Learning As a Geographical Process

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Learning As a Geographical Process FOCUS: LEARNING AS A GEOGRAPHICAL PROCESS Introduction: Learning as a Geographical Process Dragos Simandan Brock University In this introduction to the Focus Section “Learning as a Geographical Process,” I provide a context for the four articles that follow, by means of (1) making explicit the threefold rationale for this initiative; (2) relating this initiative with previous geographical scholarship on the problematic of learning; (3) highlighting the significance of the self-referential character of our work; (4) providing a brief outline of the articles that follow; and (5) pointing out the important fact that both “learning” and “geographical process” constitute semantically rich categories and that relating the two involves a many-to-many type of logical mapping. Key Words: geographical process, learning, many-to-many mapping, self-reference. En esta introduccion´ al tema de “El aprendizaje como un proceso geografico”´ de la Seccion´ Focus contribuyo un contexto para los cuatro artıculos´ que siguen, mediante las siguientes acciones: (1) haciendo explıcita´ la triple racionalidad de esta iniciativa; (2) relacionando la iniciativa con la erudicion´ geografica´ precedente relacionada con la problematica´ del aprendizaje; (3) destacando la significacion´ del caracter´ auto-referencial de nuestro trabajo; (4) suministrando un breve esquema de los artıculos´ de la Seccion;´ y (5) senalando˜ el hecho notable de que tanto “aprendizaje” como “proceso geografico”´ constituyen unas categorıas´ sistematicamente´ ricas y que para relacionarlas entre sı´ se involucra una logica´ cartografica´ del tipo “muchos-a-muchos” [many-to-many]. Palabras clave: proceso geografico,´ aprendizaje, mapeo de muchos-a-muchos, auto-referencia. he rationale for this Focus Section is strumentalist and pluralistic flavor and espouse T threefold. First, by embracing an instru- the implicit belief that there is no one cor- mentalist epistemological viewpoint, we aim to rect way to think about learning. Instead, in explore the potential usefulness of learning as the spirit of pluralistic epistemology (Preston a geographical process as a way of thinking. 2005), we operate from the belief that the best Can we learn something new and interesting way to understand a given topic is to tackle about learning if we think of it as a geograph- it from a diversity of perspectives, each con- ical process? What problems is the learning as ceived as a constellation of epistemic gains and a geographical process perspective particularly losses. By not asking the question that a real- suited to address? Enlightened by looking at ist epistemologist would ask—is learning as a learning in geographical terms, what new phe- geographical process the correct way to think nomena might we get the chance to observe? about learning?—we cast our work as a use- Note that these questions share a distinctly in- ful application of geographical reasoning to the The Professional Geographer, 65(3) 2013, pages 363–368 Copyright C 2013 Crown copyright. Initial submission, June 2011; revised submissions, October and December 2011; final acceptance, December 2011. Published by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC. 364 Volume 65, Number 3, August 2013 problematic of learning and not as an imperi- organized this Focus Section. Indeed, it seems alistic disciplinary attempt to turn the study of to me that much of the previous geographi- learning into exclusive geographical territory. cal work on learning shares two less desirable Second, by choosing to dwell on this par- properties: it engages the theme of learning ticular topic, we hope to attack from a new in implicit ways, more as a background for angle the problem of the excessive internal some other interest (for a recent exception see divisiveness and lack of communication among McFarlane 2011), and it usually discusses the the diverse subdisciplines that constitute con- learning of humans and their institutions with- temporary human geography. The work culmi- out pointing out that the very process of nating in this Focus Section started in Septem- learning can be thought of as quintessentially ber 2010, when I began to put together a panel geographical. for the annual meeting of the Association of An illustration of the first property is the American Geographers (AAG) in Seattle. From recent debate in human geography over the the very beginning, I aimed to select a num- centrality of representations (Anderson and ber of distinguished geographers from a variety Harrison 2010). Whereas the conventional of subdisciplinary backgrounds: cultural geog- view emphasizes that we learn by acquiring, raphy, economic geography, geographic infor- creating, and updating mental representations mation systems (GIS), urban geography, po- of the world out there, nonrepresentational litical geography, and so on. It seems to me theorists (Thrift 2008) have challenged this en- that learning is a topic that can play the role trenched perspective, suggesting instead that of connective tissue for the discipline of hu- the world is its own representation, that we get man geography, precisely because it is a central by through being immersed in the world and attribute of what makes us human. By high- dwelling in the middle of things. Put differ- lighting the spatiality of a process that all hu- ently, if a representationalist would argue, like mans engage in—learning—we foreground a Simon (1996), that memory is the environment unifying concern for all human geographers, of thought, a nonrepresentationalist would flip regardless of the particular subdiscipline with things around to claim that the environment which they identify themselves. is the memory of our thought. Even though These last observations bring me to the third I worded this debate to show that it really is rationale for this Focus Section, namely, the de- about how we learn, the fact of the matter is sire to make the problematic of learning truly that the organizing label for it has been repre- central to the concerns of human geographers. sentation(s) or lack thereof and that this explicit The history behind this rationale consists, in focus has obfuscated the underlying theme of my detection, of a discrepancy between the fun- learning. damental role ascribed to learning in the hu- Two illustrations of the second property are man species by philosophers, psychologists, and (1) the earlier scholarship undertaken within comparative biologists and the relatively pe- behavioral geography (including cognitive ge- ripheral place it has occupied on the agenda of ography; for historical overviews of these fields human geographers. One of the chief concerns see Johnston and Sidaway [2004, 139–63]; Gold of the former group of scholars has been with [2009]; and Montello [2009]), and (2) the trying to single out those attributes that most surge of interest about the learning of eco- distinguish our species from the rest (Adler and nomic agents in economic geography (Hudson Rips 2008). Although the debate is far from be- 1999; Bathelt, Malmberg, and Maskell 2004). ing concluded, one of the key ideas springing Behavioral geography is a label used to de- from it is that humans have an ability to learn note, first, a broader intellectual movement that is unprecedented in any other species and highly influential in geography between 1965 that the success of the human race in achieving and 1980 and concerned with the role of ecological dominance can be traced back to that cognitive processes in spatial decision mak- ability. If learning carries such a significant ex- ing and, second, a distinct subdiscipline of planatory power for the human saga, it stands to human geography, incorporating cognitive ge- reason that human geographers should system- ography as one of its foci. Although behav- atically consider it in their research and schol- ioral geography has generated useful research arship. Had this been the case, I would not have on topics such as the learning of geographical Introduction: Learning as a Geographical Process 365 knowledge (Golledge and Stimpson 1997), the The Problem of Self-Reference role of cognitive processes in spatial decision making (Wolpert 1964; Kitchin and Blades That it is hard work to explicitly think of learn- 2002), cognitive maps (Gould and White ing as a geographical process became apparent 1974; Portugali 1996), human perceptions of to me in an ironic kind of way at the AAG natural hazards (Burton, Kates, and White meeting in Seattle. Even though I had been re- 1978), and the relation among geography, flecting on the topic for months, it was only the experience, and imagination (Lowenthal 1961), immediate pressure of finding something intel- it has come under the attack of both humanis- ligent to say in the introduction to the panel tic and radical geographical approaches, which that made me realize in the evening before the has resulted in its “increasing peripherality and panel debate that our discussion bears the in- remnant status” (Gold 2009, 283) and its being teresting characteristic of being self-referential. perceived as a “limited extension of spatial sci- Self-referentiality has long been identified as ence” (Gold 2009, 282). From the standpoint of the most perplexing attribute with which lo- this Focus Section, my main criticism of behav- gicians have had to wrestle and refers to the ioral geography is that it left the processes of curious worlds of thought that open up when learning themselves undertheorized and failed a set of statements refers to itself (Hofstadter to think of them as geographical through and 1980; Adler and Rips 2008). Metaphorically, through. It is one thing to study the learning the image of a dog vainly chasing its own tail is of geographical knowledge (e.g., mental maps, apt to elicit the same kind of intellectual dizzi- navigation, way-finding); it is quite another to ness that accompanies any and every attempt analyze how the process of learning itself is ge- at self-referential reasoning. It occurred to me ographical. Not surprisingly, even some of the that there are two loops of self-referentiality key proponents of the field were led to admit in our discussion of learning as a geographical in retrospect “the conceptual weakness that be- process.
Recommended publications
  • Navigating Without Vision: Principles of Blind Spatial Cognition
    Navigating without Vision: Principles of Blind Spatial Cognition Chapter 15, Handbook of Behavioral & Cognitive Geography (edited by D.R. Montello) Nicholas A. Giudice Spatial Informatics Program: School of Computing and Information Science Virtual Environments and Multimodal Interaction (VEMI) Laboratory (umaine.edu/vemi) The University of Maine Abstract This chapter considers what it means to learn and navigate the world with limited or no vision. It investigates limitations of blindness research, discusses traditional theories of blind spatial abilities, and provides an alternative perspective of many of the oft-cited issues and challenges underlying spatial cognition of blind people. Several provocative assertions pertaining to visual impairment and spatial abilities are advanced that help to better understand navigation without vision, provide greater explanatory power relevant to many of the current debates, and offer some needed guidance on the development of new spatial learning strategies and technological solutions that will ultimately have a significant positive impact on the independence and quality of life of this demographic. An underlying and related theme of the chapter emphasizes the importance of ‘space’ in spatial cognition research, rather than vision as its principle mechanism. There is no debate that vision is an amazing conduit of spatial information, but it is also important to remember that it does not have a monopoly on space. Indeed, all of our senses encode spatial information to one degree or another, and as we will discuss, this commonality allows for equivalent performance on many of the same spatial behaviors, independent of whether they originate from visual or nonvisual perception. 1. Introduction For most sighted people, navigating from place to place is a fundamental component of daily life, performed without much conscious thought or active attention (Merleau-Ponty 1962).
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Neuroscience of Spatial and Geographic Thinking
    Bond University Research Repository Cognitive neuroscience of spatial and geographic thinking Schinazi, Victor R.; Thrash, Tyler Published in: Handbook of Behavioral and Cognitive Geography DOI: 10.4337/9781784717544.00016 Licence: Other Link to output in Bond University research repository. Recommended citation(APA): Schinazi, V. R., & Thrash, T. (2018). Cognitive neuroscience of spatial and geographic thinking. In D. R. Montello (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral and Cognitive Geography (pp. 154-174). Edward Elgar Publishing. https://doi.org/10.4337/9781784717544.00016 General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. For more information, or if you believe that this document breaches copyright, please contact the Bond University research repository coordinator. Download date: 28 Sep 2021 Cognitive neuroscience of spatial and geographic thinking Victor R. Schinazi and Tyler Thrash Chair of Cognitive Science, ETH Zurich, Switzerland Abstract Cognitive neuroscience has provided additional techniques for investigations of spatial and geographic thinking. However, the incorporation of neuroscientific methods still lacks the theoretical motivation necessary for the progression of geography as a discipline. Rather than reflecting a shortcoming of neuroscience, this weakness has developed from previous attempts to establish a positivist approach to behavioral geography. In this chapter, we will discuss the connection between the challenges of positivism in behavioral geography and the current drive to incorporate neuroscientific evidence. We will also provide an overview of research in geography and neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • The Linguistic Encoding of Landscape in Lokono
    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) The linguistic encoding of landscape in Lokono Rybka, K.A. Publication date 2016 Document Version Final published version Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Rybka, K. A. (2016). The linguistic encoding of landscape in Lokono. LOT. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:02 Oct 2021 337 English summary: The linguistic encoding of landscape in Lokono This thesis investigates the how landscape elements are expressed linguistically in Lokono (Arawakan). As a background to the analysis, an account of language vitality and a description of the grammar of space are given. The linguistic analysis of landscape starts with landform terms, which form a system of non-lexicalized phrases sharing a generic term horhorho ‘landform’.
    [Show full text]
  • Varenius: NCGIA's Project to Advance Geographic Information Science
    UC Santa Barbara Varenius Initiatives (1995-1999) Title Varenius—NCGIA's Project to Advance Geographic Information Science: 1997 Annual Report Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/53q629nv Authors Goodchild, Michael F. Kemp, Karen K. Publication Date 1997-12-01 eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California VARENIUS: NCGIA’S PROJECT TO ADVANCE GEOGRAPHIC INFORMATION SCIENCE ANNUAL REPORT AND INCREMENT REQUEST, DECEMBER 1997 Summary The National Center for Geographic Information and Analysis is funded through its University of California, Santa Barbara, site to carry out a new program of research and agenda-setting in the area of geographic information science. Funding is provided by the National Science Foundation under Cooperative Agreement SBR 96-00465. This report summarizes progress from the inception of the project, February 15, 1997, through the end of November 1997, a period of just over nine months, and is submitted in accordance with the requirements of the cooperative agreement. It requests the second increment of funding for the project, for the period 2/1/98 through 1/31/99. The first section summarizes the progress made in setting up the project, recruiting members for the Strategic Area Panels, setting topics and dates for the Specialist Meetings, and initiating the Advisory Board. The second section reports on activities of the National Center for the period 1/1/97 through 11/30/97, a period of transition between activities under the cooperative agreement which had funded NCGIA since its inception in 1988 (SBR 88-10917) and the Varenius project. The appendices to this section include reports of publications, visitors, courses taught, and degrees granted.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to the Special Issue
    INTRODUCTION TO THE SPECIAL ISSUE MAPPING EMOTIONAL CARTOGRAPHY Sébastien Caquard Amy L. Griffin Concordia University RMIT University INTRODUCTION Maps are somehow shy. They tend to hide their emotional side behind their clear lines, precise points, minimalistic words, numerical data and informative purpose. But when we scratch the cartographic surface, maps appear to be impregnated with all sorts of emotions. The emotions associated with the topic mapped and the ones evoked through the carto- graphic design. The emotions felt by the mapmaker while drawing the map and the ones felt by the map user when discovering it. The anger and sadness triggered by social injustices revealed on a map, or the simple pleasure felt while admiring a beautiful cartographic design. The emotional experiences we clearly remember and the most common ones we hardly notice or we simply forget. Beneath the surface, maps and mapping teem with emotions of all sorts. In this introduction to the special issue on Maps and Emotions, we will reveal the multiple relationships that exist between maps, mapping, and emotions. Before looking at these relationships, let’s first clarify what we mean by “emotions” in the context of mapmaking. The term emotion is often used interchangeably with the term affect, since both are generally understood by scholars as embodied experiences. Beyond this commonality there are distinct differences between affect and emotion that vary depending on the disciplinary lens through which each is viewed. While both arise from our interaction with the environment, affects are generally conceived as arising from the body, while emotions are usually linked to the conscious mind and expressed through emotion concepts (e.g., fear, joy, embarrassment).
    [Show full text]
  • Human Geography
    INTERNATIONAL ENCYCLOPEDIA OF HUMAN GEOGRAPHY EDITORS-IN-CHIEF ROß KITCHIN National Institute of Regional and Spatial Analysis (NIRSA) John Hume Building, National University of Ireland Maynooth, County Kildare Ireland NIGEL THRIFT University House, University of Warwiek Coventry UK ELSEVIER AMSTERDAM • BOSTON. HEIDELBERG • LONDON • NEW YORK • OXFORD PARIS. SAN DrEGG. SAN FRANCISCO. SINGAPORE • SYDNEY • ToKYO CONTENTS Senior Editors v Section Editors vii Guide to use of the Encyclopedia ix Contributors xi Contents xxxvii Subject Classification lxiii Foreword by Mary Robinson lxxv Foreword by Rob Kitchin lxxvii VOLUME 1 A Activism L M Takahashi Activist Geographies P Routledge 7 Actor-Network Theory/Network Geographies G T J6hannesson and J 0 Beerennokit 15 Affect J 0 Dewsbury 20 Africa G A Myers 25 Ageing and Health G J Andrews 31 Ageing and Mobility A M Warnes 36 Ageism and Age L Maxey 42 Agglomeration A Malmberg 48 Agoraphobia J Davidson 54 Agrarian Transformations J Renes 58 Agricultural Land Preservation K B Beesley and 0 Ramsey 65 Agriculture, Sustainable T Marsden 70 Agri-Environmentalism and Rural Change C Potter 79 Aid V Desai 84 Arnenaqernent du Territoire: Territorial Development P Merlin 91 Americas R B Kent and K AButIer 99 Anarchism/Anarchist Geography MM Breitbart 108 Anglo-American/ Anglophone Hegemony M B Aalbers and U Rossi 116 Animal Geographies C Wilbert 122 Animal Welfare, Agricultural H Buller 127 xxxvii xxxviii Contents Antarctica S Chaturvedi 133 Anthropogeography (Atter Ratzei) J Lossau 140 Anthropology and Human
    [Show full text]
  • MONTELLO 9781784717537 PRINT (4Col).Indd 3 13/03/2018 15:21 4 Handbook of Behavioral and Cognitive Geography
    1. Behavioral and cognitive geography: introduction and overview Daniel R. Montello 1.1. INTRODUCTION: WHAT IS BEHAVIORAL AND COGNITIVE GEOGRAPHY? A tenet of everyday lay reasoning is that people do what they do because of what they believe—about themselves, about other people, about opportunities and threats in the sur- rounding world. About precedents in the past and consequences in the future. Researchers have dubbed such lay explanations for what people do as “commonsense,” “intuitive,” or “naive” reasoning (Egenhofer and Mark 1995; Hobbs and Moore 1985; Malle 2004; Ross 1977). The claim that people act in response to their beliefs has also attracted its share of serious scientific investigation, especially since the mid-20th century. Geographers and other scholars of human activity in natural and built environments joined these investigations during this time (see Chapter 2). Within the academic discipline of geography, such an expla- nation for human behavior came to be dubbed the “behavioral” approach. More recently, it is often referred to as “cognitive” geography; we explain below that these terms are not strictly synonymous. Briefly, behavioral and cognitive geography is an approach, or collection of approaches, to the study of human mental and physical activity in and concerning space, place, and environment. This Handbook presents a comprehensive and up-to-date overview of concepts, theories, and empirical research in behavioral and cognitive geography. Perhaps many people are unaware that there is any effort within academic geography to understand human mind and behavior. Of course, many people probably don’t under- stand academic geography very accurately; perhaps they don’t understand behavioral science very well either (most behavioral scientists don’t study rats or the neuroses you have from an overbearing mother).
    [Show full text]
  • International Encyclopedia of Human Geography
    International Encyclopedia of Human Geography CONTENTS: Cartography Cartography in Islamic Societies Map Libraries and Archives Art and Cartography Atlases Biodiversity Mapping Cartographic Animation Census Mapping Children and Mapping Colour, Mapping Mapping, Commercial Counter-Mapping Critical Cartography Critical GIS Digital Earth Mapping, Distributed Electoral Cartography Performative and Embodied Mapping Feminism, Maps and GIS Generalization Geodemographics Geodesy GIScience and Systems Geomatics Geo-spatila Intelligence Geovisualization GIS and Cartography Global Positioning/GPS Maps and Governance Information Graphics Cartography, History of Integrated Spatial Data Infrastructure Internet/Web Mapping Map Hacking Map Interactivity Map Perception and Cognition Map Types Mapping Agencies Mapping, Cyberspace Mapping, Race and Ethnicity Maps Maps and the State GIS, Mobile and Locational Based Services Indigenous Mapping Mapping, Non-Western Mapping, Philosophy Photogrammetry/Aerial Photography Projections Maps and Protest GIS, Public Participation Representation - Mapping Science and Scientism, Cartography Spatial Databases Spatial Ontologies Qualitative Spatial Reasoning Surveying Symbolism, Iconography Mapping, Topographic Development Aid Brain Drain Brandt Commission Brown Agenda Child Labour Civil Society Cultural Imperialism Debt Deforestation Dependency Desertification Development I Development II Digital Divide East Asian Miracle Empowerment Eurocentrism Extended Metropolitan Region Fair Trade Famine First World Governance, Good
    [Show full text]
  • 11, January-June 2021
    Volume: 11, January-June 2021 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENT IN SOCIAL SCIENCE AND HUMANITIES e-ISSN:2455-5142; p-ISSN: 2455-7730 Cognitive Developments, Geographical Models and Theories Dr Mohammadhossein Ramesht, Azhar Abbas Sabr, Kamal Mohammed Ayyash Isfahan University, Department of Geography, Geomorphology Speciality Paper Received: 17th May, 2021; Paper Accepted: 12th June, 2021; Paper Published: 13th June, 2021 How to cite the article: Dr Mohammadhossein Ramesht, Azhar Abbas Sabr, Kamal Mohammed Ayyash, Cognitive Developments, Geographical Models and Theories, IJDSSH, January-June 2021, Vol 11, 22-44 22 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENT IN SOCIAL SCIENCE AND HUMANITIES Volume: 11, January-June 2021 ABSTRACT Theories and models in geography are an expression of a new conception or understanding of a phenomenon or process that geographers attempt to gain the attention of others by suggesting conceptual metaphors[2]. The theory can be understood simply as understanding something else that the researcher is trying to express in words that can have the closest meaning to his discoveries such as "the butterfly effect expressing an objective truth in the communication system". The theory of atmospheric phenomena must be considered an abstract concept of tangible things and in the realm of perception of the human mind and a question of nature the theories and models are always supported by a knowledge base so that the theory cannot exist and has been found to have no epistemological roots so when classifying geographical theories it appears from It is necessary to design cognitive systems first because the foundation of any theory is supported by the conceptual system of its cognitive system.
    [Show full text]
  • 2612C22d2fd1b5209b65ad7169
    KNOWLEDGE IN THE AGE IN THE KNOWLEDGE CDSMS MARIANO ZUKERFELD KNOWLEDGE OF OF IN THE AGE DIGITAL CAPITALISM DIGITAL DIGITAL CAPITALISM An Introduction to Cognitive Materialism Knowledge in the Age of Digital Capitalism: An Introduction to Cognitive Materialism Mariano Zukerfeld Translated from Spanish by Suzanna Wylie University of Westminster Press www.uwestminsterpress.co.uk Competing interests The author declares that they have no competing interests in publishing this book. Published by University of Westminster Press 101 Cavendish Street London W1W 6XH www.uwestminsterpress.co.uk Text © Mariano Zukerfeld 2017 First published 2017 Series cover concept: Mina Bach (minabach.co.uk) Printed in the UK by Lightning Source Ltd. Print and digital versions typeset by Siliconchips Services Ltd. ISBN (Paperback): 978-1-911534-24-2 ISBN (PDF): 978-1-911534-25-9 ISBN (EPUB): 978-1-911534-26-6 ISBN (Kindle): 978-1-911534-27-3 DOI: https://doi.org/10.16997/book3 This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution- NonCommer cial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ or send a letter to Creative Commons, 444 Castro Street, Suite 900, Mountain View, California, 94041, USA. This license allows for copying and distributing the work, provid ing author attribution is clearly stated, that you are not using the material for commercial purposes, and that modified versions are not distributed. The full text of this book has been peer-reviewed to ensure high academic standards. For full review policies, see: http://www.uwestminsterpress.co.uk/site/publish/ Suggested citation: Zukerfeld, Mariano.
    [Show full text]
  • Practice, Communication and Space: a Reflection on the Materiality Of
    Space and the Communicative Constitution of Social Practice 1 Practice, Communication and Space A reflection on the materiality of social structures A thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Vinicius de Moraes Netto The Bartlett School of Graduate Studies University College London University of London Space and the Communicative Constitution of Social Practice 2 …symbols are required to make traces of what is not given apparent in what is given. Jürgen Habermas [on Cassirer], On the Logic of the Social Sciences, 1988 The imageries of space are the dream of society. Wherever the hieroglyph of space lies deciphered, there is offered the grounds of social reality.1 Siegfried Kracauer, Über Arbeitsnachweise, 1929 1 “Die Raumbilder sind die Träume der Gesellschaft. Wo immer die Hieroglyphe irgendeines Raumbildes entziffert ist, dort bietet sich der Grund der sozialen Wirklichtkeit dar” – personal translation. I would like to thank Marion Röhrig. Space and the Communicative Constitution of Social Practice 3 Abstract The general issue of relations between sociality and spatiality, until recently profoundly ignored outside spatial studies, has become a focus of great theoretical attention in a number of disciplines – what has been called, remarkably, the “spatial turn” in social and cultural theory. The thesis wishes to address a central problem in that debate: the connection of practice and space. It does so emphasising a dimension that has not been previously explored to a significant degree: the conditions of sociation of practice, or a material account of how action becomes social action and practice social practice. In other words, it investigates the place of space in semantically mediated interactions that constitute the knots in (spatialised) networks of sociation – or communication.
    [Show full text]
  • Research Article
    int. j. geographical information science, 1999, vol. 13, no. 8, 747± 774 Research Article Cognitive models of geographical space DAVID M. MARK National Center for Geographical Information and Analysis, and Department of Geography, University at Bu alo, Box o, NY 14261-0023, USA. e-mail: [email protected] alo.edu CHRISTIAN FREKSA Fachbereich Informatik, UniversitaÈ t Hamburg, Vogt-Koelln-Strasse 30, 22527 Hamburg, Germany STEPHEN C. HIRTLE School of Information Sciences, 735 IS Bldg, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA ROBERT LLOYD Department of Geography, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC 29208, USA and BARBARA TVERSKY Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305-2130, USA Abstract. This paper reviews research in geographical cognition that provides part of the theoretical foundation of geographical information science. Free- standing research streams in cognitive science, behavioural geography, and carto- graphy converged in the last decade or so with work on theoretical foundations for geographical information systems to produce a coherent research community that advances geographical information science, geographical information sys- tems, and the contributing ® elds and disciplines. Then, we review three high- priority research areas that are the topics for research initiatives within the NCGIA’s Project Varenius. Other topics consider but ranked less important at this time are also reviewed. 1. Introduction As geographical information becomes ubiquitous in a variety of domains and ® eld applications, computational models of geographical cognition become increas- ingly important to the growth of a science of geographical information. This paper reviews the history of research in cognition of geographical space, summarizes the current state of the ® eld, and suggests several important open issues regarding the cognitive component of geographical information science.
    [Show full text]