Word Processing in Languages Using Non-Alphabetic Scripts

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Word Processing in Languages Using Non-Alphabetic Scripts Word processing in languages using non-alphabetic scripts. The cases of Japanese and Chinese Published by LOT Phone: +31 30 253 6006 Trans 10 3512 JK Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://www.lotschool.nl Cover photo by Rinus Verdonschot ISBN: 978-94-6093-059-1 NUR 616 Copyright © 2011: Rinus Verdonschot. All rights reserved. Word processing in languages using non-alphabetic scripts. The cases of Japanese and Chinese PROEFSCHRIFT Ter verkrijging van de graad van Doctor aan de Universiteit Leiden op gezag van Rector Magnificus prof. mr. P.F. van der Heijden, volgens besluit van het College voor Promoties te verdedigen op donderdag 12 mei 2011 klokke 13.45 uur door Rinus Verdonschot Geboren te Geldrop in 1977 Promotie commissie Promotor: Prof. Dr. N.O. Schiller Co-Promotor: Dr. W. La Heij Overige Leden: Prof. Dr. B. Hommel Prof. Dr. L.L. Cheng Prof. Dr. K. Tamaoka (Nagoya University) Dr. C.C. Levelt Dr. P.A. Starreveld (Universiteit van Amsterdam) Acknowledgements I am indebted to my supervisor (Prof. Niels Schiller) and co- supervisor (dr. Wido La Heij) for allowing me to explore my fascination for the processing of logographic scripts. I have learned a lot from both of you and our discussions were always held in a constructive and pleasant way. I am also very grateful to the LUCL and the Cognitive Psychology department for supporting me when I was collecting data in Japan and China. In this respect I want to especially mention the involvement of Albertien Olthoff and Gea Hakker. Without you I could not have efficiently planned and carried out the work necessary for this dissertation. I also want to thank Prof. Katsuo Tamaoka, Prof. Qingfang Zhang, dr. Sachiko Kiyama, dr. John Phillips, Nanae Murata, Xuebing Zhu, Haoran Zhao and Chikako Ara for their invaluable assistance in recruiting and testing participants in Japan and China. During my PhD period I was lucky to have met so many nice, smart and crazy colleagues here in the Netherlands as well as in Japan and China, it would unfortunately take up too much space to personally acknowledge everybody I have a good connection with but I do want to mention my two office-mates (Marieke and Jun) for making our time together in (and outside) the office so enjoyable. Last but not least I am grateful to my friends, my two ‘paranimfen’ (Johan and Vincent) and above all to my family for their constant support! ϕ5ϟ3ϙϧϗ) ኽ Contents Acknowledgements Chapter 1: General Introduction........................................................... 7 Chapter 2: Japanese kanji primes facilitate naming of multiple katakana targets.................................................................................. 25 Experiment 1: Reading aloud katakana targets preceded by kanji primes with Equal Reading Preference...................................... 33 Experiment 2: Reading aloud katakana strings preceded by kanji primes with Equal and high-ON/high-KUN readings................ 38 Chapter 3: Semantic context effects when naming Japanese kanji, but not Chinese hànzì ............................................................................... 53 Experiment: Semantic context effects in Japanese and Chinese word reading .............................................................................. 59 Chapter 4: Context effects when naming Japanese (but not Chinese), and degraded Dutch nouns: evidence for processing costs? .............. 73 Experiment 1: Naming Japanese kanji with homophonic distractor pictures ...................................................................... 81 Experiment 2: Naming Chinese hànzì with homophonic pictures .................................................................................................... 86 Experiment 3: Dutch bare noun, det+N, and degraded-word naming........................................................................................ 89 Chapter 5: The functional unit of Japanese word naming: evidence from masked priming. ...................................................................... 105 Experiment 1: Segment versus mora priming .......................... 115 Experiment 2: The effects of kana and romaji scripts on masked onset priming............................................................................ 118 Experiment 3: Effect of hiragana primes on romaji targets .... 121 Experiment 4a: Distinguishing the CV from the syllable: MOPE group. ....................................................................................... 123 Experiment 4b: Distinguishing the CV from the syllable: MORA group. ....................................................................................... 125 Chapter 6: Summary and Conclusion............................................... 149 Samenvatting in het Nederlands....................................................... 163 Curriculum Vitae.............................................................................. 169 - 7 - Chapter 1: General Introduction - 8 - General introduction. This thesis is concerned with reading aloud words written in a logographic script such as Japanese kanji and Chinese hànzì which are amongst the most complex writing systems in the world. Imagine therefore, for a second, that you have to read aloud a word presented on a screen. How long would that take you? The answer is: not very long, generally around half a second. The ability and speed with which we convert visually presented material (such as alphabetic words, logographic words, symbols and pictures) to intelligible speech output is a quite impressive cognitive achievement. However, the exact amount of time it takes to name aloud visually presented words or pictures depends on a large number of factors, such as the language or script used, specific properties of a word or picture and the setting (or experimental condition) in which a target word or picture has to be named. Word properties that affect naming times are e.g. language, frequency and length, but also regularity. Glushko (1979) found, for instance, that irregular words (i.e. words that do not have a consistent spelling-to-sound correspondence) are named slower compared to regular words; however, others (e.g. Jared, McRae & Seidenberg, 1990) found this to hold only for low-frequency items (such as the first syllable /die/ in DIESEL and DIET which is pronounced /di/ or /daI/). Picture naming latencies are also found to be dependent on properties such as frequency. Oldfield and Wingfield (1964) for instance, found a linear correlation between picture name frequency and naming latency (i.e. higher frequency leads to faster RTs). For many decades now scientists have designed chronometric experiments and investigated speech errors to better understand which components of information processing are involved and which routes are taken when producing words. Models of language production Models of language (and word) production usually discriminate between different processing levels which may or may not (depending on the model) include: conceptualization, assignment of syntactic features, phonological word-form encoding, and ultimately articulation (for overviews see e.g., Caramazza, 1997; Dell, 1986; Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999; Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon & Ziegler, 2001). In this thesis, I do refer to other models to account for my findings, but my primary focus lies on the most - 9 - detailed model of language production called “Word Encoding by Activation and VERification” or WEAVER++ (which is largely based on evidence from chronometric experiments) developed by Roelofs (1992) and Levelt et al. (1999). According to this model, speech production involves a specific number of levels (see Figure 1). Figure 1. The route a to-be-named picture follows in naming in the WEAVER++ model of language production (Roelofs, 1992; Levelt et al., 1999; sg = singular) First of all, at the conceptual level, contents of the utterance related to the communicative situation or intention become activated. For instance, if one is instructed to name a picture (e.g., of a “house”), the presented object is visually processed and activates the appropriate concept. It is commonly assumed that not only the target concept “house” but also semantically related concepts (such as “building” or “farm”) receive activation (Collins & Loftus, 1975; Levelt et al., 1999; Glaser & Düngelhoff, 1984; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995). Subsequently, the activated conceptual nodes will spread activation to the level of lexical representations containing the word’s lexical-syntactic (or lemma) representation. This involves accessing parameters concerning morpho-syntactic make-up of the words. For - 10 - instance, if one has been presented with multiple houses (“housepl”), then at this level the parameter for plural (+s) for this specific word will be set. Obviously, syntactic information for irregular words, such as “fish” having an irregular plural form (e.g. “fish”), is also stored and incorporated at this level. The time it takes to select the target lexical-syntactic representation depends on the number of co-activated representations (and their respective activation strengths) that are assumed to compete for selection (but see Mahon, Costa, Peterson, Vargas, & Caramazza, 2007 for an alternative proposal). Competition at the lexical-syntactic level results from cascading activation between the conceptual and lexical-syntactic levels. According to Levelt et al. (1999; see also Levelt et al., 1991), one lexical-syntactic representation is ultimately selected and only this representation
Recommended publications
  • Mora Or Syllable? Which Unit Do Japanese Use in Naming Visually Presented Stimuli?
    Applied Psycholinguistics 25 (2004), 1–27 Printed in the United States of America DOI: 10.1017.S0142716404001018 Mora or syllable? Which unit do Japanese use in naming visually presented stimuli? KATSUO TAMAOKA Hiroshima University YASUSHI TERAO University of Shizuoka ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Katsuo Tamaoka, PhD, International Student Center, Hiroshima University, 1-1, 1-Chome, Kagamiyama, Higashi-Hiroshima 739-8523, Japan. E-mail: [email protected] ABSTRACT Because the Japanese phonetic script (i.e., kana) represents moraic units, it is often claimed that Japanese people assemble phonology at the moraic unit. Two experiments were conducted to investigate the unit for naming visually presented stimuli, focusing on the special nasals /N/, geminates /Q/, long vowels /R/, and dual vowels /J/. The special sounds create two morae when there is only one syllable. Experiments 1 and 2 compared the production of three-mora, three-syllable and three-mora, two-syllable real, and nonsense words. The findings indicated that native Japanese speakers named the three-mora words containing the special sounds more quickly. Accordingly, it is posited that special sounds are named as syllable units rather than moraic units. Because symbols of the Japanese kana phonetic script (see details of kana in Inagaki, Hatano, & Otake, 2000) basically correspond to morae,1 because the well- known Japanese poetic form called haiku is composed on the basis of 17 morae (i.e., three phrases of 5–7–5 morae), and even because Japanese children play shiritori (a word game requiring the segmentation of an ending mora), it is often believed that mora play a major role in the phonological processing of the Japanese language.
    [Show full text]
  • Japanese Flash Card Game
    Japanese flash card game Play the “Concentration game” game with online hiragana/katakana cards. movies, drag-and-drop-style exercises, worksheets and flashcards in PDF format.​Verb/Adjective Conjugation · ​Kanji Reading Practice · ​Lesson 1 Vocabulary. A list of resources to help students study Japanese. Learn Japanese Online for free - it's fun using easy flash quizes. If you masterd it, take a shot at the Kanji glyphs, using our flashcards. memory game.​Fujisan Hiragana Quiz · ​Kana Invaders! · ​Writing · ​Memory Game. Start studying Hiragana Review game. Learn vocabulary, terms, and more with flashcards, games, and other study tools. Try these fun Japanese practice games to become proficient in the key skills of reading, writing, speaking and listening in Japanese. Hills Learning offers flashcards online for learning Japanese based on our tested and proven curriculum, learning Hiragana, Katakana, Kanji, and through. Language - Japanese Flashcards. 漢字 's - 99 cards; 怪盗ジョーカーの第章単語 - 76 cards; 怪盗ジョーカーの第章単語 - 71 cards; 怪盗ジョーカーの. Using the right Japanese flashcards app puts you at a huge advantage for fun through the use of colorful, image-based flashcards, games and quizzes. How to study effectively? Want to master Japanese vocabulary? Japanese flashcards are images/text/sound on cards, used to help remember new vocabulary in. Free Web-Based Japanese Language Study Quizzes, Flashcards and More. Here are two other "listening and vocabulary" games using the same data. Real Kana. Learn Hiragana and Katakana. It's easy to use. Click Hiragana or Katakana and choose which characters you'd like to study. Then, click study, and. When presented with a flashcard, think of the answer and then click to have It's not a game-app – it's not supposed to be particularly fun or to.
    [Show full text]
  • Table of Hiragana Letters Pdf
    Table of hiragana letters pdf Continue Hiragana is one of three sets of characters used in Japanese. Each letter of Hiragana is a special syllable. The letter itself doesn't make sense. Hiragan is widely used to form a sentence. You can download/print the Hiragan chart (PDF) of all Hiragana's letters. The origin of Hiragan あ か た ま や the original 安 加 太 末 也 of Hiragan was developed in the 8th-10th century by simplifying the shape of specific kanji symbols. Compared to Katakana, Hiragana's letters have more curved lines. Number of letters In modern Japanese 46 basic letters of Hiragana. In addition to these 46 main letters, called gojon, there are modified forms to describe more time - 20 dakun, 5 handakuon, 36 y'on, 1 sokuon and 6 additional letters. Frequently asked questions: What are the letters with the bar on top ( Yap.) ? Gojaon 【五⼗⾳】 Goyon-【五⼗⾳図】 In Japanese, syllables are organized as a table (5 x 10). This table is called goj'on-zu (literally means a table of 50 sounds). The alphabets of Hiragan and Katakana are used to describe these sounds. Letters い, う and え appear more than once in the table. These 5 duplicates (grey) are usually missed or ignored. It includes ん syllable. It does not belong to any row or column. In total, 46 letters (45'1) are considered goj'on (50 sounds). You can learn the goj'on letters on the Hiragan course: Part 1-10. The structure of table First row - あ, い, う, え and お - five vowels of Japanese.
    [Show full text]
  • Identity and Ideology in Sakura Katakana Shimbun
    IDENTITY AND IDEOLOGY IN SAKURA KATAKANA SHIMBUN KUROHI REI Department of Japanese Studies Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences National University of Singapore 2018 i Declaration of Academic Integrity This thesis represents my own work and I have duly acknowledged in the footnotes and bibliography the sources and information which I have consulted for the purpose of this study. This thesis has not transgressed the maximum word limit of 12,000 words. The total word count for this thesis is 12,000 words. _____________________ Kurohi Rei April 17, 2018 Abstract This thesis focuses on Sakura Katakana Shimbun, a children’s newspaper published in Singapore during the Japanese Occupation, as an example of Japanese imperial propaganda targeted specifically at children. In Japan, child-oriented propaganda was widespread in the form of kamishibai paper plays, but in occupied Southeast Asia, Sakura is perhaps the only such specimen. In examining the discontinuities between the first and second halves of Sakura’s publication run, this thesis aims to shed light on the roles and ideologies of Japanese propagandists in wartime Singapore. In addition, it considers how local children’s identities were imagined, constructed, and manipulated by the authors of Sakura to various ends. When the 25th Army captured Singapore, they sought to Japanise it not only by making formal changes to its systems, institutions, and name but also by transforming the minds of its people. For military leaders and conscripted literati, this meant implementing a strong Japanese language policy. Sakura was one such tool used to educate the local children in both Japanese language and cultural knowledge.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Preference and Cognitive Availability of Sexual Identity in Gay Japanese Bilinguals: an Analysis of Habitual Use of Linguistic Categories"
    City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Dissertations and Theses City College of New York 2016 "LANGUAGE PREFERENCE AND COGNITIVE AVAILABILITY OF SEXUAL IDENTITY IN GAY JAPANESE BILINGUALS: AN ANALYSIS OF HABITUAL USE OF LINGUISTIC CATEGORIES" Sebastian Cordoba CUNY City College How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/cc_etds_theses/588 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] i LANGUAGE PREFERENCE AND COGNITIVE AVAILABILITY OF SEXUAL IDENTITY IN GAY JAPANESE BILINGUALS: AN ANALYSIS OF HABITUAL USE OF LINGUISTIC CATEGORIES by SEBASTIAN CORDOBA Principal Advisor: Dr. Vivien Tartter Additional Readers: Dr. Susan Fischer & Dr. Robert Melara The City College of New York, CUNY 2016 ii Language Preference and Cognitive Availability of Sexual Identity in Gay Japanese Bilinguals: An Analysis of Habitual Use of Linguistic Categories Principal Investigator: Sebastian Cordoba Advisor: Dr. Vivien Tartter Committee members: Dr. Susan Fischer Dr. Robert Melara iii Acknowledgment I would like to express my deepest gratitude to my advisor Dr. Vivien Tartter for her unwavering support and mentorship throughout the process of researching and writing my thesis. I am thankful for her stimulating guidance and valuable feedback. I would also like to thank my committee members, Dr. Fischer and Dr. Melara, for their very valuable comments on this thesis. I am gratefully indebted to them. Special thanks to my partner Cooper Gatewood for his amazing proofreading skills and continuous encouragement. This project would not have been possible without them.
    [Show full text]
  • IPU New Zealand Newsletter
    IPU NEW ZEALAND Japanese Taster Weekends! IPU New Zealand Every year we open our doors to Year 11-13 students who want Newsletter to experience life as a university student, whilst enjoying some Japanese language and cultural activities. In 2015, we hosted over April 2015 80 students from high schools all across the country, who enjoyed activities such as calligraphy, UPCOMING EVENTS taiko drumming, tea ceremony Apr 13-15 Orientation 2016 - IPU New and lesson with our three native Zealand will be welcoming new Japanese lecturers. students from all over the world . Apr 18 IPU Term One starts. This year we will be hosting four weekends on the following dates: Apr 25 ANZAC Day. May 7-8 Golden Week. This is one of June 18-19 Apr 29 - May 5 the most important holidays in July 2-3 the Japanese calendar, a seven August 27-28 day holiday made up of four national holidays and well placed Email us at [email protected] for more information. We hope weekends. The national holidays to see you there! are; 昭 和 の 日( し ょ う わ の ひ ) Showa Day, the celebration of the former こんにちは from IPU NZ! Japanese Intensive Weekends registration forms must be received two weeks prior to Emperor’s birthday, 健保記念日(け the start of the event. Participants may arrive on a Friday night, and return home on ん ぽ き ね ん び ) Constitution Day, 緑 Hello again! Another month, another newsletter. How are the Sunday night or Monday morning. Registration fees cover all accommodation and activity costs.
    [Show full text]
  • Pitch Contour of Japanese Traditional Verse
    University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 2011 Pitch Contour of Japanese Traditional Verse Shiho Yamamoto The University of Montana Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Yamamoto, Shiho, "Pitch Contour of Japanese Traditional Verse" (2011). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 752. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/752 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. PITCH CONTOUR OF JAPANESE TRADITIONAL VERSE by SHIHO YAMAMOTO BA in English, Kansai Gaidai University, Osaka, Japan, 2004 Thesis presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Linguistics, Applied Linguistics Option The University of Montana Missoula, MT May 2011 Approved by: Perry Brown, Associate Provost for Graduate Education Graduate School Dr. Mizuki Miyashita, Chair Linguistics Dr. Timothy Vance National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics Dr. Irene Appelbaum Linguistics Jeanie Castillo Linguistics Yamamoto, Shiho, MA in Linguistics, Spring 2011 Linguistics Pitch Contour of Japanese Traditional Verse Chairperson: Dr. Mizuki Miyashita In this thesis I conduct experiments to investigate how pitch pattern is realized in Japanese Traditional Verses. My observation consists of four parts: (i) investigation of lexical pitch and accentual combinations of Contemporary haiku in prose and in verse (ii) investigation of lexical pitch and accentual combinations of Basho’s traditional haiku (iii) observation of Nonsense haiku and (iv) pitch range measurements of Contemporary haiku in prose and in verse.
    [Show full text]
  • A Study of Motivation Among Learners of Japanese In
    PASSION & PERSISTENCE: A STUDY OF MOTIVATION AMONG LEARNERS OF JAPANESE IN AUSTRALIA Barbara M. Northwood A thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Japanese Studies School of International Studies Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences University of New South Wales, Sydney September 2013 Originality Statement ‘I hereby declare that this submission is my own work and to the best of my knowledge it contains no materials previously published or written by another person, or substantial proportions of material which have been accepted for the award of any other degree or diploma at UNSW or any other educational institution, except where due acknowledgement is made in the thesis. Any contribution made to the research by others, with whom I have worked at UNSW or elsewhere, is explicitly acknowledged in the thesis. I also declare that the intellectual content of this thesis is the product of my own word, except to the extent that assistance from others in the project's design and conception or in style, presentation and linguistic expression is acknowledged.’ …………………………………………………. 31 March 2013 ii List of publications Northwood, B. and Thomson, C.K. 2010. “Why stop studying Japanese? A case in Australia.” In A.M. Stoke (Ed.), JALT2009 Conference Procedings. Tokyo: JALT. (Written about initial findings in the high school study, Chapter 4). Northwood, B. and Thomson, C.K. 2012. “What Keeps Them Going? Investigating Ongoing Learners of Japanese in Australian Universities.” Japanese Studies, 32(3): 335-355. London, U.K.: Routledge. (Written about findings from Round I of the University study, including Interviews 1 and 2).
    [Show full text]
  • Sample Teaching and Learning Outline
    SAMPLE TEACHING AND LEARNING OUTLINE JAPANESE: SECOND LANGUAGE YEAR 5 Copyright © School Curriculum and Standards Authority, 2018 This document—apart from any third party copyright material contained in it—may be freely copied, or communicated on an intranet, for non- commercial purposes in educational institutions, provided that the School Curriculum and Standards Authority is acknowledged as the copyright owner. Copying or communication for any other purpose can be done only within the terms of the Copyright Act 1968 or with prior written permission of the School Curriculum and Standards Authority. Copying or communication of any third party copyright material can be done only within the terms of the Copyright Act 1968 or with permission of the copyright owners. Any content in this document that has been derived from the Australian Curriculum may be used under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International licence. Disclaimer Any resources such as texts, websites and so on that may be referred to in this document are provided as examples of resources that teachers can use to support their teaching and learning programs. Their inclusion does not imply that they are mandatory or that they are the only resources relevant to the learning area syllabus. 2018/48158v4 Japanese: Second Language | Year5 | Sample Teaching and Learning Outline The sample teaching and learning outline provides one possible approach through which the Japanese: Second Language syllabus within the Western Australian Curriculum: Languages can be taught. Consistent with the rationale of the Languages curriculum, this outline supports students’ ability to communicate proficiently in a language other than English, with essential communication skills in that language, an intercultural capability, and an understanding of the role of language and culture in human communication.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Representations of the Japanese Language: Levels of Processing and Orthographic Influences
    Doctoral Dissertation Academic Year 2015 Phonological Representations of the Japanese Language: Levels of Processing and Orthographic Influences Graduate School of Media and Governance Keio University Miyoko Nakamura CONTENTS Acknowledgments …………………………………………..... iv Abstract (English) ..…………………………………………. vii Abstract (Japanese) ..………………………………………… ix List of Figures ………………………………………….xi List of Tables ……………………………………….. xiii Chapter 1: Introduction …………………………………. 1 1.1 Phonological Representations ……………………………………………... 3 1.2 Orthographic Influences …………………………………………….. 5 1.2.1 From Sound to Letters: from phonology to orthography …………………….. 5 1.2.2 Orthographic Influences on Speech Processing ……………………………. 6 1.3 The Objectives of the Present Study ……………………………………………8 1.4 Approach ………………………………………………………………………. 8 1.4.1 General Approach ………………………………………………………... 8 1.4.2 Specific Approach: Levels of Speech Processing …………………………. 9 1.5 Dissertation Outline ………………………………………………………. 14 Chapter 2: Related Studies ………………………………..16 2.1 Japanese Phonology and Writing System ………………………………... 16 2.1.1 Japanese Phonology on Syllable and Mora ……………………………… 16 i 2.1.2 Japanese Phonology and the Writing System …………………………… 18 2.2 Research Background of Linguistic Awareness ....………………………... 22 2.2.1 Metalinguistic Ability and/or Linguistic Awareness ……………………. 22 2.2.2 Language as an Object: Metalinguistic Thinking …………………….. 24 2.2.3 Phonological Awareness and Phonemic Awareness …………………….. 26 2.2.4 Previous Studies on Phonological Awareness in Alphabetic Language Speakers
    [Show full text]
  • Phonemic Awareness in Alphabetically Literate
    416 A. Dowkeret al. Cahiers de Psychologie Cogrffive/ Current Psychology of Cognition 1998,17 (2), 417-450 (c) Poems containing similes (1) English, about the picture of the bird: The bird is flying in the sky Like a tiny aeroplane. Phonemic awareness in alphabetically literate (2) Italian, about the picture of the dog: Japanese adults: The influence of the first "Ho visto un cane chc abbaiava forte come un tuono E aveva gli occhi neri come il carbone". acquired writing system (I saw a dog who barked as loudly as thunder And had eyes as black as coal.) Miyoko Nakamura,1 Régine Kolinsky,2-3 (3) French, about the picture of the rabbit: Carmela Spagrioletti,4 and José Morais2 "Le lapin est rapide comme l'éclair Et léger comme une plume." (The rabbit is as fast as lightning 1. Keio University, Japan And as light as a feather.) 2. Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium 3. Belgian F.N.R.S. (4) Polish, about the picture of the horse: 4. Institut des Langues yi^ptntes et de Phonétique, U.L.B., Belgium "Konjestpracowity Jak ta mala mrowka." (The horse is hardworking Abstract. The present study examined the nature of the metapho­ Like a little ant.) nological units that are used by Japanese native speakers who know both mora-based and phoneme-based writing systems. In three experiments, (5) Brazilian, about the picture of the cat: "Eu vi um gato preto como o carvao participants were asked to perform a reversal task. The results show Com os olhos azuls como o eel." that, in comparison to phonemes and syllables, moras are the most pro­ (I saw a cat as black as coal minent units in spontaneous reversal.
    [Show full text]
  • Proquest Dissertations
    Developing critical consciousness: Shutaisei in teaching and learning a foreign language Item Type text; Dissertation-Reproduction (electronic) Authors Iventosch, Mieko Shimizu, 1956- Publisher The University of Arizona. Rights Copyright © is held by the author. Digital access to this material is made possible by the University Libraries, University of Arizona. Further transmission, reproduction or presentation (such as public display or performance) of protected items is prohibited except with permission of the author. Download date 27/09/2021 10:31:33 Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/284322 INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly fi'om the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be fi'om any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing fi'om left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book.
    [Show full text]