery, Stores, & Wiggs, 2004; Reed, et al., 2009). Among behavioral interventions, most tend to address one specific sleep- related behavior at a time (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b), such as night terrors (Durand, 2002); disrupted sleep-wake cycle (Piazza, Hagopian, Hughes, & Fisher, 1998), and excessive daytime sleepiness (Friedman & Luiselli, 2008). In reality, parents of children with ASD tend to report a multitude of co-occurring sleep problems (e.g., bedtime resistance, delayed sleep onset, frequent night waking, and undesired co-sleeping) (Christodulu &

Durand, 2004; Durand & Christodulu, 2004; Durand, Gernert-Dott, & Mapstone, 1996;

Reed, et. al., 2009; Weiskop, Matthews, & Richdale, 2001).

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 10 10

Parents of children with ASD report that some sleep disturbances evolve from their own attempts to address other long-standing sleep problems, such as engaging in undesired co-sleeping to reduce night awakenings (Reed, et al., 2009). Undesired co- sleeping can be a part of the initial bedtime routine (Weiskop, Matthews, & Richdale,

2001; Weiskop, Richdale, & Matthews, 2005) or a response to difficulty falling back asleep and disruptive behaviors (i.e., tantrums, refusal to sleep in own , or escaping from the bed and ) following night awakenings (Christodulu & Durand, 2004;

Durand & Christodulu, 2004; Durand, Gernert-Dott, & Mapstone, 1996; Howlin, 1984).

Therefore, it is not surprising that undesired co-sleeping experienced by children with

ASD often coincides with a wide variety of sleep problems, including frequent night awakenings, bedtime disturbances, difficulty falling asleep and reinitiating sleep independently, decreased duration of sleep, early morning awakenings, and refusal to sleep in one’s own bed (Christodulu & Durand, 2004; Durand & Christodulu, 2004;

Durand, Gernert-Dott, & Mapstone, 1996; Howlin, 1984; Reed, et. al., 2009; Weiskop,

Matthews, & Richdale, 2001; Weiskop, Richdale, & Matthews, 2005).

Bedtime Fading with Response Cost (BFRC) is a promising behavior-based intervention that targets a multitude of sleep problems concurrently (Piazza, Fisher, &

Sherer, 1997). Specifically, BFRC has been demonstrated to decrease disruptive sleep- related behaviors, reduce sleep latency, reduce night awakenings, and increase sleep duration in children with ASD (DeLeon, Fisher, & Marhefka, 2004; Moon, Corkum, &

Smith, 2010; Piazza, Fisher, & Moser, 1991; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997). BFRC has also been effective in reducing (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b; Piazza, Fisher, & Moser, 1991) and even eliminating (Ashbaugh & Peck, 1998) undesired co-sleeping in children without

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 11 11

ASD. Thus far, undesired co-sleeping has not been the primary sleep variable in studies evaluating BFRC with children on the autism spectrum. To date, undesired co-sleeping has been used as the primary dependent variable in only two studies (Weiskop,

Matthews, & Richdale, 2001; Weiskop, Richdale, & Matthews, 2005) in children with

ASD. Both studies focused on parent-defined sleep disturbances and treatment goals.

Original BFRC Protocol

BFRC involves temporarily delaying the child’s typical bedtime, establishing a set wake-time (regardless of the amount of sleep acquired the night before), and eliminating access to daytime sleep (for non-napping children ages 4 and older) (Piazza

& Fisher, 1991b). Developmental sleep norms in conjunction with parent sleep goals are used to set the appropriate bedtime and wake-time based on the amount of sleep required for each child. Parents are informed of the average hours of sleep per night needed by their child according to his or her chronological age and asked to indicate their preferred time of sleep onset, awakening, and (if applicable). The original BFRC protocol began with collecting baseline data on whether the child was asleep or awake every 30 minutes, throughout a 24-hour period, until data were determined stable. Once baseline data are stable, the child’s mean sleep onset time was calculated and the child’s bedtime was temporarily delayed by adding 30 minutes to the average time the child typically fell asleep (i.e., faded bedtime) (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b). For example, if the child’s average time of sleep onset was10:45 p.m. during baseline, the initial bedtime was set to

11:15pm.

The caregivers placed the child in bed at the scheduled bedtime and observed the child 15 minutes later to determine whether the child was asleep or awake. A caregiver

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 12 12 stood within one foot of the child and whispered his or her name. If the child did not respond to his or her name (i.e., the absence of motor or verbal response), then the child was considered to be asleep, and the caregiver recorded the time of sleep onset. If the child was not asleep, he or she was removed from his or her bed, and restrict access to the bed, bedroom, and sleep for 45 minutes (i.e., response cost). During that time, the child was allowed to partake in regular nighttime activities (e.g., with toys, watch TV) not related to sleep. At the end of the 45 minutes the child was returned to bed. If the child did not fall asleep within 15 minutes of being returned to bed, the caregivers repeated the response cost procedure by removing the child from bed for 45 minutes and allowed the child to engage in non-sleep related activities. This procedure was repeated until the child fell asleep within 15 minutes of being placed in bed (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b).

Each subsequent night the child’s bedtime was adjusted by 30 minutes based on the time of sleep onset during the previous night. For example, if bedtime was 10:30 p.m., and the child did not fall asleep until 12:00 a.m., the following night the bedtime would be 11:00 p.m.; similarly, if the child fell asleep at 10:30 p.m., the following night the bedtime would be adjusted to 10:00 p.m. For instances of nighttime awakenings, caregivers were encouraged to prompt the child to return to his or her bed every 30 minutes. Caregivers allowed the child to engage in regular nighttime activities. The predetermined wake-time was strongly enforced by the caregivers every day, regardless of the amount and quality of the child’s sleep during the previous night. Caregivers eliminated access to daytime sleep, unless were appropriate for the age and developmental needs of the child (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b).

BFRC Procedural Variants

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 13 13

Modifications to the original BFRC protocol have been reported in subsequent studies (Ashbaugh & Peck, 1998; DeLeon, Fisher, & Marhefka, 2004; Moon, Corkum, &

Smith, 2010; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997). Variations include the amount of time the bedtime is faded, the duration of time allowed to achieve sleep before response cost is implemented, the duration of the response cost procedure itself, and the duration of faded bedtime. The amount of time added to the faded bedtime and the duration of response cost, i.e., time spent out of bed when not achieving sleep onset, have been evaluated using both 30- and 60-minute intervals. The amount of time allotted between placing a child in bed and checking sleep onset has included both 15- and 20-minute intervals.

Fading the bedtime once sleep onset is achieved has been evaluated with both 15- and 30- minute intervals and after either one or two consecutive nights. In addition, the original authors of BFRC suggest fading the bedtime by 15 minutes, rather than 30 minutes, if the child’s bedtime fading plateaus (does not progress) before reaching the bedtime goal

(Piazza & Fisher, 1991b). With and without modifications to the original protocol, BFRC has been shown to be effective in addressing various co-occurring sleep disturbances with a typically developing child (Ashbaugh & Peck, 1998), children with intellectual disability (ID) (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b), children with autism (DeLeon, Fisher, &

Marhefka, 2004; Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997) and related disorders (Piazza, Fisher, & Moser, 1991).

BFRC and Children with Non-ASD Neurodevelopmental Disorders

The bedtime fading procedure without the response cost component was first developed by Piazza and Fisher (1991a) to increase the duration of appropriate nighttime sleep of two children with severe sleep problems. Treatment included utilizing baseline

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 14 14 data of each participant’s average time (i.e., within 15 minutes) of sleep onset that was most likely to occur. Faded bedtime was then determined by calculating the baseline average for sleep onset and adding one half hour. For example, if the average sleep onset time is 10:00 p.m. during baseline, then the initial bedtime during the treatment phase is

10:30 p.m. Treatment required the child to remain awake until the faded bedtime and access to the bed was not permitted until the designated bedtime. Additionally, a predetermined wake time was established and the participant was not allowed to sleep past this time (Piazza & Fisher, 1991a). Based on the previous night’s time of sleep onset, the bedtime was adjusted accordingly, such that if the child fell asleep within 15 minutes of the designated bedtime, the bedtime was adjusted 15 minutes earlier. Correspondingly, if the child did not fall asleep within the allotted 15 minutes, the bedtime was pushed later an additional 15 minutes. Bedtime adjustment required two consecutive nights of the same established sleep onset time before fading the bedtime on the third night.

Two participants, a 6-year-old female diagnosed with attention deficit- hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and a 4-year-old female diagnosed with profound ID and comorbid tuberous sclerosis were included in the study (Piazza & Fisher, 1991a). Both participants experienced multiple sleep problems, including bedtime refusal, bedtime disruption, and a lack of independent sleep initiation. The 6-year-old participant experienced frequent night awakenings and obtained only four to six hours of sleep per night. Treatment was initiated while the participant lived at home with her grandmother.

The 4-year-old participant experienced excessive daytime sleepiness and fell asleep multiple times during the day. Treatment was conducted while the participant resided at an inpatient facility for children with developmental disabilities. A momentary time

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 15 15 sampling procedure (every 30 minutes during a 24-hour cycle) was utilized to record whether the child was in or out of bed and awake or asleep. Trained observers collected data on the participant in the inpatient unit. The grandmother of the in-home participant collected data until she went to bed, with the addition of two scheduled checks at 2:00 a.m. and 4:00 a.m. (Piazza & Fisher, 1991a).

Results indicate that bedtime fading procedure increased the percentage of appropriate sleep (i.e., hours of sleep occurring within the determined timeframe) and increased the duration of sleep for both participants. In addition, bedtime fading eliminated the problem sleep behaviors of night awakening and bedtime refusal for the 6- year-old participant. The authors suggested that bedtime fading is potentially useful for combating multiple sleep problem concurrently as it may help to institute a consistent and establish the bed as a conditioned reinforcer for sleep (Piazza & Fisher,

1991a). One limitation of the study was that the authors did not use an objective measure

(e.g., actigraphy) of the participants’ target behaviors. Skill acquisition and the elimination of problem sleep behaviors were based on observers’ and caregiver reports.

The same authors were the first to evaluate bedtime fading with the added component of response cost (BFRC) (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b). The additional treatment component included removing the child from bed and keeping him or her awake for one hour if sleep was not initiated within 15 minutes of the initial bedtime. During the hour of response cost, the child was permitted access to the same items and attention as found during baseline. At the end of the one-hour interval, the child was returned to bed and allowed 15 minutes to fall asleep. If the child did not fall asleep within the allotted amount of time, he or she was removed from the bed again (i.e., response cost) and

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 16 16 required to remain awake for an additional hour. The outlined procedure was repeated until sleep onset was achieved within 15 minutes of being placed in bed. The investigators hypothesized that the response cost component would “enhance treatment efficacy” by allowing for “a more rapid fading of the bedtime”, thus increasing the reinforcing value of sleep (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b).

Participants were four nonverbal children diagnosed with profound ID and living in an inpatient unit for the treatment of ID and self-injurious behaviors. All participants experienced co-occurring sleep problems such as delayed sleep onset, inconsistent sleep duration, night awakenings, early morning awakenings, disruptive bedtime behaviors, and excessive daytime sleep, whereas one participant experienced undesired co-sleeping.

Similarly to the first bedtime fading study (Piazza & Fisher, 1991a), the investigators employed a momentary time-sampling (e.g., each half hour) method conducted by trained observers to record whether or not the participants were in or out of bed and awake or asleep. The effectiveness of the procedure was evaluated using a non-concurrent multiple baseline across subjects. The parameters for the bedtime fading component as found in the preliminary study were used in the same manner, with the response cost component previously described (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b).

Two different observers scored the child’s behavior (i.e., awake or asleep, in or out of bed) to assess inter-observer agreement (IOA). Results of the study indicated that all participants either improved in the percentage of intervals of appropriate sleep or decreased in the percentage of intervals of inappropriate sleep. Three out of four participants demonstrated a decrease in the frequency of night awakenings. Furthermore, disruptive bedtime behaviors decreased from 100% to 16% for one participant and

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 17 17 undesired co-sleeping decreased by nearly 50% for another. The authors concluded that each participant benefited from the BFRC procedure and that both classical and operant conditioning may have facilitated treatment efficacy (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b).

A third study, coauthored by Piazza, Fisher, & Moser (1991), sought to evaluate whether BFRC would be effective in improving the aberrant sleep patterns of three girls with Rett Syndrome (RS). Standard BFRC procedures were used for all three participants who were participating in inpatient (N=2) or outpatient (N=1) services for the treatment of behavior problems. Sleep problems included night awakenings, unwanted co-sleeping, delayed sleep onset, and excessive daytime sleep. Data recording methods and IOA employed by the trained observers and parent participants remained consistent with methods outlined in previous studies (Piazza & Fisher, 1991a; Piazza & Fisher, 1991b).

Results demonstrated an increase in the percentage of appropriate sleep for all participants, although clinical significance was not determined (Piazza, Fisher, & Moser,

1991). In addition, time of sleep onset decreased for one participant, and the duration of night awakenings, as well as the percentage of daytime sleep, decreased for two participants. Outcomes for undesired co-sleeping were not directly reported, although the authors did indicate that the parents of one participant were engaging in co-sleeping less frequently and spending less time soothing their child back to sleep. Similarly to the previous bedtime fading and BFRC studies (Piazza & Fisher, 1991a; Piazza & Fisher,

1991b), no information was provided on how the parent participants were trained to implement the BFRC procedure or collect data on the observable sleep variables (Piazza,

Fisher, & Moser, 1991). In addition, sleep variables were not measured objectively.

BFRC and Typically Developing Children

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 18 18

To date, only one study replicated the BFRC procedures with a typically developing child. Ashbaugh and Peck (1998) employed an ABAB experimental design with a typically developing 2-year-old female experiencing frequent night awakenings, disrupted sleep, and undesired co-sleeping. The participant lived at home with her biological father and mother, the latter being a member of the investigative team.

Ashbaugh and Peck (1998) followed the BFRC protocol and data collection procedures as presented in the Piazza and Fisher (1991b) study, with two minor modifications: 1)

The procedure included returning the child to her own bed if she awoke during the night, as requested by her parents; and 2) The response cost component was reduced from 1 hour to 30 minutes. Bedtime for the subsequent night was adjusted earlier by 30 minutes if the child was able to fall asleep within the allotted 15 minutes or later by 30 minutes if the child was not able to fall asleep within 15 minutes of bedtime (Ashbaugh & Peck,

1998). Parent and teacher observations of napping behavior were conducted and compared for IOA. Results of the study indicated that the participant was able to establish a more regular sleep schedule and undesirable co-sleeping was eliminated. The authors noted that slight modifications to the BFRC might not diminish the effectiveness of the intervention (Ashbaugh & Peck, 1998).

BFRC and Children with ASD

Three studies have evaluated the effects of BFRC in children with autism and sleep disturbance (DeLeon, Fisher, & Marhefka, 2004; Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010;

Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997). All three studies concluded that faded bedtime, with or without response cost, was successful in improving sleep problems in children with autism. Piazza, Fisher, and Sherer (1997) compared the relative effectiveness of BFRC to

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 19 19 simple bedtime scheduling, which involved establishing a set bedtime and wake time

(naps if age appropriate) and permitting sleep only during designated times. Bedtime scheduling also included implementing a consistent bedtime routine and placing the child in bed at the designated times (Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997). Five of the fourteen participants, ages 5 to 8 years, were diagnosed with ASD. Participants were randomly assigned to either the BFRC or the bedtime scheduling group. Treatment took place in an institutionalized setting as all 14 participants were being treated for severe behaviors in an inpatient unit (Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997).

Sleep problems experienced by the sample population included early awakenings, difficulties initiating sleep independently, delayed sleep onset, and nighttime awakenings.

BFRC procedures, data collections methods, and IOA techniques were adopted from previously described studies (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b; Piazza, Fisher, & Moser, 1991).

Result indicated that for all participants the BFRC intervention, compared with bedtime scheduling, produced a greater reduction of sleep problems (Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer,

1997). Of the two children with ASD in the bedtime scheduling group, one showed slight improvement with night waking and falling asleep, while the other child’s sleep problems

(i.e., night awakening and early awakening) maintained at baseline levels. All three of the participants with ASD in the BFRC group demonstrated improvements with the near elimination of early awakenings and a decrease in the duration and frequency of night awakenings. Outcomes were maintained at a 4-month follow-up. Strength of this study was the inclusion of a comparison group to demonstrate the effectiveness of BFRC.

A subsequent study evaluated the effectiveness of the faded bedtime component without response cost in solving sleep problems and preventing self-injurious behaviors

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 20 20

(SIBs) in a 4-year-old male with ASD (DeLeon, Fisher, & Marhefka, 2004). The participant awoke frequently throughout the night and engaged in head banging, flopping on the floor, and body hitting, until receiving contingent reinforcement of attention in the form of hugs and comfort. Treatment was conducted in an inpatient unit for the treatment of severe SIBs. BFRC treatment procedures, data collection methods, and IOA criteria were replicated from previous studies (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b; Piazza, Fisher, & Moser,

1991; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997). The investigators employed an AB design without a reversal condition due to the severe nature of the participants’ SIBs. Results demonstrated a decrease in the total number of night awakenings, which, in turn, lead to the reduction in the frequency and rate of the wake-related SIBs displayed by the participant (DeLeon, Fisher, & Marhefka, 2004).

The most recent BFRC study examined the effectiveness of a manualized BFRC program in the reduction of problem sleep behaviors in children with ASD (Moon,

Corkum, & Smith, 2010). All participants (N=3) had a diagnosis of ASD, lived at home with their biological , and experienced delayed sleep onset and difficulties with initiating sleep. The parents of all participants were trained to provide the BFRC procedure at home. Each parent received a handbook on the treatment of sleep difficulties, including information on the principles of , importance of consistent bedtime routines, and behavioral strategies for managing child’s daytime and sleep-related behaviors. Parents met with a therapist for 5 consecutive weeks to review the information provided in the handbook. In addition, at the end of each week, the therapist contacted parents by phone to review the content of the materials learned and to monitor progress (Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010).

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 21 21

The BFRC procedure, data collection method, and IOA criteria remained the same as described in preceding studies (Piazza & Fisher, 1991b; Piazza, Fisher, & Moser,

1991; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997) with minor exceptions: The duration of time allowed to attain sleep onset was increased from 15 minutes to 20 minutes (Moon,

Corkum, & Smith, 2010). Sleep diaries and actigraphy were used to record sleep outcomes. Results indicate a reduction in sleep onset latency for all three participants.

Outcomes were maintained at a 12-week follow-up, and parents reported satisfaction with the intervention. Strengths of this study include objective recording of sleep variables

(i.e., actigraphy), clear use of behavioral definitions needed to replicate the study, and engaging parents to implement the BFRC protocol in the home environment (Moon,

Corkum, & Smith, 2010). A weakness of the study is that it is impossible to isolate BFRC as the only independent variable when the intervention was presented in combination of other treatment strategies, such as the didactic sessions and handbook on behavioral strategies. As a result, the study makes it difficult to determine if treatment outcomes are the result of the BFRC intervention itself.

Conclusion and Future Directions

In summary, children with ASD experience various co-occurring sleep problems and disruptive sleep-related behaviors at a greater frequency and severity than typically developing children. Consequently, in children with ASD and their families quality of life and day-to-day functioning suffer. Parents of children with ASD who also experience sleep disturbances are especially at risk for mood disorders and marital dissatisfaction.

Effective behavioral treatments that parents can reliably implement at home are limited.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 22 22

BFRC is a promising intervention that successfully addresses sleep problems and sleep- related problem behaviors in children with and without ASD.

At this time, BFRC has been restricted to controlled studies by several groups of investigators implemented by trained professionals in institution settings. Consideration has yet to be given to how parents uniquely define sleep disturbance within the context of their own home and family. Primary sleep variables tend to be researcher defined, which may not capture the most salient aspects of a child’s sleep disturbances from the parent’s perspective. For example, undesired co-sleeping is a commonly occurring sleep disturbance reported by parents of children with ASD. However, parent-defined primary dependent variables have not been considered in any of the existing BFRC studies. Only one BFRC study engaged parents in implementing treatment of sleep problems in children with ASD in the home setting. However, none of these studies utilized parent- derived sleep goals to determine the onset of treatment or evaluate the effectiveness of

BFRC for the reduction of sleep disturbance in children with ASD. Therefore, the primary purpose of this study was to evaluate the effectiveness of BFRC as a parent- implemented home-based treatment for parent-defined sleep disturbances and related sleep problems experienced by their child with ASD.

Method

Participants Children between ages 4 and 8 years with a clinical diagnosis of autism and sleep disturbance were recruited through a day treatment program for children with ASDs and an outpatient behavioral health clinic located in the Midwest. Participant recruitment continued until a sufficient number of eligible participants (N=3) needed for a single

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 23 23 subject research design was recruited. Participant selection criteria were adapted from other studies in the area of behavioral treatment of sleep disturbance in children (Mindell

& Durand, 1993; Weiskop, Richdale, & Matthews, 2005). Recruited participants had a confirmed diagnosis of autism based on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule,

Second Edition (ADOS-2) (Lord, et al. 2012) and the Childhood Autism Rating Scale,

Second Edition (CARS-2) (Schopler, Van Bourgondien, Wellman, & , 2010), which is standard practice in diagnostics. For the purposes of this study, individuals with a classification of Autism or Autism Spectrum on the ADOS-2, in combination with a classification of either Severe or Mild-to-Moderate on the CARS-2, met the criteria for a diagnosis of autism and were deemed eligible for participation in the study. Individuals classified as Non-Spectrum on the ADOS-2 and/or Minimal-to-No-Symptoms on the

CARS-2 were excluded from the study. Eligible participants were required to be living at home with at least one parent at the time of the study and present with a minimum of four-week history of childhood sleep disturbances as denoted by

▪ resistance to going to bed, defined as one or more verbal protests and/or

physical protests a night, for a minimum of three nights per week; or

▪ delayed sleep onset, defined as more than 20 minutes between bedtime and

falling asleep, for a minimum of three nights per week; or

▪ any of the following situations at bedtime for a minimum of three nights per

week:

• undesired co-sleeping with a parent or sibling, being rocked or held to

fall asleep, falling asleep in a location other than his/her own bed; or

• falling asleep with a bottle or pacifier; or

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 24 24

• spontaneous awakening that requires parental intervention to return to

sleep at least once per night, at least three times per week.

Children using pharmacological agents to regulate sleep and/or other behaviors

were not necessarily excluded. Unless medically necessary, parents were asked to

attempt to maintain their child’s medication regiment throughout the duration of

the study. Excluded from the study were families who had received professionally

derived information directly from a psychologist, therapist, and/or physician

and/or used a behavior-based intervention to address their child’s sleep problems

within the past 6 months (Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010).

Three potential participants were screened and met criteria for participation. All three parents-child dyads enrolled and completed the study. All three children had been diagnosed with ASD prior to enrolling in the study. Each participant met the classification of Autism on the ADOS-2 and the Severe Symptoms of Autism Spectrum

Disorder group on the CARS-2 measures. All names were changed to protect participants’ right to confidentiality. Participant 1, Bill, was a 7-year-old Caucasian male attending the 1st grade. Bill’s parents reported that he exhibited bedtime resistance, refused to sleep in his own bed, and took longer than 20 minutes to fall asleep each night.

He woke repeatedly throughout the night and each time sought parental presence to reinitiate sleep by crawling into his parents’ bed. His parents often engaged in undesired co-sleeping with Bill in their bed, as they were asleep when he crawled into their bed and remained asleep with Bill in bed. Participant 2, Holly, was an 8-year-old Caucasian female attending the 3rd grade. Her parents reported that she refused to sleep in her own bed and never fell asleep without parental presence. A parent had to remain next to Holly

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 25 25 each night in their bed and engaged in undesired co-sleeping to get her to fall asleep.

Participant 3, Keith, was a 4-year-old Caucasian male attending preschool. His parents reported that he resisted bedtime, had difficultly falling asleep, and experienced frequent awakenings throughout the night. Keith required parental presence to fall asleep at bedtime and following any night awakening, which resulted in undesired co-sleeping in either his bed or his parents’ bed. His parents reported that Keith sometimes stayed awake throughout the entire night.

All children were living with their biological parents and siblings. Each parent reported participating in implementing the BFRC protocol, whereas one designated parent from each dyad was responsible for collecting data and taking telephone calls from a member of the investigative team. During the study, all children continued their previously prescribed regiment of medication (Bill: dexmethylphenidate [Focalin], guanfacine, and melatonin; Holly: risperidone and melatonin; Keith: cetirizine [Zyrtec]).

Table 1 provides a summary of participant characteristics including chronological age, gender, ADOS-2 scores and classification, CARS-2 scores and severity group, and sleep problems meeting selection criteria.

Materials A trained clinician administered the ADOS-2 and CARS-2 to evaluate each child’s verbal, social, and cognitive abilities (Gotham, Risi, Pickles, & Lord, 2007). The ADOS-2 is a semi-structured, standardized assessment for children ages 12 months through adulthood that allowed the examiner to observe and rate behaviors related to communication and social interaction and play skills identified as important to the diagnosis of ASD. Scores based on the examiner’s observations were calculated through

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 26 26 the use of a diagnostic algorithm to yield one of three classifications (i.e., Autism, Autism

Spectrum, and Non-spectrum) (Lord, et al., 2012). The use of the ADOS-2 alone does not provide justification for a diagnosis of ASD; therefore, the CARS-2 was used as a screening tool to compliment ADOS-2 outcomes.

The CARS-2 is a valid and reliable clinician- and parent-completed rating scale for children ages 2-13 years (Schopler, Van Bourgondien, Wellman, & Love, 2010). The

CARS-2 is comprised of descriptors of childhood social, developmental, and motor abilities related to ASD symptoms. A total score classifies children into one of three severity groups of ASD symptoms (i.e., Severe, Mild-to-Moderate, and Minimal-to-No-

Symptoms).

Dependent measures of sleep included the following: (a) Sleep Diary; (b)

Actigraphy; (c) Behavior Diary; and (d) the Children’s Sleep Habit Questionnaire

(CSHQ). All measures were collected during baseline (4 to 14 days), end of treatment (11 to 14 days), and at one month post-treatment (4 to 7 days). A Sleep Diary is a standardized parent-completed measure in the field of behavioral interventions and sleep medicine with a reasonable validity and high internal consistency (Corkum, Tannock,

Moldofsky, Hogg-Johnson, & Humphries, 2001; Mindell & Durand, 1993).

A traditional sleep diary, based on considerations from the literature (Patzold,

Richdale, & Tonge, 1998; Weiskop, Matthews, & Richdale, 2001), was developed for this study. One parent per family recorded nightly information regarding their child’s sleep in the sleep diary throughout the duration of the study (see Appendix A). Outcomes recorded in the diary specific to sleep included (a) frequency and duration of night awakenings after sleep onset (FNA; WASO); (b) sleep onset latency (SOL); (c) total

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 27 27 sleep time (TST); (d) time in bed (TIB); and (e) independent versus dependent sleep initiation (ISO; DSO). Sleep efficiency (SE) outcomes were extrapolated from the sleep diary data [(TST/TIB) x 100] and expressed as a percentage.

A slightly modified sleep-related behavior diary (Allen, Kuhn, DeHaai, &

Wallace, 2013; Burke, Kuhn, & Peterson, 2004) was added to this study. The same parent recorded nightly information regarding their child’s sleep-related behaviors in the behavior diary throughout the duration of the study (see Appendix B). Diary outcomes specific to sleep-related behavior included (a) frequency and duration of undesired co- sleeping (UC-S) with a parent or a sibling; (b) frequency and duration of bedtime resistance (BR; DBR); (c) frequency of sleeping in a non-designated area; and (d) the location of non-designated sleep (refer to Table 2 for a list of all dependent variables on the sleep and sleep related behavior diaries). Bedtime resistance (BR) frequencies were the sum total of the number of disruptive behaviors that occurred during the bedtime routine once the child was placed in bed and the number of times the child got out of the bedroom after being put in bed for the night. Disruptive Behavior Composite (DBC) mean frequency was determined by dividing the sum total of BR per data collection phase by the number of days during that period. Duration of bedtime resistance (DBR) outcomes were obtained by calculating the duration of disruptive behaviors during bedtime routine and once the child was placed in bed with the duration of time the child was out of the bedroom after being put in bed for the night.

Treatment integrity is a measure of the extent to which treatment procedures are implemented as intended (Vollmer & Sloman, 2008). An integrity-monitoring sheet on the BFRC protocol (see Appendix C) was created for this study based on clinical

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 28 28 considerations and the literature (Burke, Kuhn, & Peterson, 2004; Durand & Mindell,

199b; Vollmer & Sloman, 2008). Telephone calls were made to the designated parent responsible for data collection at least three times a week during the treatment phase

(Burke, Kuhn, & Peterson, 2004; Durand & Mindell, 1999b). On randomly selected nights, parents answered a series of questions related to components of the BFRC procedure they implemented. Parent responses were recorded on the integrity-monitoring sheet, and a random sample of data collected, ranging from 43% to 71% of participant treatment data, was scored for fidelity. Treatment integrity outcomes were calculated by dividing the sum of correct parent responses by the total number of possible correct responses and multiplying the result by 100 (Vollmer & Sloman, 2008). The bedtime fading components were implemented with 96% integrity for Holly, 97% integrity for

Bill, and 100% integrity for Keith. The response cost components were implemented with

100% integrity for all three participants.

To assess the reliability of data collection, periodic telephone checks were used to compare the level of agreement between parent-reported data and actual recorded data for the previous one to three nights during the treatment phase (Burke, Kuhn, & Peterson,

2004; Durand & Mindell, 1999b). The researcher recorded parent verbal reports during each telephone check using a blank sleep diary and sleep-related behavior diary (see

Appendix A and B). Random samplings of these telephone checks (i.e., 43% of data for

Keith, 50% for Bill, 71% for Holly) were later matched and compared with the data forms returned by parents. Data points were considered reliable if the difference between the reported and recorded durations varied by no more than 15 minutes and observed frequencies varied by no more than one point between data sets (Burke, Kuhn, Peterson,

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 29 29

2004). Reliability was calculated by dividing the sum of agreements by the sum total of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying the result by 100 [(∑ of agreements / [∑ of agreements + ∑ of disagreements]) x 100]. The result was expressed as a percentage

(Durand & Mindell, 1999b). Level of agreement between the telephone checks and parent-recorded sleep diaries was 95% for Holly, and 97% for Bill and Keith each. The level of agreement between parent-recorded sleep-related behaviors was 94% for Holly,

98% for Keith, and 100% for Bill.

Actigraphy is an objective measure of motor activity, which, in turn, provides reliable estimates of sleep-wake patterns in children (Meltzer, Montgomery-Downs,

Insana, & Walsh, 2012). Actigraphy is considered a relatively unobtrusive method for recording sleep patterns in children. Children with autism and/or sensory issues may not tolerate wrist placement of the actigraph device; therefore, ankle placement is recommended (Ancoli-Israel, et al. 2015). The MicroMini Motionlogger actigraph

(Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc.), a small sensor about the size of a wristwatch, was secured with a hospital band to each child’s ankle to continuously record movement.

Parents were asked to have their child constantly wear the small actigraph on the same ankle, except during bathing or swimming. Zero Crossing Mode (ZCM) was used to count frequencies of signal-based movement for each epoch (e.g., 30 seconds or one minute). Activity data collected were downloaded from the sensor using Act Millennium

(version 3.68.0.1) software. Data were scored using Action W (version 2) software and the Sadeh scoring algorithm (Sadeh, Sharkey, & Carskadon, 1994) to make inferences about the child’s sleep-wake cycles. Primary outcome variables included SOL, TST, SE,

WASO, and TIB.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 30 30

The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ) (Owens, Spirito, &

McGuinn, 2000) is a widely used multidimensional measure to determine the frequency and topography of sleep behaviors for school-aged children between 4-10 years. The parent-completed questionnaire yields reliable psychometric properties with typically and non-typically developing children (Lewandowski, Toliver-Sokol, & Palermo, 2011).

CSHQ abbreviated version (CSHQ-A) is composed of 33 items regarding bedtime, sleep behaviors, waking during the night, and morning waking/daytime sleepiness (Owens,

Spirito, & McGuinn, 2000). Parents retrospectively reported their child’s sleep for the past week using a Likert scale indicating if the problem occurred “usually” (five to sever nights per week), “sometimes” (two to four nights per week), or “rarely” (zero to one night per week). Parents also indicated if the sleep habit was a problem by circling “Yes”,

“No”, or “Not applicable (N/A)”. Items were scored within 8 subscales (i.e., bedtime resistance, sleep onset delay, sleep duration, sleep anxiety, night awakenings, , sleep disordered breathing, and daytime sleepiness). Total sum of each subscale yielded a total sleep disturbance score (range of 33 to 87), with a proposed cut- off score of 41; higher scores indicate more sleep problems.

Three measures were selected to capture the impact of and satisfaction with treatment for the family. The Goal Achievement Scale (GAS) quantifies progress on parent-identified treatment goals and assesses the clinical significance of change in sleep behavior (Hudson, Wilken, Jauering, & Raddler, 1995). For each identified treatment goal (i.e., sleep related behavior), 0% success (i.e., no improvement) was the set baseline rate. Before treatment, parents (with guidance from a member of the research team on developmentally appropriate behaviors) decided what would constitute 100% (total)

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 31 31 success for each goal. Success was dependent on the parents’ level of desired improvement, not on the total elimination of a problem behavior. Change in each goal was expressed as a percentage of success over baseline. For example, parents reported wanting to reduce the frequency of co-sleeping. Parents’ goals represented a personally acceptable level of co-sleeping (e.g., 2 out of 7 nights per week). Baseline rate of undesired co-sleeping (e.g., occurring 7 out of 7 nights per week) was set at 0% success, whereas the goal for 100% total success depended on parents’ preference (e.g., eliminating all but 2 of the 7 nights of co-sleeping per week).

The Parent’s Customer Satisfaction Questionnaire (PCSQ) is a 42-item questionnaire to evaluate treatment satisfaction (McMahon & Forehand, 2003). For the purposes of this study, only 16 questions specific to treatment satisfaction were used; 11 items corresponded to a Likert scale (e.g., 1 (considerably worse) to 7 (greatly improved)). Wording of each response option varied according to the question being asked (e.g., 1 = Very dissatisfied to 7 = Very satisfied; 1 = Strongly not recommended to 7

= Strongly recommended; 1 = Very inappropriate to 7 = Very appropriate; 1 = Very pessimistic to 7 = Very optimistic). Parents evaluated the overall treatment protocol by rating the degree to which they endorsed each statement and answered 5 open-ended questions related to usefulness of the treatment.

The Kansas Marital Satisfaction Scale (KMSS) is a brief 3-item measure designed to rapidly assess relationship satisfaction of married individuals or couples (Crane,

Middleton, & Bean, 2000). The KMSS is described as a “reliable and economical” tool for measuring satisfaction with marriage, partners, and marital relationships. Parents separately completed the KMSS by reading each item and marking on the corresponding

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 32 32 scale of 1 (extremely dissatisfied) to 7 (extremely satisfied) the degree to which they agreed with each statement (Schumm, Nichols, Schectman, & Grigsby, 1983). A possible score for each item ranges from 1 to 7; a cut-off score of 17 distinguishes between distressed and non-distressed couples, with a total individual score of 16 or lower indicating marital distress (Crane, Middleton, & Bean, 2000).

Procedure Initial Interview Parents of prospective participants completed a 15-minute telephone screening followed by a face-to-face interview to determine eligibility. During this interview, parents and children met with a member of the investigated team to complete the CARS-

2, baseline CSHQ-A, and a demographic questionnaire. A classification of either Severe or Mild-to-Moderate ASD resulted in the administration of the ADOS-2. Depending on parents’ availability, the ADOS-2 was completed either during the initial session or scheduled for another time that week. Parents were invited to participate in the study once a diagnosis of autism was confirmed and all other eligibility criteria were met. The consent form was reviewed and parents completed the KMSS.

Pre-Baseline Prior to baseline data collection, parents meet with a member of the investigative team for a second session to identify their specific treatment goals. Parents were instructed in the recording of data in the sleep and behavior diaries. They received feedback from a trained clinician as they completed sample diaries for two vignettes describing detailed sleep patterns and sleep-related behaviors (see Appendix D). Once parent dyads correctly completed the sample diaries, they were provided blank diaries

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 33 33 and an actigraph to begin collection of data on their child’s sleep patterns and sleep- related behaviors (Christodulu & Durand, 2004; Reed et al, 2009).

Baseline Parents met with a member of the investigative team once a week during the baseline phase of the study for approximately 30 minutes to review the sleep and behavior diaries and exchange actigraphs. Once baseline data were determined stable, or

14 consecutive days of data was collected, the treatment phase was introduced, and parents were taught to implement the BFRC protocol. Parents were instructed in the implementation of a modified BFRC protocol (i.e., bedtime procedures, response cost, and bedtime fading) and received an outline of procedures (see Appendix E).

Bedtime fading instructions included:

(i) Adjust bedtime for the following night according to child’s previous time of

sleep onset by 15 minutes. For example, if child’s initial bedtime was 10:30

p.m., and he/she fell asleep within 15 minutes of being placed in bed, fade the

bedtime to 10:15 p.m.; and

(ii) If child did not fall asleep within 15 minutes of the initial bedtime, push back

the bedtime to 10:45 p.m.

Bedtime procedures included:

(i) Follow regular bedtime routine and place child in his/her bed at the initial

adjusted bedtime;

(ii) After 15 minutes, check to see if child is awake by standing within one foot of

the bed and whispering his/her name. If there are no observable motor or

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 34 34

verbal responses (i.e., opening eyes, talking, etc.) it is safe to assume child is

asleep; and

(iii) If determined that child is awake (i.e., he/she responded to whisper),

implement response cost.

Response cost procedures included:

(i) Remove child from the bed and bedroom and allow him/her to partake in

activities not related to sleep for 30 minutes, such as playing with toys or

watching TV; and

(ii) At the end of 30 minutes, return child to his/her bed;

(iii) Repeat the procedure if your child does not fall asleep within 15 minutes of

being returned to bed; and

(iv) Address nighttime awakenings by returning child to his/her bed with minimal

parental attention and interaction.

Parents received feedback from a trained clinician as they verbally described how to implement BFRC with their child. Once parent dyads correctly reported how to implement each component of BFRC, they were provided blank diaries and an actigraph to begin treatment phase data collection. Finally, each parent completed the KMSS independently, once weekly for the duration of the study.

Treatment A member of the investigative team assisted parents in determining parent- preferred bedtime goals. A consistent wake time and naptime (if applicable) were derived using developmental norms (Iglowstein, Jenni, Molinari, & Largo, 2003) and parent sleep goals. Baseline data were used to determine the initial bedtime by calculating the average

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 35 35 sleep onset time and adding 30 minutes. Parents were asked to enforce a consistent wake time every morning regardless of their child’s sleep behaviors the night before. Parents were also asked to eliminate access to non-established daytime sleep, such as naps, unless appropriate for the age and developmental needs of their child. Parents were required to regulate the timing and duration for any established naps, regardless of the amount or quality of sleep the child had the night before, and to not permit access to sleep outside of the designated times. If the child eloped from the bedroom, parents were asked to return him or her to bed and ignore all disruptive behaviors as long as they were not harmful to the child or others.

Parents were taught a modified BFRC protocol and received an outline of the following procedures: (a) Bedtime protocol: (i) Follow the regular bedtime routine and place child in his/her bed at the initial adjusted bedtime. (ii) After 15 minutes, check to see if child is awake by standing within one foot of the bed and whispering his/her name.

If there are no observable motor or verbal responses (i.e., opening eyes, talking, etc.), it is safe to assume the child is asleep. (iii) If the child is awake (i.e., he/she responded to your whisper), implement response cost. (b) Response cost: (i) Remove child from the bed and bedroom and allow him/her to partake in activities not related to sleep for 30 minutes, such as playing with toys or watching TV. (ii) At the end of 30 minutes, return child to his/her bed. (c) Repeat the procedure if child does not fall asleep within 15 minutes of being returned to bed. (d) Address nighttime awakenings by returning child to his/her bed with minimal parental attention and interaction. (e) Bedtime fading: (i) Adjust the bedtime for the following night according to child’s previous time of sleep onset by 15 minutes. For example, if child’s initial bedtime was 10:30 p.m., and he/she fell asleep

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 36 36 within 15 minutes of being placed in bed, fade the bedtime earlier by 15 minutes to 10:15 p.m.. If the child did not fall asleep within 15 minutes of the initial bedtime, push the bedtime later by 15 minutes to 10:45 p.m.

After being trained to implement the BFRC protocol, parents continued to meet with a member of the investigative team once a week for approximately 30 minutes to review sleep and behavior diaries and exchange actigraphs. During the treatment phase, a researcher contacted the designated parent responsible for data collection by telephone approximately every other day to check on “data collection efforts” (Durand & Mindell,

1999). Parents answered a series of questions related to the BFRC procedures and data collected. Information provided was recorded in a modified diary and compared with parent completed sleep and behavior diaries at the end of the study. Parents were asked to complete the CSHQ-A at the end of the treatment phase.

Follow-up Approximately four weeks after the end of treatment, parents met with a member of the investigative team for approximately 30 minutes to receive data sheets and an actigraph. Parents collected sleep and behavior data for a minimum of 4 and maximum of

7 consecutive days. At the end of the follow-up data collection phase, parents attended a closing session to complete the CSHQ-A and PCSQ and return data and the actigraph.

Parents attended between five and eight sessions to complete the study, depending on outcomes (see Table 3). For example, if a stable trend in the baseline and treatment data was observed, then sessions two, three, four, and five were consolidated into two sessions.

Research Design

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 37 37

A non-concurrent multiple baseline design across participants was used (Harvey,

May, & Kennedy, 2004; Watson & Workman, 1981). Baseline lengths were staggered across participants and information was collected until data stability and trend were observed or for a maximum of 14 days if stability was not observed, followed by a maximum of 14 days for the treatment phase. It was expected that changes may occur in each participant’s sleep behavior under the treatment condition, while participant’s sleep behavior during baseline may remain unchanged. Follow-up measures were collected one month following the conclusion of treatment, for a maximum duration of one week.

Visual inspection of baseline data was utilized to assess stability and trends in the data and determine the introduction of the treatment phase for each individual participant

(Harvey, May, & Kennedy, 2004; Watson & Workman, 1981). For example, once the frequency of Bill’s undesired co-sleeping was stable and the trend was not decreasing

(after 6 days), the treatment phase was initiated. Similarly, when the frequency of Keith’s undesired co-sleeping was not stable or the trend was decreasing, treatment was delayed until baseline data demonstrated a stable trend in the desired direction (or for a total of 14 days). This time-lagged approach demonstrates experimental control through the sequential introduction of treatment once stable baseline levels in the dependent variables were observed (Harvey, May, & Kennedy, 2004).

Data analysis Means and standard deviations were calculated for WASO, SOL, TST, and SE from data recorded by actigraphy and in sleep diaries (Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010).

Mean frequencies and standard deviations for FNA, DSO, and non-designated sleep were calculated from data recorded in the sleep and behavior diary. Means and standard

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 38 38 deviations for undesired co-sleeping were also calculated using data recorded in behavior and sleep diaries (Weiskop, Matthews, & Richdale, 2001). Raw scores on the CSHQ-A subscales of bedtime resistance, sleep onset delay, sleep duration, sleep anxiety, night awakenings, parasomnias, sleep disordered breathing, and daytime sleepiness were reported (see table; Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010). Daily means for the frequency and duration of disruptive behaviors were calculated using data from the sleep and behavior diaries (Weiskop, Richdale, & Matthews, 2005). Level of success of parents’ treatment sleep goals was calculated using the GAS at the end of baseline, intervention, and follow- up conditions and expressed as % (Weiskop, Matthews, & Richdale, 2001; Weiskop,

Richdale, & Matthews, 2005). Mean level of satisfaction (%) was also calculated from

KMSS raw scores for each parent during all three phases.

Results

Data from each child were analyzed individually because a non-concurrent multiple baseline design across participants was used in this study. Changes in trends and patterns for the primary dependent variables are provided below. Each of the three parents dyads generated sleep-related goals for their own child and worked with a member of the investigative team to refine and determine the levels of success for each goal (see Table 4).

Undesired co-sleeping

Undesired co-sleeping was the only sleep problem consistently reported by the parents of all three participants. Parents generated a goal using the GAS to reduce undesired co-sleeping in the parent’s bed (see GAS section for details). Frequency and

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 39 39 duration of undesired co-sleeping with parents reduced after the onset of BFRC for all three children as measured by the sleep-related behavior diaries (see Figures 1 and 2). For

Holly, undesired co-sleeping occurred once per night during baseline, averaging 653 minutes per night. This decreased to 0 minutes during treatment and was maintained at that level at follow-up. Bill’s frequency of undesired co-sleeping was once per night throughout baseline, with a mean duration of 230 minutes per night, which also decreased to 0 minutes during treatment and was maintained at that level at follow-up. It should be noted that on the third night of treatment, Bill’s parent chose to co-sleep with him in the child’s bed for 90 minutes after an instance of bedwetting. This occasion did not meet the definition of undesired co-sleeping; therefore, the frequency and duration were not included in the final data set. Alternatively, had this one-time occurrence been included, undesired co-sleeping decreased from baseline to 0.1 occurrences and an average of 6 minutes per night at the end of treatment. Keith demonstrated a dramatic decrease from the mean frequency of 2 occurrences per night (mean duration of 57 minutes per occurrence) during baseline to 0.1 occurrences and an average of 1 minute per night during treatment. One episode of undesired co-sleeping occurred on the last night of the treatment phase in the parents’ bed for 15 minutes. Undesired co-sleeping for Keith slightly increased to 0.3 occurrences, with a mean duration of 9 minutes per night, throughout follow-up. These instances occurred on separate days in the parent’s bed, lasting less than 45 and 15 minutes each.

Non-designated sleep

Frequency and location of sleeping in a non-designated area were reported for Bill and Keith, whereas Holly maintained zero occurrences during all three phases of the

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 40 40 study. Both Bill and Keith showed reduction in the frequency of sleeping in a non- designated area. For Bill, mean frequencies of sleep in a non-designated area (location not reported) decreased from 0.16 occurrences during baseline to zero occurrences throughout treatment. For Keith, mean frequencies of sleep in a non-designated area decreased by half from baseline (0.13) (located in the car and on the couch) to treatment

(0.06) (located in the car). This single episode of sleeping in a non-designated area occurred during a change in routine. Keith’s parents reported they had traveled by car out of town for the day, and the return trip coincided with Keith’s bedtime, at which time he fell asleep and remained asleep for the duration of the car ride (2.5 hours). Follow-up data demonstrated a total elimination of sleeping in a non-designated area for Bill and

Keith.

Bedtime resistance

Bedtime resistance (BR) was a reported concern for Bill’s parents only, although all three children engaged in disruptive behaviors during the bedtime routine and after being placed in bed, as recorded in the sleep-related behavior diaries (see Figure 3). For

Holly, mean duration of BR per night decreased from 15 minutes during baseline to 0.4 minutes at the end of treatment and further decreased to 0.3 minutes during follow-up.

Night-by-night inspection of Holly’s sleep-related behavior diary indicated a one-time change in her bedtime routine (i.e., attending a party for 30 minutes past bedtime), which may have contributed to the duration of BR during follow-up. Both Keith and Bill experienced extinction bursts (increase in mean duration of BR) during the treatment phase, with a sharp decrease beyond baseline levels during follow-up. Bill’s mean duration of BR per night greatly increased from baseline (12.4 minutes per night) to

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 41 41 treatment (55 minutes per night) and then significantly decreased throughout follow-up (4 minutes per night). For Keith, BR mean duration per night increased from baseline (1.3 minutes per night) to treatment (6.3 minutes per night), and decreased to near 0 minutes during follow-up (0.1 minutes per night). Overall, Keith’s BR duration increased by only an average of 5 minutes during treatment and reduced by 6.2 minutes at follow-up.

Parents in this study reported no occurrences of aggressive behavior when returning the child to their own bed following elopement. Night-by-night sleep-related behavior diary for Bill indicated a spike in BR duration by 150 minutes the same day he experienced a significant deviation in routine (i.e., attended only ½ day of school and no swim class), which was preceded and succeeded by nights with BR rates of zero. Bill’s parents noted this change in schedule might have greatly contributed to his resistance to bedtime. Bill’s CSHQ-A bedtime resistance subscale scores decreased from baseline (9) to treatment (6) and were maintained during follow-up (6). Likewise, Keith’s CSHQ-A bedtime resistance subscale scores decreased from baseline (12) to treatment (7) and were maintained during follow-up (6). Holly’s CSHQ-A bedtime resistance baseline subscale score of 14 decreased to 6 during follow-up.

Disruptive behavior composite

Mean scores of Disruptive Behavior Composite (DBC) were extrapolated from the sleep-related behavior diaries and patterns similarly to the mean durations and frequencies of BR for all three children (see Figure 4). Holly’s DBC mean scores decreased slightly from the baseline (1.3 per night) to the end of treatment (1.2 per night) and displayed a downward trend. Her DBC further decreased during follow-up (0.5 per

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 42 42 night). Night-by-night inspection indicated that disruptive behaviors during her bedtime routine or after she was placed in her bed for the night were eliminated, and only the number of times she escaped from her bedroom after being put in bed for the night contributed to her DBC mean scores. Holly’s nightly DBC scores varied throughout treatment with a subtle downward trend that remained below baseline at the end of follow-up. A change in Holly’s routine may have contributed to one of two instance of

BR during follow-up, thus increasing her DBC. Bill’s DBC increased slightly from baseline (5.3 per night) to treatment (5.7 per night).

Similarly to Holly’s, Bill’s night-by-night data indicated that disruptive behaviors during his bedtime routine decreased to almost none. Furthermore, Bill’s disruptive behaviors occurring after he was placed in his bed for the night decreased, while the number of times he came out of his bedroom after being placed in bed for the night increased during treatment. Visual analysis of the plotted data showed an unexpected uptick in the frequency of BR on the same day Bill experienced a significant deviation from his daily routine. Such deviation may have negatively impacted his DBC. His DBC decreased throughout follow-up to 2 per night. As for Keith, his DBC doubled from baseline (1.1 per night) at the end of treatment (2.2 per night) and decreased to near 0 minutes during follow-up (0.1 per night) with no clinically relevant differences across topography.

Night awakenings

Frequent night awakenings (FNA) were a parent-reported sleep problem for Bill and Keith. Both children showed some decrease in the number of night awakenings

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 43 43 during the implementation of BFRC as measured by sleep diaries (see Figure 5). Bill’s

FNA slightly decreased from 1.2 occurrences per night at baseline to an average of 1 per night throughout treatment, whereas Keith’s FNA per night decreased from 0.6 per night at baseline to 0.3 per night during treatment. Night-by-night sleep diary data for Bill indicated a progressive decrease in FNA during treatment with four consecutive nights of zero night awakenings nearing the end of treatment with an increase in FNA the following night. Bill used the bathroom independently once that night, which accounts for one FNA. Bill’s parents indicated a deviation from his typical daytime routine resulting in a lack of exercise, which might have had a negative impact on his sleep and contributed to the unexpected increase of FNA. Additionally, at least one night awakening on each of the first, third, and fourth nights of the treatment phase were the result of bedwetting and required parental assistance in changing and .

These instances were not recorded as FNAs. Follow-up data demonstrated a total elimination of FNA for Bill and Keith.

Holly experienced an increase and fluctuation in the frequency of night awakenings from baseline (0 per night) to a mean frequency of 0.7 per night during treatment, which returned to near baseline levels during follow-up (0.5 per night). Prior to

BFRC, Holly slept through the night in her parents’ bed with at least one parent in the room at all times. This standard might have been greatly reducing the likelihood of FNA.

Therefore, the adjustment to sleeping alone in her own bed may have produced FNA during treatment. Visual analysis of the data showed FNA decreased to zero the last night of treatment, maintained the first three nights of follow-up, and increased by 2 instances the final night of follow-up due to a change in routine (i.e., party attendance and delayed

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 44 44 bedtime). Bill’s CSHQ-A night wakings subscale scores decreased from baseline (7) to treatment (6), and further decreased during follow-up (4), whereas Holly’s CSHQ-A bedtime resistance subscale score remained stable from baseline (5) throughout follow- up. Keith’s CSHQ-A night wakings subscale scores decreased from baseline (7) to treatment (4) and then slightly increased during follow-up (5).

Dependent sleep onset

Dependent sleep onset (DSO) was a parent-reported problem for Holly and Keith as both children required a parent in the room to fall asleep. DSO, as measured by the sleep diaries, was eliminated for all three children (see Figure 6). Holly’s DSO occurred steadily once per night during baseline, whereas Keith’s baseline DSO varied between 1 and 3 times per night with a mean frequency of 1.7 per night. Bill’s baseline DSO generally occurred once a night with a mean frequency of 1.2 occurrences per night. Each child’s DSO decreased to zero after the onset of BFRC and remained at the 0-per-night level at follow-up.

Sleep onset latency

Night-by-night inspection of mean sleep onset latency (SOL), as measured by sleep diaries, revealed an overall reduction in SOL at during follow-up for Bill and Keith, whereas Holly’s mean SOL increased above baseline (see Table 5). Bill’s baseline mean

SOL (57.5 minutes) decreased by over 50% by end of treatment (25.7 minutes), and again at follow-up (11.3 minutes). This clinically meaningful improvement highlights the effectiveness of BFRC in the reduction of SOL for a child with ASD that experienced significant sleep onset delays. Visual analysis of Bill’s night-by-night sleep diary data

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 45 45 showed an increase in SOL the same day he experienced a change in routine, suggesting a more consistent schedule may have resulted in greater improvement of SOL. Keith’s mean SOL baseline (9.6 minutes) – already an acceptable level of latency – greatly increased during treatment (33.2 minutes). This abrupt increase in SOL occurred the first night of treatment; SOL progressively decreased and remained at a lower level during follow-up (4.3 minutes).

Mean SOL recorded by actigraphy decreased from baseline to the end of treatment for Holly and Bill (see Table 5). Actigraphy treatment phase data for Keith were lost due to a software malfunction. Actigraphy data on Holly’s mean SOL indicated a decrease of 29.7 minutes from baseline (43.5 minutes) to treatment (13.8 minutes) with a return toward baseline levels (32 minutes) at the end of follow-up. Bill’s mean SOL decreased by 50 minutes from baseline (74 minutes) to treatment (24 minutes) and increased by 7.3 minutes at follow-up (31 minutes). Keith’s remaining actigraphy data indicated his mean SOL decreased from baseline (13 minutes) to follow-up (5.6 minutes) by 7.4 minutes. Bill’s CSHQ-A sleep onset delay subscale scores decreased from baseline

(3 minutes) to treatment (1 minute) and were maintained during follow-up (1 minute).

Holly and Keith’s CSHQ-A sleep onset delay subscale scores of 1 were maintained throughout the study.

Waking after sleep onset

Mean values of night awakenings after sleep onset (WASO) in minutes, as measured by sleep diaries, revealed a progressive reduction in night-awakenings after sleep onset for Keith, whereas Holly’s and Bill’s mean WASO increased from baseline levels at the end of treatment (see Table 5). Keith’s WASO in minutes decreased from 16

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 46 46 minutes at baseline to 6.4 minutes during treatment and to zero during follow-up. Holly’s

WASO increased from 0 minutes at baseline to 29 minutes at the end of treatment, followed by a decrease to 11.3 minutes during follow-up. Visual analysis of the data showed WASO maintained duration of 0 minutes on the first three out of four nights of follow-up and increased by 45 minutes on the final night. This increase corresponds with the uptick of FNA on the same night and may also be contributed to a change in Holly’s bedtime. Bill’s WASO increased by 6 minutes from baseline (35 minutes) to treatment

(41 minutes) and decreased to 0 minutes at follow-up. Akin to his FNA sleep diary data,

Bill’s night-by-night data showed a progressive decrease in mean WASO during treatment. Specifically, Bill experienced four consecutive nights of 0 minutes awake after sleep onset toward the end of treatment. The following night, WASO increased by 180 minutes (3 hours), which may have resulted from the unexpected change in his daytime routine and activity level.

WASO recorded by actigraphy decreased from baseline to the end of treatment for Holly and Bill (see Table 5). As previously reported, actigraphy treatment phase data for Keith were lost due to a software malfunction. Holly’s mean WASO decreased by

101 minutes from baseline (192 minutes; 3.2 hours) to treatment (91.3 minutes; 1.5 hours), with a return toward baseline levels (132.5 minutes; 2.2 hours) at the end of follow-up. Bill’s mean WASO decreased by 55 minutes from baseline (105 minutes; 1.8 hours) to treatment (50 minutes), and the decrease was maintained at follow-up. Keith’s remaining actigraphy data indicated his mean WASO increased slightly from baseline (51 minutes) to follow-up (57 minutes).

Time in bed

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 47 47

Time in bed (TIB) means measured in minutes by sleep diaries revealed an overall reduction in time spent in bed from baseline through follow-up for all three children. This calculation included the total amount of time the child was physically in bed during a 24-hour period. Holly and Keith’s TIB progressively reduced throughout the study, whereas Bill’s TIB slightly increased during follow-up (see Table 5). Bill’s mean

TIB decreased by 1 hour and 14 minutes from baseline (595 minutes; 9.9 hours) to treatment (521 minutes; 8.7 hours). His mean TIB slightly increased – by 27 minutes – from treatment to follow-up (548 minutes; 9.1 hours). Holly’s mean TIB baseline (671 minutes; 11.2 hours) decreased by 45 minutes at the end of treatment (626 minutes; 10.4 hours), followed by an additional decrease of 11 minutes during follow-up (615 minutes;

10.3 hours). Keith’s mean TIB decreased from baseline (638 minutes; 10.6 hours) by 13 minutes at the end of treatment (625 minutes; 10.4 hours), and another 12 minutes at the end of follow-up (613 minutes; 10.2 hours). Overall, TIB means reduced from baseline to follow-up by 56 minutes for Holly, 47 minutes for Bill, and 25 minutes for Keith.

Correspondingly, TIB recorded by actigraphy decreased for all three children (see

Table 5). Holly’s mean TIB decreased by approximately 1 hour from baseline (667 minutes; 11.1 hours) to treatment (605 minutes; 10.1 hours) and increased by 37 minutes from treatment to the end of follow-up (642 minutes; 10.7 hours). Similarly, Bill’s mean

TIB decreased by 1 hour and 41 minutes from baseline (609 minutes; 10.2 hours) to treatment (508 minutes; 8.5 hours) and increased by 41 minutes throughout follow-up

(549 minutes; 9.2 hours). Holly experienced an overall reduction in TIB of 25 minutes

(31 minutes less than recorded in sleep diaries), whereas Bill’s TIB reduced by 1 hour (13 minutes more than recorded in sleep diaries). Much the same, Keith’s mean TIB

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 48 48 decreased from baseline (598 minutes; 10 hours) to follow-up (567 minutes; 9.5 hours) by 31 minutes (6 minutes more than recorded in sleep diaries).

Total sleep time

Total sleep time (TST) means measured by sleep diaries decreased from baseline to the end of treatment for all three children (see Table 5). TST was calculated by adding the durations of daytime and nighttime sleep within a 24-hour period. None of the parent dyads scheduled naptimes for their child; therefore a majority of sleep took place during the night. Holly’s baseline mean TST (653 minutes; 10.9 hours) decreased by the end of treatment (551 minutes; 9.2 hours) by 1 hour and 42 minutes and then increased by 27 minutes during follow-up (578 minutes; 9.6 hours). Her average decrease in mean TST was 1 hour and 15 minutes. Bill’s baseline mean TST (503 minutes; 8.4 hours) decreased by 57 minutes by the end of treatment (446 minutes; 7.4 hours) and reverted above baseline levels by 30 minutes (533 minutes; 8.9 hours) during follow-up.

Although none of the parents were interested in adding naps to their child’s sleep routine, Keith’s parents reported he had been allowed to nap during the day occasionally due to not sleeping either most or all of the night. This decreased the likelihood of him successfully sleeping through the night. Keith took two naps throughout the entire study, each on separate days during baseline. The first nap (15 minutes, 11:45 a.m.to 12:00 p.m.) occurred after sleeping for only 8.25 hours the night before with a terminal wake time of 2:00 a.m. Two days later, the second nap (3.75 hours, 8:15 a.m.to 12:30 p.m.) occurred after sleeping only 4.5 hours the night before with a terminal wake time of

11:00 p.m. Naps were eliminated for Keith at the onset of BFRC; this practice was

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 49 49 maintained through follow-up. Both naps included, Keith’s mean baseline TST (599 minutes; 10 hours) decreased by 20 minutes by the end of treatment (579 minutes; 9.7 hours). His TST also reverted above baseline levels by 10 minutes (609 minutes; 10.2 hours) during follow-up.

Mean TST recorded by actigraphy increased for Holly and Bill and decreased for

Keith from baseline to follow-up (see Table 5). Holly’s mean TST actigraphy data increased by 1 hour and 32 minutes from baseline (399 minutes) to treatment (491) and decreased by 16 minutes by the end of follow-up (475). Her overall TST increased by 1 hour and 16 minutes. Bill’s mean TST decreased by 13 minutes from baseline (415) to treatment (402) and increased by 1 hour and 2 minutes by the end of follow-up (464). His overall TST increased by 49 minutes. Keith’s mean TST decreased from baseline (525) to follow-up (502) by 23 minutes. Bill’s CSHQ-A sleep duration subscale scores did not change from baseline (5) to treatment (5) and decreased during follow-up (3), whereas

Holly’s CSHQ-A sleep duration subscale scores did not change from baseline (3) through follow-up. Keith’s CSHQ-A sleep duration subscale scores decreased from baseline (6) to treatment (3) and did not change during follow-up (3).

Sleep efficiency

Sleep efficiency (SE) outcomes extrapolated from sleep diaries data indicate all three children overall maintained or improved the percentage of their sleep efficiency.

Holly’s baseline SE of 97% decreased to 88% at the end of treatment and increased to

94% during follow-up. Bill’s baseline SE increased slightly from 85% to 86% at the end of treatment, and further increased to 97% during follow-up. Keith’s baseline SE of 94%

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 50 50 decreased slightly to 92% at the end of treatment and increased to 99% throughout follow-up. Compared with these data, SE outcomes extrapolated from actigraphy data were lower for each child. For example, Holly’s baseline SE of 60% increased to 82% at the end of treatment, and decreased to 75% during follow-up. Bill’s baseline SE increased from 68% to 80% at the end of treatment, and further increased to 85% during follow-up. Keith’s baseline SE of 89% was maintained during follow-up.

Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire, Abbreviated Version

BFRC led to a significant reduction in the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire,

Abbreviated Version (CSHQ-A) total sleep disturbance scores for all three children. Total sleep disturbance scores drastically decreased from the clinical range during baseline to the non-clinical range by follow-up for two children, whereas the third child’s follow-up score was only 1 point above the proposed clinical cutoff score (41). Specifically, Bill’s baseline CSHQ-A total score of 48 decreased to 43 at the end of treatment, and further decreased to 36 during follow-up, with an overall decrease of 12 points. Keith’s total baseline score of 60 significantly decreased to 46 at the end of treatment, with an additional decrease to 42 during follow-up, with a decrease of 18 points, which represents the the greatest improvement among the study participants. Likewise, Holly’s CSHQ-A total score at baseline (56) decreased by the end of follow-up (40), leaving her with the second best reduction in score of 16 points.

As previously mentioned, Holly’s CSHQ-A subscale scores for sleep onset delay

(1), sleep duration (3), and night wakings (5) remained unchanged from baseline throughout follow-up (EOT data not available), whereas bedtime resistance significantly decreased from 14 to the minimum score of 6 (see Table 5). Similarly, Keith’s CSHQ-A

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 51 51 subscale score for sleep onset delay remained unchanged throughout the study at a baseline score of 1. In contrast to Holly’s score, Keith’s CSHQ-A subscale score for sleep duration decreased from baseline (6) by half at the end of treatment (3) and remained unchanged during follow-up. His CSHQ-A subscale score for night wakings also decreased from baseline (7) to treatment (4) but then slight increased at follow-up (5).

Keith’s CSHQ subscale score for bedtime resistance decreased from baseline (12) to the end of treatment (7), with an additional decrease at follow-up (6).

Bill’s CSHQ-A subscale score for sleep onset delay decreased from baseline (3) to the minimum score at the end of treatment (1) and did not change during follow-up.

His CSHQ-A subscale score for bedtime resistance followed a similar pattern with a decrease from baseline (9) to the minimum score at the end of treatment (6), which did not change during follow-up. Bill’s CSHQ-A sleep duration subscale score maintained at baseline through end of treatment (5) and decreased to the minimum score (3) at the end of follow-up, whereas night wakings slightly decreased from baseline (7) to end of treatment (6), with a near minimal level decrease during follow-up (4). CSHQ-A sleep disordered breathing scores were reported within 1 point (range 3 to 4) of the minimal level (3) and determined non-clinically significant for all three children.

Goal Achievement Scale

Parents set sleep-related goals for their child and worked with a member of the investigative team to refine and determine the levels of success for each goal (see Table

4). Keith had four goals, whereas Holly and Bill had two goals each. The only similar parent-set goal across all three children was to reduce undesired co-sleeping in the parent’s bed by having the child sleep alone in their own bed. The percentage of nights

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 52 52 during the treatment phase when each child slept in their own bed dramatically increased for all three children as measured by the sleep-related behavior diaries. Bill’s goal of sleep through the night in [his] own bed dramatically increased from 0% success (0 out of 6 nights) during baseline, to 100% success (14 out of 14 nights) at the end of treatment phase, which remained at 100% success (4 out of 4 nights) through follow-up.

Similarly, Holly’s goal of sleep in [her] own bed (every night) increased from 0% success (0 out of 4 nights) during baseline to 100% success (11 out of 11 nights) at the end of the treatment phase and remained at 100% success during follow-up (4 out of 4 nights). Keith’s goal of falling asleep on own (in [his] own bed) increased from 0% success (0 out of 14 nights) during baseline to 100% success (14 out of 14 nights) at the end of the treatment phase and remained at 100% success (7 out of 7 nights) during follow-up. Keith’s goal of sleeping through the night in [his] own bed (return to sleep during the night by himself) went from 0% success (0 out of 14 nights) during baseline to

93% success (13 out of 14 nights) at the end of treatment. This goal’s success decreased to 75% (5 out of 7 nights) during follow-up due to briefly engaging in undesired co- sleeping in the parent’s bed (mean of 9 minutes) on two separate nights.

Bill’s and Keith’s parents had different goals related to the time their child went to sleep each night. Bill’s parents’ goal to go to sleep sooner (reduce SOL each night) was set at equal to or less than 15 minutes in order to achieve 100% success. He improved from 0% success (average of 57.5 minutes per night) during baseline to 75% success (average of 25.7 minutes per night) at the end of the treatment phase, which further increased to 100% success (average of 11.3 minutes per night) during follow-up.

Keith’s parents’ goal of bedtime between 7:00 and 7:30 p.m. was set to 7 out of 7 nights

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 53 53 to achieve 100% success. This goal increased from 1% success (1 out of 14 nights) during baseline to 43% success (6 out of 14 nights) at the end of the treatment phase, and further increased to 86% success (6 out of 7 nights) during follow-up, thus indicating

BFRC provided the structure necessary to achieve success.

Holly’s and Keith’s parents had similar goals for the amount of sleep their child received each night. Members of the investigative team used sleep norms (Iglowstein, et al., 2003) to help parents set the goals. Norms for total sleep duration per 24-hour period range between 9.6 and 11 hours for children ages 8 and 9 years and between 10.5 and

12.8 hours for children ages 4 to 5 years (Iglowstein, et al., 2003). Based on these norms,

Holly’s goal to get required amount of sleep for [her] age (every night) decreased from

50% success (2 out of 4 nights) during baseline to 18% success (2 out of 11 nights) at the end of the treatment phase, followed by a return to baseline level 50% (2 out of 4 nights) during follow-up. Keith’s goal to get enough sleep (developmentally appropriate amount per night) also decreased from baseline 36% success (5 out of 14 nights) to 21% success

(3 out of 14 nights) at the end of the treatment phase and increased to 43% success (3 out of 7 nights) during follow-up.

In contrast to the recommendations published by Iglowstein and colleagues

(Iglowstein, et al., 2003), the National Sleep Foundation recommendations on sleep duration per 24 hours are between 10 to 13 hours of sleep for children ages 3 to 5 years and between 9 and 11 hours for children ages 6 to 13 years (Hirshkowitz, et al., 2015).

Even though these recommendations were not available at the beginning of our study, these norms were compared with Holly’s and Keith’s total sleep time (TST) values. The results of this comparison revealed a steady improvement in sleep duration goals for both

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 54 54 children. Holly’s baseline level of 50% success (2 out of 4 nights) increased to 64% success (7 out of 11 nights) at the end of the treatment phase, and further increased to

100% success (4 out of 4 nights) during follow-up, whereas Keith’s baseline level of 57% success (8 out of 14 nights) remained stable through the end of the treatment phase and further increased to 71% success (5 out of 7 nights) during follow-up.

Although sleep duration norms for children with ASD have yet to be established,

Hodge and colleagues (Hodge, Carollo, Lewin, Hoffman, & Sweeney, 2014) found that children with ASD between ages 3 and 17 years experienced significantly less overall sleep (mean, 8 hours and 47 minutes; standard deviation, 93 minutes) compared with typically developing peers (mean, 9 hours and 22 minutes; standard deviation, 76 minutes). Compared with these findings, Holly’s baseline level of 25% success (1 out of

4 nights) increased to 82% success (9 out of 11 nights) at the end of the treatment phase, and further increased to 100% success (4 out of 4 nights) during follow-up, whereas

Keith’s baseline level of 50% success (7 out of 14 nights) increased to 64% success (9 out of 14 nights) at the end of the treatment phase and decreased to 29% success (2 out of

7 nights) during follow-up (Hodge, Carollo, Lewin, Hoffman, & Sweeney, 2014).

Kansas Marital Satisfaction Scale

Parent-completed Kansas Marital Satisfaction Scale (KMSS) indicated a possible ceiling effect for two of the three parent dyads (see Table 6). Bill’s and Keith’s parents each reported being extremely satisfied (7) on all three KMSS probes (i.e., 1) How satisfied are you with your marriage?; 2) How satisfied are you with your husband/wife as a spouse?; and 3) How satisfied are you with your relationship with your

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 55 55 husband/wife?) at baseline, end of treatment, and follow-up. Therefore, Bill’s and Keith’s parents’ total individual KMSS scores were maintained at the ceiling level of 21 for each parent throughout the study, thus staying above the cut-off score of 17 for distressed couples. In contrast, Holly’s parents each endorsed being somewhat satisfied (5) with their marriage and very satisfied (6) with their husband or wife as a spouse and with their relationship with their husband or wife at baseline and end of treatment. Holly’s parents’

KMSS scores with regard to their marriage increased by 1 point each from somewhat satisfied to very satisfied (6), while the other scores remained unchanged through to follow-up. Total individual KMSS scores for Holly’s parents remained stable from baseline (17) through end of treatment and increased at follow-up (18), also staying above the cut-off score indicating marital distress.

Parent’s Customer Satisfaction Questionnaire

Parent’s Customer Satisfaction Questionnaire (PCSQ) mean-ratings indicate high levels of satisfaction with treatment implementation and outcomes across all three families. Holly’, Bill’s, and Keith’s parents endorsed that the major problems that originally prompted treatment and the problems treated during this study had greatly improved (mean rating of 7). All three parent dyads endorsed feeling very positive (mean rating of 7) about the treatment program, feeling very satisfied (mean rating of 7) about their child’s progress, and would strongly recommend (mean rating of 7) the treatment program to a friend or relative. Parental expectations for a satisfactory outcome of the treatment at the end of follow-up were optimistic (Keith) or very optimistic (Holly and

Bill; mean rating of 6.66).

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 56 56

Each parent endorsed being very confident (mean rating of 7) in their ability to manage future in-home sleep problems based on what they learned during the study, whereas only Holly’s and Bill’s parents also endorsed being very confident (7) in their ability to manage current in-home sleep problems independently, resulting in an overall mean ratings of 5. This discrepancy in endorsements by Keith’s parents may due to the reverse selection of the response options for this particular question, suggesting a rating of 7 may have been intended as opposed to 1 (i.e., 1 = Very unconfident verses 7 = Very confident). Problems related to the children not treated during the study have either remained the same (Holly and Keith) or slightly improved (Bill; mean rating 4.33), whereas the degree to which the treatment program helped parents with other personal or family problems have either helped (Holly and Keith) or helped slightly (Bill; mean rating 5.66).

In the open-ended questions, parents were invited to share their opinions on the best and worst components of the treatment program. The most helpful components included ‘sleeping in parents’ bed stopped completely’ for Bill; ‘getting her to sleep in her own bed’ for Holly; and ‘creating strict bedtime rules and sticking to it’ for Keith.

Parents reported liking most the ‘results; treatment solved problems’ (Bill); ‘easy to follow/implement’ (Holly); and ‘having us have rules to establish a better bedtime routine’ (Keith). Bill and Keith’s parents liked least the ‘lack of sleep a few nights’ (Bill) and ‘first few days were difficult, but it got better. those days were tough! and data entry.’ (Keith). Holly’s parents indicated that there was nothing they liked least about the treatment program, whereas each parent dyad endorsed none of the treatment program

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 57 57 components as least helpful. Likewise, none of the parents reported any part of the treatment program as not helpful, nor did they offer recommendations for improvement.

Discussion

The results of this study add to the literature supporting BFRC as an effective treatment option for co-occurring sleep problems experienced by children with ASD.

This study evaluated the effectiveness of BFRC as an in-home treatment with parents as change agents in decreasing parent-defined sleep disturbances, such as undesired co- sleeping, night awakenings, difficulty falling asleep, and bedtime disturbances experienced by children with ASD. While this study centered on improving the sleep of children diagnosed with ASD by means of BFRC, innovative concepts of the study were to utilize parents to determine and implement treatment according to parent-derived sleep goals for their children.

Previous BFRC research on sleep disturbances in children with ASD (DeLeon,

Fisher, & Marhefka, 2004; Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer,

1997) had not considered such novel aspects simultaneously. For example, two studies

(DeLeon, Fisher, & Marhefka, 2004; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997) were restricted to institutionalized settings with BFRC implemented by trained professionals, one study

(Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010) involved training parents to implement BFRC in the home setting, and one study (Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997) used sleep norms in conjunction with parent goals to establish ideal sleep and wake times for their child. The current study extends both the science and practice of increasing parental agency while providing BFRC as a treatment option for children with ASD and co-occurring sleep disturbances.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 58 58

The primary sleep problem across all three families – frequency and duration of undesired co-sleeping – improved for each child. Visual analysis showed improvement in baseline levels immediately following treatment onset. These improvements were maintained at one-month follow-up; thus, BFRC was effective in reducing (Keith) and eliminating (Holly and Bill) the frequency and duration of undesired co-sleeping. Neither frequency nor duration of undesired co-sleeping was negatively impacted by changes in routine for any of the children. Furthermore, parent goal for their child to sleep in their own bed were accomplished after the onset of treatment, and these outcomes were maintained across all children. Each of these outcomes offers compelling evidence for experimental control in a single-subject, non-concurrent multiple baseline design and meaningful implication for the effectiveness of BFRC as a parent-implemented home- based intervention.

This is the first study to demonstrate BFRC as an effective intervention for the elimination of undesired co-sleeping in children with ASD. Previously, Ashbaugh and

Peck (1998) found that BFRC eliminated undesired co-sleeping with a typically developing child, whereas others (Piazza and Fisher, 1991b; Piazza, Fisher, and Moser,

1991) noted reductions in undesired co-sleeping among children diagnosed with non-

ASD neurodevelopmental disorders. Previous BFRC studies in children with ASD primarily focused on changes in disruptive sleep-related behaviors, sleep latency, night awakenings, or sleep duration (DeLeon, Fisher, & Marhefka, 2004; Moon, Corkum, &

Smith, 2010; Piazza, Fisher, & Sherer, 1997). The current study evaluated additional parent-determined sleep goals (i.e., reduce night awakenings and sleep onset, set bedtime, and obtain developmentally appropriate amount of sleep) as primary sleep problems and

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 59 59 sleep problems not identified as parent goals (i.e., BR, DBC, DSO, WASO, TIB, TST,

SE) as secondary sleep problems. Nevertheless, these outcomes provide further evidence for a positive change following BFRC.

Above and beyond the complete elimination of undesired co-sleeping, Holly also demonstrated meaningful improvements in sleeping in her own bed, falling asleep independently, and bedtime resistance. At the onset of treatment, Holly’s parents no longer had to remain next to her to get her to fall asleep or to sleep in her own bed throughout the night. Moreover, Holly no longer engaged in disruptive behaviors during her bedtime routine or after she was placed in her bed for the night. Time spent engaging in bedtime resistance dramatically improved after the onset of BFRC. These changes were clinically important to Holly’s parents because a substantial portion of their evenings were no longer occupied by conflict over sleep. Occasionally, she continued to come out of her bedroom after being put in bed for the night; however, frequencies of these minor disturbances remained below baseline levels at follow-up.

Depending on the measurement method, Holly’s overall total sleep time decreased (sleep diary) or increased (actigraphy) from baseline to the end of follow-up.

Unlike the other two children in this study, Holly did not experience difficulties with night awakenings prior to treatment. Nonetheless, both the number and the duration of night awakenings varied greatly during treatment and eventually returned to near baseline levels. This putative suppression of night awakenings during baseline may stem from her sleeping through the night in her parents’ bed with at least one parent in the room.

Therefore, Holly’s night awakenings began after access to co-sleeping with a parent and dependent sleep onset were eliminated. It is worth noting that Holly experienced a change

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 60 60 in routine the last night of follow-up, which might have contributed to sleep problems experienced that night.

In addition to the elimination of undesired co-sleeping, Bill also experienced noteworthy improvements in sleep latencies, bedtime resistance, and the number of night awakenings and started sleeping in his own bed and falling asleep independently. After the onset of BFRC, Bill no longer sought parental presence to initiate or reinitiate sleep and he was able to sleep through the night in his own bed independently. He also engaged in fewer disruptive behaviors during his bedtime routine that continued to improve throughout treatment and follow-up. By the end of follow-up, Bill was able to consistently achieve sleep onset within 15 minutes of bedtime. This outcome was clinically significant because prior to intervention Bill often took one to two hours a night to fall asleep, which may have inadvertently disrupted his parents’ sleep.

According to both subjective and objective measures, Bill’s overall total sleep time increased from baseline to the end of follow-up. Night awakenings also progressively improved throughout treatment, however, Bill might have achieved greater improvement had it not been for a change in schedule and lack of daytime activity the second to last day of treatment. That same night, he experienced more frequent and longer lasting disruptive behaviors than he had the previous ten nights of treatment and spent the least amount of time sleeping out of any night during the entire study. Bill also took longer to fall asleep than he did the two nights before and had more night awakenings than he did the previous four nights. Nonetheless, his night awakenings were completely eliminated during follow-up.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 61 61

Keith also demonstrated considerable improvements over and above a significant reduction in undesired co-sleeping, specifically in night awakenings, sticking to a set bedtime, sleeping in his own bed, falling asleep independently, and bedtime resistance.

After the onset of BFRC, Keith experienced fewer night awakenings, which completely disappeared toward the end of treatment and remained absent throughout follow-up. As opposed to Holly’s data, Keith’s overall total sleep time from baseline to the end of follow-up increased, based on sleep diary data, or decreased, based on actigraphy data.

Furthermore, Keith no longer required a parent in the room or in bed with him to fall asleep at bedtime or after night awakenings. Keith also engaged less often in bedtime refusal, following an expected extinction burst the first night of BFRC, when he was no longer permitted to sleep in his parents’ bed or with a parent in the room.

Overall, the occurrence of Keith’s sleep-related disruptive behaviors gradually declined throughout treatment and remained rare at follow-up, with the exception of an unexplained uptick. A steady upward trend in the length of time Keith engaged in bedtime resistance began the third night of treatment, reaching its peak on the sixth night.

The following night marked the descent to near baseline levels. Unfortunately, no information was provided that could reasonably account for this change in trend. As for sleep latencies, Keith’s data showed a sharp increase on the first night of treatment, followed by a progressive decline that was maintained during follow-up. This pattern likely indicates that for this child, the onset of treatment temporarily altered his sleep latency, and he was able to eventually adjust to his original set point of sleep onset within

15 minutes of being placed in bed. Furthermore, this improvement was not clinically

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 62 62 meaningful as he was already achieving sleep within an acceptable range of latency

(average within 10 minutes of being placed in bed) before treatment.

In all BFRC studies with children on the autism spectrum, researchers determined treatment onset and primary sleep outcomes, with the exception of Piazza and colleagues

(1997), who used sleep norms in conjunction with parent preferences to establish children’s sleep and wake times. In the current study, two children (Holly and Keith) had parent-determined goals for sleep duration based on sleep norms (Iglowstein, et al.,

2003). At the time of our study, the most current and complete sleep duration norms available were based on the combined results of two longitudinal studies conducted with

Swiss children (Iglowstein, et al., 2003). Based on these norms and lofty parent goals, it is not surprising that neither Holly nor Keith attained total sleep time goals after the onset of treatment. This may be partly because children with ASD tend to sleep less compared with typically developing peers (Hodge, Carollo, Lewin, Hoffman, & Sweeney, 2014).

Compared with other studies with children on the autism spectrum (Durand & Mindell,

1999; Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010; Schreck & Mulick, 2000), Holly’s and Keith’s sleep durations appeared to fall within the normal range for their age. Therefore, both parent dyads might have overestimated their child’s need for sleep.

As previously described, two children (Bill and Keith), whose parents had a goal of reducing the number of night awakenings, experienced an overall improvement during treatment and total elimination during follow-up. These results are consistent with the findings of two studies (Piazza, Fisher, and Sherer,1997; DeLeon, Fisher, and Marhefka,

2004) in that night awakenings were nearly eliminated following BFRC in children with

ASD. Consistent with findings by Moon, Corkum, and Smith (2010), mean sleep onset

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 63 63 latency as measured by both actigraphy and sleep diary revealed an overall decease from baseline following BFRC for two children (Bill and Holly) with ASD. BFRC was also effective in reducing the duration and frequency of disruptive sleep-related behaviors for all three children. Moreover, BFRC produced a clinically significant improvement in

CSHQ-A total scores for three children with severe ASD.

Prior to treatment, parent-reported total sleep disturbance scores indicated that their children experienced -type sleep problems in the clinical range. Two children (Holly and Bill) greatly improved, their problems moving from the clinical range to the non-clinical range by the end of follow-up. However, one child (Keith), who experienced the greatest improvement with the largest reduction in scores out of all three children, was only one point above the clinical cut-off score by the end of the follow-up.

These parent-reported scores on each subscale are consistent with night-by-night data from sleep and behavior diaries, indicating an overall positive change with less bedtime resistance for all three children, fewer night awakenings and longer sleep duration for Bill and Keith, and less sleep onset delays for Bill. Actigraphy data further support these positive changes in sleep onset delays and sleep duration for Bill. These outcomes offer strong support for BFRC successfully treating a wide variety of multiple co-occurring disruptive sleep and related problem behaviors in children on the autism spectrum.

The total amount of time spent sleeping in a 24-hour period was greater when recorded in sleep diaries vs. by actigraphy across all three children. Furthermore, the average amount of time spent in bed as recorded by both sleep diaries and actigraphy significantly reduced from baseline to follow-up for all three children. This reduction in time spent in bed likely produced the observed improvements in sleep efficiency.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 64 64

According to sleep diary data, each child was already achieving sleep efficiency within the low-end of normal (85%) to high (97%) range prior to treatment. However, actigraphy data produced lower sleep efficiencies, compared with sleep diary data, for each child.

In contrast to findings by Moon, Corkum, and Smith (2010), there was a pattern of change in mean sleep efficiency as measured by actigraphy for two children. Holly’s and Bill’s sleep efficiency increased after treatment, and mild improvements were maintained at follow-up, whereas Keith’s mean sleep efficiency remained at 89% throughout the study. For all three children, the amount of time spent awake after sleep onset recorded by actigraphy was greater than time recorded in sleep diaries, which was nearly zero. Similarly to the improvements of night awakenings recorded in sleep diaries, two of the three children spent no time awake after sleep onset by follow-up, whereas the third child might have continued the pattern of zero time awake after initially falling asleep had she not experienced a change in routine on the final night of follow-up.

In the present study, all parent dyads reported high levels of satisfaction with

BFRC and its results and were willing to strongly recommend this treatment to others.

This outcome is similar to high parent satisfaction with treatment reported in the one other BFRC study that used parents as change agents in the home setting with children with ASD (Moon, Corkum, & Smith, 2010). In our study, parents reported the use of structured guidelines and the elimination of undesired co-sleeping as being the most helpful components. In sum, these parent opinions suggest that benefits outweighed the challenges and support the social importance of BFRC as an effective parent- implemented treatment. Only one parent satisfaction item was rated negatively. This

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 65 65 discrepancy might be explained by a change in response item value and topographical similarity between Very unconfident and Very confident rather than a true endorsement of low confidence in their ability to manage their child’s current problem behaviors. For example, these parents wrote, “The treatment was awesome. It gave us the tools we needed.” Therefore, this score should be interpreted with caution. As such, this minor inconsistency does not diminish the overall high level of parent satisfaction across all three families and the social validity of BFRC as a feasible parent-implemented in-home treatment for children with ASD and multiple co-occurring sleep problems.

We explored the impact of changes in sleep problems on marital satisfaction.

Holly’s parents each experienced some minor improvement in marital satisfaction at the end of treatment. This slight improvement does not provide sufficient evidence to support the impact of BFRC on marital satisfaction; however, previous findings (Fisman, Wolf,

& Noh, 1989; Mihaila & Hartley, 2016) suggest various explanations for this change. For instance, an increase in martial satisfaction might indicate that Holly’s parents’ sleep also improved, thus increasing each parent’s positive affect (Mihaila & Hartley, 2016).

Likewise, removing Holly from her parents’ bed might have had a positive impact on the quality of their marriage by allowing for more time alone with each other (Fisman, Wolf,

& Noh, 1989). Bill’s and Keith’s parents consistently endorsed being extremely satisfied with their marriage, his or her spouse, and his or her relationship with their spouse.

Although a ceiling effect left little room for improvements in martial relations, these results do indicate that BFRC did not produce a decline in couple’s marital satisfaction.

Piazza, Fisher, and Sherer (1997), DeLeon, Fisher, and Marhefka (2004), and

Moon, Corkum, and Smith (2010) provided foundational evidence for BFRC as a

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 66 66 promising intervention for sleep disturbances experienced by children with ASD. Our study expands upon this research in a number of ways. First, the current study addresses the lack of agency parents have been given when children participate in treatment-related research. Parents, not researchers, determined the treatment goals and the primary outcome variables. The current method enhances the social validity of BFRC and the social significance of the positive findings. Second, the current study obtained actigraphy data as an objective measure of the sleep-related outcome variables.

Third, a non-concurrent multiple baseline across subjects design was employed in place of a more rigorous design due to its feasibility in applied settings and in an attempt to avoid potential temporal delays in participant enrollment (Watson & Workman, 1981).

Nonetheless, we met experimental design requirements (Harvey, May, & Kennedy, 2004) by staggering baseline durations across the three participants and planning for the implementation of BFRC. For example, BFRC was introduced after baseline data on the frequency of the primary target behavior (i.e., undesired co-sleeping) were determined stable (4 consecutive days for Holly and 6 consecutive days for Bill) or after 14 consecutive days of data collection, as done for Keith. The maximum of 14 consecutive days represents a potential methodological weakness; however, this two-week limit was chosen based on clinical considerations for the participants and their families.

Experimental control was demonstrated by a functional relationship between the implementation of BFRC and observable changes in the frequency of undesired co- sleeping for each child.

In addition, results from a small sample might not generalize to a wider population. We did not evaluate the extent to which each parent dyad worked together.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 67 67

We did encourage both parents to implement treatment and designated one parent responsible for data recording; therefore, it is unclear if a could effectively implement treatment and achieve similar results. In addition, this study focused on preschool and school-aged children, who largely depend on parents to structure their sleep routine and sleep environment. It would be beneficial to determine if parents of adolescents with ASD can effectively implement BFRC with comparable outcomes.

Nevertheless, BFRC was effective for three young children with severe autism and multiple co-occurring sleep problems, who were living in a two-parent household. A randomized controlled trial would strengthen the external validity of these findings and help to determine effective levels of parental participation.

Lastly, we did not include an objective measure such as video recording to confirm procedural integrity or collect IOA to document the reliability of data collection procedures. Based on clinical considerations (Burke, Kuhn, & Peterson, 2004; Durand &

Mindell, 1999; Vollmer & Sloman, 2008), we selected telephone checks, a less intrusive method, to ensure ongoing and timely data collection and to obtain data on treatment integrity.

Behavior analysts have a great appreciation for precise terminology that conveys information effectively. Response cost is best described as a punishment procedure in the form of a penalty, which may not be the most accurate label for the response extinction components of the BFRC sleep intervention. Although terminological refinement may be warranted, perhaps the more salient endeavor lies in conceptualizing the effectiveness of

BFRC using behavioral processes and concepts. Respondent conditioning, operant conditioning, and the value altering effects of motivating operations all contribute to

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 68 68 understanding the socially significant phenomenon of sleep. As such, the process of respondent and operant conditioning and motivating operations may all contribute to the effectiveness of the BFRC procedure.

Respondent conditioning may contribute through a stimulus-stimulus pairing process, in which an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) becomes a conditioned stimulus (CS) that elicits the conditioned response (CR). From a respondent conditioning standpoint, the UCS is (i.e., physiological state of sleepiness), which is often associated with delaying bedtime and limiting access to the unconditioned response (UCR) of sleep.

Delaying the initial bedtime component of the BFRC procedure increases the likelihood of the child achieving sleep onset within 15 minutes of being placed in bed. The behavior of lying down in bed may have become a CS that functions as a signal for initiating sleep.

If the child falls asleep soon after being placed in bed, the CS effectively elicited the CR of sleep.

Operant conditioning, that is, the basic process of learning that occurs when a stimulus changes immediately following a response, resulting in changes in the behavior when under similar conditions in the future, may also contribute to the effectiveness of the BFRC procedure. Prior to treatment, the child had access to preferred activities, parental attention, and the opportunity to fall asleep whenever he or she became sleepy, whereas during treatment the parent controls such accessibility and opportunities. In the

BFRC treatment, the child has 15 minutes to fall asleep once being placed in bed, otherwise he or she is removed from bed and does not have another opportunity to sleep for 30 minutes. The stimulus change of being removed from bed occurs immediately following the child’s response of being awake in bed. Although the child has access to

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 69 69 preferred activities and parental attention, access to sleep is restricted until the next scheduled bedtime. As the child’s sleepiness increases, being removed from bed becomes an unpleasant event. The child learns to avoid being removed from bed by engaging in sleep-compatible behaviors or achieving rapid sleep onset.

Motivating Operation (MO) is a behavioral concept used to explain variations in the strength of value-altering and behavior-altering effects (Michael, 1982; Michael,

2000). is one of the primary MOs identified in humans. It is the current strength of sleepiness that can alter the reinforcing value of consequences and change the probability of behaviors. That is, extreme sleepiness can increase the effectiveness of sleep as reinforcement and increase the current frequency of sleep-compatible behavior

(e.g., lying down in bed) that has been reinforced by sleep. As such, MOs alter the effectiveness of both respondent and operant conditioning. Delaying the child’s bedtime and restricting access to sleep alters the reinforcing value of sleep and changes the probability of achieving rapid sleep onset and engaging in sleep-compatible behaviors.

When the duration of time children spend awake in bed is reduced, the experience of sleepiness and sleep onset comes under stimulus control of the bedtime routine, the bed, and other aspects of the bedroom environment. Therefore, the BFRC procedure makes the most out of naturally occurring sleep processes (i.e., motivating operations) while teaching more appropriate sleep associations (i.e., stimulus control).

In summary, this investigation expands research on BFRC as a home-based, parent-delivered intervention for undesired co-sleeping with co-occurring sleep problems experienced by children with severe ASD. This investigation sets the stage for empowering parents to function as the agents of change by utilizing their own treatment

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 70 70 goals and making them responsible for implementing BFRC. Moreover, this study provides evidence that elevates BFRC toward to a new classification tier as a well established empirically supported treatment (Chambless & Hollon, 1998). In conclusion, the results of this study provide evidence for parent implementation of BFRC as a behavioral treatment option for children with ASD and multiple sleep problems. Future research should center on generalizing these favorable outcomes.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 71 71

References

Adams, H. L., Matson, J. L., & Jang, J. (2014). The relationship between sleep problems

and challenging behavior among children and adolescents with autism spectrum

disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 1024-1030.

doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.05.008

Allen, K. D., Kuhn, B. R., DeHaai, K. A., & Wallace, D. P. (2013). Evaluation of a

behavioral treatment package to reduce sleep problems in children with angelman

syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 676-686.

doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.10.001

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Ancoli-Israel, S., Martin, J. L., Blackwell, T., Buenaver, L., Liu, L., Meltzer, L. J., Sadeh,

A., Spira, A. P., & Taylor, D. J. (2015). The SBSM guide to actigraphy

monitoring: clinical and research applications. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 13, 4

38. doi:10.1080/15402002.2015.1046356

Anders, T. F., Iosif, A., Schwichtenberg, A. J., Tang, K., & Goodlin-Jones, B. L. (2011).

Six month sleep-wake organization and stability in preschool-age children with

autism, developmental delay, and typical development. Behavioral Sleep

Medicine, 9, 92-106. doi:10.1080/15402002.2011.557991

Andersen, I. M., Kaczmarska, J., McGrew, S. G., & Malow, B. A. (2008). Melatonin for

in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Neurology,

23, 482-485. doi:10.1177/0883073807309783

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 72 72

Ashbaugh, R., & Peck, S. M. (1998). Treatment of sleep problems in a toddler: A

replication of the faded bedtime with response cost protocol. Journal of Applied

Behavior Analysis, 31, 127-129. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-127

Blampied, N. M., France, K. G. (1993). A behavioral model of infant sleep disturbance.

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 477-492. doi:10.1901/jaba.1993.26-477

Braam, W., Keijzer, H., Struijker Boudies, H., Didden, R., Smits, M., & Curfs, L. (2012).

CYPIA2 polymorphisms in slow melatonin metabolizers: a possible relationship

with autism spectrum disorders? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 1-8.

doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2012.01595.x.

Bramble, D. & Feehan, C. (2005). Psychiatrists’ use of melatonin with children. Child

and Adolescent Mental Health, 10, 145-149. doi:10.1111/j.1475-

3588.2005.00358.x

Burke, R. V., Kuhn, B. R. & Peterson, J. L. (2004). Brief report: a ‘storybook’ ending to

children’s bedtime problems – the use of a rewarding social story to reduce

bedtime resistance and frequent night waking. Journal of Pediatric Psychology,

29, 389-396. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsh042

Center for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2016, April 1). Prevalence and

characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years – autism

and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2012.

Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Retrieved from

https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/volumes/65/ss/ss6503a1.htm

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 73 73

Chambless, D. L., Baker, M. J., Baucom, D. H., Beutler, L. E., Calhoun K. S., Crits-

Christoph, P., et al. (1998). Update on empirically validated therapies, II. The

Clinical Psychologist, 51, 3-16.

Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining empirically supported therapies.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 7-18.

Chambless, D. L. & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically supported psychological

interventions: controversies and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 685-

716. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.685

Christodulu, K. V., & Durand, V. M. (2004). Reducing bedtime disturbances and night

waking using positive bedtime routines and sleep restriction. Focus on Autism and

Other Developmental Disabilities, 19, 130-139.

doi:10.1177/10883576040190030101

Corkum, P., Tannock, R., Moldofsky, H., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Humphries, T. (2001).

Actigraphy and parental ratings of sleep in children with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Sleep, 24, 303-312. doi:10.1586/ern.11.7

Crane, D. R., Middleton, K. C., & Bean, R. A. (2000). Establishing criterion scores for

the Kansas marital satisfaction scale and the revised dyadic adjustment scale. The

American Journal of Family Therapy, 28, 53-60. doi:10.1080/019261800261815

DeLeon, I., Fisher, W., & Marhefka, J. (2004). Decreasing self-injurious behavior

associated with awakening in a child with autism and developmental delays.

Behavioral Interventions, 19, 111-119. doi:10.1002/bin.154

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 74 74

Dodge, N. N., & Wilson, G. A. (2001). Melatonin for treatment of sleep disorders in

children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Child Neurology, 16, 581-

584.

Dominick, K. C., Ornstein Davis, N., Lainhart, J., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Folstein, S.

(2007). Atypical behaviors in children with autism and children with a history of

language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28, 145-162.

doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2006.02.003

Durand, V. M. (2002). Treating sleep terrors in children with autism. Journal of Positive

Behavior Interventions, 4, 66–72. doi: 10.1177/109830070200400201.

Durand, V. M. & Christodulu, K. V. (2004). A description of a sleep restriction

program to reduce bedtime disturbances and night waking. Journal of

Positive Behavioral Interventions, 6, 83-91.

doi:10.1177/10983007040060020301

Durand, V. M., Gernert-Dott, P., Mapstone, E. (1996). Treatment of sleep

disorders in children with developmental disabilities. The Journal of the

Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 21, 114-122.

doi:10.1177/154079699602100302

Durand, V. M., & Mindell, J. A. (1999). Behavioral intervention for childhood sleep

terrors. Behavior Therapy, 30, 705–715. doi:005-7894/99/0705

Fisman, S. N., Wolf, L. C., & Noh, S. (1989). Marital intimacy in parents of

exceptional children. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34, 519-525.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 75 75

Friedman, A., & Luiselli, J. K. (2008). Excessive daytime sleep: behavioral assessment

and intervention in a child with autism. Behavior Modification, 32, 548-555.

doi:10.1177/0145445507312187

Garstang, J. & Wallis, M. (2006). Randomized controlled trial of melatonin for

children with autistic spectrum disorders and sleep problems. ,

Health, & Development, 32, 585-589. doi:10.1111/j.1365

2214.2006.00616.x

Giannotti, F., Cortesi, F., Cerquiglini, A., & Bernabei, P. (2006). An open-label study of

controlled-release melatonin in treatment of sleep disorders in children with

autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 741-752.

doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0116-z

Goodlin-Jones, B. L., Tang, K., Liu, J. Y., & Anders, T. F. (2008). Sleep patterns in

preschool-age children with autism, developmental delay, and typical

development. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 47, 930-938. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181799f/c

Gotham, K., Risi, S., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2007). The autism diagnostic observation

schedule: revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders, 37, 613-627. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0280-1

Gupta, R. & Hutchins, J. (2005). Melatonin: a panacea for desperate parents? (hype or

truth). Archives of Disease in Childhood, 90, 986-987.

doi:10.1136/adc.2005.075218

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 76 76

Harvey, M. T., May, M. E., & Kennedy, C. H. (2004). Nonconcurrent multiple baseline

design and the evaluation of educational systems. Journal of Behavioral

Education, 13, 267-276. doi:1053-0819/04/1200-0267/0

Hirshkowitz, M., Whiton, K., Albert, S. M., Alessi, C., Bruni, O., DonCarlos, L., et al.,

(2015). National sleep foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations:

methodology and results summary. Sleep Health, 1, 40-43.

doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.010

Hodge, D., Carollo, T. M., Lewin, M., Hoffman, C. D., & Sweeney, D. P. (2014). Sleep

patterns in children with an without autism spectrum disorders: developmental

comparisons. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 1631-1638.

doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2014.03.037

Hoffman, C. D., Sweeney, D. P., Lopez-Wagner, M. C., Hodge, D., Nam, C. Y., Botts, B.

H. (2008). Children with autism, sleep problems and mothers’ stress. Focus on

Autism and Other Disabilities, 23, 155-165. doi:10.1177/1088357608316271

Howlin, P. (1984). A brief report on the elimination of long tern sleeping problems in a

6-year-old autistic boy. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 12, 257-260.

doi:10.1017/S014134730001082X

Hudson, A., Wilken, P., Jauering, R., Raddler, G. (1995). Regionally based teams for the

treatment of challenging behavior: a three year outcome study. Behavior Change,

12, 209-215. doi:10.1017/S081348390000406X

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 77 77

Iglowstein, I., Jenni, O. G., Molinari, L., & Largo, R. H. (2003). Sleep duration from

infancy to adolescence: reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics, 111,

302-307. doi:10.1542/peds.111.2.302

Jan, J. E., Freeman, R. D., Wasdell, M. B., Bomben, M. M. (2004). ‘A child with severe

night terrors and sleep-walking responds to melatonin therapy’. Developmental

Medicine and Child Neurology, 46, 789. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2004.tb01003.x

Jin, C. S., Hanley, G. P., & Beauliue, L. (2013). An individualized and comprehensive

approach to treating sleep problems in young children. Journal of Applied

Behavior Analysis, 46, 161-180. doi:10.1002/jaba.16

Johnson, K. P., & Malow, B. A. (2008). Assessment and pharmacologic treatment of

sleep disturbance in autism. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North

America, 17, 773-785. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2008.06.006

Kennaway, D. J. (2015). Potential safety issues in the use of the hormone melatonin in

paediatrics. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 1-6. doi:10.1111/jpc.12840

Knight, R. M. & Johnson, C. M., (2014). Using a behavioral treatment package for sleep

problems in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child & Family Behavior

Therapy, 36, 204-221. doi:10.1080/07317107.2014.934171

Krakowiak, P., Goodlin-Jones, B., Hertz-Picciotto, I., Croen, L. A., & Hansen, R. L.

(2008). Sleep problems in children with autism spectrum disorders,

developmental delays, and typical development: a population-based study.

Journal of Sleep Research, 17, 197-206. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00650.x.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 78 78

Levin, A., & Scher, A. (2016). Sleep problems in young children with autism spectrum

disorders: a study of parenting stress, mothers’ sleep-related cognitions, and

bedtime behaviors. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 22, 921-927.

doi:10.1111/cns.12651

Lewandowski, A. S., Toliver-Sokol, M., & Palermo, T. M. (2011). Evidence-based

review of subjective pediatric sleep measures. Journal of Pediatric Psychology,

36, 780-793. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsp119.Epub2011

Lopez-Wagner, M. C., Hoffman, C. D., Sweeney, D. P., Hodge, D., & Gilliam, J. E.

(2008). Sleep problems of parents of typically developing children and parents of

children with autism. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169, 245-259.

doi:10.3200/GNTP.169.3.245-260.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., Bishop, S. (2012).

Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition. Torrance, CA:

Western Psychological Services.

Malow, B., Adkins, K. W., McGrew, S. G., Wang, L., Goldman, S. E., Fawkes, D.

(2012). Melatonin for sleep in children with autism: a controlled trial examining

dose, tolerability, and outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

42, 1729-1737. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1418-3

Malow, B., Adkins, K. W., Reynolds, A., Weiss, S. K., Loh, A., Fawkes, D. et al. (2014).

Parent-based sleep education for children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal

of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 216-228. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-

1866-z

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 79 79

Malow, B. A., & McGrew, S. G. (2008). Sleep Disturbances and Autism. Sleep Medicine

Clinics, 3, 479-488. doi:10.1016/j.jsmc.2008.04.004

Mazurek, M. O., & Sohl, K. (2016). Sleep and behavioral problems in children with

autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46,

1906-1915. doi:10.1007/s10803-016-2723-7

McMahon, R. J. & Forehand, R. L. Parent’s consumer satisfaction questionnaire.

Adapted from Helping the Noncompliant Child (2nd ed.). The Guildford

Press (2003).

Meltzer, L. J. (2011). Factors associated with depressive symptoms in parents of

children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum

Disorders, 5, 361-367. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.05.001

Meltzer, L. J., Montgomery-Downs, H. E., Insana, S. P. & Walsh, C. M. (2012).

Use of actigraphy for assessment in pediatric sleep research. Sleep

Medicine Reviews, 16, 463-475. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2011.10.002

Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational

functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37,

149-155.

Michael, J. (2000). Implications and refinements of the establishing operation

concept. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 401-410.

Mihaila, I., & Hartley, S. L. (2016). Parental sleep quality and behavior problems

of children with autism. Autism, 1-9. doi:10.1177/1362361316673570

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 80 80

Mindell, J. A., & Durand, V. M. (1993). Treatment of childhood sleep disorders:

generalization across disorders and effects on family members. Journal of

Pediatric Psychology, 18, 731-750.

Mindell, J. A., Kuhn, B., Lewin, D. S., Meltzer, L. J., & Sadeh, A. (2006).

Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night waking in infants and

young children: an American academy of sleep medicine review. Sleep,

29, 1263-1275.

Montgomery, P., Stores, G., & Wiggs, L., (2004). The relative efficacy of two

brief treatments for sleep problems in young learning disabled (mentally

retarded) children: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of Disease in

Childhood, 89, 125-130. doi:10.1136/adc.2002.017202

Moon, E. C., Corkum, P., & Smith, I. M. (2010). Case Study: A case-series

evaluation of a behavioral sleep intervention for three children with autism

and primary insomnia. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 1-8.

doi:10.10.1093/jpepsy/jsq057

Murray, M., Hsia, Y., Glaser, K., Simonoff, E., Murphy, D., Asherson, P.,

Eklund, H., & Wong, I. (2014). Pharmacological treatments prescribed to

people with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in primary health care.

Psychopharmacology, 231,1011-1021. doi:10.1007/s00213-013-3140-7

Owens, J. A., Spirito, A., & McGuinn, M. (2000). The children’s sleep habits

questionnaire (CSHQ): psychometric properties of a survey instrument for

school aged children. Sleep, 23, 1043-1051.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 81 81

Owens, J. A., & Whitmans, M. (2004). Sleep problems. Current Problems in

Pediatric & Adolescent Health Care. 154-179.

Paavonen, E. J., Nieminen-Von, W. T., Vanhala, R., Aronen, E. T., von Wendt, L.

(2003). Effectiveness of melatonin in the treatment of sleep disturbances in

children with asperger disorder. Journal of Child & Adolescents

Psychopharmacology, 13, 83-95. doi:10.1089/104454603321666225

Patzold, L. M., Richdale, A. L. & Tonge, B. J. (1998). An investigation into sleep

characteristics of children with autism and asperger’s disorder. Journal of

Paediatrics and Child Health, 34, 528-533.

Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1991a). Bedtime fading in the treatment of

pediatric insomnia. Journal of Behavior, Therapy, & Experimental

Psychiatry, 22, 53-56. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(91)90034-3

Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Moser, H. (1991). Behavioral treatment of sleep

dysfunction on patients with the rett syndrome. Brain & Development, 13,

232-237.

Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1991b). A faded bedtime with response cost

protocol for treatment of multiple sleep problems in children. Journal of

Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 129-140. doi:10.1901/jaba.1991.24-129.

Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Sherer, M. (1997). Treatment of multiple sleep

problems in children with developmental disabilities: faded bedtime with

response cost versus bedtime scheduling. Developmental Medicine &

Child Neurology, 39, 414-418.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 82 82

Piazza, C. C., Hagopian, L. P., Hughes, C. R., & Fisher, W. W. (1998). Using

chronotherapy to treat severe sleep problems: a case study. American Journal on

Mental Retardation, 102, 358–366.

Powers, S. W. (1999). Empirically supported treatments in pediatric psychology:

procedure-related pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 24, 131-145.

doi:10.1093/jpepsy/24.2.131.

Reed, H. E., McGrew, S. G., Artibee, K., Surdkya, K., Goldman, S. E., Frank, K.,

Wang, L., & Malow, B. A. (2009). Parent-based sleep education

workshops in autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 24, 936-945.

doi:10.1177/0883073808331348

Reynolds, A. M. & Malow, B. A. (2011). Sleep and autism spectrum disorders.

Pediatric Clinics of North America, 58, 685-698.

doi:10.1016/j.pcl.2011.03.009

Rossignol, D. A., & Frye, R. E. (2011). Melatonin in autism spectrum disorders: a

systematic review and meta-analysis. Developmental Medicine & Child

Neurology, 1-10. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.03980.x

Sadeh, A., Sharkey, K. M., & Carskadon, M. A. (1994). Activity-based sleep

wake identification: an empirical test of methodological issues. Sleep, 17,

201-217.

Schopler, E., Van Bourgondien, M. E., Wellman, G. J., & Love, S. R. (2010). Childhood

Autism Rating Scale (2nd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 83 83

Schreck, K. A. (2001). Behavioral treatments for sleep problems in autism: empirically

supported or just universally accepted? Behavioral Interventions, 16, 265-278.

doi:10.1002/bin.98

Schreck, K. A., & Mulick, J. A. (2000). Parental report of sleep problems in children with

autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 30, 127-135.

Schumm, W. A., Nicholas, C. W., Schectman, K. L., & Grigsby, C. C. (1983).

Characteristics of response to the Kansas Marital Satisfaction Scale by a sample

of 84 married mothers. Psychological Reports, 53, 567-572.

Tilford, M. J., Payakachat, N., Kuhlthau, K. A., Pyne, J. M., Kovacs, E., Bellando, J. et

al., (2015). Treatment for sleep problems in children with autism and caregiver

spillover effects. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 3613-3623.

doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2507-5

Turner, K. S., & Johnson, C. R. (2012). Behavioral interventions to address sleep

disturbances in children with autism spectrum disorders: a review. Topics in Early

Childhood Special Education, 33, 144-152. doi:10.1177/0271121412446204

Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., & St. Peter Pipkin, C. (2008). Practical implications of

data reliability and treatment integrity monitoring. Behavior Analysis in Practice,

1, 4-11.

Vriend, J. L., Corkum, P. V., Moon, E. C., Smith, I. M. (2011). Behavioral interventions

for sleep problems in children with autism spectrum disorders: current findings

and future directions. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 1-13.

doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsr044

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 84 84

Wasdell, M. B., Jan, J. E., Bomben, M. M., Freeman, R. D., Rietveld, W. J., Tai, J.,

Hamilton, D., & Weiss, M. D. (2008). A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of

controlled release melatonin treatment of delayed sleep phase syndrome and

impaired sleep maintenance in children with neurodevelopmental disabilities.

Journal of Pineal Research, 44, 57-64. doi:10.1111/j.1600-079X.2007.00528.x

Watson, P. J., & Workman, E. A. (1981). The non-concurrent multiple baseline across-

individuals design: an extension of the traditional multiple baseline design.

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 12, 257-259.

doi:10.1016/0005-7916(81)90055-0

Weiskop, S., Matthews, J., & Richdale, A. L. (2001). Treatment of sleep problems in a 5-

year-old boy with autism using behavioural principles. Autism, 5, 209-221.

doi:10.1177/1362361301005002009

Weiskop, S., Richdale, A., & Matthews, J. (2005). Behavioural treatment to reduce sleep

problems in children with autism or fragile x syndrome. Developmental Medicine

& Child Neurology, 47, 94-104. doi:10.1017/S0012162205000186

Wiggs, L., & Stores, G. (2004). Sleep patterns and sleep disorders in children with

autistic spectrum disorders: insights using parent report and actigraphy.

Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 46, 372-380.

doi:10.1017/S0012162204000611

Wirojanan, J., Jacquemont, S., Diaz, R., Bacalman, S., Anders, T. F., Hagerman, R. J.,

Goodlin-Jones, B. L. (2009). The efficacy of melatonin for sleep problems in

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 85 85

children with autism, fragile x syndrome, or autism and fragile x syndrome.

American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 5, 145-150.

Wolf, M., Risley, T., & Mees, H. (1964). Application of operant conditioning procedures

to the behavior problems of an autistic child. Behavior Research & Therapy, 1,

305-312.

Wright, B., Sims, D., Smart, S., Alwazeer, A., Alderson-Day, B., Allgar, V., Whitton, C.,

Tomlinson, H., Bennett, S., & Jardine, J. (2010). Melatonin versus placebo in

children with autism spectrum conditions and severe sleep problems not

amendable to behavior management strategies: a randomized controlled crossover

trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 175-184.

doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1036-5

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 86 86

Table 1 Participant Information

Holly Bill Keith Age in years 8.5 7 4.5 Gender Female Male Male ADOS-2 Overall total 13 24 24 Comparison score 6 10 10 Classification Autism Autism Autism CARS-2 Raw score 31* 44 39 T-score; percentile 51; 54th% 57; 76th% 51; 54th% Severity group Severe Severe Severe Sleep problems meeting selection criteria Requires parent in room to fall asleep   Refusal to sleep in own bed   Undesired co-sleeping    Bedtime resistance  Sleep onset latency   Night awakenings   Note: An ADOS-2 Comparison score of 6 indicates a moderate level of autism spectrum, while a score of 10 indicates a high level of autism spectrum. * Scores for this participant were taken from the CARS-2 High Functioning (HF) version, as opposed to the Standard (ST) version of the measure.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 87 87

Table 2 Dependent Variables on Sleep Diary & Sleep Related Behavior Diary

Dependent Sleep Variables Definitions Frequency of Night-Awakenings Total number of times child awoke during the (FNA) night and got out of bed; Marked by time with an up arrow, not including final rising Waking-up After Sleep Onset Total minutes the child is awake after initially (WASO) falling asleep; Non-shaded in boxes of time (small box = 15 min.; large box = 1 hour) after initial sleep onset, until time of final rising Sleep Onset Latency (SOL) The time between initially being placed in bed awake, marked by time with a down arrow, and the time of sleep onset each night, marked by shaded in boxes Total Sleep Time (TST) Total duration of nighttime and daytime sleep acquired within 24-hours; Shaded in boxes for when the child is asleep (small box = 15 min.; large box = 1 hour) Time in Bed (TIB) Amount of time the child was physically in bed during within 24-hours; Time child gets into bed marked by a down arrow until final rising Dependent Sleep Onset (DSO) The achievement of sleep with the required assistance of a parent; Marked by the letter D next to a down arrow Independent Sleep Onset (ISO) The achievement of sleep without the assistance of a parent; Marked by the letter I next to a down arrow Dependent Sleep Related Behavior Definitions Variables Undesired Co-Sleeping (UC-S) Unwanted co-sleeping with a parent or sibling in any room during a 24 hour period; Frequency and duration of undesired co-sleeping with parent(s) or siblings by child Sleeping in a Non-Designated Area Frequency of child sleeping in a location other than the child’s bed (e.g., couch, car, floor, etc.) during a 24 hour period Location of Non-Designated Sleep Location of undesired co-sleeping with child

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 88 88

Table 3 Session Timeline

Weekly Activities Sessions Session 1: Intake interview, CARS-2, ADOS-2, KMSS, and consent Session 2: Identify parent sleep goals, teach data collection, give actigraphy, and start baseline data collection Session 3: Review week one baseline data and determine treatment or cont. baseline Session 4: Review week two baseline data or week one treatment data. Cont. or discontinue treatment Session 5: Review week one or week two of treatment data. Cont. or discontinue treatment Session 6: Review week two of treatment data or begin follow-up data collection (4-weeks post treatment) Session 7: Begin (4-weeks post treatment) or end (7 days) follow-up data collection. Session 8: End follow-up data collection

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 89 89

Table 4 Goal Achievement Scales (GAS)

c

a

c

a

b

b

FU

00%

a

75%

86%

29%

71%

43%

50%

1

100%

100%

100%

100%

100%

o

ng t

s

c

a

c

a

b

b

e

s

ordi

ha

93%

43%

75%

EOT

c

64%

57%

82%

64%

21%

18%

100%

100%

100%

P

c

a

s

c

a

c

a

b

b

L

y, 2013.

0%

1%

0%

0%

0%

0%

ne

BS

50%

57%

25%

50%

36%

50%

e

p norm p

e

e

e

w

l

)

)

s

e

e

S

l

s

s

a

nt

n, &

e

a

r pha r

r pha r

m

f

pe

pe

opm

l

s

s

of

ve

ght

ght

e

n, H

i

ni

ni

D

w

y;

e

nc

e

t

(% of (%

(% of (%

o, L

a

l

);

);

l

f

t

l

rol

e

e

ght

%)

s

)

S

m

e

, Ca ,

s

A

h ni

p ons

c

)

e

a

(G

e

e

s

l

by hi

odge

);

r pha r

p e

S

s

%)

H

l

e

S

c

ght

e

pe

=

%)

l

s

r pha r

A

S

L

s

ni

A

O

o goa

pe

ght

(G

)

he

of

s

t

S

e

);

t

e

l

s

ni

(G

di

)

);

ng t

)

up;

l

e

ount

nut

)

-

e

i

s

e

rgo, 2003; rgo,

s

d e

m

s

a

r pha r

d goa

e

m

ow

t

a

(% of (%

e

s

l

L

t

e

l

p duri p

d goa

);

pe

t

r pha r

ol

e

e

s

a

r pha r

t

oa

r pha r

e

, &

bora

l

ght

pe

G

bora

a

ri

ght

(% of (%

pe

l

s

a

= F =

pe

s

l

o s

);

s

bora

na

ol

U

ni

i

a

ol

ght

ry ni ry

l

c

ght

ppropri

F

t

ol

ght

ght

c

;

s

t

ol

ve

urn t urn

ni

s

t

ni

nt

y a

c

ni

pi

l

t

e

, M

(e

pi

h ni

l

(% of (%

s

ra

c

a

m

ra

d;

a

t

ge

pi

nni

d (re d

he

nt

a

(% of (%

(% of (%

t

e

e

he

e

ra

.

-

t

(% of (%

re

L

-

or a or

n be

d);

t

);

nt

he

n be

O

n, J

t

nt

i

-

p.m

re

opm

p f

e

S

l

re

ght

t

e

nt

e

n be

s

e

n ow

30

l

nd of

ve

n ow

re

i

i

s

7:

ow

duc

-

ry ni ry

de

nd (pa nd

t

n ow

ght

= E =

nd (pa nd

of

ght

a

Igl

00

ve

T

s

a

b

r (re r

l

ni

nd (pa nd

l

ni

n (i n

O

a

ount

n 7:

p (ge p

d (e d

he

l

he

E

e

e

;

m

e

e

oone

l

d goa

d goa

ne

w

n be

e

e

i

t

d a

t

t

l

p s

., 2015;

d goa

a

a

e

l

e

p on ow

re

t

e

t

s

e

t

be

e

s

s

a

l

-

hrough t hrough

a

e

-

e

t

hrough t hrough

n ow

t

l

qui

s

s

nt

m

nt

nough s

o s

p t

-

, e

i

p t

p i

a

e

z

e

re

re

re

= Ba =

e

l

e

t

nt

t

t

dt

e

l

o t

i

e

e

l

e

e

L

l

a

l

re

pa

pa

S

Be

F

G

G

S

BS

pa

y’s

h’s

:

l

hkow

t

4.

3.

2.

1.

2.

1.

2. G

1. S

’s

e

i

!

l

rs

e

ol

l

i

K

H

H

Not

Bi

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 90 90

Table 5

Sleep Variables (Actigraphy, CSHQ-A, Sleep and Behavior Diaries)

d

e

e

;

r

t

77)

)

)

)

)

b

37)

18)

31)

46)

e

)

e

17)

4

5

6

0

t

3

t;

(

f

0.

0.

0)

0)

0)

7.

4.

85;

57;

14;

one

6

3

1

9

s

28;

pl

(

(

(

(

(

(

(

e

27.

.

.

.

.

1

a

t

3

6

5

3

1

32%

.

02%

(

s

42

N

9

7

0

9

FU

n

n

0;

0;

0;

e

(6

57;

57.

om

up

(5

(5

(6

(3

r

612.

608.

567.

99.

14;

07;

0;

28;

57;

89.

o

-

8.

p

g

nc

0.

0.

4.

5.

502;

Pa

I

e

n

i

;

le

=

)

d

ak

e

0)

77)

t s

C

)

)

)

94)

46

4)

25)

b

n

W

6

0

1

h

(

2.

N

T

t

e

7.

0)

0.

64;

57;

C

C

C

C

C

(

I

0.

1

2

2

i

12.

21;

5%

(

(

(

.

.

.

=

d

(

ts

3

7

4

3

1

(

;

46

car

1

0

8

n

n

e

IN

IN

IN

IN

IN

07;

EO

0;

Ke

O

e

e

33.

91.

21;

(5

(6

(3

r

624.

578.

bl

32;

28;

p

S

1.

06;

42;

a

e

2.

l

6.

0.

A

0.

Pa

i

6.

D

,

W

;

va

d

3)

=

;

a

e

)

ed

9

83)

)

)

)

)

)

)

ch

07)

09)

91)

74)

O

b

14)

35)

5

b

cy

7

7

2

9

3

4

u

ot

7.

5;

S

3

L

1.

2.

0.

0.

92;

12;

92;

(

0.

s

7

8

9

9

.

1

6

13.

78;

07;

78;

s

(

(

(

(

.

.

.

.

.

.

N

en

(

4

7

6

1

(

co

t

10.

0

60

12

1

2

9

0

1

1

(

d

; D

,

n

2

ci

94%

BS

=

l

i

s

i

56.

637.

10.

50.

e

57;

(7

(5

(1

(6

(5

(9

598.

598.

524.

f

07;

32;

71;

64;

r

(1

g

13;

64;

A

car

ch

/

13;

ef

1.

1.

1.

0.

0.

in

9.

88.

Pa

N

n

e

;

e

k

eep

t

l

a

i

)

S

)

w

)

C

5)

;

=

)

pos

C

15

15)

N

)

)

t a

7.

e

6

(

(

I

5;

(

E

h

0)

0)

0)

0)

0)

0)

75;

N

(

C

C

one

n

9

(

(

(

(

(

(

I

.

S

3

6

4

3

1

om

49%

(

5;

5;

ig

36

N

5

FU

c

97%

No

2;

4;

0;

0;

0;

0;

25;

e;

(IN

(IN

n

548.

75;

(1

463.

f

0;

84.

or

m

31;

i

547.

532.

o

11.

t

vi

49.

y

c

ha

n

;

)

eep

e

)

)

)

)

)

)

)

)

)

l

66)

be

27)

)

)

u

s

e

4

4

0

9

7

6

9

5

1

(

l

11

T

4.

0

0

71;

71;

85;

64;

l

q

one

n

2

9

1

0

4

7

7

5

9

10;

71;

71;

71;

(

(

(

.

.

.

.

.

.

.

.

.

4

6

6

5

1

ve

e

82%

al

1.

68%

43

i

N

r

t

0

0

2

5

6

2

8

4

2

(

Bi

85;

EO

No

0;

0;

o

55.

40.

25.

23.

F

(7

(4

(5

(6

(3

(4

(1

(9

(3

520.

445.

507.

401.

85.

71;

0:

79.

T

1;

upt

r

=

49.

5.

s

=

i

A

;

)

T

d

D

N

e

9)

S

;

16)

81)

99)

65)

58

)

)

)

)

=

b

98)

40)

41)

40)

T

; F

74)

3

3

5

6

8.

9.

5;

5;

;

L

3.

0.

0.

45.

83;

(

(

d

A

C

0.

4

3

1

9

s

24.

6%

(

(

(

(

56.

22.

7.

.

.

.

.

(

t

3

9

7

5

3

e

(

23%

(

(

(

48

B

t

6

0

0

6

cy

n

5;

N/

BS

a

5;

e

D

502.

414.

i

84.

41;

5;

(3

(7

(1

(5

r

104.

en

33;

16;

16;

68.

16;

;

v

35;

e

e

at

5.

1.

1.

57.

230;

595;

0.

Pa

r

l

12.

608.

73.

b

nc

et

a

t

s

Ab

)

i

5)

5)

ons

s

e

;

)

r

e

98)

74

24)

)

)

)

i

p

r

6

23)

57)

a

e

1)

8

9

2

e

22.

22.

5;

5;

8

(

0)

0)

e

25;

(

(

one

n

n

9

.

0

3

e

0.

0.

16.

(

(

36.

70.

.

.

.

l

3

6

5

3

1

(

(

n

2

75%

(

(

40

m

N

5

1

8

FU

5;

S

i

o

0

94%

No

0;

0;

i

577.

132.

5;

25;

25;

5;

t

25;

(2

(1

(4

0.

475.

=

dt

0;

74.

(1

s

0.

0.

e

L

615;

642;

be

11.

26.

31.

u

O

=

Q

S

;

s

)

)

)

)

)

)

)

)

98)

23)

94)

R

t

e

y

9

8

1

6

7

0)

3

9

9

i

T

l

B

0.

0.

0)

0)

0.

90;

90;

18;

27;

one

n

A

A

A

A

A

A

7

4

4

0

0

1

7

6

8

b

l

up;

63;

72;

27;

81;

(

(

(

(

(

.

.

.

.

.

.

.

.

.

85%

-

a

08%

N

1

2

8

2

0

4

0

5

7

p;

N/

N/

N/

N/

N/

N/

EO

w

H

No

0;

0;

e

Ho

28.

17.

91.

13.

(1

(3

(4

(2

(4

(7

(1

(7

(1

625.

550.

605.

491.

o

87.

e

18;

36;

0;

72;

81.

l

p

l

l

s

e

1.

0.

0.

o

e

d

F

d

e

Sl

t

e

5)

;

2)

=

5)

72)

)

)

)

)

)

b

n

)

7.

3

6

3

4

6

e

0.

0)

5;

5;

(

1

U

L

0)

0)

0)

25;

25;

25;

(

(

r

one

69.

6

5

6

7

.

0

gna

s

39.

(

(

(

.

.

.

.

.

i

(

t

F

4

5

3

1

18%

d

(

0

03%

56

14

N

s

l

6

9

6

3

8

n

;

i

BS

t

1;

0;

0;

75;

e

(1

652.

652.

25;

15;

5;

h

(4

(4

(4

(5

(6

r

671.

667.

399.

de

n

97.

0;

60.

-

e

C

1.

192;

18.

n

Pa

43.

m

=

t

a

No

A

e

-

r

=

21)

t

-

Q

f

H

o

: 3

DS

S

e

-

d

)

g

C

n

N

n

n

;

s

E

;

o

18)

on.

a

i

e

d

-

i

g

r

t

l

3)

s

t

=

e

6

-

n

a

(

e

b

i

b

)

:

r

1

c

a

g

T

p

9)

9

i

e

o

o

:

n

-

unc

-

r

e

)

)

l

i

O

in

g

c

)

e

f

3

e

3

l

s

n

)

g

E

D

D

e

)

th

:

D

a

:

)

a

)

sl

Va

)

)

n

;

S

S

D;

)

a

e

)

e

r

-

w

m

)

D)

s

t

a

e

m

(

e

;

;

i

S

g

D

g

D

o

SD

a

r

; S

D

.

e

D

.

r

n

S

SD

n

r

(

D

SD

n

SD

c

;

s

r

n

e

e

T

S

S

i

;

n

n

e

.

S

;

;

.

l

r

a

e

i

a

i

;

b

;

c

S

o

.

.

e

;

;

y

d

d

q

.

r

.

;

r

r

e

a

n

.

.

=

l

c

d

n

.; S

.

;

q

q

(

)

i

(

a

e

e

n

s

m

n

m

n

o

.

a

n

s

n

l

e

a

r

e

n

i

i

r

e

n

w

n

i

i

e

t

s

c

r

c

i

B

n

i

i

t

f

r

r

e

l

n

m

n

n

i

o

s

I

s

p

s

s

f

f

y

e

g

m

m

Ba

a

d

i

o

i

m

m

a

a

r

m

m

n

t

e

t

n

i

b

of

d

n

T

s

)

m

n

e

t

e

n

n

t

a

i

s

a

n

r

n

a

a

d

=

n

n

e

;

De

a

n

e

n

a

a

a

e

a

a

k

t

c

e

o

r

n

a

a

n

m

y

m

y

To

r

s

Su

e

a

di

a

o

e

e

e

e

L

(

r

(

a

o

e

e

t

SD

h

e

is

u

n

e

e

m

l

m

U

se

a

e

m

m

m

(

)

;

(

(

wa

A

A

p

m

i

d

o

m

m

m

or

n

(

(

(

O

d

O

m

-

m

-

(

(

a

=

(

(

t

BS

d

m

i

(m

(

o

%

%

S

S

p

p

p

r

S

S

t

(

vi

h

Q

Q

due

(

(

:

C

-

-

A

L

L

O

e

e

e

S

p

g

T

T

d

p

DS

g

e

B

i

-

e

e

e

a

-

H

H

t

t

e

ha

IB

le

t

e

e

DB

BR

N

UC

UC

DS

FN

T

WA

SE

TS

SO

S

Be

Ni

Sl

Sl

TI

WA

SE

TS

SO

da

UC

sl

No

Be

Sl

CS

CS

Ac !

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 91 91

Table 6

Marital Impact Variable, Kansas Marital Satisfaction Scale (KMSS)

l

s

21

21

FU

a

t

nt

ri

re

a

a

P

M

21

21

s

EOT

a

h’s

t

ns

i

a

e

L

K

21

21

BS

= K =

S

S

M

K

21

21

FU

s

up;

-

nt

re

ow

a

l

ol

21

21

P

EOT

’s

l

l

= F =

Bi

L

U

!

21

21

F

;

BS

nt

e

m

t

a

re

s

18

18

t

FU

nt

re

a

nd of

P

17

17

EOT

= E =

y’s

l

T

ol

O

L

H

E

17

17

;

BS

ne

i

l

.

e

e

s

l

a

ore

c

c

ore

s

ore

c

s

l

21)

c

21)

= Ba =

s

-

-

a

l

s

on S

L

l

i

ore

3

3

ot

t

:

:

c

c

t

rna

BS

S

a

rna

S

e

f

:

t

e

nge

nge

s

t

e

a

i

a

t

a

(ra

P

(ra

M

S

Not

KMS

!

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 92 92

Table 7

Parent’s Consumer Satisfaction Questionnaire1 (PCSQ)

7

7

7

7

6

6

7

4

7

7

1*

s

s

1, 2,

Keith

a

w

ore

ons

i

c

l

t

re

l

he

i

s

S

7

7

7

7

7

7

5

7

5

7

7

Bi

nt

y be

que

re

a

onna

:

i

a

t

e

y

P

s

l

pl

7

7

7

7

7

7

6

7

4

7

7

nd m

ol

m

2003 by T

H

r a r

xa

h que

ght

rc

a

e

orde

on (e on

s

rom

i

e

t

d

s

y

e

rs

l

d f

t

nt

ve

c

y. Re y.

e

rne

re

nd. Copyri nd.

m

a

e

e

n re n

onl

ha

di

nt

e

pl

d by que

d i

e

ore

m

us

not

ri

i

l

poi

s

you l

ore

s

t

. F

c

nt

m

va

hi

e

ha

ona

re

t

x L

t

rs

nd s

ons

pa

i

) a )

ng w

d a

i

of

re

or pe or

nd Re nd

y probl y

opt

l

e

i

us

(a

n?

s

e

ype

orde

m

t

a

s

d i

w

hon a

l

pons

a

hi

s

re

book f

hom

hi

s

or f or

M

l

c

hi

he

ng t

y c

t

i

s

nt

nd re nd

n t

s

ona

nd 9 a

. M

i

i

of

on your ow

y i

) a )

rs

s

l

J

e

or m or

i

poi

d

rs

nt

rt

s

8 a

m

f

e

e

e

by us

m

i

pe

s

e

?

f

e

l

hi

a

nt

m

s

t

t

e

i

hom

ons

ha

ve

ra

re

t

a

i

i

t

t

m

a

t

nd f

he

s

re

ne

a

hom

l

a

Sat

m

t

re

y

ue

p probl p

n t

d a

re

he

r

udy a

e

l

he

i

r ge r

o purc o

t

e

I a I

e

s

t

t

l

n t

hi

s

d.) by Robe by d.)

n t

V

*Q

ons

he

s

d t

e

m

i

i

udy a

gi

t

);

ha

e

of

t

e

he

e

s

s

y c

nd or re or nd

d

e

t

nd

s

nt

e

e

m

h ot

ur

7 = 7

t

ue

re

;

(2

e

ri

i

o be

ng t

he

om

ut

ov

d

a

Q

f

f

d

t

c

or m or

gra

e

s

l

e

i

s

pr

s

f

f

p probl p

d w

ng t

i

o a

s

ge

e

m

i

m

t

e

pe

i

d duri d

Chi

l

d m

l

na

p probl p

at

y

e

s

m

e

a

e

l

s

t

orm

ory out ory

s

e

a

pt

t

ant

f

l

he

at

nt

d duri d

progre

i

s

c

e

re

e

e

s

di

m

o m

a

t

r

r

pl

progra

y

hi

f

a

d’s

r

s

n t

G

l

nt

y t

ur

i

d’s

e

progra

e

re

t

t

l

.

om

e

c

i

hi

a

V

l

y

nt

y prom y

hi

be

m

l

n t

opy t

s

e

t

l

7 = 7

bi

ng

e

progra

nc

y c

;

a

m

e

y c

oc

e

t

na

gi

t

nt

not

re

s

Nonc

m

a

or a or

be

(1 = (1

s

e

ot

i

t

gi

e

na

l

re

he

m

ve

ve

a

a

t

he

t

ng m

on f

wor

c

a

i

ori

ons

bout

t

i

o ph

t

t

n your a your n

y

t

ha

ha

he

s

ng t

t

t

a

re

a

n m

a

nc

t

rt

ha

t

pi

i

re

nt

c

abl

ha

ha

l

on t

bout

r

i

nd t

) t )

ke

e

he

t

t

i

s

e

o t

you i

s

s

t

ore

(s

s

H

de

xpe

poi

you i

i

s

c

i

L

m

re

m

m

s

h t

ng a

s

m

e

e

s

re

i

d?

c

e

a

nt

i

l

rm

l

hi

y e

s

ha

a

e

om

e

rom

e

t

nt

m

hi

hi

e

c

t

Cons

poi

nt

, m

f

-

. P

a

de

d f

l

gre

i

s

l

probl

probl

e

de

pproa

nt

or t or

7

s

i

ra

f

a

pt

ngs

de

or probl or

re

onf

i

d’s

d’s

e

r 1 = 1 r

t

j

l

l

l

progra

poi

onf

e

he

a

da

c

ng a

e

bl

s

f

o your c your o

d you re you d

i

t

ha

hi

hi

i

nt

e

c

A

l

e

m

hi

1

of

y ove

udy?

e

t

ow

d t

ord P ord

t

oul

e

:

y f

y c

y c

f

m

e

ow

t

s

o w

he

d us

l

t

t

e

!

pons

e

s

a

a

s

t

l

ui

nd 3

hi

re

re

a

ra

G

Not

11. M

t

10. H

9. H

8. W

t

7. I f I 7.

6. A

re

5. T

4. M

3. M

2. M 1. T

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 93 93

Frequency of Undesired Co-sleeping

4 Baseline Treatment Follow-up Holly 3

M=1 M=0 M=0 2

1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

4

Bill

3

y

c

n e

u M=1 M=0

q 2 M=0 e

r 7 F 1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

4 M=2 M=.07 Keith M=.29 3

2

1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31 Nights Figure 1. Frequency of undesired co-sleeping per night with parents across children. !

! !

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 94 94

Duration of Undesired Co-sleeping

800 Baseline Treatment Follow-up 700 Holly 600 500 M=653 M=0 M=0 400 300 200 100 0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

400 350 Bill

300

s e

t 250

u n

i 200 M 150 M=230 M=0 M=0 100 50 0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

400 350 Keith 300 250 M=57 M=1 M=9 200 150 100 50 0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31 Nights Figure 2. Duration of undesired co-sleeping with parents across children. !

! !

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 95 95

Duration of Bedtime Resistance

20 Baseline Treatment Follow-up

Holly 15

M=15 M=.36 M=.25 10

5 Change in routine

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

200 Change in routine Bill

150

t

h

g

i

n

r M=12.4 M=55.1 M=4

e 100

p

s

e

t

u n

i 50

M

0

1 6 11 16 21 26 31

25

Keith 20 M=1.3 15 M=6.3 M=.07

Change 10 in routine 5

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

Nights Figure 3. Duration of bedtime resistance acros! s children. ! !

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 96 96

Disruptive Behavior Composite

4 Baseline Treatment Follow-up

Holly 3 M=1.3 M=1.2 M=.05

2 Change in routine

1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

20 Change in 16 routine Bill M=5.3

M=5.7 M=2

s

e r

o 12

c

S

e t

i 8

s

o

p m

o 4

C

0

1 6 11 16 21 26 31

8

Keith 6

M=1.1 M=2.2 M=.14 4 Change in routine 2

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

Nights Figure 4. Disruptive behavior composite scor!es across children. ! !

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 97 97

Frequency of Night Awakenings

5 Baseline Treatment Follow-up

4 Holly M=0 M=.73 M=.50

3 Change in routine

2

1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

5

4 Bill

M=1.2 Change in M=1 M=0

3 routine

y

c n

e 2

u

q

e

r F

1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

5

Keith 4

M=.64 3 M=.36 M=0 Change in 2 routine

1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

Night!s ! Figure 5. Frequency of night awakenings acros s children. !

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 98 98

Frequency of Dependent Sleep Onset

4 Baseline Treatment Follow-up Holly 3

M=1 M=0 2

1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

4

Bill 3 M=1.2 M=0 M=0

2

y c

n 1

e

u

q

e

r F

0

1 6 11 16 21 26 31

4

M=1.7 M=0 M=0 3 Keith

2

1

0 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

Nights Figure 6. Frequency of dependent sleep onset (DSO) per night across children. ! ! !

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 99 99

Appendix A Sample sleep diary data sheet.

Instructions: Please complete the sleep diary during the evening and morning each day within the same 24-hour period. The same person should complete all diaries. Record the date and the following information: 1. Mark the time your child gets into bed with a down arrow “”; 2. Mark the time your child gets out of bed with an up arrow “”; 3. Shade in boxes when your child is asleep (small box = 15 minutes; large box = 1 hour); 4. Mark a “W” next to the arrow if your child was awaken by a parent or alarm; mark an “S” if your child awakened by his/herself; and 5. Mark a “D” if your child falls asleep dependently (with parental presence) or an “I” if your child falls asleep independently (without parental presence).

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 100 100

Appendix B Sample sleep related behavior diary data sheet.

Instructions: Please complete the sleep related behavior diary during the evening and morning each day within the same 24-hour period. The same person should complete all diaries. Mark a tally in the corresponding box and record the duration of each instance the child engages in a disruptive behavior during the bedtime routine and once placed in bed, undesired co-sleeping, and sleep in a non-designated area. Record the date, the time the child gets ready for bed, the time and reason for removing the actigraphy, and the locations of undesired co-sleeping and non-designated sleep. Please use the following definitions: 1. Disruptive behaviors are defined as one or more verbal protests and/or physical protests a night when instructed by a parent to go to bed or go to sleep; 2. Sleeping in a non-designated area is defined as one or more occurrences of sleeping in a location other than the child’s bed during a 24 hour period; and 3. Co-sleeping is defined as unwanted sleeping with a parent or sibling in any room during a 24-hour period.

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 101 101

Appendix C Sample integrity-monitoring data sheet.

Instructions: Record parent responses as reported in the corresponding boxes. Record “Y” for “yes”, “N” for “no”, or “N/A” for not applicable to the context. Tri-weekly 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 phone-check Date and time of call (e.g., 8/14/13; 8:00am) Indicate the caller’s initials Parent answer the phone (Y, N, or N/A) Return call by 5:30 p.m. (Y, N, or N/A) Bedtime Fading 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (BF) Was BF implemented? (Y, N, or N/A) Time placed child in bed last night? Time checked to see if child was sleeping? How did you determine if your child was sleeping? What did you do after your child was or was not sleeping? Response cost 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (RC) Was RC implemented? (Y, N, or N/A) Time child was taken out of bed and bedroom? What did your child do during that time? Time returned child to bed last night? Time checked to see if child was sleeping? How did you determine if your child was sleeping? What did you do after

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 102 102 your child was or was not sleeping? How many times was RC repeated? What time did your child finally fall asleep?

Call # Notes: Barriers: Solutions:

Call # Notes: Barriers: Solutions:

Call # Notes: Barriers: Solutions:

Call # Notes: Barriers: Solutions:

Call # Notes: Barriers: Solutions:

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 103 103

Appendix D Sample vignettes of sleep patterns and related behaviors.

Please complete the sleep diary and sleep related behavior diary using the information presented below. If you have any questions, please do not hesitate to ask the investigator before leaving your session.

Vignette 1. On Saturday 6/22/13, James went to bed by himself at 9:00 p.m. and fell asleep at 9:45 p.m., alone. He awoke on his own at 12:30 a.m. and was put back to bed at 1:45 a.m. by his mother. James fell asleep at 2:00 a.m. by himself and slept till his father woke him at 7:30 a.m..

Vignette 2: On Sunday 6/30/13, Chelsea was put to bed by her father at 8:30 p.m. and fell asleep at 10 p.m. with her father in the room. Chelsea awoke at 2:30 a.m. and went to sleep by herself around 3:30 a.m.. She woke up at 5:00 a.m. on her own, but did not leave her bed until 6:00 a.m.. Chelsea fell asleep in the car on the way to daycare from 7:30 a.m. till 8:00 a.m. and woke-up independently. On the way home from daycare she slept in the car from 5:30 p.m. till her father woke her up at around 6:35 p.m..

BEDTIME FADING WITH RESPONSE COST 104 104

Appendix E Sample bedtime fading with response cost treatment protocol.

f

,

, i

s

e

. If .

e

te

pl

u

m

o

m

n

t

e

xa

r na r

p.

m

i

e

30 mi 30

.

or e or

e

dt

l

s

or he or

or or

. F

s

a

s

be

on.

s

i

i

p f

te

45pm

t

he

e

u

c

d

t

e

l

l

n

ng hi

ra

de

hi

e

ri

.

o 10:

a

.

o s

e

t

nt

t

pe

e

m

s

d t

os

i

d, f

15 mi 15

e

m

hi

t

c

i

nd i

dt

a

e

your c your

l

dt

e

by

n be

t

re

e

on a

d be

nd w

be

i

d i

um

e

pons

t

e

s

s

nt

he

s

c

d a

not

e

us

a

t

s

re

d.

p ons

t

k t

e

o a

dj

e

be

i

c

a

nt

t

e

t

l

a

l

e

e

he

l

a

vi

o be

f

ng pl

t

s

i

a

t

m

i

t

a

i

n

h ba

e

t

c

d t

s

of

re

of

s

be

pl

ni

e

a

i

i

n a

t

m

i

m

p

, pus

i

of

oot

i

l

urne

s

he

e

t

f

a

t

.)

e

t

r), i r),

m

ke

t

c

m

i

t

a

ous

pe

nut

ni

dt

rt

i

d a

i

s

ng re ng

i

n one

vi

hi

be

ng, e

hi

l

t

r be r

o pa

h m

be

i

a

ki

t

pre

i

l

i

t

r t r

of

a

n 15 m

s

ni

d’s

, t

i

l

hi

d w

ng w

s

or he or

t

te

i

s

hi

u

o your w your o

be

ye

or he or

he

n

ndi

.

r

t

a

d t

m

d

p w

t

n hi

e

s

ng e

of

e

be

hi

l

d i

s

l

r

s

ni

e

15 mi 15

or he or

o your c your o

by

ow

hi

s

a

ponde

l

n

l

l

s

l

ke

nut

hi

e

i

hi

a

ng t

re

. ope

t

or he or

o

f

i

s

w

.e

nd a

he

he

d t

a

hi

your c your

s

(i

ordi

l

a

s

s

p w

/

/

e

s

c

o

e

hi

c

e

c

n 15 m

e

d i

c

a

l

l

a

d t

. he

hi

s

l

t

hi

i

.e

a

nd he

droom

hi

pons

l

.

ght

l

c

s

a

(i

nd pl

a

.V

p w

a

f

re

ke

e

l

ng your

a

e

nd be

ng ni

ne

l

your c your

i

i

s

f

w

30pm

not

ng T

rba

i

s

d a

a

a

urni

l

e

ow

hi

t

s

l

l

e

urn your urn

c

a

10:

t

rout

t

be

s

ol

d i

e

a

f

or ve or

l

a

o s

f

he

d doe

m

, re ,

l

hi

i

or

t

by re by

s

w

k t

he

e

hi

e

dt

c

ot

or w or

d not

c

m

ngs

rom

he

or t or

i

m

nut

i

r be r

e

ni

f

oys

dt

d di

, c

your c your

a

e

d f

l

s

l

bl

ke

your

h t

m

hi

ne

a

t

be

f

i

hi

te

i

i

gul

l

i

rva

u

w

30 m

a

:

dt

i

e

s

n

t

rm

a

e

e

t

ni

be

ng w

dure

i

m

your c your

e

nd of

i

your c your

dure

he

yi

t

:

your re your

e

e

a

t

t

15 mi 15

d’s

t

. If .

ng:

no obs

l

r

ove

he

os

pl

e

ow

di

ght

t

us

re

proc

hi

s

t

l

you de

a

m

c

t

f

a

proc

dj

e

f

ol

ni

he

e

h a

e

s

15pm

re

) If )

t

s

) A )

i

) A )

t

m

) A )

i

) Re )

i

i

) F )

m

i

a

uc

he

i

10:

your c your

(i

(i

s

(i

(i

t

(i

(i

pons

dt

s

dt

pe

ddre

) Be )

) A )

) Be )

) Re )

(a

!

(e

(d

(c

(b) Re (b)