<<

Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine DigitalCommons@PCOM

PCOM Dissertations Student Dissertations, Theses and Papers

2004 Relationship Between Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorders Across Diagnostic Axes Bradley Michael Rosenfield Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.pcom.edu/psychology_dissertations Part of the Commons

Recommended Citation Rosenfield, Bradley Michael, "Relationship Between Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorders Across Diagnostic Axes" (2004). PCOM Psychology Dissertations. Paper 119.

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Dissertations, Theses and Papers at DigitalCommons@PCOM. It has been accepted for inclusion in PCOM Psychology Dissertations by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@PCOM. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine

Department of Psychology

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN COGNITIVE DISTORTIONS AND

PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS ACROSS DIAGNOSTIC AXES

By Bradley M. Rosenfield

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements for the Degree of

Doctor of Psychology

December 2004

PHILADELPHIA COLLEGE OF OSTEOPATHIC MEDICINE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY

Dissertation Approval

th This is to certify that the thesis presented to us by Bradley Rosenfield on the 5 day of

October 2004, in partial fulfillment ofthe requirements for the degree of Doctor of

Psychology, has been examined and is acceptable in both scholarship and literary quality.

Committee Members' Signatures:

Robert A. DiTomasso, Ph.D., ABPP, Chairperson

Michael Ascher, Ph.D.

Carrie L. Yurica, Psy.D.

Robert A. DiTomasso, Ph.D., ABPP, Chair, Department of Psychology

- iv

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Sincere thanks to all of those who have encouraged, mentored and otherwise supported my academic and professional development leading up to and throughout the dissertation process. Specifically, Dr. Robert DiTomasso, Committee Chairperson, has provided invaluable professional, didactic, and technical guidance. Beginning in my very first class at PCOM and continuing throughout the entire internship and dissertation process, I have been grateful for his selfless support, , and supervision. He has been incredibly helpful to me and to my fellow students despite his ever-increasing responsibilities.

Equal is extended to my committee members: Dr. Carrie Yurica, for her pioneering research into cognitive distortions, which served as the foundation for the present study, and for her selfless guidance and support whenever it was most needed. I would also like to thank Dr. Michael Ascher for his professionalism, guidance, support, and good sense of humor.

Special thanks to Dr. Arthur Freeman for his keen ability to identify and nurture each student’s unique and often undiscovered talents. Dr. Freeman has always gently guided and encouraged many students’ professional development, while keeping them on the right academic and professional path. He has always gone well beyond the norm in making himself cheerfully accessible to hard-working students.

Sincere appreciation is also extended to study collaborators and participants, who have brought this research to fruition and furthered science, in general.

Finally, deepest thanks to my parents for their efforts, sacrifice, and sincere for all of their children. They are the two most caring and, yet competent and dynamic people with whom

v a child could be blessed. They have both always modeled the deepest concern for others, and the highest ethical behavior. Of course, neither I, nor the present study, would be here without them.

vi

Abstract

The Relationship Between Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorders Across Diagnostic

Axes

The purpose of this study was twofold: First, the study sought to determine whether or not cognitive distortions correlated with the number and severity of psychological disorders across diagnostic axes. Second, the study endeavored to assess further the validity and reliability of a promising new self-report measure, The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions (ICD), by correlating this instrument with clinical diagnoses as determined by the Millon Clinical

Multiaxial Inventory-III. The sample was selected from a heterogeneous adult outpatient population. Participants meeting inclusion criteria presented for psychological treatment or assessment. Excluded were participants who had less than an eighth grade education, or who met diagnostic criteria for a number of neurological disorders. The study was correlational in design.

Results supported the reliability and validity of the ICD as an assessment instrument measuring cognitive distortions, because the ICD determined that approximately half of the variance in both the number and severity of psychological dysfunction, on both Axis I and Axis II, was accounted for by the frequency of cognitive distortions.

vii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

CHAPTER 1 1 INTRODUCTION 2 Prevalence of Mental Health Disorders ………………………………………… 6 Rise of the Cognitive Model ……………………………………………………. 8 Future Directions for ………………………………………. 9 Future Directions for Cognitive Therapy ………………………………………. Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorder: Statement of the Problem ….

CHAPTER 2

LITERATURE REVIEW Beck’s Original Model of Cognitive Therapy ………………………………….. 14 Contemporary Models of Cognitive Therapy …………………………………... 15 Contemporary Models of Cognitive Behavior Therapy ………………………... 19 Varieties of Cognitive Distortions ……………………………………………… 20 Cognitive Distortions: Origins of Terms ……………………………………….. 21 Cognitive Processing Models of Cognitive Distortions ………………………… 22 Evolutionary Models of Cognitive Distortions …………………………………. 26 Developmental Theories, , and Cognitive Distortions ……………….. 30 The Efficacy of Cognitive Therapy ……………………………………………... 34 Cognitive Therapy for ………………………………………………. 36 Depression Complicated by Personality Disorders …………………………. 38 Cognitive Therapy for Disorders ………………………………………. 43 Anxiety Complicated by Personality Disorders ……………………………… 45 Demonstration of Treatment Outcome …………………………………………. 48 Outcome and Measures of ……………………………….. 48 Measuring Cognitive Distortions ……………………………………………….. 49 Limitations of Existing Measures of Cognitive Distortion ……………………... 51 The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions ………………………………………… 52 Outcome and Measures of …………………………………… 55 The Minnesota Multipahasic Personality Inventory-2 ………………………. 55 The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III ………………………………… 56 Limitations of the MCMI—III ……………………………………………… 66 Cognitive Distortions in Personality Disorders and Clinical Syndromes ………. 67 Rationale for the Study …………………………………………………………. 73 The Purpose of the Study ……………………………………………………….. 75 Research Hypotheses …………………………………………………………… 78 Definition of Terms ……………………………………………………………... 86

viii

CHAPTER 3

METHODOLOGY Participants ……………………………………………………………………… 89 Inclusion Criteria ……………………………………………………………….. 90 Exclusion Criteria ………………………………………………………………. 90 Participant Recruitment ………………………………………………………… 91 Research Design ………………………………………………………………… 91 Measures: The Million Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-Third Edition …………… 92 MCMI—III reliability ………………………………………………………. 93 MCMI—III validity …………………………………………………………. 94 Measures: The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions …………………………….. 94 ICD Psychometrics …………………………………………………………. 95

CHAPTER 4

RESULTS Descriptive Statistics ……………………………………………………………. 98 Relationship to Demographic Variables ………………………………………... 99 The Relationship between the MCMI-III and the Total ICD Scores …………… 99

CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION Demographic Characteristics ……………………………………………... 107 Major Findings: Presence of psychopathology …………………………………. 107 Axis I Clinical Syndromes: Number of Axis I diagnoses ………………………. 108 Axis I Clinical Syndromes: Severity of Axis I clinical syndromes …………….. 109 Axis II Personality Disorders: Number of personality disorders ……………… 110 Axis II Personality Disorders: Severity of personality disorders ………………. 111 Comorbid Axis I and Axis II Disorders ………………………………………… 111 Number of comorbid clinical syndromes and personality disorders …………… 112 Total severity of all ps ychological dysfunction ………………………………… 113 The Relationship between Cognitive Distortions and Individual Disorders …… 114 Limitations of the Study ………………………………………………………… 120 Recommendations for Future Research ………………………………………… 121 Conclusions ……………………………………………………………………... 122

REFERENCES ……………………………………………………………………... 124

ix

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1. Summary of meta-analytic findings ………………………………………. 35 Table 2. Hypothesis Summary ……………………………………………………... 78 Table 3. Analysis of Variance for Comorbid Axis I and Axis II Conditions ……… 102 Table 4. Number and Severity of Psychological Disorders across Axis I and Axis II and the Relation to Total Frequency of Cognitive Distortion ………………………………………………... 103 Table 5. The Relationship between Frequency of Cognitive Distortion and Individual Psychological Disorders ………………………………... 105

x

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Illustration of interpersonal dimensions …………………………………. 62

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

Prevalence of Mental Health Disorders

Psychological disorders are surprisingly common in the United States and abroad.

According to The U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (1999) approximately 22% of

American adults over the age of 18 years—or over 44 million people—suffer from a diagnosable mental disorder in a given year. The most common maladies are the anxiety and depressive disorders, which 16.4 and 7.1%, respectively, of the American population each year.

Personality disorders (PDs) are believed to cause distress for 2.1% of the American population and for many of those with whom they interact. It is also estimated that schizophrenia affects

1.3% of the U.S. population. Comorbidity among the various disorders, even across clinical axes is also common (Reiger, Narrow, Pae, Manderscherd, Lockem & Goodwin, 1993; Kessler,

McGonagle, Zhoa, Nelson, Hughes, Eshleman et al, 1994; Weissman, Bland, Canino, Faravelli,

Greenwald, Hwa et al, 1997; Millon, 1999).

It is logical that millions of those from these disorders would seek treatment to alleviate their distress and to improve their functioning. Empirically supported psychological treatments have found increasing among professionals, given the pressures arising from managed care, from fiscal constraints, from the public’s growing psychological sophistication, and from potential accusations of malpractice should therapy be ineffectual, or worse, cause harm (e.g, American Psychological Association [APA], 2004; Hawley & Weisz, 2

2002). Cognitive therapy (CT) is in the vanguard of evidenced-based psychological treatments,

both in the U.S. and, increasingly, around the world (Dobson & Khatri, 2000).

The Rise of the Cognitive Model

CT evolved during a tumultuous time in the development of effective psychotherapy. During

this period two competing, dominant factions, the psychoanalytic (e.g., Freud, 1899/1962;

Sulloway, 1979) and behavioral schools (e.g., Skinner, 1958; Wolpe, 1958) were competing with

a popular nonconformist theory: the client-centered humanistic approach (Rogers, 1951).

While yet a practicing psychoanalyst, Beck (1961) adroitly discerned that his depressed patients were often vaguely aware of certain negative thoughts that they frequently failed to report during free association. These negative thoughts composed part of a constellation of , which he described as “verbal and pictorial events in the stream of consciousness… based on attitudes, assumptions,” expectations, and (Beck, , Shaw, & Emery, 1979, p. 4). Because these cognitions resided mainly outside of conscious control and occurred without volition, they were labeled automatic. As a trained psychoanalyst, Beck compared these automatic thoughts to Freud’s notion of the preconscious. This original cognitive model of depression proposed that patients may not have been aware of the automatic thoughts, yet they could become acutely aware of the negative affect that such cognitions could engender. These automatic thoughts were subject to specific types of logical errors or cognitive distortions, which were labeled “selective abstraction, overgeneralization, dichotomous thinking, and

(of the negative aspects of their experiences)” (Beck, 1991, p. 368). One’s cognitions, arising from past experience, become habitual patterns of thought about the self, the world, and the

3 future, which profoundly influence one’s affect and behavior (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 1979; Beck, Freeman, Davis, & Associates, 2004; Beck Wright, & Newman, 1993; Ellis &

Geiger, 1977).

Moreover, both Ellis (1958, 1962, 1973) and Beck (1961, 1964, 1967, 1976) recognized that neither excavation of early childhood trauma nor modification of stimulus-response patterns was sufficient to produce enduring change in human and behavior. They similarly viewed the non-directive humanistic approach as inefficient and without empirical support. Instead, these theorists discerned a number of patterns of dysfunctional thinking among their patients. For example, Ellis (1958, 1962, 1973) proposed 11 irrational beliefs that he believed predisposed individuals to negative emotional and behavioral consequences.

Similarly, Beck (1967) coined the term “cognitive therapy” after discovering a correlation between the negative thinking and the moods of his depressed patients. By identifying, exploring, and modifying these frequently inaccurate cognitions, Beck (1964, 1967, 1979) witnessed a distinct improvement in his patients’ moods. Consequently, Ellis, Beck and their adherents viewed dysfunctional cognitive processes as primary targets for psychotherapy.

Beck further proposed that automatic thoughts, which arise from interpretations of events, are themselves based on a network of secondary beliefs, assumptions, formulas, and rules that are often connected to relevant memories. At a still deeper level are more absolute, dysfunctional beliefs about the self, or schemas, such as “I am unlovable.”; “I am a loser.”; “I am worthless.”

These cognitive structures develop early in life from personal experiences, from identification with others, and from (Beck, 1967; Beck & Weishaar, 1995). Thus persistent patterns of generalizing, deleting, and otherwise distorting internal and external stimuli foster a confirmatory which supports a constellation of beliefs that may not accurately reflect one’s

4 current environment (Alford & Beck, 1997; Yurica, 2002). When this occurs, cognition is said to be distorted.

For example, one’s belief system can shape experience in ways that are analogous to optical lenses. Prescription glasses that are carefully crafted to help a child to see the world more clearly may need to be adjusted as the child matures. Lenses that are not properly altered may later distort events so drastically that they impair functioning and cause great distress.

Similarly, schemas or subjective beliefs about one’s own self, shaped by early childhood experience may foster rules, assumptions, and expectations that no longer suit the real world.

When this occurs, cognitive distortions may arise that systematically misrepresent reality, cause impairment and distress, and prevent individuals from resolving their own problems. Thus, cognitive distortions play a fundamental role in the onset, maintenance, and ultimately, the amelioration of all manner of psychological dysfunction, be they Axis I and II disorders (e.g.,

Beck et al., 2004). Beck et al. (1979) proposed that individuals distort reality in six distinct ways. Since then, many types of cognitive distortions have been postulated (Burns, 1980, 1999;

Freeman & Oster, 1999; Yurica, 2002), all of which represent inaccuracies and misattributions in interpreting interpersonal and contextual information (Giancola, Mezzich, Clark, & Tarter,

1999).

From the original theoretical cognitive distortions, Yurica (2002) provides empirical support, via factor-analysis, for 11 cognitive distortions, including 1) Externalization of Self-

Worth, 2) Fortune-Telling, 3) Magnification, 4) Labeling, 5) Perfectionism, 6) Comparison with

Others, 7) , 8) Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusions, 9)

Minimization, 10) Mind-Reading, 11) Emotional Reasoning and Decision-Making.

5

Cognitively based therapies have been conceptualized in a variety of ways. Ellis (1962) originally po sited the ide a that irrational beliefs foster emotional distress and functional impairment. Alternately, Meichenbaum (1975) suggested that self-talk, that is, one’s internal dialogue is the fundamental mediator of affect and behavior. Finally, it has been postulated that both emotional disorders (Beck, 1967, 1976) and PDs (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al.,

2004) result from maladaptive cognitive processing, manifested by dysfunctional core beliefs and associated cognitive distortions.

CT and cognitive-behavior therapies (CBT) have subsequently evolved to include CT (Beck,

1967; Beck et al., 1979), rational emotive behavioral therapy (Ellis & Grieger, 1977), problem- solving therapy (D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1990; Nezu, 1986), narrative therapy, (e.g., White & Epston,

1989), and self-instructional therapy (Meichenbaum, 1993).

Moreover, cognitively oriented therapies have gained growing acceptance throughout the mental health community and, more generally, across the entire healthcare industry

(Meichenbaum & Turk, 1987; Robins, Gosling, & Craik, 1999). In fact, cognitively oriented psychotherapy has gained such prominence that Robins, et al. (1999) determined this modality to be the fastest growing choice for treatment throughout the last four decades, as measured by the subject matter of published dissertations, leading psychological journals, and subdisciplinary journals. Consequently, according to these objective measures, the preeminent psychotherapy models today appear to be the cognitive and cognitive behavioral. And, common to these schools of thought is the recognition of the fundamental role of cognitive distortion in psychopathology.

6

Future Directions for Cognitive Therapy

Progress in psychotherapy has been predicated upon a number of important variables. First,

the science and profession of clinical psychology has become increasingly concerned with

incorporating scientifically supported treatment into progressively more evidence-based clinical

practice. Thus, the synthesis of the scientist-practitioner (Raimy, 1950) and practitioner-scholar

models has allowed the field to advance, incrementally and systematically, by incorporating

empirical findings and scholarly theories directly into clinical practice (Belar & Perry, 1992).

Next, legitimate empirical research is itself based upon the use of standardized procedures in

order to allow consistent application of procedures and to facilitate replication. This requires valid and reliable instruments to measure results (Kazdin, 1998). Consequently, the validation of psychometrically sound instruments is a prerequisite for the advancement of empirically validated treatments for wide range of psychological disorders.

Finally, pressures from managed care and from distressed patients seeking relief have

increased the need for optimally efficient and effective therapy (McDaniel, 1995). This has

generated increasing demand for manualized treatment approaches with proven efficacy, which

can be further augmented by incorporating empirically validated, standardized assessment

measures (Gilson & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004). Moreover, it is incumbent upon both

scientists and practitioners to demonstrate successful treatment outcome objectively. This

requires the measurement of cognitive and behavioral variables (Dobson & Khatri, 2000).

Beck (1993) asserted that CT has gained preeminence because of its reliance on scientific

method and ability to assimilate empirical findings. Evidence for effectiveness of CT and CBT

has been demonstrated for the treatment of the following disorders: major depressive disorder,

7 disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, generalized , eating disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder, sexual dysfunction, substance , and PDs (Agras, Rossiter,

Arnow, Schneider, Telch, Raeburn, et al., 1992; Butler & Beck, 2000; Beck, & Freeman, 1990;

Chambliss & Olendick, 2001; Deblinger, Steer & Lipman, 1999; DeRubeis & Crits-Christoph,

1999; Woody, McLellan, Luborsky, & Obrien, 1990; Young, 1990/1999). Cogntive therapy is showing promise even in the treatment of schizophrenia (Beck, 2000) and

(Newman, Leahy, Beck, Reilly-Harrington, & Gyulai, 2004).

There is also empirical support for the association between cognitive distortions and a number of other maladaptive social and clinical conditions, including sexual assault (Baumeister,

Catanese, & Wallace, 2002), pathological gambling (Steenbergh, Meyers, May, & Whelan,

2002), adolescent anxiety and depressive disorders (Kendall, Kortlander, & Brady, 1992), violence and in marital relationships (Ekhardt, Barbour, & Davison, 1998), and adolescent depression and anxiety (Kolko, Brent, Baugher, Bridge, Birmaher, 2000). To the extent that dysfunctional cognition and its corollaries contribute to these problems, there is —and evidence—for the effectiveness of CT in their amelioration.

Given the future trends in the evolution of psychological research, clinical practice, and CT, this ability to provide empirical validation for a promising new measure of cognitive dysfunction could provide further support for the cognitive model, provide clinicians with a valuable assessment instrument, and further advance the field. Moreover, identifying the extent to which cognitive distortions contribute to psychological dysfunction across Axis I and Axis II should add practical knowledge to aid professionals when assessing and treating these troubling disorders.

8

Current Clinical Practice

Identifying cognitive distortions in current clinical practice among cognitive therapists

generally involves the use of the various thought records, such as the Dysfunctional Thoughts

Record (Beck, et al., 1979), Thought Record (Persons, Davidson, & Thompkins, 2001), and

Burns (1999) Daily Mood Log. Although these instruments may illuminate distorted cognition, they fail to identify specific cognitive distortions; instead, this task is left to the clinician.

Moreover, even after completing various thought records, the process of identifying cognitive

distortions is generally neither standardized nor quantitative (Yurica, 2002).

Results of randomized clinical trials supported the efficacy of such thought records for modifying dysfunctional thinking in depressed outpatients (Craighead, Craighead, & Ilardi,

1998; DeRubeis & Crits-Christoph, 1998). However, there are a number of impediments to using thought records. For instance, Persons et al., (2001) found that the following factors were impediments to proper application: (a) difficulty eliciting patients’ automatic thoughts, (b) patients’ reluctance to use the instruments both in and outside of treatment sessions, (c) noncompliance with homework assignments, (d) patients’ beliefs that the procedure would not be helpful.

In addition, it appears that clinicians use indirect and non-standardized methods for identifying specific types of cognitive distortions because the various assessment tools intended to assess dysfunctional thinking are neither widely known nor employed in the field and inadequately explain specific varieties of cognitive distortions independent of clinical diagnosis; some of these assessment tools include: the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS; Weissman &

9

Beck, 1978; Weissman, 1979), Cogntive Errors Questionnaire (CEQ; Lefebvre, 1981), and

Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ; Hollon & Kendall, 1980) In practice, this makes

identifying, modifying, documenting, and predicting cognitive distortions pr oblematic and

unnecessarily time-consuming (Yurica, 2002).

Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorders:

Statement of the Problem

Although the cognitive model recognizes the fundamental role of cognitive distortion in the genesis and maintenance of all manner of psychological disturbance, only one recently validated measure exists that identifies the frequency and categorical use of cognitive distortions by mental health patients. Although promising, this instrument, the ICD, has been validated only for

anxious and depressed populations. Moreover, a review of the literature revealed that the DAS

was useful in distinguishing individuals with either Axis I or Axis II conditions from controls;

however, there was no support for discriminant validity of the DAS in identifying specific PDs or Axis II Clusters, and there was little research into how individuals with specific Axis I and

Axis II conditions differ in their tendency to engage in cognitive distortion (Ilardi & Craighead,

1999).

The overall purpose of the present study was twofold: First, the study sought to determine whether or not cognitive distortions correlated with the number and severity of psychological disorders across Axis I and Axis II. Second, the study intended to assess further the validity and reliability of a promising new self-report measure of cognitive distortions, the ICD and was to correlate this instrument with clinical diagnoses as determined by the MCMI-III.

10

A literature review revealed no research addressing the contribution of the frequency of both cognitive distortions in a heterogeneous adult outpatient sample meeting criteria for a wide range of DSM-IV disorders across both Axis I and Axis II. Most studies were limited to investigations of circumscribed diagnoses, such as depression or anxiety, with the possibility of another added variable, that is, comorbid PDs (e.g., Ilardi and Craighead, 1999). Moreover, no studies were found assessing the relative contribution of the frequency of cognitive distortions both to the number and the severity of disorders. This lacunae is problematic; because if, as cognitive theory and research have indicated, cognitive distortions contribute to psychological disorders, an additional understanding is needed of the many patients, especially those with PDs, who present with multiple diagnoses across both axes (Beck et al., 2004; Millon & Davis, 1996).

Consequently, providing further validation for the ICD, a brief, portable, and objective measurement of these key cognitive processes should be beneficial both to research and to practice in a number of important respects. First, cognitive therapists can provide convincing

post-treatment evidence of successful treatment outcome by using the ICD to establish objective

baselines and regularly monitor behavior and mood utilizing empirically valid and reliable instruments. Although such measures are helpful in diagnosis and treatment planning for a variety of disorders, there is a paucity of empirically supported measures designed to assess

cognitive distortions directly—conceptualized as one of the fundamental cognitive

dysfunctions—that have been validated for the a wide range of clinical diagnoses. Accurately

identifying specific patterns of cognitive distortions early in the course of treatment might assist

the clinician to target more efficiently dysfunctional thought patterns that maintain and

exacerbate problems. Moreover, an instrument which can provide regular assessment of

cognitive distortions throughout treatment can provide an objective measure of progress because

11

cognitive distortions have been shown to correlate with a number of Axis I disorders; for

example, depression, (e.g., Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erdbaugh, 1961; Butler & Beck,

2000; Hollon & Kendall, 1980; Yurica, 2002) anxiety (Ross, Gottfredson, Christensen, &

Weaver, 1986; Yurica, 2002) and PDs (Ilardi and Craighead, 1999).

Second, it has been shown that treatment occurring earlier in the course of psychological

disorders correlates with a number of therapeutic benefits, including positive treatment outcome,

fewer office visits to physicians, and reduced health care costs (Bruns, 1998; Kaplan, 2000).

Thus, early identification of cognitive distortions might allow the therapist and patient to

collaboratively and more quickly illuminate and modify the underlying secondary beliefs and

schemas, which maintain those same cognitive distortions.

Third, the instrument could facilitate psychoeducational processes regarding patients’ particular patterns of cognitive distortions. If specific distorted thought patterns are noted, the therapist may teach more adaptive cognitive and behavioral skills explicitly targeting those distortions. For example, if the patient exhibits rigid patterns of perfectionism, it might be more efficient to use decentering or scaling techniques as a first line of treatment (Beck & Freeman,

1990).

Fourth, there may emerge characteristic patterns of distortions that delineate particular

Axis I versus Axis II conditions. If the ICD supports this difference, its use may increase diagnostic accuracy, which, in turn, may lead to more effective treatment selection.

Fifth, demonstrating a correlation between the amelioration of cognitive distortions and the improvement of clinical symptoms for Axis I and Axis II conditions would lend further empirical support to the cognitive model of psychological disorders on both diagnostic axes.

12

Sixth, the ICD has already demonstrated efficacy in correlating the chronicity of cognitive

distortions with depression and anxiety. If the same pattern emerges with PDs, the ICD will be

able to provide researchers and clinicians with an elegant tool to gauge quickly and accurately

the severity of pathology (Yurica, 2002). Furthermore, because the ICD directly assesses

cognitive processes that are central to maladaptive functioning and treatment, ascertaining the

severity of distortions may provide a roadmap to guide appropriate treatment planning by

allowing the clinician to adapt to the patient’s level of cognitive functioning.

Seventh, this study also sought to expand further the utility of the ICD beyond the

evaluation of anxiety and depression (Yurica, 2002) for use with additional Axis I and Axis II

conditions. It is hoped, also, that a sufficiently validated ICD will allow for greater flexibility in

empirical research by providing an additional assessment tool for the design and standardization

of empirical procedure, which can be employed to measure pathology and progress, aiding as

well in the replication of empirical research.

Finally, in a review of 136 research studies, Grove and Meehl (1996) determined that

empirically based personality assessment instruments are consistently equal to or superior to

less-structured clinical interview methods; these instruments increase the efficiency of the assessment process, understand patients more fully, establish rapport, formulate an accurate diagnosis, develop insight, plan optimum empirically guided treatment planning, and predict the course of treatment (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Consequently, a brief self-report measure, such as the ICD, administered early in the treatment process can efficiently increase diagnostic accuracy and improve treatment planning; this may facilitate these benefits for clinicians and patients alike. Such an instrument might also lessen the burden on the psychological and medical communities by reducing the course of treatment and, by extension, health care costs.

13

Stated more psychometrically, the goals of the study are to 1) evaluate the construct validity of the ICD in a sample of Axis I and Axis II patients, 2) determine the relationship between the ICD with specific Axis I and II disorders, 3) establish the relationship between the

ICD and the MCMI—III, a well-established, multidimensional, valid and reliable measure of

Axis I and Axis II conditions, 4) further confirm the internal consistency of ICD content by confirming the acceptable alpha coefficient levels originally found by Yurica (2002), and 5) provide an instrument that will assist clinicians to target specific maladaptive cognitive distortions regardless of diagnosis.

In summary, the purpose of this study is to further assess a promising instrument, the ICD, for the measurement of cognitive distortions, as defined by Beck (1976), Beck et al., (1979),

Burns (1980, 1999), Freeman & Oster (1999), Gilson & Freeman, (1999), and Yurica (2002) as these occur in various psychological disorders. It is hoped that this instrument will aid in cognitive-behavioral treatment for adult clinical populations.

The subsequent literature review offers an empirical and theoretical basis for the further validation of the ICD, as supported by relevant literature regarding 1) the original model of cognitive therapy. 2) contemporary models of cognitive therapy 3) contemporary models of cognitive behavior therapy 4) varieties of cognitive distortions 5) cognitive processing models of cognitive distortions 6) evolutionary models of cognitive distortions 7) developmental theories cognition and cognitive distortions, 8) efficacy of cognitive therapy 9) outcome and measures and 10) limitations of existing measures of cognitive distortion.

14

CHAPTER 2

LITERATURE REVIEW

Beck’s Original Model of Cognitive Therapy

Beck (1976; Beck et al. 1979) originally posited the theory that one’s affect and behavior are powerfully influenced by one’s cognition, including one’s past, present, and future interpretations of oneself, the world, and one’s future. and experience were believed to be active processes based on the interpretation of both internal and external stimuli. Further, individuals were said to manifest certain trait-like cognitive patterns that left them vulnerable to emotional distress (Beck, 1976; Beck et al., 1979). These cognitions did not necessarily correspond to reality but were thought to be “based on attitudes or assumptions (schema), developed from previous experiences.” (Beck et al., 1979, p.3). In other words, cognition may be corresponding to previous experience rather than extant reality and may, therefore, be distorted.

Consequently, CT is founded on the premise that individuals can learn to recognize and correct their distorted thinking and that more realistic thinking can result in amelioration of clinical symptoms.

Consistent with the cognitive model of psychopathology, CT is designed to be structured, directive, active, and time-limited, with the express purpose of identifying, reality-testing, and

15

correcting distorted cognition and underlying dysfunctional beliefs (Beck, 1967, 1976, Beck et

al., 1979).

Although modern CT employs interventions, which may be cognitive, behavioral,

interpersonal, or imaginal, (e.g., Beck, 1996; McMullin, 2000), the ultimate goal is to disconfirm

maladaptive beliefs and to correct cognitive distortions (Beck et al., 1979). Assumptions

underlying CT include the following:

1. Perception and experience are active processes involving both inspection and introspection.

2. Cognitions represent a synthesis of internal and external stimuli.

3. The way one views an event is evident in one’s cognitions (thoughts and visual images).

4. Cognitions—reflecting on oneself, on one’s past and future, and on other people—constitute

one’s stream of consciousness or phenomenological field.

5. Modifications in cognition will alter affect, mood, and behavior.

6. Psychotherapy allows the patient to become aware of cognitive distortions.

7. Correcting these dysfunctional cognitions can lead to amelioration of clinical disorders.

Contemporary Models of Cognitive Therapy

There are a several competing theoretical models within the field of CT today. Despite

their differences, there are a number of common assumptions among cognitively oriented

therapists which still unite the field. For example, modern cognitively oriented therapists respect

the patient’s subjective experience. In addition, cognitively oriented therapy is active, directive,

empirical, and behavioral. Cognitive models generally draw on Beck's general schema theory

and the notion of automatic thoughts (Beck, 1976; Beck et al., 1979). This theory holds that core

16 beliefs and the content of schema are responsible for a myriad of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral interactions—some of which may be dysfunctional. The maladaptive cognitive processes, which may cause symptoms that arise to the level of clinical significance, include cognitive distortions and maladaptive assumptions, rules, and other secondary beliefs (Alford &

Beck, 1997; Beck, 1996; Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004). Additionally, psychological disorders may be maintained by schema- driven processes, particularly schema-related avoidance and maladaptive compensatory strategies (Young, 1990/1999). To complicate matters, conflicting and interacting beliefs and unhelpful strategies are involved in maintaining certain disorders (Wells, 1997). In other words, the patients’ subjective are the central focus of CT.

CT requires an active approach, with the therapist and patient collaboratively defining problems, identifying goals, and negotiating the tactics and strategies to achieve those goals

(Beck et al, 1979, J. Beck, 1996; Freeman & Oster, 1979). Patients’ active collaboration is essential for identifying, exploring, and modifying distorted cognitions and maladaptive behavior.

Another salient characteristic of modern CT is the wide use of behavioral interventions.

However, as opposed to conditioning models, CT theorists attribute resulting change in emotion and behavior to change in “verbal behavior” (Alford & Beck, 1997, p.60). In other words, the cognitive model asserts that the ultimate goal of even the most behavioral interventions is reducing distorted cognition and increasing rational thinking. It is then essential to generalize these changes to important areas of the patient’s life (Beck et al, 1979; Freeman & Oster, 1999).

To facilitate the generalization, patients are oriented to the cognitive model and encouraged to engage actively in between-session homework assignments (Beck et al., 1979; Freeman &

17

Rosenfield, 2002). Contemporary CT models are empirical. For instance, both intrasession CT and intersession homework allow patients to test and to modify, empirically, the idiosyncratic beliefs that maintain various clinical disorders on both Axis I and Axis II (Ilardi & Craighead,

1999; Alford & Beck, 1997, Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004; Freeman & Rosenfield,

2002). Homework activities may include, but are not limited, to activity scheduling, completing thought records, self-monitoring, breathing retraining, graded task performance, mastery and tasks, social skill building, and .

The original cognitive model has evolved as Alford and Beck (1997) synthesized the subsequent two decades of research into their modified cognitive theory to include 10 “formal axioms” (p. 15). First, schemas are conceptualized as meaning-making structures, through which, individuals interpret given contexts in relation to the self.

Second, assigning meaning controls behavior, emotion, , and memory. Meaning assignment can occur both at the automatic and deliberative levels. Third, there is a reciprocal influence between cognition, affect and behavior. Four, “cognitive content specificity” (p. 16) occurs when meaning is translated into particular patterns of attention, memory, emotion, and behavior.

The fifth axiom, which is particularly germane, states that because meaning is constructed by the individual, it may be incorrect in relation to specific contexts and goals. The authors state,

“When cognitive distortion or bias occurs, meanings are dysfunctional or maladaptive” (p. 16).

Cognitive distortions may include errors in interpretation and cognitive processing, or both. Six, individuals are predisposed to particular cognitive distortions, leaving them predisposed to specific syndromes. This is termed cognitive . Seven, psychopathology is said to result from cognitive distortions related to a slightly modified cognitive triad, reflecting

18

maladaptive interpretations “regarding the self, the environmental context (experience), and the

future (goals)” (p.16). Eight, events are interpreted on two levels: The Public or objective level

and a private level. The latter incorporates generalizations, implications, and significance. Nine,

“Three Cognitive Systems” provide for three levels of cognition, including (a) the automatic

level (unintentional/preconscious), (b) the conscious level, and (c) the realistic, adaptive,

rational, or metacognitive level. Changes occurring at the conscious level primarily account for

progress in CT.

Finally, the tenth axiom posits the idea that individuals’ schemas have evolved to assist them

to adapt to their environments. Schemas become maladaptive only when they are out of context

with the general social or physical environment.

In summary, Alford & Beck (1997) asserted that cognitive distortions arise from “patterns”

and “predispositions”, indicating that this maladaptive pattern of thinking may be trait-like rather than state-like. This is a theory that has received some support, particularly with Axis II patients.

For example, Ilardi & Craighead, (1999) found that Axis II patients’ distortions, as measured by the DAS, seemed to persist beyond amelioration of Axis I syndromes. However, in individuals with only Axis I diagnoses, cognitive distortions receded as depressive symptoms remitted.

Thus, the tendency to engage in cognitive distortion was more trait-like in individuals with Axis

II disorders, whereas this propensity was more state-like in those with Axis I conditions. Once

again, theory indicates that cognitive distortions play a pivotal role in the genesis, maintenance,

and eventual amelioration of psychological disorders.

19

Contemporary Models of Cognitive Behavior Therapy

CT has always included cognitive and behavioral interventions in its repertoire (Beck et al.

1979; Ellis, 1962). In, perhaps, one of the most comprehensive reviews of cognitive-behavior

therapy (CBT), Brewin (1996) contended that CBT had evolved to a point at which it

encompassed diverse sets of theories, terms, and procedures. Historically, Brewin asserted that

the cognitive and behavioral camps suffered from an “absence of an explicit role for conditioning

in cognitive therapies and the absence of a role for verbal mediation in behavior therapies (p.

34)”; this led to a period of estrangement and disparagement between the two factions.

Today, various CBT schools are distinguished by their targeting of circumscribed disorders

or by targeting various generalized disorders. Additionally, some CBT theorists suggest that

interventions should be aimed at modifying conscious beliefs and representations rather than

modifying unconscious representations in memory. Interventions aimed at altering consciously

accessible beliefs subscribe to the theoretical notion of appraisal theories of emotion and

cognitive theories of emotion and motivation. Interventions aimed at modifying unconscious

representations are related to their theoretical bases in learning theory and in findings from

experimental (e.g. Chambless & Gillis 1993, Dobson 1989, Hollon,

Shelton, & Davis, 1993). Regardless of theoretical underpinning, CBT is focused on cognitive change, behavioral change, or both. A comprehensive review of the literature (DeRubeis & Crits-Cristoph, 1997), indicated differential results for interventions that were principally cognitive versus those that were predominantly behavioral. For instance, predominantly behavioral interventions have demonstrated good outcome with certain Axis I conditions, such as specific phobias, social

20 phobia, , PTSD and agoraphobia. On the other hand, both PDs and depressive disorders responded better to predominantly cognitive interventions. Leichsenring & Leibing

(2003) also demonstrated efficacy for CBT in treating PDs. Other studies have found that both cognitive and behavioral interventions were effective (DeRubeis & Crits-Cristoph, 1997;

Rachman, 1999).

These facts indicate that there are cognitive and behavioral components both in Axis I and

Axis II disorders which can be ameliorated through either predominantly cognitive or mainly behavioral means. However, one cannot discount the fundamental role of cognitive change resulting from even the most behavioral interventions. Although both modern CT and CBT approaches employ interventions which may be cognitive, behavioral, interpersonal, or imaginal

(e.g., McMullin, 2000), the ultimate theoretical goals are to disconfirm maladaptive beliefs and to correct cognitive distortions (Beck et al., 1979; Butler & Beck, 1998). Consequently, it would be beneficial both for CT and CBT practitioners to be able to assess these cognitive components, which are pivotal correlates of treatment success; this is possible with valid, reliable instruments for the myriad of disorders and comorbidities with which they are confronted. A literature review related to cognitive distortions is considered next.

Varieties of Cognitive Distortions

The term cognitive distortion first appeared in the literature in Beck’s (1967) discussion of depression. At that time, Beck theorized that his patients frequently engaged in cognitive processing patterns that were systematic but erroneous. He recognized that such faulty information processing predictably produced maladaptive emotion and behavior.

21

Cogntive Distortions: Origins of Terms

A number of leading CT and CBT theorists have proposed a variety of cognitive distortions

Beck (1967) originally postulated six varieties of distortion, including the following: (1) absolutistic/dichotomous thinking, (2) arbitrary inference (3) minimization and magnification,

(4) overgeneralization, (5) personalization, (6) selective abstraction.

In an effort to make Beck’s concepts even more comprehensible to the average patient, and borrowing from Ellis & Grieger (1986), Burns (1990, 1999) offered 10 cognitive distortions, including: (1) all-or-nothing thinking (2) discounting the positive, (3) emotional reasoning, (4) jumping to conclusions, (5) labeling, (6) magnification, (7) mental filter, (8) overgeneralization,

(9) blaming and personalization, (10) should-statements.

Additional cognitive distortions added important interpersonal and subjective dimensions, such as (1) comparison, (2) externalization of self-worth, and (3) perfectionism (Freeman &

DeWolf, 1990; Freeman & DeWolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).

Later, Gilson & Freeman (1999) suggested that, in addition to distorted cognitive processing, individuals often engage in fallacious thinking, such as: (1) of change, (2) fallacy of worrying, (3) fallacy of fairness, (4) fallacy of ignoring, (5) fallacy of being right, (6) fallacy of attachment, (7) fallacy of control, and (8) heaven’s reward fallacy.

Other investigators have suggested disorder-specific cognitive distortions that may manifest in particular disorders. For example, in pathological gamblers: reframed losses and the illusion of control over luck, (Toneatto, 1999); in incarcerated teenagers: self-serving and self-

debasing distortions (Barriga, Landau, Stinson, Liau, & Gibbs, 2000), and in PTSD patients:

22 preoccupation with danger and self- (Briere, 2001; Briere, Runtz, Giancola, Mezzich,

Clark, & Tarter, 1993).

. Najavits (1993; Najavits, Gotthardt, Epstein, 2004) proposed a series of distortions specifically related to substance use disorders, including, but not limited to: (1) The escape:

Inability to tolerate or solve problems. The only escape appears to be drugs, self-cutting, sleep, food, etc. (2) Dangerous permission: Patients give themselves permission for self- destructive behavior. (3) Time warp: It feels as if negative feelings will go on forever. (4) Short- term thinking: Focus on the short-term only, how one feels for a few minutes. (5) The good old days: Remembering only the wonderful highs from a drug or an abusive relationship.

A survey of the above catalog of cognitive distortions, though not complete, illustrates the fact that these processing errors may affect virtually every aspect of individuals’ lives. The following review of cognitive processing models more fully illustrates the cognitive mechanisms involved in these maladaptive thought patterns.

Cognitive Processing Models of

Cognitive Distortions

The concept of cognitive distortions has enjoyed wide spread acceptance in clinical practice and considerable validation in empirical research (e.g., Najavitis et al., 2004; McGrath

& Repetti, 2002; Yurica, 2002). Kelly’s Personal Construct Theory (1955) proposed that people develop anticipatory cognitive attitudes that results from past experience. A portion of this cognitive organization revolves around schemas, which Kelly theorized were mental organizations of information developed from past experience that are used to categorize new events. Schemas facilitate encoding new events, orienting individuals to novel events, suggesting

23

where to look for more information, and providing default information to fill gaps in perception

and interpretation. These constructs direct, filter, encode, and evaluate new experience. In

addition, schema can influence memory, both proactive and retroactive. For instance, beliefs,

such as “others cannot be trusted” may lead to expectations and misinterpretation of events in

ways that are congruent with that belief. Additionally, language and the particular labeling of

events can also transform experience, so that a man with “shifty eyes” will be experienced in a

way that may be much different from perceiving that same man to be “cautious, observant, or

thoughtful” (Andersson, 1994; Chiu, Krauss, & Lau, Whorf, 1956). Beck (1963, p. 120)

recognized that one of the deleterious effects of “inexact labeling” was that it contributed to

distortion. Consequently, expectations and language can distort appraisals of objective reality.

There are a number of extant cognitive processing models (see Ingram, Miranda & Segal,

1998 for a review), which at the most fundamental level, attempt to explain how individuals

process information and perform cognitive tasks that affect all aspects of behavior and emotion.

However, Kendall’s cognitive taxonomy model is the most relevant to the present research

(Ingram & Kendall, 1986; Kendall, 1992). Kendall theorized that human cognition consisted of

the following four fundamental elements:

1. Cognitive products or content: Thoughts, beliefs, and images. Self-referent speech

composed of stored and organized memories.

2. Cognitive processes: Attention, encoding, and retrieval procedures.

3. Cognitive structures. Mental templates that attend to or filter certain stimuli.

4. Cognitive products: Thoughts, assumptions, beliefs.

Kendall (1985, 1992) further delineated faulty cognitive processing into active, cognitive

distortion rather than cognitive deficiencies; the latter is postulated as the result from de ficits in

24

cognitive functioning. According to the model, dysfunctional cognition is the predictable

byproduct of maladaptive cognitive processing, manifested as cognitive distortions. However,

according to Yurica (2002) it is also possible that the failure to think more actively or mindfully

(Teasdale, Segal, Williams, Ridgeway, Soulsby, & Lau, 2000) distorts one’s experience.

Social and cognitive psychologists alike have observed that cognition occurs on two levels, one deep and effortful, and the other automatic and superficial (Chaiken & Trope, 1999;

Petty & Cacioppo, 1984; 1986). When individuals engage in more superficial thinking, they are more liable to make errors in judgment based on emotion or on salient, but not necessarily important aspect of the situation.

Consistent with this theory, there is increasing evidence that humans have at least two partially independent ways of reaching decisions about important (often social) events. The first uses an experiential system, employs heuristics, takes mental short cuts to reach judgments quickly, resorts to emotional reasoning, is automatic, seems to rely more on conditioning, and is congruent with early childhood schemas (Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Gilbert, 1998; Petty &

Cacioppo, 1984).

The second mode of cognitive processing involves a volitional, rational system, which operates more slowly as it integrates stored information from memory and knowledge. This system has the ability to incorporate logical deductive and abstract forms of reasoning in a more conscious way and without undue influence of past experience and conditioning (Chaiken &

Trope, 1999; Power & Brewin, 1991; Gilbert, 1998; Petty & Cacioppo, 1984).

Investigators have determined that it is possible to reduce superficial processing by increasing the perception of personal relevance (Petty & Caciappo, 1984) and allowing more time for deliberation (Ratneswar & Chaiken, 1991). For example, students are more likely to

25

reduce rational decision-making strategies regarding the time needed to study for an upcoming

exam when they believe that will be exempt. Most relevant to the cognitive model of

psychotherapy, personal experience fosters cognitive processes, cognitive structures, and

cognitive products that confirm expectancies whether those expectations are rational or are

distorted. This complicated cognitive interaction is consistent with the notion of cognitive or

confirmatory bias (Beck et al., 1979). In CT, patients learn to identify instances of heuristic,

distorted thinking and then they are encouraged to engage actively in more rational, mindful

thinking (e.g., Teasdale, Moore, Hayhurst, Pope, Williams, Segal, 2002).

Interestingly, it is also possible that cognitive distortions can be beneficial and,

conversely, that promoting more accurate thinking may produce distress. In other words, there

are instances when cognitive distortions may be adaptive and that accurate thinking may

correlate with maladaptive behavior and distress. For example, it was determined that although

depressed patients engaged in more negative cognition than nondepressed individuals, the

depressed patients were actually more accurate in their thinking (Alloy & Abramson, 1988).

Support for this depressive realism was also demonstrated in more accurate thinking among

depressed breast cancer patients, as compared with their nondepressed counterparts; this type of

realism motivated the depressed individuals to treatment adherence (Keller, Lipkus, & Rimer,

2002). Opponents of this theory caution that the phenomenon has been demonstrated in severely

limited settings and suffers from lack of empirically derived criteria for the term accuracy (e.g.,

Pacini, Muir, & Epstein, 1998; Vazquez, 1987).

In addition, comparing oneself favorably with those perceived to be less fortunate, that is,

downward social comparison (Wood, Taylor, & Lichtman, 1985), increased self-esteem and reduced stress among breast cancer patients. Similarly, the mere perception of control allowed

26

enhanced performance, inspired , and produced higher self-esteem (Tafarodi, Milne,

& Smith, 1999).

Thus, the research is not without controversy regarding the costs and benefits of distorted thinking. However, there is little debate that cognitive distortions are complex phenomena that occur outside of conscious awareness and correlate with a host of psychological disorders, dysfunctional behavior, and emotional problems.

Evolutionary Models of

Cognitive Distortions

In seeking to gain a deeper understanding of personality, Millon (1990) believed that it was necessary to explore the common elements of nature and science beyond the unnecessarily restrictive realm of human psychology. Evolution, Millon postulated, was the fundamental process that explained and unified nature, the sciences, and, more specifically, emotion, behavior, and cognition. Personality and PDs could be explained as attempts at adaptation and as a product of phylogenetic (specific to genus or group) as well as ontogenetic (specific to the individual) evolutionary progression.

Millon (1990; Millon et al, 1996) further asserted that personality was the individual’s distinctive attempt at adapting to a given environment. PDs were the result of maladaptive and deficient functioning. Such dysfunction was said to occur because of “protective constriction”

(akin to and avoidance) and cognitive distortion that restricted prospects for new learning, misconstrued essentially benign events, and provoked undesirable reactions from others; these problems foster a self-defeating vicious cycle of personal problems and new

27

interpersonal predicaments that further magnify distress and drive people further away from an

unbiased appraisal of reality (Millon et al, 1996, p. 15).

Gilbert (1998) viewed cognitive distortions as an evolutionarily adaptive response to the

perception of threat, rather than mere maladaptive thinking. Gilbert posited the idea that

“humans beings evolved to think adaptively, not rationally” (p. 448). Consistent with the

previously cited cognitive psychological research, Gilbert also asserted that negative thinking,

even when rational, might be dysfunctional at times.

For example, because the environment bombards people with so much data that it is

impossible to perceive, attend to, analyze, interpret and draw conclusions about every aspect of

all stimuli, humans are necessarily forced to make heuristic judgments about many events

(Kahneman & Tversky, 1972). This is especially true in the face of emergency situations—or

even the mere perception of threat—when there is little time to cogitate. For example, natural selection might favor an individual who responds to a encounter with a shadowy figure in a darkened alley with the heuristic judgment of threat and a subsequent fight or flight response; it might, therefore, favor another individual less who attempts logical discourse and careful analysis during such an event (Cosmides, 1989). In this case, distorted thinking in the form of a threat bias might prove to be adaptive. Thus, there are advantages to acting quickly and in favor of a threat bias.

There is also evidence that moderate positive is a variety of cognitive distortion that may be adaptive. For example, Krebs and Denton (1997) determined that when individuals operate in groups, self-serving attributions, overestimation of personal abilities, illusion of control, and increased risk-taking behavior correlated with greater self-esteem, improved affect, and more adaptive behavior. Gilbert (1998) offered the following evolutionary

28 explanation and some empirical support for seven cognitive distortions that are frequently cited in the literature:

1. Selective Abstraction. Beck et al. (1979) referred to a tendency to focus on

negative details out of context. Gilbert hypothesized that this tendency resulted from

attentional . There is now much evidence that mood states do affect attentional

mechanisms on tasks such as the Stroop Test (Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996) and

that attentional biases and processing can occur without conscious awareness (Power &

Brewin, 1991) and that the perception, for example, of threat can be adaptive.

2. Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusions (Beck et al., 1979). Emergency

situations require categorical thinking when determining whether or not an event poses a

threat. Decision-making is much more rapid when the number of possible choices is reduced,

for instance, to threat or no threat (Epstein, Lipson, Holstein, & Huh, 1992) . The principle of

“better safe than sorry” when dealing with threat will likely result in jumping to conclusions

and, according to Gilbert (1997; 1998) is the most salient cognitive distortion.

3. Dichotomous/All-or-Nothing Thinking (Beck et al., 1979). Similar to jumping to

conclusions, dichotomous thinking illustrates rapid categorical thinking. Once the brain

perceives threat, it will resort to more categorical processing to reduce response time and

risk, which will result in action that may reduce the threat (Epstein et al., 1994; Gilbert,

1998).

4. Emotional Reasoning (Burns, 1990). According to Gilbert (1998, p. 457), “… for

millions of years animals and humans have relied on fast track affect (emotional reasoning)

to make decisions about actions when under threat.” Once again, this is fast track thinking

29

that may increase genetic fitness in life and death situations by increasing the probability of

erring on the side of safety (Nesse, 1998).

5. Disqualifying the Positives (Burns, 1990). Downplaying one’s own attributes, as

manifested in the form of modesty, is appropriate and even admired in certain contexts,

cultures, and in moderation (Gilbert, 1998). Numerous studies support the hypothesis that

modest people are better liked than those who boast (Baumeister & Jones, 1978; Rosen,

Cochran, & Musser, 1990; Wosinka, Dabul, Whetston-Dion, & Cialdini, 1996). Of course,

being liked enhances one’s attractiveness and increases opportunities to mate and pass on

those modest traits (Langlois, & Musselman, 1995).

6. Social Comparison (Festinger, 1954). Both upward and downward social

comparison can enhance self-esteem, boost confidence, reduce stress, and encourage

perseverance on difficult tasks (Gibbons & Gerrard, 1989; 1991; Gilbert, 1998). It has long

been recognized that people compare themselves socially with others because it would be

adaptive to identify who is one’s superior or inferior before determining the most

advantageous way to interact with him or her. Comparison occurs even when this makes one

unhappy or depressed. Gilbert, (1998) posited the theory that social comparison is probably

one of the oldest forms of social cognition.

7. Personalization and Blame (Beck et al., 1979). Attributions of self-blame may be

adaptive in a number of ways. First, it may offer some illusion of control, because negative

events that are perceived to result from one’s own negligence should be controllable. Second,

blaming oneself avoids attacks on others as well as the resultant retaliation. As Gilbert (1998)

suggested, “Even if not expressed, the to counter-attack when blaming others (non-

submissive attitude) could be detected by the other and escalate the conflict” (p. 450). In

30

support of the adaptive nature of personalization of blame, Andrews & Brewin (1990), found

that abused women blamed themselves more for abuse while still in the abusive relationship,

but blamed their partners more after separation; that is, when the women were removed to a

safe distance, the risk of counter-attacks from the partners were reduced. Self-blame may

also elicit secondary reinforcement in the form of social support (Driscoll, 1988).

Gilbert (1998) concluded that evolutionary explanations for cognitive distortions have implications for CT. For example, framing such distortions as the product of normal adaptations that, with treatment may be modulated, normalizes the patient’s responses, provides insight, and increases mindfulness. Although these seemingly automatic cognitive mechanisms may have been adaptive throughout our evolutionary history, they can, however, cause immense problems today, in creating, maintaining, and exacerbating psychopathology. The question remains, how do cognitive distortions arise on the ontogenetic level?

Developmental Theories, Cognition, and

Cognitive Distortions

Yurica (2002) considered many of the following developmental theories when formulating the ICD. Head and Holmes (1911) were among the first to use the term schema.

These investigators were interested in patients’ spatial assessment of their own bodies, referring to their perceptions as the postural schema. Later, Piaget (1952) conceptualized the idea that the human mind organized schema as internalized regularities (operations) into dynamic cognitive structures, which he termed schema. Schema represented the relationships between perceived environmental regularities, or concepts. With experience, humans develop associations and

31

perceive relationships among concepts and develop a rich variety of schema, which encompass

virtually every aspect of life.

Erikson (1963) proposed that schema or unconditional core beliefs develop when

individuals are infants and that children progress through four critical psychosocial stages of

development, occurring at circumscribed junctures from infancy through puberty. Layden,

Newman, Freeman, & Morse, (1993) produced a seminal work on the development of borderline

patients by integrating Erikson and Piaget’s theories into a cognitive

framework. Synthesizing Erikson’s theory into a cognitive model of schema development,

Layden et al. (1993) suggested that failure at specific Eriksonian stages predisposes individuals

to acquiring maladaptive schemas. Success or failure at each of these stages results in the

following, respective outcomes:

Stage 1: Ages birth to 18 months. versus mistrust. Mistrust and abandonment

schemas develop in response to failure to develop loving and trusting relationships

Stage 2: Ages: 18 months to 3 years. Autonomy versus and . Failure to

develop physical control, such as in walking, grasping, and rectal-sphincter response may result

in schemas regarding competence which are related to shame and .

Stage 3: Ages 3 to 6 years. Initiative versus : Failure to take initiative or being unusually assertive may lead to schemas related to dependence and emotional deprivation

Stage 4: Ages 6 to 12 years. Industry versus inferiority: Inability to deal with the growing demands associated with new skills may lead to schemas of incompetence. Additional negative schemas of unlovability and defectiveness may result at any stage (Layden et al., 1993).

Although infants begin with scant cognitive processing skills, these abilities, along with reasoning, and problem-solving skills develop as infants interact with their new social and

32

physical environment (Piaget, 1952). As skills develop at certain phases of development, so too

may inaccurate thinking patterns or cognitive distortions (Layden et al., 1993). Layden et al.

further posited that cognitive distortions may also develop at discrete stages of cognitive

development as proposed by Piaget (1952), as follows:

Stage 1: Ages birth to 2 years. The Sensory Motor Stage. Consistent with Piaget’s notion

of and a lack of object permanence occurring at this phase, Layden et al. (1993)

propose that the cognitive distortion of mind reading, in addition to and lack of

develop in BPD patients at this stage.

Stage 2: Ages 2 to 7 years. The Preoperational Stage. During this stage, children think

concretely but are unable to engage in abstract logic. However, many cognitive distortions may

develop at this stage. For instance, when decentration or the ability to see the whole in

relationship to the parts, fails to develop adequately, Layden et al. (1993) proffer the idea that the

cognitive distortion of overgeneralization can result as individuals categorize similar objects on the basis of only one or of unimportant characteristics. Additionally, the cognitive distortions of emotional reasoning, dichotomous thinking, catastrophization, and perfectionism are thought to develop during this phase.

Stage 3: Ages 7 to 11 years. The Concrete Operations Stage. With the accumulation of

physical experience, the children begin to conceptualize, creating logical structures to explain

physical experiences. This plants the seed for abstract reasoning, such as in arithmetic equations.

Stage 4: Ages 11 to 15 years of age. The child becomes capable of abstract thought, and

meatacognition—the capacity to think about thinking—allowing them to see wide varieties of

possible solutions for increasingly complex pr oblems.

33

Layden et al. (1993) asserted that individuals with BPD exhibit a lack of cognitive development during Stages three and four. This produces deficits in metacognition, theory building, generation of alternatives, imaginal hypothesis testing, and a lack of deductive and inductive reasoning skills; these may, in turn, distort reality, prevent adaptive functioning, and produce immense interpersonal difficulties.

To summarize, the synthesis of the cognitive model with the Erikson’s and Piaget’s developmental theories provides a cogent explanation for the generation of maladaptive schemas and cognitive distortions. Moreover, the deficits in cognitive adaptation from these earlier stages of cognitive development are quite appropriate for reciprocal CT techniques intended to reduce the distortions, including “… teaching patients alternative ways of thinking (e.g., examining the evidence, weighing the evidence, generating alternative explanations, imagining hypothetical scenarios” and self-monitoring (Yurica, 2002, p. 37).

The above models, cognitive processing, evolutionary, and developmental, provide elegant and convincing evidence for the genesis, maintenance, process, and amelioration of cognitive distortions. The following is an explication of how these maladaptive thought patterns are expressed in psychological disorders across Axis I and Axis II; an explation of treatment procedures is also included.

34

The Efficacy of Cognitive Therapy

CT is based on the principle that human emotion and behavior are heavily influenced by

cognition. Thus, consistent with cognitive theory, personality and clinical pathology correlate

with cognitive distortion (e.g., Beck, 1976; Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004; Yurica,

2002).

In research and practice, CT enjoys considerable empirical support for its efficacy in treating

many frequently occurring disorders, not only in a broad array of populations, but also in all

formats, such as individual, couples, group and family, and in both inpatient and outpatient

populations (Beck, 1993). Butler and J. Beck (2000) evaluated 14 meta-analyses, examining the efficacy of CT in 325 studies with a total of 9,138 participants. Meta-analysis is a statistical strategy that permits researchers to aggregate the results of several studies and to translate them into standard units known as an effect sizes. In their review, these investigators examined how

CT outcomes compared with the outcomes of various control groups in terms of their effect sizes, including the percentage of those participants’ responses that was superior to a variety of no-treatment controls. Table 1 clearly illustrates the efficacy of CT for a host of Axis I conditions.

35

Table 1

Summary of meta-analytic findings: Cognitive therapy vs. no-treatment, wait list, and placebo controls (Butler & Beck , 2000).

SUMMARY OF META-ANALYTIC FINDINGS Comparisons of cognitive therapy to no-treatment, wait list, and placebo controls.

% of CT Average effect patients size superior to Disorder controls

Adult unipolar depression .82 79% Adolescent unipolar depression 1.11 87% Generalized anxiety disorder 1.04 85% Panic disorder with or without agoraphobia .91 82% Social phobia .93 82% Childhood depression and anxiety disorders .90 82% Marital distress .71 76% Anger .70 76% Childhood somatic disorders .47 68% Chronic (not headache) .46 68%

Note. Empirical evidence for the efficacy of CT over controls in magnitude of effect and percentage of superior response. Reprinted with the permission of Judith Beck. Source: http://www.beckinstitute.org/beck.html

36

Cognitive Therapy for Depression

The efficacy of CT for depression is now widely accepted. Empirical studies have demonstrated the relationship between positive CT outcome and cognition. For example, Oei and

Sullivan (1999) found that participants who had recovered from depression, operationalized as

BDI scores below 10, engaged in less frequent negative automatic thoughts and reported higher activity levels than their non-recovered counterparts.

In their comprehensive meta-analysis, Butler and Beck (2000) demonstrated the fact that

79% of adult unipolar patients responded to CT for depression with an average effect size of .82.

Even better results were found for adolescents, with 87% responding and an average effect size of 1.11. This meta-analysis also determined that CT was superior to antidepressant medication with an average effect size of .38 for adult unipolar depression. Perhaps the greatest benefit that

CT offered as being superior to medication alone was the fact that CT patients experienced less than half the relapse rate of medicated patients at one-year follow up, 30% vs. 60%, respectively.

Additional mounting evidence also suggests that CT reduces relapse and recurrence of depressive episodes. For example, a number of studies found that outpatients who recovered from major depression following CT experienced less subsequent relapse or perceived need for treatment than patients who recovered using pharmacotherapy; they are subsequently withdrawn from antidepressant medication (Blackburn, Eunson, & Bishop, 1986; Evans, Hollon, DeRubeis,

Piasecki, Grove, Garvey et al, 1992; Shea, Elkin, Imber, Sotsky, Watkins, Collins, et al., 1992;

Simons, Murphy, Levine, & Wetzel, 1986). The utilization of CT following recovery with

37

pharmacotherapy also reduced subsequent relapse and recurrence of depression (Fava, Grandi,

Zielezny, Rafanelli, Canestrari, 1996; Fava, Rafanelli, Grandi, Conti, & Belluardo, 1998). Even

for patients responding only partially to antidepressant medication, the addition of CT to clinical management and continuing antidepressant medication significantly reduced rates of relapse

(Paykel, Scott, Teasdale, Garland, Moore, Jenaway et al., 1999).

Additional evidence also suggests that CT reduces relapse and recurrence of depressive

episodes. For example, outpatients who recovered from major depression following CT

experienced less subsequent relapse or perceived need for treatment than patients who recovered

using pharmacotherapy and then were subsequently withdrawn from antidepressant medication

(Blackburn, et al., 1986; Evans, Hollon, DeRubeis, Piasecki, Grove, Garvey, & Tuason, 1992;

Shea, Elkin, Imber, Sotsky, Watkins, Collins et al, 1992; Simons, Murphy, Levine, & Wetzel,

1986). CT following recovery from pharmacotherapy also reduced subsequent relapse and

recurrence of depression (Fava, Grandi, Zielezny, Rafanelli, & Canestrari, 1996; Fava, Rafanelli,

Grandi, Conti, & Belluardo, 1998).

In a controlled clinical trial, Teasdale, Moore, Hayhurst, Pope, Williams, Segal (2002)

determined that the prevention of relapse from depression was mediated through changes in an

all-or-none, absolutistic thinking style, operationalized as an extreme dichotomous negative or

positive response set to a number of self-report instruments; these included the Attributional

Style Questionnaire (Peterson et al., 1982), Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Weissman & Beck,

1978), and the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1979). Dichotomous distortions were

inferred when questions received responses similar to the following: Causes of negative

outcomes "will never again be present." Interestingly, such extreme responding has been

determined as a marker of rapid, seemingly automatic processing, which is also consistent with

38

negative correlations between attitude extremity and response latency, as previously reported

(e.g., Bargh et al., 1992). Teasedale (et al., 2002) affirmed the pivotal role of such automatic

cognitive distortions as predicted by the cognitive model of depression. Also supported was the

hypothesis that relapse into depression was correlated with the sum total of dichotomous

cognitive distortions.

Depression Complicated by Personality Disorders

Millon, Davis, & Millon (1997) conceptualize Axis I clinical symptoms as “extensions

or distortions of the patient’s basic personality pattern... waxing and waning over time depending

on the impact of stressful situations.” (p. 22). The multiaxial model posits an interaction between

Axis I and Axis II symptomatology in which personality is defined as the “…overall capacity to

perceive and to cope with our psychosocial world…” (Millon et al., 1997, p.120). Personality is

further conceptualized as analogous to an immune system, protecting the individual from the

vicissitudes of external stressors. However, Axis I symptoms may occur because the PD patient’s

dysfunctional “immune systems” fails to protect the individual from stress. When this occurs,

Axis I clinical syndromes occur in addition to the preexisting PD symptoms. This increases the

total severity of the individual’s dysfunction (Millon et al., 1997) . In other words, all other things

being equal, an individual who meets the diagnostic criteria for both major depressive disorder

and dependent personality disorder would be expected to evince more impairment and/or distress

than another person with a diagnosis on either Axis I or Axis II.

According to Hirschfeld and Shea’s (1992) model, like a compromised

immune system which fails to insulate one from disease, the distorted cognition associated with

39

PD’s may leave one vulnerable to the stressors that are noted on Axis IV. As Millon et al. (1997,

p. 124) assert:

Faced with recurrent failures, anxious about old and unresolved conflicts reemerging, and

unable to recruit new adaptive strategies, they (the personality disordered) may revert to

pathological ways of coping, to less adequate control over , and ultimately, to

increasingly subjective and distorted perceptions of reality and to the production of clinical

symptoms.

This leads to a pattern in which cognitive distortion can constrict behavior in a fashion that may actually exacerbate existing difficulties, leading to vicious circles of cognitive distortion,

distress, and interpersonal impairment. When this occurs, one’s cognition may be further

impaired by the resulting Axis I clinical syndromes. Prolonged Axis I symptoms can, in turn,

exacerbate personality dysfunction.

This reciprocal interaction between Axis I and Axis II explains why Millon et al. (1997)

posited the notion that the presence of Axis I pathology supports the presence of personality

pathology. Consequently, it is expected that individuals with greater total pathology, independent

of whether this pathology arises from Axis I or Axis II, will experience more frequent cognitive

distortions. Cognitive distortions have been reported in PDs (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al.,

2004) and account even for an overlap between some PDs, such as borderline and schizotypal

(Rosenberger & Miller, 1989).

Further evidence for the additive effects of depression and Axis II syndromes is offered by studies in which depressed patients with at least one PD were found to have greater frequency of previous episodes of depression, higher levels of distress, and more severe symptomatology

(Diguer, Barber, & Luborsky, 1993; Farmer & Nelson-Gray, 1990; Shea et al., 1987). Other

40

evidence suggests that personality disordered inpatients tend to suffer from earlier initial onset of

illness, longer duration of episode, and more suicidal thoughts and/or suicide attempts than those

without Axis II comorbidity (Black, Bell, Hulbert, & Nasrallah, 1988).

Moreover, Skodol, Gundserson, MacGlashan, & Dyck, (2002) recently conducted the first study to document and quantify the extent of functional impairment experienced by patients with varying types of PDs in contrast to patients having only an impairing Axis I disorder. This study

supported Millon et al.s’ (1997) contention that schizotypal and borderline personality disorders

are more severe, because they produced greater dysfunction on virtually every measure of

impairment than patients with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder or major depressive

disorder alone produced, even after controlling for comorbid Axis I psychopathology and

regardless of whether the assessment was interview-based or by patient self-report. Avoidant

personality disorder impairment was intermediate. No gender effects were found. It should be

noted schizotaypal, borderline, and to a lesser extent, avoidant personality disordered patients

were significantly more impaired at work, in social relationships, and at leisure than patients with

obsessive-compulsive personality disorder or major depressive disorder. These authors

convincingly concluded “Personality disorders are a significant source of psychiatric morbidity,

accounting for more impairment in functioning than major depressive disorder alone.”

Most patients with PDs enter treatment, seeking relief for an Axis I disorder. There have

been a number of studies evaluating treatment response in those meeting criteria for comorbid

Axis I and Axis II conditions. Ilardi and Craighead (1999) found that, in formerly depressed

patients, rates of relapse into depression occurred 7.4 times faster for patients with comorbid PDs

than for those without Axis II personality pathology. Additionally, Axis II pathology accounted for approximately 29% of the variance in dysfunctional attitudes (as measured by the DAS) and

41

14% of the variance in maladaptive negative event attributions (as measured by the ASQ-N), regardless of whether or not the patient relapsed into depression. This dysfunctional thinking may suggest depressogenic vulnerability in individuals with Axis II disorders. Interestingly, for each Axis II criterion met, there was an 8% decrease in expected survival of remission.

Moreover, there was a significant interaction between specific PD Clusters and outcome with

Clusters B and C associated with shorter remission times. Surprisingly, the presence of Cluster A diagnoses was actually associated with longer remission. Otherwise, analysis of Axis II clusters was largely consistent with a hypothesized general personality pathology factor associated with

dysfunctional cognitions. This study illustrates the fact that more effective identification and

amelioration of dysfunctional cognitions associated with comorbid PDs may pr ovide gr eat

benefit not only for predicting outcome, but also for combating both personality pathology and

depression. Thus, more effective identification and amelioration of dysfunctional cognitions

associated with PDs and depression may provide great benefit for predicting outcome and

combating depression.

Barber and Muenz (1996) found CT to be significantly more effective than interpersonal

therapy (IPT) in treating depression in Avoidant PD patients. Although IPT and CT were both

effective in treating PD patients with comorbid depression, those with obsessive personality

disorder (OCPD) responded significantly better when treated with IPT rather than CT. However,

CT was significantly superior to IPT for depressed patients with comorbid avoidant PD. It was hypothesized the IPT differentially addressed perfectionistic, internal coping strategies characteristic of OCPD, whereas CT was more effective in ameliorating the externalized coping

strategies, such as the need for approval, more common to avoidant PD. Nevertheless, common

to both of these effective treatment modalities is that both CT and IPT, in part, ameliorate

42

dysfunctional cognition (e.g., Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004; Klerman, Weissman,

Rounsaville, & Chevron, 1984).

Additional evidence for the interactive and additive nature of Axis I and Axis II

symptomatology arises from a naturalistic study demonstrating an important fact; in a

comparison of depressed patients without a personality disorder and of patients with depression

and PDs, the latter reported more severe depressive symptoms at intake and more residual

problems at the conclusion of CT. However, there was no evidence that patients with depression

and PDs benefited less from CT than depressed patients without a PD. Rather, treatment outcome

was negatively influenced by avoidant and paranoid beliefs whether or not a PD was diagnosed.

Specifically, avoidant beliefs predicted changes in self-reported depressive symptoms and

paranoid beliefs predicted changes in therapist-rated general functioning (Kuyken, Kurzer,

DeRubeis, Beck, & Brown, 2001).

Similarly, Hardy, Barkham, Shapiro, Stiles, Rees, & Reynolds, (1995) found that patients with depression, which was complicated by Cluster C PDs also presented with more severe symptomatology than patients with uncomplicated depression. Interestingly, there was an interaction between the presence of severe PD and treatment modality in outcome. At posttreatment and one-year follow-up, there was no significant difference between complicated and uncomplicated depressed patients when treated with CBT. However, severely depressed PD patients who were treated with psychodynamic—interpersonal psychotherapy continued to maintain more severe symptomatology at posttreatment and at 1-year follow-up. This lends further support to the central role of dysfunctional cognition, targeted, specifically by CBT, in the maintenance and treatment of Axis I and Axis II conditions.

43

Cognitive Therapy for Anxiety Disorders

Butler and Beck (2000) included many of the following studies in their meta-analysis of treatment outcome for CT with anxiety disorders:

Panic Disorder

CT research has been particularly impressive in regard to panic disorder. For example,

Sokol, Beck, Greenberg, Wright, & Berchick (1989) found CT to be significantly superior to supportive therapy in eliminating panic attacks. Another study found CT to be significantly more effective than behavior therapy, imipramine, and placebo control in ameliorating panic attacks at the end of treatment, and at 1-year follow-up (Clark, 1991). Another important advantage of CT was far lower attrition rates, as compared with behavior therapy (often employing aversive exposure methods) or wait-list controls.

Gould et al. (1995) conducted a meta-analysis comparing cognitive interventions with more behavioral interventions based on exposure treatments and various control conditions. The most effective treatments were those that combined with interoceptive exposure (ES = 0.88), the mainstay of the traditional CT approach (e.g., Barlow, 1988; Taylor,

2000). CT was superior to pharmacological interventions (ES = 0.47) or combination treatments

(ES = 0.56). One-year follow indicated excellent resiliency of treatment effects, with virtually no slippage in effect size. Another advantage for CT was a significantly lower attrition rate, with only 5.6% of those treated with CT relapsing within the first year after treatment, compared with

44

19.8% of those treated pharmacologically. Finally, Oei et al. (1999) demonstrated that CT

reduced panic symptoms to levels near or below those found in the general population.

Social Phobia

Butler & Beck (2000) also reviewed meta-analyses related to CT for social phobia (Feske

& Chambless, 1995; Gould, Buckminster, Pollack, Otto, Yap, 1997). Gould et al. (1997) found

that CT was superior to exposure interventions without cognitive restructuring (ES = 0.89) and,

surprisingly, the combination of the two interventions (ES = .80) Consistent with studies on

numerous other Axis I disorders, CT was also superior to wait-list and placebo attention controls

(ES = 0.93) for treating social phobia.

Generalized Anxiety Disorder

CT has also been found to be effective in treating Axis I conditions, such as generalized anxiety disorder (Butler Fennell, Robson, & Gelder, 1991). In a meta-analysis of studies comparing CT with controls and pharmacotherapy for treating generalized anxiety disorder,

Gould, Otto, Pollack, and Yap (1997) determined that CT was significantly more effective than nondirective therapy, no-treatment controls, wait list controls, or placebo controls. However, CT combined with drug therapy, the latter consisting mainly of Benzodiazepines, were not statistically different at post-treatment.

45

Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

According to (Butler & Beck, 2000) exposure and response prevention is often the frontline psychological treatment of choice for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; e.g., Riggs

& Foa, 1993). However, Abramowitz (1997) found no significant differences in effect sizes

when comparing the exposure and response prevention with CT for treating OCD. Simailarly,

Van Balkom, van Oppen, Vermeulen, van Dyck, Nauta, & Vorst, (1994) determined, via meta- analysis, that both cognitive and cognitive-behavioral treatments resulted in substantial reductions in OCD symptoms.

Anxiety Complicated by Personality Disorders

Although PDs are subject to increasing investigation (Reich, 2003; Ruegg & Allen, 1995), relatively few studies have focused on comorbid anxiety and PD, as compared with comorbid depression and Axis II disorders. This issue has important ramifications for assessment and treatment, as well, because of a growing emphasis on the use and cost of mental health services

(Phillips, Shea, Warshaw & Dyck, 2001).

Three naturalistic, prospective, longitudinal studies of panic disorder (Faravelli & Albanesi,

1987; Noyes, Reich, Christiansen, Suelzer, Pfohl & Coryell, 1990; Pollack, Otto, Rosenbaum,

Sachs, O'Neil, Asher et al., 1990) reported that the presence of PDs was associated with poor treatment outcome for panic disorder symptoms and functioning.

Moreover, another longitudinal study linked panic disorder and comorbid PDs with poor treatment outcome for panic disorder symptoms and functioning (Pollack, Otto, Rosenbaum,

46

Sachs, O'Neil, Asher, Meltzer-Brody, 1990; Pollack Otto, Rosenbaum, & Sachs 1992). However, it should be noted that this study was limited by the use of self-report PD instruments and a briefer follow-up of only 1 year. Although convincing, the empirical data regarding the contribution of PDs to anxiety disorder outcome is limited, inconsistent, and contradictory.

Other studies have shown that the presence of PDs decreases treatment effectiveness for specific anxiety disorders. For example, in a multisite, pr ospective, longitudinal, naturalistic study assessing PDs over a five-year period, Massion, Dyck, Shea, Phillips, Warshaw, & Keller

(2002) found that the presence of a personality disorder predicted a 39% lower likelihood of social phobia remission and a 30% lower likelihood of remission GAD, even after controlling for the effects of major depression. However, contrary to previous naturalistic studies, these researchers determined that PDs had no effect on remission form panic disorder remission. But the effects of PDs on anxiety were differential. Specifically, dependent personality disorder predicted a 41% lower probability of remission from GAD; avoidant personality disorder was associated with a 34% reduced chance for remission in GAD, and a 14% reduced remission rate in social phobia. Surprisingly, the presence of borderline personality disorder failed to predict negative outcome.

In other studies of generalized anxiety disorder, Yonkers, Warshaw, Massion, & Keller

(1996) reported an interaction effect between personality and clinical disorders. These investigators demonstrated the fact that the presence of cluster B or C PDs were differentially negative treatment indicators. Avoidant personality disorder predicted a poor outcome for both generalized anxiety disorder and social phobia. It is interesting to note that the presence of a PD diagnosis has deleterious treatment effects even in pharmacotherapy (Versiani, Nardi, Mindim,

Pinto, Saboya, & Kovacs, 1996; Versiani & Amrein, 1997). Accurately assessing Axis II

47

pathology is crucial because another study found that fully 37% of individuals meeting criteria

for social phobia also met criteria for two PDs, namely, avoidant and OCPD (Turner, Beidel,

Borden, Stanley, Jacob, 1991).

In summary, it appears that when Axis I disorders are complicated by PDs, outcome is often less favorable. The literature has long reflected that the presence of PDs may correlate with clinical syndromes to a degree that is far more than anticipated and that such comorbidity may impede treatment success (see Reich, 2003 for review). This is consistent with the notion that there may be an additive effect when comorbidity is present whether or not the disorders appear on either of the two DSM-IV-TR diagnostic axes (Millon et al, 1997). It should be noted that most patients with PDs do benefit from treatment, but not always as much as those without Axis

II pathology (Beck et al., 2004). Additionally, cognitive distortions have demonstrated a correlation with a number of Axis I and Axis II conditions and CT has been shown to be effective in ameliorating these distorted thinking patterns and subsequent dysphoria and impairment (Beck et al., 2004). These facts also illustrate the need for adequate assessment prior to treatment planning. In research and in practice, CT enjoys considerable empirical support for treating many frequently occurring disorders across Axis I and Axis II, in a broad array of populations and formats (Beck, 1993).

48

Demonstration of Treatment Outcome

Outcome and Measures of Cognitive Distortion

According to Pickstone (2001), the contemporary revolution in healthcare, in general, has

been stunning. Clinicians have come under increasing pressure to demonstrate treatment

effectiveness in the modern cost-accountable and resource-limited world. Forces militating for

efficiency include managed care, rising costs, demands of distraught patients, and therapists’

own to alleviate distress. Clinicians are under increasing pressure to demonstrate the fact that their methods are not only effective, but also efficient. Pressure arises, in large part, from managed care insurers who may limit the number of sessions for which they reimburse clinicians

for specific disorders. This has spurred the need for clinicians to provide more effective therapy

in less time (McDaniel, 1995). This is especially true of CT and CBT practitioners, who

endeavor to provide effective, brief therapy (Beck, 1996). As a consequence, although motivation may vary, insurers, patients and clinicians ultimately share the same goal: salubrious outcome provided in an optimally expeditious manner (Budman, & Gurman, 1988).

Demonstrating treatment effectiveness requires objective, psychometrically sound

measures. According to Kazdin (1998), self-report instruments provide cost- and time-efficient

tools to establish baseline data, assess current levels of functioning, supply a basis to modify

treatment strategies, and substantiate treatment outcome. Although the foundation of CT and

CBT is the exploration and modification of cognition (e.g., Beck, 1979; Burns, 1999), relatively

few instruments have been validated to detail adequately one of the most fundamental cognitive

process involved in psychological dysfunction, specifically, cognitive distortions (Yurica, 2002).

49

At this time, in fact, the literature reflects only five instruments with acceptable psychometric properties that profess to measure some aspect of cognitive distortions. In chronological order of publication, these measures include: The Cognitive Errors Questionnaire

(CEQ; Lefebvre, 1981), The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS-A, DAS-B; Weisman, 1979),

The Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ; Hollon & Kendall, 1980), The Cognitive

Distortions Scale (CDS; Briere, 2001), and The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions (ICD; Yurica

& DiTomasso, 2002. In research, measures of cognitive distortion have been used to document levels of distress and psychopathology (e.g., Yurica, 2002), response to psychotropic medication

(Meyer, Kenedy, Korman, Brown, DaSilva, et al., 2003), response to CT and CBT (DeRubeis,

Evans, Hollon, Garvey, Grove & Tuason, 1990), and even as predictors by organizational and consumer psychologists (Netmeyer, Williamson, Burton, & Biswas, 2002). A review of the professional literature at this time indicates that the DAS and, to a lesser extent the CEQ, have become used increasingly for the measure of cognitive distortion in research. However, no data exists regarding their application in clinical practice. The question arises: How can one best measure cognitive distortion?

Measuring Cognitive Distortions

Cognitive distortions are generally assessed via self-report inventories. A number of such instruments have been produced purporting to measure features of cognition and behavior relating to specific Axis I disorders. Examples of such instruments include the Beck Depression

Inventory—II (BDI—II; Beck, Steer, & Brown 1996) the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck,

& Steer, 1990), the Panic Attack Cognition Questionnaire (PAC; Clum, Boyles, Bordin, &

50

Watkins, 1990) the Agoraphobia Cognitions Questionnaire (ACQ; Chambless, Caputo, Bright, &

Gallagher, 1984), Social Anxiety Thoughts Questionnaire (SAT; Hartman, 1984), and the of

Negative Evaluation Scale (Watson & Friend, 1983). Although these instruments assess distorted

cognitions, they are limited because they identify only cognitions related to specific disorders or

syndromes.

On the other hand, additional instruments have been developed to assess the quantity and

content of positive and negative cognitions, such as the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

(ATQ; Hollon & Kendall, 1980) the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire—Revised (ATQ-R;

Kendall, Howard, & Hayes, 1989), and the Cognitive Triad Inventory (CTI; Beckham, Leber,

Watkins, Boyer, & Cook, 1986), Another instrument, the Young Schema Questionnaire-Long

Form, 2 nd ed. (YSQ-L2; Young & Brown, 1990), assesses cognition for purposes of identifying

underlying schema. Finally, several inventories have been developed to measure irrational

beliefs and attitudes such as the Rational Behavior Inventory (RBI; Shorkey & Whiteman, 1987),

the Irrational Values Scale (IVS; MacDonald, 1972) the Irrational Beliefs Scale (IBS; Malouff &

Schute, 1986) and the General Attitude and Belief Scale (GABS; DiGiuseppe, Leaf, Exner, &

Robin). Although these instruments assess constructs related to cognitive distortion, they cannot

claim to quantify specific distortions or their severity.

A literature review revealed five instruments specifically designed to assess the construct of

cognitive distortions in a clinical context. These instruments are as follows: The Dysfunctional

Attitude Scale (DAS; Weissman & 1979), the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ, Hollon

& Kendall, 1980), the Cognitive Error Questionnaire (CEQ; Lefebvre, 1981), the Cognitive

Distortion Scale (CDS; Briere, 2000), and the Inventory of Cognitive Distortions (Yurica &

DiTomasso, 2002). Of these measures, only the ICD assesses the frequency of 11 factor-

51 analyzed cognitive distortions culminating in a Total Cognitive Distortion score—the dependent variable of in the present study.

However, most of these instruments suffer from a number of critical limitations.

Limitations of Existing Measures of Cognitive Distortion

Many of the existing measures of cognitive distortion are lacking in a number of ways that are relevant to the present research. Specifically, the ATQ (Hollon & Kendall, 1980), DAS

(Weissman & 1979), and CEQ (Lefebvre, 1981) were designed for assessing cognitive distortions attendant to depression. Furthermore, the DAS yields only a total distortion score.

This is problematic in research because the DAS fails to illuminate a clear picture about which specific distortions correlate with particular disorders. Moreover, in assessment and clinical practice, the DAS is designed to assess dysfunctional beliefs and fails to identify many important distortions that may be targeted in treatment.

Second, of the existing measures of cognitive distortion, only the ICD measures 11 different cognitive distortions on separate subscales, thereby offering a significant advantage over both the

DAS and CEQ, because the DAS is limited to only six types of distortions (arbitrary inference, overgeneralization, selective abstraction, magnification or minimization, dichotomous reasoning, and personalization), whereas the CEQ is further limited to only four varieties of distortion

(overgeneralization, arbitrary inference, selective abstraction, and magnification or minimization).

Third, although the CDS (Brierre, 2000) is more useful for identifying specific cognitive distortions, it is still limited because it yields only the following five distortions: helplessness,

52

hopelessness, self-, self-blame, and preoccupation with danger. Consequently, the

instrument fails to identify a number of important theoretical cognitive distortions, such as those

identified by Yurica (2002).

Third, as Yurica (2002) explicated, all four instruments lack specificity in their definitions of

the term cognitive distortions, and suffer from “… poor consensus in definition, variable

measurement across instruments, limited applicability, and outdated, limited scope of the

measurement of cognitive distortions….” (p. 57). However, of all of these measures, only the

ICD assesses the frequency of Yurica’s (2002) 11 factor-analyzed cognitive distortions and

yields a total score of cognitive distortions—the dependent variable of interest in the present

study.

The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions

The ICD (Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002) provides the latest and most comprehensive,

structured, psychometrically validated self-report instrument for measuring cognitive distortions

in a heterogeneous, adult, clinical, outpatient population. The ICD is a 69-item self-report

inventory designed specifically to measure the frequency of distorted cognitions in an outpatient

clinical population. The instrument consists of single sentence items, answered on a five-point

Likert scale. The ICD provides scores on 11 factors/cognitive distortions. According to Yurica

(2002), the ICD was specifically designed to assess “self-statement cognitive products representative of particular types of cognitive distortions in differing mental health disorders” (p.

103). CT experts and factor analysis have established good content validity. Three cognitive therapy experts came to a 100% independent agreement on the original 69 items.

53

Yurica (2002) explicitly designed the ICD to validate the 16 theorized cognitive distortions

(Beck et al., 1979; Burns, 1980, 1999; Freeman & DeWolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).

However, of the original theorized cognitive distortions, factor analysis revealed 11 fundamental

factors that closely resembled 10 theory-derived cognitive distortion subscales (Externalization of Self-Worth, Fortune-Telling, Magnification, Labeling, Perfectionism, Comparison to Others,

Emotional Reasoning, Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusion, Minimization, Mind-

Reading), in addition to one important new cognitive distortion (Emotional Reasoning and

Decision-Making). It should be noted that the ICD did not retain the theoretical construct of

overgeneralization because this was the only distortion that failed to receive a 100% agreement

among independent cognitive therapy experts.

Despite convincing theoretical and empirical evidence for the fundamental role of cognitive

distortions in both the cognitive model and cognitive-behavioral therapies, there has been

surprisingly little effort to objectively quantify the frequency with which these cognitive events

occur. In fact, to date, only two instruments, the ICD and the CDS, have been empirically

validated via factor-analysis of the theoretical cognitive distortions to test the frequency of

cognitive distortions with a clinical sample of heterogeneous, adult, mental health outpatients.

ICD also offers several advantages over other measures of cognitive distortion. First, it provides

the latest factor analysis of the original theorized cognitive distortions. Second, it identifies and

evaluates a larger number of cognitive distortions than any of the other competing instruments.

Third, the distortions identified by the ICD have been demonstrated to span diagnostic

categories, rather than being restricted to a particular diagnosis (Yurica, 2002).

Yurica (2002) determined that the ICD significantly and positively correlated with widely

accepted measures of depression, the (BDI-II) anxiety (BAI), and dysfunctional attitudes (DAS).

54

Comparing scores of outpatient psychiatric patients (n=122) with a comparison control group

(n=66), the ICD demonstrated impressive internal consistency, having a total scale estimate of

internal reliability measuring a Coefficient alpha of .998 (N=28), indicating good homogeneity

of item content. Additionally, the ICD total scale scores produced test-retest reliability of r = .98, indicating good stability. Patterns of cognitive distortion appeared to be stable and enduring, even after a cognitively and emotionally traumatic event of historic proportion, the attacks of

September 11, 2001. Such an enduring pattern of cognition is more consistent with trait- versus

state-like thinking. However, it is also possible that such an unprecedented mass trauma may

have artificially affected ICD reliability.

Nonetheless, such reliability is consistent with Alford and Beck’s (1997) revised cognitive

theory, in which cognitive distortion was postulated to include both errors in content/meaning,

on the one hand, and cognitive processing/meaning elaboration, on the other hand. It was

proposed that predisposition to specific cognitive distortions or cognitive vulnerabilities leaves

individuals susceptible to specific disorders. This proposal is also supported by research of the

interaction of parenting and children’s coping styles, as well as developmental theories relating

to cognitive distortions (Yurica, 2002).

In summary, Yurica, (2002) found a robust and significantly positive correlation between

cognitive distortions on the one hand, and anxiety, depressive symptoms and dysfunctional

attitudes, on the other hand. A review of the literature revealed no similar research examining the

relationship between the frequency of the 11 factor analyzed cognitive distortions and Axis II

conditions. Thus, this study endeavors to expand further the utility of this promising instrument

into additional specific clinical syndromes, and beyond that, into the personality disorder

domain.

55

Outcome and Measures of Psychopathology

The Minnesota Multipahasic Personality Inventory-2

A number of widely accepted, standardized self-report measures exist for psychopathology outcome measurement. These instruments are also employed for the diagnostic classification of research participants and clinical patients. Such measures are valuable because they provide an indication both of the number and severity of clinical disorders, mainly along accepted nosology. Since Hathaway & McKinley (1943) originally developed The Minnesota

Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), it has become the most widely used and researched clinical assessment inventory, with over 10,000 published research references (Groth-Marnat,

1999). Despite its enormous value, however, its most recent incarnation, the MMPI-2 (Butcher,

Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989), has its limitations. Most significantly, according to Groth-Marnat (1999), the MMPI-2 may be prohibitively time-consuming; it is, in fact, with 567 items, the lengthiest of such measures. Additional MMPI-2 limitations include high item overlap, resulting in high intercorrelations among various scales, the use of obsolete diagnostic labels (such as, Psychasthenia), ineffectiveness in diagnosing either Axis I or Axis II conditions, including the criteria of the current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association

[APA]; 2000).

56

The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III

Another widely accepted self-report measure is the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-

III (MCMI-III; Millon, 1994). The MCMI-III purports to “help the clinician assess DSM-IV-

related personality disorders and clinical syndromes” and provide information relating to

emotional adjustment, and attitude toward taking the test (Millon, 1994, p. 17). A review of the

literature reveals that various versions of the MCMI have been used as outcome measures in

hundreds of studies, for such disorders as post-traumatic stress disorder (Allen, Coyne, &

Huntoon, 1998), PDs (Alnaes & Torgensen, 1989; Bayon, Hill, Svrakic, Przybeck, & Cloninger,

1996; Joiner & Rudd, 2002; de Groot, Franken, van der Meer, & Hendriks, 2003), personality cluster traits (Bender, Farber, & Geller, 2001; Espelage, Mazzeo, Sherman, & Thompson, 2002), narcissistic personality disorder (Auerbach, 1984), forensic patients (Blackburn, 1998; Rogers,

2003)., substance abusers (Craig, 1997a, 1997b; Craig, & Olson 1998), depression (Davis &

Hayes, 1997; Libb, Stankovic, Sokol, Freeman, Houck, & Switzer, 1990), schizophrenia (del

Rosario, McCann, & Navarra, 1994), bipolar disorder (Turley, Bates, Edwards, & Jackson,

1992), anxiety disorders (Freeman, Kablinger, Rolland, & Brannon, 1999), aggressiveness (Holt,

Meloy, & Strack, 1999), and for all manner of psychiatric outpatients (Piersma & Boes, 1997).

The MCMI-III is also used extensively in clinical practice (Groth-Marnat, 1999).

The MCMI—III was normed on a heterogeneous sample of 998 males and females representing a wide variety of diagnoses and demographic variables. The sample included patients seen in independent practices, in mental health centers, in clinics, in residential settings,

57 and in hospitals. Because norms were based exclusively on a clinical sample, the instrument is not appropriate for use with nonclinical populations due to a high frequency of diagnostic false- positives (Millon & Davis, 1996).

The MCMI-III has become a frequently used measure of clinical syndromes and PDs and has good empirical validation (e.g., Choca & VanDenburg, 1997; Craig, 1993, 1997a; Hsu,

2002). The MCMI—III PD scales include all of the PDs contained in the DSM—IV plus four additional, nonofficial PDs (depressive, passive—aggressive, self-defeating, and sadistic).

Although these additional diagnoses are no longer in the official nosology, Millon et al (1997) maintain that these disorders, nonetheless “exist in nature,” despite having been deleted from the

DSM. The MCMI—III is widely used by clinicians and researchers, and there is an extensive literature reporting favorable psychometric properties (Craig & Weinberg, 1993; Millon &

Davis, 1996; Retzlaff, 1995). More generally, studies comparing a variety of self-report and interview forms of diagnostic assessment have provided little evidence that one test or method is superior to others, particularly in evaluating PDs (Widiger & Sanderson, 1995).

In discussing the assessment and quantification of psychological disorders, Millon (1999. p. 4) philosophically observed the following:

The natural world is complex and intricate, and its phenomena are not easy to classify.

Our concepts and categories are only tools to guide observation, interpret its results, and

solve problems. Alternative concepts may help us to understand a single subject of

inquiry… In psychology and other soft sciences, we sometimes have to impose arbitrary

systems that suggest more clarity and coherence than the imprecise nature of the subject

may warrant…. Debates on these issues may sometimes degenerate into semantic

arguments and theoretical hair-, but conceptual distinctions and the assumptions

58

they reflect are a necessary part of scientific decision-making. However, clinicians and

researchers are ultimately concerned with the welfare of real people, holistic collections

of inextricably interconnected configurations of cognition, behavior, and affect.

Millon (1999) acknowledges this reality by advocating a “psychosynergistic” assessment that integrates Axis I clinical syndromes with Axis II PDs into a single dynamic psychological profile, which recognizes that, “Each person becomes a synthesized and substantive whole that is greater than the sum of its multiaxial parts.” (p. 4). Thus, impairment and distress may be manifested as an Axis I syndrome, an Axis II disorder, or in problems across both axes. To this end, the MCMI—III, does not proceed through a serial interpretation of individual scales; rather, it transforms their meanings into a holistic profile—a profile that Millon (1999) posited can be legitimately explained by behavioral, psychodynamic or cognitive perspectives.

According to Millon et al. (1997, p. 124) “the greater the number of scales elevated above

BR 75, the greater the personality pathology” and the higher the BR scale score elevation above

BR 75, the more severe the disturbance. Although all MCMI-III BR scores above 75 are interpretable, a score of BR 75 is expected to indicate the presence of a clinically significant personality “style” or “syndrome”; BR 85, however, is indicative of symptoms sufficient to warrant a diagnosis of PD (p. 120). Thus, for purposes of assessing Axis II pathology, the sum of the positive distance from the BR 75 cutoff score for any of the specific MCMI-III Clinical

Personality Pattern and/or Severe Personality Pathology scales should capture the total pathology on Axis II.

Millon specifically intended the original Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (Millon &

Davis, 1977) to assist in diagnosing PDs as well as clinical syndromes (Groth-Marnat, 1999;

Millon & Davis, 1977). The instrument has undergone a number of revisions in order to

59

accommodate relevant empirical findings. The most recent version, the MCMI—III (Millon,

1994) is a self-report measure consisting of 175 true or false statements, rendering scores on 28

scales in the following categories: three Modifying Indices, one Validity Scale, three Severe

Personality Pathology scales, 11 PD scales, seven clinical syndrome scales, and three additional

Severe Clinical Scales.

Clinical Syndrome Scale.

Separate MCMI—III categories distinguish Axis I from more enduring Axis II disorders.

Within clinical axes, scales are further categorized according to severity. Thus, three Severe

Clinical Syndromes (Thought Disorder, Delusional Disorder, and Major Depression) are

assessed independently of more moderate or “neurotic” Clinical Syndromes Scale scores,

composed of the following seven subscales: Anxiety, Somatoform, Bipolar Manic, Dysthymia,

Alcohol Dependence, Drug Dependence, and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (Millon & Davis,

1996, p. 3). The instrument also provides data regarding psychosocial stressors and their therapeutic implications. It should be noted that the presence of certain Axis I conditions, such as particular dysthymic and anxiety states can elevate or depress certain PD scales (Shea et al,

1987). However, the MCMI automatically compensates for the influence of this state (Axis I)—

trait (Axis II) interaction (Millon, 1994; Millon & Davis, 1996).

60

Personality Disorder Scales.

The MCMI-III consists of two personality disorder scales, both consisting of a total of 14 theorized PDs. The first scale, Clinical Personality Pattern Scales, contains the following 11 personality subscales: Schizoid, Avoidant, Depressive, Dependent, Histrionic, Narcissistic,

Antisocial, Aggressive/Sadistic, Compulsive, Passive-Aggressive (negativistic and Self-

Defeating (Masochistic). The second personality scale, Severe Personality Scales consists of the following three subscales: Schizotypal, Borderline, and Paranoid (Millon, 1994; Millon & Davis,

1996).

Axis II PDs are scored in a manner similar to Axis I, with BR scale scores of 75 to 84 on

Axis II suggestive of personality traits; however, scores of 85 or greater provide strong empirical support for the presence of personality disorder(s), and according to Millon, resultant Axis I clinical pathology as the personality disorder deficits exacerbate distress and interpersonal difficulties (Millon & Davis, 1996).

In a recent study conducted with a combined sample of clinical and nonclinical populations,

O’Connor & Dyce (2001) demonstrated empirically that the MCMI—III (Millon, 1994) differentiated individuals with PDs from those failing to meet Axis II diagnostic criteria. Normal and disordered personalities were found to coexist in a variety of regions of the multivariate space, based on the 5-factor model (FFM) of personality (Widiger, 2000). Examination of the interpersonal dimensions chart (Figure 1) reveals that within regions, the profiles of normal and disordered personalities were very similar in characteristic configuration or vector but notably different in vector length. Moreover, this difference was detected by the MCMI-III. Those with

PDs appear to be “out there” in inner experience, interpersonal behavior, and in this case,

61 geometric configuration. Significantly, although length of vector predicted the presence of Axis

II pathology, it failed to predict any specific personality disorder. Consequently, it is predicted that the severity of cognitive distortion will predict the presence and severity of Axis II conditions. Although the severity of cognitive distortion may be able to predict the extremity of the vector (severity) and presence of PD(s) or clinical disorder(s), distorted thinking is not, however, considered capable of predicting the direction of the vector, that is, specific personality disorder.

62

Figure 1. Illustration of interpersonal dimensions.

Note. O’Connor & Dyce (2001) empirically demonstrated that the MCMI—III and, to a lesser extent, the MMPI—2 differentiated individuals with PDs from those failing to meet Axis II diagnostic criteria. Although normal and disordered personalities were found to coexist in a variety of regions of the multivariate space based on the 5-factor model (FFM) of personality (Widiger, 2000), those with PDs appear to be “out there” in inner experience, interpersonal behavior, and in this case, geometric configuration. Source: O'Connor, B. P., & Dyce, J. A. (2001).

Rigid and extreme: A geometric representation of PDs in five-factor model space. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 81, 1119-1130.

63

Personality-disordered individuals tended to be located in the perimeters or outer regions of

the FFM space, as indicated by their longer vector lengths. This indicates that the presence and

severity of a personality disorder was manifested in a difference in the quantity, rather than the

quality of four of the five FFM dimensions: , extraversion, agreeableness, and

conscientiousness were differentially more extreme in individuals with PDs. Specifically, those

reaching the threshold for significance on MCMI-III, with BR scores equal to or greater than BR

75 or BR 85, exhibited significantly more extremity and rigidity on these dimensions.

In summary and in support of the Cognitive Model, evidence exists suggesting that as the

severity or the number of Axis I or Axis II conditions increases, there will be a correspondent

increase in pathology. In other words, there should be a positive correlation between measures of

pathology and measures of cognitive distortion.

MCMI Scoring Methods

Scale scores have been established by actuarial base-rate-transformed and base rate-adjusted

PD scale scores reflecting the prevalence of the syndromes or characteristics in the general

population, as described in the MCMI—III manual (Millon & Davis, 1996). Empirical evidence

suggests that BR scores offer more diagnostic accuracy than T scores (Duthie & Vincent, 1986).

Derived from a standardization sample of 1079 clinical mental health patients, Millon & Davis

(1997) recommended cutoff scores are as follows:

• 35 = median score for “normal”/nonpsychiatric groups.

64

• 60 = median score for psychiatric groups.

• 75 = presence of features.

• 85 = definite characteristics.

The scales were explicitly designed to measure the official DSM—IV diagnostic criteria and

Millon’s evolutionary theory of PDs (Millon, 1990, Millon et al., 1996). In addition, with a

completion time of approximately 25 minutes, the MCMI—III is shorter and thus more practical

and appealing than comparable instruments for patients and clinicians (Millon & Davis, 1996).

The three-stage validation process is an additional strength of the MCMI.

Evidence for the validity of the scales has been provided in the form of correlations with

ratings by clinicians, correlations with “collateral tests measuring of identical constructs”, and

strong diagnostic efficiency statistics (Millon, 1994, p. 30). The instrument has been validated

with a variety of clinical samples including inpatients, outpatients and drug and alcohol center

patients (Hsu, 2002; Millon, 1994; Millon & Davis, 1996). The MCMI—III is widely used both

by clinicians and researchers, resulting in an extensive literature reporting favorable

psychometric properties (e.g., Craig & Weinberg, 1993; Retzlaff, 1995).

The MCMI-III has demonstrated high test-retest validity with a median for all scales of .91

(Millon, 1994). Test-retest reliability reflected high stability, ranging from a low of .82 for

debasement to a high of .96 for Bipolar: Manic with a median stability coefficient of .91. It

should be noted, though, that the delay between test and retest was only 5 to 14 days. These

results indicate high stability, at least over short periods of time. However, Overholser, (1989)

found .69 and .67 test-retest validities for the personality scales and clinical scales, respectively,

with retests occurring at a mean interval of 379 days.

65

Strong internal consistency was also confirmed with alpha coefficients surpassing .80 for 20 scales, ranging from a high of .90 for Depression and a low of .66 for the Compulsion scale

(Millon, 1994). Correlations between the MCMI-I and MCMI-III scales are moderately high

(Groth-Marnat, 1999). Later versions of the MCMI have also demonstrated little difference

between stability on personality scales versus clinical scales (Groth-Marnat, 1999). In fact, the

MCMI-III manual (Millon et al., 1997) actually indicates a slightly lower mean of .89 for the

personality scales versus .91 for the clinical scales.

Concurrent validity.

The MCMI-III also demonstrated good concurrent validity, with the Major Depression and

Dysthymia scales correlating with the Beck Depression Inventory, .74 and .71, respectively

(Millon & Davis, 1996). However, although the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI;

Spielberger, 1983) correlated more highly with the Major Depression scale (.59), the Anxiety

scale still correlated moderately with state (.55) and trait (.58) anxiety. Consequently, caution

might be appropriate regarding discriminant validity; for example, the BDI also correlated .63

with the PTSD and .62 with the Thought Disorder scales (Millon et al, 1997) .

Predictive validity.

The aggressive personality disorder scales and several of the neurotic scales correlated with

future institutional violence in prison populations (Retzlaff, Stoner, & Kleinsasser, 2002).

Discriminant validity.

The MCMI—III has demonstrated both convergent and discriminant validity as a measure

of sleep disturbance (Allen, Console, Brethour, 2000).

66

Limitations of the MCMI—III

Because the MCMI-III data is based exclusively on individuals who were undergoing psychotherapy, psychological assessment, or on those who were evincing emotional or interpersonal problems, the instrument is not appropriate for use in nonclinical populations.

Moreover, the instrument provides valuable data regarding personality and psychopathology; however, scores are best regarded as a cross-sectional snapshot of the individual. Consequently, it might be ideal to augment the instrument with extensive corroborating information from collateral sources and biographical interviews of the type that are prohibitive to this sort of experimental procedure (Kazdin, 1998; Millon & Davis, 1996). A lack of discriminant validity has been noted between clinical and nonclinical populations (Boyle & Loick, 2000). Millon &

Davis, (1996) recommend a multimethod approach in which MCMI results are evaluated in the light of independent clinical evidence such as other psychological tests, observed behavior, case history, and data gathered through clinical interviews.

Claims of “on the mark” validity for the MCMI-III range from 55 to 65% with “off-target” errors in 10 to 15% of cases indicating a quantitative range of five to six times greater than chance (Millon & Davis, 1996, p. 7). The instrument is most accurate for individuals in the moderate range of disturbance. This means that more acutely disturbed or psychotic individuals may be underdiagnosed, whereas those experiencing milder adjustment disorders or transient stressors are more liable to be overdiagnosed (Millon & Davis, 1996).

The MCMI shares limitations of many other self-report measures, including vague items and language, cultural assumptions, and state-bias (Hsu, 2002).

67

Although, studies comparing various self-report and interview forms of PD assessment have

provided little evidence that one test or method is superior to the other (Widiger & Sanderson,

1995), the relative brevity, diagnostic breadth, and psychometric validity make the MCMI-III an appealing instrument.

Summary

Overwhelming empirical evidence supports the cognitive model of psychological disorders, which posits the idea that cognitive distortions may precipitate, attend, maintain, and exacerbate a wide range of clinical problems. To measure these problems, the MCMI—III provides a practical, psychometrically sound instrument for assessing psychological disorders on both Axis

I and Axis II. Similarly, the ICD appears to be the most comprehensive, psychometrically validated measure of cognitive distortion to date.

Cognitive Distortions in

Personality Disorders and Clinical Syndromes

Personality Disorders have long represented one of the most perplexing and frustrating facets of psychopathology confronting clinicians and researchers (Nigg & Goldsmith, 1994). The lives of the personality disordered are often unusually stressful for themselves and their families.

The impact of PDs can range from to increased sucidality for both patients and for those with whom they interact. In fact, high stress combined with concurrent PD diagnosis has

68 been correlated with elevated risk of suicidal behavior (Zimmerman, Pfohl, Stangl, & Coryell,

1985).

According to Beck, Butler, Brown, Dahlsgaard, Newman and Beck (2001, p. 1214), “…the essence of a personality disorder is revealed in the dysfunctional beliefs that characterize and perpetuate it.” Cognitive distortions (Beck et al., 1979; Burns, 1980; 1999; Freeman & Oster

1999) contributing to those dysfunctional beliefs may foster and maintain an enduring, rigid, and pervasive pattern of inner experience and behavior that deviates markedly from what is expected by one’s culture (Beck & Freeman, 1990). When this occurs, one may experience subjective distress, interpersonal problems, and/or deficits in impulse control. Once these symptoms are of sufficient quantity, chronicity, and/or severity, the diagnosis of a PD may be appropriate. Thus, cognitive distortions can cause severe and chronic problems for PD patients (Beck, & Freeman,

1990; Beck et al., 2004; Freeman & Oster 1999).

The following illustration, typical of many patients diagnosed with obsessive-compulsive

PD (OCPD), illustrates virtually all of Yurica and DiTomasso (2001) empirically validated cognitive distortions (distortions italicized): Perfectionism, characteristic of many OCPD patients

(Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004), can lead to avoidance of personal and occupational tasks for fear that they will be unable to live up to unobtainable idealistic standards. When complicated by dichotomous/black and white thinking, this can lead OCPD patients to expect inevitable and abject failure in everything perceived to be significant. Even if they attempt an activity, magnification of minor errors and minimization of success maintains a negative confirmatory bias, making the individual liable to make arbitrary inferences/jump to conclusions that they have failed before they have ever given themselves a chance to succeed. In this way, externalization of self-worth occurs as the individual’s self-esteem becomes conditional on the

69 quality and quantity of his or her work product. Because the patient inevitably fails to meet these impossible standards, he or she may engage in chronic upward social comparison to others. The

OCPD patient, then, may come to label him- or herself as defective, helpless, or unlovable, leading to a further magnification of the perceived problem. Thus, it is easy to understand how this interlocking knot of cognitive distortions may lead to shame, , anger, and a host of other dysphoric emotions as well as maladaptive behavior, which may maintain dysfunctional schema at the core of the process (Young, 1999). These processes are theorized to be similar across the official DSM-IV-TR nosology (APA, 2000).

There is evidence that cognitive distortions, as measured by the Dysfunctional Attitude

Scale (Weissman & Beck, 1978), remit in most patients when their depression abates, rendering their cognition indistinguishable from nondepressed controls (Blackburn, Eunson, & Bishop,

1986). These results support Beck’s (1976) notion that depression results from dormant maladaptive schema(s), which are often activated by negative life events.

However, it has also been demonstrated empirically that the cognitive distortions of patients diagnosed with PDs remain intact even after depression remits (Ilardi & Crgaihead, 1999).

Moreover, individuals with comorbid PDs and clinical depression exhibited higher levels of dysfunctional attitudes, operationalized as DAS scores, than do depressed patients without PDs

(Evans & Graighead, 1995). These results lend additional empirical support to the conclusion that individuals manifesting PDs engage in more distorted thinking than controls or Axis I patients without PDs. Moreover, the cognitive distortions of Axis II patients are trait-like, because they are stable and fail to remit when Axis I conditions abate. Such dysfunctional cognitive style may carry the prediction of leaving them more vulnerable to future episodes of

Axis I disorders (Ilardi & Crgaihead, 1999; Millon, 1994).

70

DSM-III (APA, 1980) originally delineated PDs along a separate axis to encourage the consideration of enduring characterlogical problems in the context of comorbid and more immediate mood, anxiety, and psychotic disorders (Frances, 1980; Spitzer, Williams, & Skodol,

1980). Although distinguishing PDs from other clinical syndromes diagnostically can be useful and valid, the demarcation often remains problematic and illusory whether or not diagnoses are placed on separate axes (Widiger & Shea, 1991).

Cognitive patterns exhibited in PDs (Axis II) differ from so-called clinical disorders on

Axis I in a number of interesting ways. First, in Axis I conditions, such as depressive, anxiety, and psychotic disorders, the frequency and intensity of dysfunctional automatic thoughts are expected to subside as the Axis I syndrome abates. Axis I conditions are expected to be more episodic, meaning that patients typically return to premorbid levels of cognitive function as symptoms remit. Conversely, dysfunctional beliefs and distorted thinking patterns exhibited in

PDs are more systematically “built into the ‘normal’ cognitive organization” and therefore more persistent and less amenable to change (Beck & Freeman, 1990, p. 58). Thus, if PDs originate in adolescence or early adulthood and are, by definition, pervasive and inflexible, then one might expect the dysfunctional beliefs and associated cognitive distortions to be more chronic in PD patients. An additional clinical problem arises from the fact that Axis II patients are more prone to “resistance” in the form of treatment nonadherrence or “noncollaboration” (Beck & Freeman,

1990, p. 67).

This noncollaboration may stem from lack of motivation, secondary ga in, rigidity of beliefs and behavior, vague or unrealistic treatment goals, and/or of the effects of change.

All of these obstacles to change can be maintained, reinforced, and exacerbated by cognitive distortions (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004). Accordingly, correctly identifying and

71

treating patterns of cognitive distortions may illuminate underlying maladaptive beliefs, facilitate

treatment effectiveness, improve patient level of functioning, and ameliorate distress for

individua ls with recalcitrant Axis II conditions.

Distorted cognitive style also has been associated with aggressive behavior, such as conduct disorder, reactive aggression, and commission of violent crimes in a sample of highly

aggressive juvenile offenders (Dodge & Newman, 1981; Dodge, Price, Bachorowski, &

Newman, 1990). To compound matters, early aggressive behavior, associated with cognitive

distortions, has been found to be a developmental precursor of later drug use (Brook, Whiteman,

Finch, & Cohen, 1996; Lynskey & Fergusson, 1995).

There is also empirical support for the association between cognitive distortions and a

number of other troubling social and clinical conditions, including sexual assault (Baumeister,

Catanese, & Wallace, 2002), pathological gambling (Steenbergh, Meyers, May, & Whelan,

2002), adolescent anxiety and depressive disorders (Kendall, Kortlander, & Brady, 1992),

violence and anger in marital relationships (Ekhardt, Barbour, & Davison, 1998), and adolescent

depression and anxiety (Kolko, Brent, Baugher, Bridge, Birmaher, 2000).

Moreover, there is evidence of a relationship between comorbidity and cognitive

distortions. For example, Najavits, Gotthardt, Weiss, & Epstein (2004) recently demonstrated the

fact that individuals diagnosable with a dual Axis I diagnosis (PTSD and substance use

disorders) had a significantly greater tendency to engage in 10 out of 20 cognitive distortions, as

measured by the Cognitive Distortion Scale, than did individuals who only met criteria for one

Axis I diagnosis (PTSD).

Thus, consistent with the cognitive model, dysfunctional beliefs and their associated cognitive

distortions appear to be a unifying feature of psychological disorders, regardless of axis

72 taxonomy or biopsychosiocial manifestations. Yet it is only recently that a single measure was developed to quantify the frequency of specific cognitive distortions independent of diagnosis

(Yurica, 2002; Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002).

Cognitive distortions serve a pivotal role in the maintenance of emotional disorders (Beck et al, 1979; Beck, Wright, & Newman, 1993; Ellis & Grieger, 1977) and PDs (Beck & Freeman,

1990; Beck et al., 2004). Cognitive distortions can become habitual patterns of thinking that are evoked by certain stimuli and are supported by underlying intermediate beliefs composed of conditional assumptions, beliefs and rules (Beck, 1996). At the foundation of these dysfunctional cognitions are maladaptive core beliefs. These core beliefs are related to the most vulnerable aspects of an individual’s self-concept (e.g., I am unlovable, helpless, worthless, vulnerable, or incompetent) and maladaptive views of others (e.g., People are rejecting, demeaning, or hostile)

(Beck, 1996). Cognitive distortions may also prevent reality testing of thoughts, thus maintaining the entire dysfunctional system of cognition, emotion, and behavior. When core beliefs are activated, as in times of , cognitive distortions may become increasingly prominent and severe (Beck & Weishaar, 1989).

According to the cognitive model of psychopathology, CT “…techniques are designed to identify, reality-test and correct underlying distorted conceptualizations and the dysfunctional core beliefs (schemas) underlying those cognitions.” (Beck et al., 1979, p. 43). Thus, CT seeks to reduce cognitive distortions by promoting more accurate thinking.

Consequently, if cognitive distortion occupies such a central role in the cognitive model and patients present with a myriad of psychological disorders and permutations of comobidites, then it would behoove therapists to use the most accurate means to assess the presence and severity

73 these cognitive distortions. This illustrates the need for a brief, portable, psychometrically sound instrument, such as the ICD (Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002).

For the assessment of psychological disorders, the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory

(MCMI—III; Millon, 1994) provides a comprehensive self-report questionnaire to assess personality and emotional adjustment according to DSM-IV criteria and Millon’s evolutionary theory of personality. In addition, the MCMI—III also provides a means to assess the validity of the test-taker’s responses. The instrument has also demonstrated good reliability and validity

(Groth-Marnat, 1999).

Rationale for the Study

The influence that cognition exerts on emotion and behavior has been postulated since the beginning of classical civilization. Similarly, CT is based on the principle that human emotion and behavior are heavily influenced by cognition (e.g., Beck, 1976). Thus, according to cognitive theory, clinical disorders (e.g., Beck, 1976) and personality pathology (Beck & Freeman, 1990;

Beck et al, 2004) should correlate with distorted cognition.

When Beck originally conceptualized depression as a “thinking disorder” in 1964 (p.13); he defined it as a disorder in which individuals viewed the world in a subjective manner largely determined by idiosyncratic cognitions. Consistent with Bartlett (1932) and Piaget’s vocabulary

(1947/1950) and Kelly’s (1955) cognitive constructs, Beck further postulated that the activation of particular schemas were fundamental to cognitive, affective, and behavioral symptoms found in various psychological disorders (Beck, 1964, 1976; 1996; Beck, Emery, & Greenberg, 1985;

Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck, Wright, Newman, & Liesse, 1985).

74

Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002 provide the latest and most comprehensive psychometric self-

report instrument for measuring cognitive distortions in an adult clinical po pulation. The ICD is

a 69-item self-report inventory designed to measure distorted cognitions in an outpatient clinical

population and in normal controls. The instrument consists of single sentence items answered on

a five-point Likert scale (Likert, 1932). The ICD provides scores on 11 factors/cognitive

distortions and a total score reflecting the total frequency of cognitive distortion. Possible scores

range from 69 to 345.

Yurica & DiTomasso (2001) designed the ICD explicitly to validate theorized cognitive

distortions (Beck et al., 1979; Burns, 1980, 1999; Freeman & DeWolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster,

1999). However, of the original theorized cognitive distortions, factor analysis revealed 11

fundamental factors that closely resembled 10 theory-derived cognitive distortion subscales

(Externalization of Self-Worth, Fortune-Telling, Magnification, Labeling, Perfectionism,

Comparison to Others, Emotional Reasoning, Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusion,

Minimization, Mind-Reading), in addition to one new cognitive distortion (Emotional Reasoning and Decision-Making).

Despite convincing theoretical and empirical evidence for the fundamental role of cognitive

distortions in both the cognitive model and cognitive-behavioral therapies, there has been

surprisingly little effort to quantify objectively the frequency with which these cognitive events

occur. In fact, to date, only one instrument, the ICD (Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002), has been

empirically validated via factor-analysis of the theoretical cognitive distortions with a clinical

sample of heterogeneous outpatient mental health patients to test the frequency of these

particular cognitive distortions (Yurica, 2002). Thus, this study endeavored to further validate

the ICD and to expand the utility of this promising instrument into specific Axis I conditions

75

and, beyond that, into the Axis II domain by correlating cognitive distortions (Total ICD scores)

with psychological disorders (MCMI-III subscale scores).

The Purpose of the Study

This study sought to determine the relationship between the frequency of cognitive distortions and the number and severity of psychological disorders across Axis I and Axis II.

Further, the study endeavored to further determine the validity and reliability of the ICD, a promising new self-report measure of cognitive distortions, by correlating this instrument with clinical diagnoses as determined by the MCMI-III, a widely accepted measure of psychological disorders. Providing further validation for the ICD, a brief, portable, and objective measure of these key cognitive processes should be beneficial to both research and practice in the following ways:

1. Accurately identifying specific patterns of cognitive distortions early during the course of

treatment might assist the clinician to target more efficiently dysfunctional thought patterns

that maintain and exacerbate social and psychological problems. It has been shown that

treatment occurring earlier in the course of psychological disorders correlates with a number

of therapeutic benefits, including positive treatment outcome, fewer office visits to

physicians, and reduced health care costs (Bruns, 1998; Kaplan, 2000). Thus, using the ICD

for early identification of cognitive distortions might allow the therapist and patient to

illuminate collaboratively and more quickly the underlying secondary beliefs and schemas

which maintain those same cognitive distortions.

2. An instrument that allows for regular assessment of cognitive distortions throughout

treatment, such as the ICD, can provide an objective measure of progress, guiding treatment

76

planning because cognitive distortions have been shown to correlate with a number of Axis I

and Axis II disorders (e.g., Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, &

Erdbaugh, 196; Hollon & Kendall, 1980; Ross, Gottfredson, Christensen, & Weaver, 1986).

3. The instrument could facilitate psychoeducational processes regarding patients’ particular

patterns of cognitive distortions, as well as the cognitive and behavioral skills explicitly

targeting those distortions (Beck & Freeman, 1990).

4. There may be an emergence of characteristic patterns of distortions that delineate particular

Axis I versus Axis II conditions. If the ICD supports this difference, its use may increase

diagnostic accuracy, which may, in turn, lead to more effective treatment selection.

5. Demonstrating a correlation between the amelioration of cognitive distortions and the

improvement of clinical symptoms for Axis I and Axis II conditions would lend further

empirical support to the cognitive model of psychological disorders on both axes.

6. In a review of 136 research studies, Grove and Meehl, (1996) determined that empirically

based personality assessment instruments are consistently equal to or superior to less-

structured clinical interview methods for increasing the efficiency of the assessment process,

for understanding patients, establishing rapport, formulating an accurate diagnosis, for

developing insight, developing optimum empirically guided treatment planning, and for

predicting the course of treatment (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Consequently, a brief self-report

measure administered early in the treatment process might also lessen the burden on the

psychological and medical communities by reducing the course of treatment and, by

extension, healthcare costs.

7. The ICD has been validated only in an anxious and depressed sample. This study also

sought to further expand the utility of the ICD beyond the evaluation of anxiety and

77

depression (Yurica, 2002) for use with additional Axis I and Axis II conditions. Additionally,

it is hoped that a sufficiently validated ICD will allow for greater flexibility in empirical

research by providing an additional assessment tool for the design and standardization of

empirical procedure, which can be employed to measure pathology and progress for a

broader range of psychological disorders, as well as aiding in the replication of empirical

research. In addition, the ICD should also provide researchers and clinicians with an elegant

tool to gauge quickly and accurately guage the severity of pathology and aid in treatment

planning.

8. Psychometrically, the goals of the study are to a) evaluate the construct validity of the ICD

in a sample of Axis I and Axis II patients, b) determine the relationship between the ICD

with specific Axis I and II disorders, c) establish the relationship between the ICD and the

MCMI—III, a well-established, multidimensional, valid and reliable measure of Axis I and

Axis II conditions, and d) further confirm the internal consistency of ICD content by

confirming the acceptable alpha coefficient levels originally found by Yurica (2002).

78

Research Hypotheses

The study included eight research hypotheses. Table 2 presents a summary of hypotheses,

rationale for individual hypotheses, and respective operational definitions.

Table 2

Table 2. Hypothesis Summary

Hypothesis Rationale Formula

H 1: Individuals with a greater If frequency of cognitive The number of significant number of diagnosable Axis I distortion correlates with the clinical disorder and clinical disorders and/or Axis manifestation of clinical and personality disorder scales r II PDs will report a higher personality disorders, then Total ICD scores frequency of cognitive individuals experiencing more distortions than will frequent cognitive distortions individuals with fewer should manifest a greater diagnosable psychological number of psychological disorders or who are otherwise disorders. free of diagnosable psychological disorders.

H2: The number of PDs for If frequency of cognitive The number of significant which an individual may be distortion correlates with the MCMI—III Clinical diagnosed will positively manifestation of personality Personality Pattern scale correlate with the reported disorders, then individuals scores and/or Severe frequency of cognitive experiencing more frequent Personality Pathology scale distortions. cognitive distortions should scores r Total ICD scores manifest a greater number of personality disorders.

H3: The number of Axis I If frequency of cognitive The number of significant clinical syndromes for which distortion correlates with the MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes an individual can be diagnosed manifestation of clinical and/or Severe Clinical will po sitively correlate with a syndromes, then individuals Syndromes r Total ICD person’s reported frequency of experiencing more frequent scores. cognitive distortion. cognitive distortions should manifest a greater number of clinical syndromes.

H4: Individuals with a greater Individuals with comorbid The number of significant number of comorbid Axis I Axis I and Axis II conditions MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes and Axis II disorders will engage in more frequent and/or Severe Clinical report more frequent cognitive cognitive distortions than Syndromes r Total ICD

79 distortion than will individuals individuals who are scores. meeting criteria for only one diagnosable on only one axis. Axis, that is either personality disorder(s) or for clinical syndrome(s).

H5: The severity of Axis I Individuals with more severe The sum of all MCMI—III conditions will po sitively Axis I pathology engage in Clinical Syndromes and correlate with the reported more frequent cognitive Severe Clinical Syndromes frequency of cognitive distortions than those with less scale BR score points of 75 or distortions. severe Axis I conditions. greater r Total ICD scores.

H6: The severity of personality Individuals with more severe The sum of all MCMI—III disorders will po sitively personality disorder pathology Clinical Personality Pattern correlate with the reported will engage in more frequent and Severe Personality frequency of cognitive cognitive distortions than Pathology BR scale score distortions. those with less severe Axis II points of 75 or greater r Total conditions. ICD scores

H7: The total severity of Individuals with more severe The sum of all MCMI—III psychological dysfunction, psychological dysfunction will BR scale score points of 75 or independent of whether the engage in more cognitive greater on both Axis I and diagnoses occur on Axis I or distortion, regardless of Axis II r Total ICD scores Axis II, will positively whether or not impairment is correlate with the reported diagnosable on Axis I or Axis frequency of cognitive II. distortions.

H8: There will be a positive The frequency of cognitive Individual MCMI-III scale correlation between reported distortion will correlate with score points of BR 75 or cognitive distortions and each individual diagnosis. greater r Total ICD scores. clinical diagnosis for which an individual tests positive.

Note: Psychological disorders will be operationalized as MCMI—III scale scores, whereas cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total ICD scores. Individuals must score at least BR 75 or greater for inclusion on a minimum of one MCMI—III personality or clinical disorder scales.

Hypothesis 1: Individuals with a greater number of diagnosable Axis I clinical disorders and/or Axis II PDs will report a higher frequency of cognitive distortions than will individuals with fewer diagnosable psychological disorders or who are otherwise free of diagnosable

80 psychological disorders. Diagnosable disorders will be operationalized as a BR of 75 or greater on any scale in the following MCMI-III categories: Clinical Syndromes (Axis I), Severe Clinical

Syndromes (Axis I), Clinical Personality Patterns (Axis II), Severe Personality Pathology (Axis

II). This will yield a score of between 0 for no disorders to 24 if all personality scales and clinical disorder scales are scored BR 75 or greater.

Cognitive distortion will be operationalized as Total ICD scores. Participants will be judged as manifesting a “diagnosable” disorder by meeting a threshold of BR 75 or greater on one or more scales of either Axis I or Axis II pathology. Although an MCMI—III score of BR 85 indicates the prominence of a clinical syndrome or the presence of a personality disorder

(Millon, 1994, Millon & Davis, 1996), a BR of 75 indicates the presence of a style or syndrome.

It should be noted that this criterion applies to all of this study’s hypotheses (See Table 2 for summary of hypotheses, rationale, and formulae.)

Hypothesis 2: The number of PDs for which an individual may be diagnosed will positively correlate with the reported frequency of cognitive distortions. The number of PDs will be operationalized as the sum of all significant MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern scales and

Severe Personality Pathology scale that score BR 75 or greater. This will yield a score of between 0 for no PDs to 14 if all personality scales scored BR 75 or greater. Cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total ICD scores.

Rationale for H1 and H2: According to Millon et al. (1997, p. 124), “the greater the number of scales elevated above BR 75, the greater the personality pathology” and the higher the BR scale score elevation above BR 75, the more severe the disturbance. Thus, the number of significant MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern and/or Severe Personality Pathology scales should correlate with total pathology on Axis II, as measured by the ICD. Although all MCMI-

81

III BR scores above 75 are interpretable, a score of BR 75 is expected to indicate the presence of

a clinically significant personality “style” or “syndrome”; however, BR 85 is indicative of symptoms sufficient to warrant a diagnosis of PD (Millon & Davis, 1996, p. 120).

Hypothesis 3: The number of Axis I clinical syndromes for which an individual can be diagnosed will po sitively correlate with a person’s reported frequency of cognitive distortion.

This means that an individuals who meet criteria for only one Axis I diagnosis is expected to

engage in less cognitive distortion than individuals with a greater number of clinical syndromes.

These effects are expected to be incremental, with the tendency to engage in cognitive distortion

increasing as the number of Axis I clinical syndromes increases.

The number of Axis I clinical syndromes will be operationalized as the total number of

MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes (Axis I), Severe Clinical Syndromes (Axis I) that are elevated

above BR 75. This will yield a potential score of 0 for no diagnosis to 10, if an individual scores

equal to or greater than BR 75 for all of the MCMI-III Axis I scales.

Hypothesis 4: Individuals with a greater number of comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders will report more frequent cognitive distortion than will individuals meeting criteria for only one

Axis; that is it meets the criteria either for personality disorder(s) or clinical syndrome(s). In other words, individuals meeting diagnostic criteria for both Axis I and Axis II conditions will engage in more cognitive distortions than an individual who is diagnosable only on Axis I or

Axis II. Additionally, these effects are predicted to be incremental, increasing as the number of disorders increases.

The number of Axis I and Axis II disorders will be operationalized as above, with a possible range of scores of 0 for no diagnosis to 24 for an individual scoring greater than or equal to BR

75 for all of the Axis II scales (n=14) and Axis I scales (n=10).

82

Hypothesis 5: The severity of Axis I conditions will positively correlate with the reported frequency of cognitive distortions. The severity of Axis I conditions will be operationalized as

MCMI-III scale scores of BR 75 or greater on for any of the Clinical Syndromes and/or Severe

Clinical Syndrome scales. Cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total ICD scores.

The severity of Axis I conditions will be quantified as the sum of all BR points for Clinical

Syndromes and Severe Clinical Syndrome scale scores of BR 75 or greater for any of the Axis I scales. A ceiling of 115 is imposed for a valid score (Millon & Davis, 1996). The scope of possible Axis I severity scores will range from 0 to a possible score of 400 for an individual scoring 115 for all 10 scales. For example, an individual scoring BR 95 for scale A and 85 for scale D would yield an Axis I severity rating of 20 +10 = 30.

Rationale for H3, H4, and H5: Axis I clinical syndromes may appear independent of personality pathology. (Millon & Davis, 1996). Empirical evidence lends strong support to the cognitive model of emotional disorders for various Axis I clinical syndromes (Beck, 1967,

1976), which posits that schema-driven “cognitive errors” powerfully influence affect and behavior (Beck et al., 1979, p. 10). Perhaps at the most fundamental level, one of the principal goals of CT is to reduce these characteristic patterns of distorted thinking. Even schema-focused approaches ultimately aim to replace maladaptive core beliefs with more adaptive cognitions, which maintain, and are perpetuated by, one’s cognitive distortions (Young, 1999). Thus, it is expected that the frequency of cognitive distortion will correlate with the severity and number of psychological disorders.

Hypothesis 6. The severity of PDs will positively correlate with the reported frequency of cognitive distortions. Axis II personality severity will be operationalized as the sum of all

MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern and Severe Personality Pathology scales scores of BR 75

83

or greater, indicating a threshold of BR 75 for significance. The MCMI—III imposes a ceiling of

BR 115 (Millon & Davis, 1996). Thus, the scope of possible Axis II severity scores will range

from 0 to a possible score of 560 for an individual scoring the maximum 115 for all 14 Axis II

pathology scales. Cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total ICD scores.

Rationale for H6: In a recent study conducted with a combined sample of clinical and nonclinical populations, O’Connor & Dyce (2001) demonstrated empirically that the MCMI—III was able to differentiate the individuals with PDs from those individuals failing to meet Axis II diagnostic criteria. Normal and disordered personalities were found to coexist in a variety of regions of the multivariate space based on the 5-factor model (FFM) of personality (Widiger,

2000). Examination of the interpersonal dimensions chart (Figure 1) reveals that within regions, the profiles of normal and disordered personalities were very similar in characteristic configuration or vector but notably different in vector length. Moreover, this difference can be detected by the MCMI-III. Those with PDs appear to be “out there” in inner experience, interpersonal behavior, and in this case, geometric configuration. Significantly, length of vector predicted the presence of Axis II pathology, but it failed to predict any specific personality disorder. Consequently, it is predicted that the severity of cognitive distortion will predict the presence and severity of Axis II conditions. Although cognitive distortion may be able to predict the extremity of the vector and presence of personality disorder(s), it is not predicted to be able to predict the direction of the vector, that is, specific personality disorder.

Personality-disordered individuals tended to be located in the perimeters or outer regions of the FFM space, as indicated by their longer vector lengths; this is indicative of the fact that the presence and severity of a personality disorder was manifested in a difference in the quantity, rather than the quality of four of the five FFM dimensions: Neuroticism, extraversion,

84

agreeableness, and conscientiousness, which were differentially more extreme in individuals

with PDs. Specifically, those reaching the threshold for significance on MCMI-III, with BR scores equal to or greater than BR 75 or BR 85, exhibited significantly more extremity and rigidity on these dimensions.

Hypothesis 7. The total severity of psychological dysfunction, independent of whether diagnoses occur on Axis I or Axis II, will positively correlate with reported frequency of cognitive distortions.

Total severity of psychological dysfunction scores will be operationalized as the sum of all relevant MCMI-III scales of BR 75 or greater within the following categories: Clinical

Syndromes (Axis I), Severe Clinical Syndromes (Axis I), Clinical Personality Patterns (Axis II),

Severe Personality Pathology (Axis II). Cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total

ICD scores. This indicates a threshold for significance of BR 75. The scope of possible total severity scores will range from 0 to a possible score of 960 for an individual scoring 115 for all

24 clinical scales.

Rationale for H7: Millon & Davis, (1997) conceptualize Axis I clinical syndromes as

“extensions or distortions of the patient’s basic personality pattern... waxing and waning over

time depending on the impact of stressful situations”(p. 22).

The multiaxial model po sits an interaction between Axis I and Axis II symptomatology in

which personality is defined as the “…overall capacity to perceive and to cope with our

psychosocial world…” (Millon & Davis, 1996, p. 120). Personality is further conceptualized as

analogous to an immune system, protecting the individual from the vicissitudes of external

stressors. However, Axis I symptoms may occur because the PD patients dysfunctional “immune

systems” fails to protect the individual from stress. When this occurs, Axis I clinical syndromes

85

arise in addition to the preexisting PD symptoms. This increases the total severity of the

individual’s dysfunction (Millon & Davis, 1996). In other words, all other things being equal, an

individual meeting the diagnostic criteria for both Major Depressive Disorder and Dependent

Personality Disorder is expected to evince more impairment and/or distress than another person

meeting diagnostic criteria for only one of these axes.

Comorbid diagnoses across axes may lead to a pattern in which cognitive distortion can constrict behavior in a fashion that may actually exacerbate existing difficulties, leading to vicious circles of cognitive distortion, distress, and interpersonal impairment. When this occurs, one’s cognition may be further impaired by the resulting Axis I clinical syndromes. Prolonged

Axis I symptoms can, in turn, exacerbate personality dysfunction.

Because of this reciprocal interaction between Axis I and Axis II (Millon et al., 1996; Millon

& Davis, 1996), it is expected that individuals with greater total pathology, independent of

whether this pathology arises from Axis I or Axis II, will experience more frequent cognitive

distortions. This explains why Millon & Davis, (1997) posit the idea that the presence of Axis I

pathology supports the presence of personality pathology.

Finally, Beck (1976) and Lefebvre (1981) suggest that a total distortion score might be of

more practical value than differentiating individual categories of distortions. Similarly, the DAS

(Weisman & Beck, 1978) was designed to yield a single total score of dysfunctional attitudes.

Consequently, this study will correlate ICD total scores with the number of diagnosable clinical

disorders, with the severity of total ps ychological dysfunction, and with various individual

disorders—all of which will be determined by the MCMI—III scale scores.

Hypothesis 8: There will be a positive correlation between reported cognitive distortions and

individual clinical diagnosis scores for which an individual tests positive. Cognitive distortions

86

will be operationalized as ICD scores; a positive diagnosis will be operationalized as BR scores

greater than 75 on any MCMI-III scale on either Axis I or Axis II. The 24 MCMI-III BR scale

scores plus a single ICD score will yield 25 x 25 multi-correlational matrix correlating cognitive

distortions with individual disorders.

Rationale for H8: According to the cognitive model, cognitive distortions will positively

correlate with psychological disorders as assessed by the MCMI-III on either Axis I or Axis II.

However, it is not expected that any differential pattern of frequency of cognitive distortion to

specific disorder will emerge.

Definition of Terms

Diagnoses: Operationally defined as a score of 75 or greater on any of the MCMI-III

subscales.

Axis II Personality Disorder: Operationally defined as a score of 75 or greater on any

MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern Scale(s) or any Severe Personality Pathology Scale(s).

Axis I Clinical Syndrome(s): Operationally defined as a score of 75 or greater on any

MCMI-III Clinical Syndrome Scale(s) or Severe Clinical Syndrome Scale(s).

The 11 theoretical factors or cognitive distortions, as defined verbatim by Yurica (2002, p.

60), are composed of the following:

Cognitive Distortions:

1. Externalization of Self-Worth: Refers to the development and maintenance of self-worth

based almost exclusively on how the external world views oneself (Freeman & DeWolf, 1992 ;

Freeman & Oster, 1999).

87

2. Fortune-Telling: The process of foretelling or predicting a future event or events and

believing that this prediction is absolutely true for oneself (Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).

3. Magnification: The tendency to exaggerate or magnify either the positive or negative

consequences of some personal trait, event, or circumstance (Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).

4. Labeling: The cognitive process of labeling oneself using derogatory names (Burns, 1980,

1989, 1999; Freeman & Dewolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).

5. Perfectionism: Refers to a constant striving to live up to some internal or external

representation of perfection without examining the evidence for the reasonableness of these

perfect standards, often to avoid the subjective experience of failure (Freeman & DeWolf, 1990;

Freeman & Dewolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).

6. Comparison to Others: The tendency to compare oneself to others whereby the outcome

typically results in concluding that they are inferior or worse off than others (Freeman, &

DeWolf 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).

7. Emotional Reasoning: Refers to the predominant use of an emotional state to form

conclusions about oneself, others, or situations (Beck et al., 1979; Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).

8. Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusion: Refers to the process of drawing a negative

conclusion in the absence of specific evidence to support that conclusion (Beck et al., 1979;

Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).

9. Minimization: Refers to the process of minimizing or discounting the importance of some

event, trait, or circumstance (Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).

10. Mind-Reading: Refers to one’s arbitrary conclusion that someone is reacting negatively, or

thinking negatively towards him or her, without specific evidence to support that conclusion

(Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).

88

11. Emotional Reasoning and Decision-Making: The tendency to rely on emotions to make

decisions (Yurica, 2002).

89

CHAPTER 3

METHODOLOGY

Participants

A total of 168 adult outpatients presenting at the Center for Brief Therapy at the

Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine for psychological treatment or evaluation were

offered the opportunity to participate in the present study of cognition. Participants were also

recruited from various patients in the care of clinicians in private practice in the Philadelphia

area.

Participation in this project was voluntary for all subjects and they retained the right to

withdraw at any time without explanation. All participants remained completely anonymous. No

identifying information was ever collected on any subjects, other than basic demographic data.

Clinicians presented the participants with a packet containing the following:

1. Letter of informed consent.

2. Brief Demographic Questionnaire

3. Millon Clinical Multliaxial Inventory-III

4. Inventory of Cognitive Distortions

5. A stamped, self addressed return envelope to the investigators

Completion time was estimated to be no more than 50 minutes.

90

Inclusion Criteria

Participants were required to meet the following conditions to be included in this study.

Individuals were between 18 to 88 years of age (Millon & Davis, 1996) and obtained a score of

BR 75 or greater on at least one MCMI-III subscale on either Axis I or Axis II. Participants currently undergoing pharmacological care were included in the study and identified by a yes or no format on the Brief Information Sheet (BIS) to determine whether or not they were currently taking psychotropic medication (Yurica, 2002). Participants verified a minimum of an eighth grade education on the MCMI-III questionnaire sheet.

Exclusion Criteria

Participants younger than 18 or older than 88 years of age were excluded. Additionally excluded were participants who met diagnostic criteria for traumatic brain injury, mental retardation, pervasive developmental disorders, tic disorders, delirium, dementia, amnestic disorders, schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders (Yurica, 2002), or individuals who appeared to be intoxicated. These diagnoses were made by the treating clinician. Also excluded were participants who reported an educational level below eighth grade either to the clinician or on the MCMI-III.

91

Participant Recruitment

Participants were recruited from the Center for Brief Therapy, an outpatient mental

healthcare clinic associated with the Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine and from

individual clinicians in private clinical practice. Study clinicians included therapists at the

Master’s level, doctoral students, doctoral candidates, as well as licensed psychologists. All

graduate-level therapists were supervised by an experienced licensed psychologist.

Participants were requested to review an Agreement to Participate in Research Study of

Cognition form prior to participating in this study. They were then given a packet including the

BIS, one copy of the ICD, and one copy of the MCMI—III. The anticipated time for completion of both instruments was approximately 50 minutes. Participation was completely voluntary and participants could have withdrawn at any time without explanation.

All participant data remained completely anonymous. Only demographic data, such as age race, gender, and a yes/no response about current psychotropic medication use was retained. No other identifiable participant information was collected or stored.

Research Design

A correlational design was employed to compare the relationship between cognitive distortions, as measured by the ICD, and psychological disorders, operationalized as scores on the MCMI-III. Additionally, the study was intended to assess the psychometric properties of the

ICD. A series of Pearson Product Moment Correlations was conducted to compare ICD scores of

92

those with significant clinical and/or PDs, as measured by the MCMI—III, versus those who

failed to meet diagnostic criteria for any Axis I or Axis II condition.

Measures

The Million Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-Third Edition

Test materials consisted of the MCMI—III (Millon, 1994), the ICD (Yurica & DiTomasso,

2002), and a brief demographic questionnaire.

MCMI—III.

The MCMI—III (Millon, 1994) is a 175-item self-report test with a total of 27 scales.

Estimated administration time is approximately 25 minutes. The test is divided into five sections with scales measuring both PDs and clinical syndromes comprising: (a) Modifying Indices (3 validity scales), (b) Clinical Personality Patterns (11 basic personality disorder scales on Axis II),

(c) Severe Personality Pathology (3 severe personality disorder scales on Axis II), (d) Clinical

Syndromes (7 moderate psychopathology scales on Axis I), and (e) Severe Syndromes (3 severe psychopathology scales on Axis I). The MCMI—III raw scores are translated into BR scores.

The BR method of scoring is used on the MCMI—III rather than the more common normalized standard t-score transformations. The rationale for the use of BR scores is to assure that the proportion of individuals who score above each scale's cutoff point approximates, as closely as possible, the actual prevalence rates of the disorder in a clinical population. A BR score of 0 is the minimum, and a BR score of 115 corresponds to the maximum valid BR score for each scale.

93

A BR score of 60 corresponds to the median BR score for each scale in a psychiatric population

(Millon, 1994; Millon & Davis, 1996).

MCMI—III reliability.

Millon & Davis (1997) reported that the MCMI—III has demonstrated acceptable psychometric properties. The instruments internal consistency, or the degree to which scale items intercorrelate, ranged from a low of Cronbach’s alpha 0f .66 for the Compulsive scale to a high of .90 for Major Depression.

MCMI—III validity.

The MCMI-III also demonstrated good concurrent validity with the Major Depression and Dysthymia scales correlating with the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck & Steer, 1987) .74 and .71, respectively (Millon & Davis, 1996). The State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI;

Spielberger, 1983) correlated more highly with the Major Depression scale (.59) than the

Anxiety scale, the MCMI—III; however, the Anxiety scale still correlated moderately with state

(.55) and trait (.58) anxiety (Millon et al., 1997). Consequently, caution might be appropriate regarding discriminant validity as, for further example, the BDI also correlated .63 with the

PTSD and .62 with the Thought Disorder scales (Millon & Davis, 1996).

94

Predictive validity.

The aggressive personality disorder scales and several of the neurotic scales correlated with

future institutional violence in prison populations (Retzlaff, Stoner, & Kleinsasser, 2002).

Discriminant validity.

The MCMI—III has demonstrated both convergent and discriminant validity as a measure

of sleep disturbance (Allen, Console, & Brethour, 2000).

The MCMI-III has also demonstrated high test-retest validity with a median for all scales of

.91 (Millon & Davis, 1996).

The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions

The ICD, a 69-item self-report inventory, is composed of short sentences reflecting 11 factor-analyzed cognitive distortions. The ICD was designed for and validated with an adult clinical population with symptoms of anxiety and/or depression (Yurica, 2002). Items are scored on a five-point Likert (1932) scale ranging from 1 = Never to 5 = Always. Total possible ICD scores range from 69 to 345, with lower scores reflecting less frequency of cognitive distortions than higher scores. The instrument has attained satisfactory construct and content validity, with

experts achieving 100% agreement that all inventory items reflected each specific distortion

construct. Only items with 100% expert agreement were retained for final factor-analysis. The intent of the ICD is to provide a total score of cognitive distortion, with higher scores signifying

95

more severe patterns of distortion and employment of more types of distortion. The scale also

yields subscale scores of distortion. Yurica (2002) demonstrated good psychometric properties

for the ICD, as follows:

ICD Psychometrics

ICD Reliability.

The initial validation study found an impressive test-retest reliability with a test-retest reliability coefficient for total ICD scores of .998 (n = 28, p <, 001).

ICD validity.

The ICD also demonstrated good concurrent validity, as Total ICD scores correlated significantly and positively with other widely accepted measures of psychopathology, such as dysfunctional attitudes, the DAS (r = .70, N = 159, p < . 0001); depression, the BDI-II (r = .70,

N = 161, p < .0001); and anxiety, the BAI (r = .59, N = 161, p < .0001). In the same study,

Yurica (2002) also found impressive criterion validity because the Total ICD scores

differentiated clinical outpatients from non-patient controls (F = 15.2, df = 169, p < .0001).

Procedure

The present study included a heterogeneous population of adult outpatients ranging in age from 18 to 88 years; these patients presented to the Center for Brief Therapy at the Philadelphia

96

College of Osteopathic Medicine or specified local clinicians for psychological treatment or evaluation. Excluded were those meeting diagnostic criteria for mental retardation, pervasive developmental disorders, amnestic disorders, schizophrenia or any of the other psychotic disorders, or those appearing to be intoxicated. Confirmation of inclusionary and exclusionary criteria was performed by the treating clinicians (study collaborators) and through analysis of the

BIS and MCMI-III. Study collaborators included therapists at the Master’s level, do ctoral students, doctoral candidates, as well as licensed psychologists. All graduate level therapists were supervised by experienced, licensed psychologists.

Placement of the instruments into packets varied randomly to control for sequence and order effects, with the MCMI—III preceding the ICD in only some cases (Kazdin, 1998). The random order for each packet will be determined by coin toss.

Participants were informed in writing about the purpose and procedures involved in the study. All participants had the right to withdraw from the study at any time without explanation.

Participation in the study was completely voluntary. Descriptive data were gathered including age, sex, marital status, educational level, and a yes-no response as to current use of psychotropic medication. Participants were informed that their responses would remain anonymous and confidential.

97

CHAPTER 4

RESULTS

A correlational research design with a sample of a heterogeneous group of adult outpatients was used to ascertain the relationship between psychometric variables encompassed by the ICD

(frequency of cognitive distortions) and the MCMI-III (psychological diagnoses). In addition, results were analyzed to test the psychometric properties of the ICD. Data gathered from these individuals comprised the entire sample used for statistical computation and analyses. Study collaborators recruited clients to volunteer from among their patients; the collaborators included practicing psychologists, counselors, practicum students or interns in the Greater Philadelphia area, recruited clients to volunteer from among their patients. Both study collaborators and the

MCMI-III screened participants for a minimum of eight years of education.

All psychological outpatients were combined to form one outpatient sample for the purposes of this study. The sample (N = 168) included those meeting objective criteria for a clinical diagnosis either on Axis I and/or Axis II.

Diagnosis was operationally defined as a score of 75 or greater on any of the ten MCMI-III

Axis I Clinical Syndrome Scales or any of the 14 Personality Scales. This provided objective clinical diagnosis on a widely accepted measure of psychopathology. A number of variables were analyzed, including participant demographics, diagnostic variables, frequency of cognitive distortions, and the intercorrelation of these factors.

98

Descriptive Statistics

Age

Participants ranged in age from 18 to 86 years of age. The mean age in both groups was 42.8 with a standard deviation of 18.1.

Gender

The gender distribution for the total sample (N = 168) consisted of 48 males (29.3%) and

116 females (70.7%).

Marital Status

Marital status was distributed in the total sample as 65 single (67.3%), 69 married (42.1%), and 29 (17.7%) endorsing “other”, the latter was further defined by the MCMI-III as separated, divorced, widowed, or cohabitating.

99

Use of Psychotropic Medication

Of the 154 participants responding, 114 (67.9%) reported not using psychotropic medication, whereas 39 (23.2%) reported current medication use.

Internal Consistency

Cronbach’s Alpha for the ICD scale was .97 for all ICD items, indicating good internal consistency and homogeneity of item content.

Relationship to Demographic Variables

The Relationship between the MCMI-III and the Total ICD Scores

Statistical Package for Social Sciences version 10.0 (SPSS 10.0) was used in the data analysis. A number of Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficients, one-tailed were gathered to test the relationship between the number (operationalized as the number MCMI-III subscale scores of 75 or greater) and severity (operationalized as the cumulative sum of MCMI-

III BR scores of 75 or greater) of clinical syndrome(s) and/or personality disorder(s) on the one hand, and the total frequency of cognitive distortion on the other hand (hypotheses 1, 2, 3, 5, 6,

100

and 7). A one-way ANOVA was conducted to determine if there was a significant difference in

Total ICD scores between individuals diagnosable on only one clinical axis versus those

diagnosable with comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders (hypothesis 4). Finally, a series of

Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficients was also conducted to illustrate the

relationship between the presence of specific Axis I and Axis II disorders on the one hand

(operationalized as MCMI-III scores of 75 or greater on any subscale), and the frequency of cognitive distortions (operationalized as total ICD scores) on the other hand (hypothesis 8).

Hypothesis 1

It was hypothesized that the number of Axis I and Axis II disorders (MCMI-III BR scores of

75 or greater on any Axis I or Axis II scale) for which an individual met criteria would correlate

with the total ICD score. The results demonstrated a significantly positive Pearson correlation

coefficient (r = .662, r2 = .438, p <.001, one-tailed) on this dimension. This illustrates a coeffient of determination, indicating that almost 44% of the variance in the number of Axis I and Axis II disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria was attributable to differences in Total

ICD scores.

Hypothesis 2

It was hypothesized that the number of PDs for which an individual may be diagnosed

(MCMI-III BR scores of 75 or greater on any Axis II scale) would positively correlate with the

total ICD score. A significant and moderate Pearson correlation coefficient (r = .563, r2 = .312, p

< .001, one-tailed) was obtained on this variable. Consequently, it appeared that 31.2% of the

variance in the number of Axis II disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria was

attributable to the differences in Total ICD score.

101

Hypothesis 3

It was hypothesized that the number of Axis I clinical syndromes for which an individual could be diagnosed (MCMI-III BR scores of 75 or greater on any Axis I scale) would positively correlate with the total ICD score. Again, a significantly positive Pearson correlation coefficient

(r = .638, r2 = .407, p <.001) was obtained, indicating that over 40% of the variance in the number Axis I disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria was attributable to the differences in Total ICD score.

Hypothesis 4

It was hypothesized that the number of comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders (MCMI-III

BR scores of 75 or greater on both Axis I and Axis II scales) would report more frequent cognitive distortion (greater total ICD scores) than will individuals meeting criteria for only one

Axis, that is for either personality disorder(s) or clinical syndrome(s). One-way ANOVA results

(see Table 3) demonstrated a significant difference between individuals diagnosable on only one clinical axis versus those diagnosable with comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders, including total

ICD scores (F = 74.778, df = 123, p < .001). It should be noted that only five participants met diagnostic criteria for only Axis I without PD pathology, making comparison with that group superfluous.

102

Table 3

Analysis of Variance for Comorbid Axis I and Axis II Conditions

Source Df F MS p

Between groups 1 74.78 83333.3 .001

Within groups 122 1114.40

Note. Individuals with a larger number of comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders experienced a

significantly greater frequency of cognitive distortion than did those who met criteria for fewer

comorbid conditions. (Participants meeting only Axis I criteria were excluded do to a small n (n

= 5).

Hypothesis 5

It was hypothesized that the severity of Axis I conditions (the cumulative sum of MCMI-III

BR scores of 75 or greater on Axis I scales) would positively correlate with total ICD scores.

Pearson correlation results indicated that there was a significant, positive relationship between

these variables (r = .693, r2 = .48. p <.001, one-tailed). Thus, almost half of the variance in severity of Axis I syndromes was attributable to differences in total ICD scores.

Hypothesis 6

It was hypothesized that the severity of PDs (the cumulative sum of MCMI-III BR scores of

75 or greater on all Axis II scales) would positively correlate with total ICD scores. Once again,

Pearson correlations indicated a highly significantly positive relationship on this dimension (r =

.757, r2 = 573, p <.001, one-tailed). In other words, 57 percent of the variance in severity of

personality disturbance was attributable to differences in total ICD scores.

103

Hypothesis 7

It was hypothesized that the total severity of psychological dysfunction, independent of

whether or not the diagnoses occurred on Axis I or Axis II (the cumulative sum of all MCMI-III

BR scores of 75 or greater on all Axis I and Axis II scales), would positively correlate with total

ICD scores. Again, results of Pearson correlations suggested a strong and highly significant

relationship in this regard (r = .748, r2 = .56, p <.001, one-tailed). This means that, once again, well over half of the variance in severity of psychological dysfunction, across clinical axes, was attributable to differences in total ICD scores.

Table 4 presents correlation coefficients for frequency of cognitive distortions in relation both to the severity and the number of disorders.

Table 4

Number and Severity of Psychological Disorders across Axis I and Axis II and the Relation to

Total Frequency of Cognitive Distortion.

Table 4

Axis I Axis II Both (Axes I and I)

Number of Disorders .638** .563** .662**

Severity of Disorders .693** .757** .748**

104

Note. Pearson coefficients, when comparing the total frequency of cognitive distortion either

with the number or the severity of psychological disorder, significant correlations were found for

both Axis I and Axis II disorders, severally and when comorbid. **p < .001.

Hypothesis 8

A series of Point-Biserial Correlations were conducted to determine the validity of the

hypothesized positive correlation between individual clinical diagnostic scores on either Axis I

and/or Axis II for which an individual tested positive (MCMI-III BR scores of 75 or greater on any Axis I and/or Axis II scale[s]) and total ICD scores. The diagnostic variables were dichotomous, meaning that individuals either met or did not meet operational criteria for each specific disorder. For example, a participant with a score of 87 on schizoid and 64 on narcissistic met criteria for the former, but not the latter. This positive relationship was confirmed by multiple, significant Pearson correlations for most Axis I and Axis II disorder, on the one hand, and Total ICD scores, on the other hand (see Table 5). However, a number of exceptions were discovered.

First, although correlated in a positive direction, both bipolar disorder (r = .104, p < .109,

one-tailed) and drug dependence (r = .063, p <, 228, one-tailed) failed to reach statistical

significance. Conversely, three Axis II disorders were significantly but negatively correlated with

frequency of cognitive distortion, specifically, narcissistic (NPD), histrionic (HPD), and

compulsive PDs (OCPD) which correlated negatively with the total ICD scores, as follows: NPD

(r = -.214, r2 = .046, p < .05, one-tailed); HPD (r = -.266, r2 = .071, p < .001, one-tailed); and

OCPD (r =-.343. r2 = .118, p < .001).

105

Table 5

The Relationship between Frequency of Cognitive Distortion (Total ICD score) and Individual

Psychological Disorders.

Axis II Axis I

Schizoid .233** Anxiety .613***

Avoidant .494*** Somatoform .247*

Depressive .491*** Bipolar .104

Dependent .518*** Dysthymia .436***

Histrionic -266*** Alcohol Dependence -

Narcissistic -.214** Drug Dependence .063

Antisocial .188* PTSD .300***

Aggressive/Sadistic .154* Thought Disorder .266***

Compulsive -.343*** Major Depression .344

Passive-Aggressive .437*** Delusional Disorder .334***

Self-Defeating .513**

Shizotypal .348***

Borderline .384***

Paranoid .354***

Note. Most Axis I and Axis II conditions were positively and significantly correlated with frequency of cognitive distortion. Exceptions included bipolar disorder, drug dependence, NPD,

HPD, and OCPD. *p <.05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

106

CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was twofold: First, the study sought to determine whether or not the frequency of cognitive distortions correlated with the number and severity of psychological disorders across Axis I and Axis II. Second, the study endeavored to further assess the validity and reliability of a promising new self-report measure of cognitive distortions by correlating the ICD with clinical diagnoses as determined by the MCMI-III in a heterogeneous adult clinical outpatient sample. Significant positive findings indicate that these goals were achieved. This chapter summarizes the study, expands on relevant findings and discusses the implications of the results in light of the existing literature. Study limitations and directions for future research are also explored.

Cronbach’s analysis of all ICD items indicated strong homogeneity of item content

(Cronbach’s Alpha = .97). This finding indicates that 97% of Total ICD scores was due to shared variance in the concept being measured, that is, cognitive distortions. Such internal consistency provides valid support for using the Total ICD scores as a measure of the frequency of cognitive distortion. In this manner, the study examined the relationship between the frequency of cognitive distortions and the number and/or severity of psychological disorders.

Participants completed comprehensive assessment instruments for Axis I and II disorders, as well as cognitive distortions; this was done to evaluate the impact of cognitive distortions on co-occurring disorders. Specifically, the MCMI-III and the ICD were measured to determine the relationship between cognitive distortion and the number and severity of psychological disorders along Axis I and Axis II. Results supported all of the eight initial hypotheses.

107

Demographic Characteristics

Study participants included an ethnically diverse sample of clinical outpatients with a wide

variety of clinical diagnoses across the entire adult age span. Participants ranged in age from 18

to 86 years of age. The mean age was 42.8 years with a standard deviation of 18.1.

The gender distribution for the total sample (N = 168) consisted of 48 males (29.3%) and

116 females (70.7%). Marital status was distributed in the total sample as 65 single (39.9%), 69 married (42.3%), and 29 (17.8%) separated, divorced, widowed, or cohabitating. Of the 154 participants responding, 114 (67.9%) reported not using psychotropic medication, whereas 39

(23.2%) reported current medication use.

Major Findings

Presence of Psychopathology

As was hypothesized, individuals meeting criteria for any diagnosis (BR of 75 or greater on any MCMI-III Axis scale[s]), on Axis I clinical disorders or on Axis II PDs, reported a higher frequency of cognitive distortions than those who were free of diagnosable psychological disorders. Moreover, individuals meeting criteria for a greater number of diagnoses, be they on

Axis I and/or Axis II PDs, reported a higher frequency of cognitive distortions than did individuals with fewer diagnosable psychological disorders or than who were otherwise free of diagnosable psychological disorders (r = .662, r2 = .438, p <.001, one-tailed). This illustrates the

108

fact that almost 44% of the variance in the ICD was attributable to the number of Axis I and Axis

II disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria—the more disorders, the more

frequently they engaged in cognitive distortion.

These findings are consistent with the cognitive model of emotional disorders (Alford &

Beck, 1997; Beck, 1967; Beck et al., 1979), which predicts that dysfunctional cognition

correlates with psychopatholgy—in this case—generally across Axis I clinical syndromes and

Axis II PDs.

Axis I Clinical Syndromes.

Number of Axis I diagnoses.

The study found that as the number of clinical disorders for which an individual met criteria

increased (the total number of MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes, Severe Clinical Syndromes

elevated above BR 75), so too did their tendency to engage in cognitive distortion. A

significantly positive Pearson correlation coefficient (r = .638, r2 = .407, p <.001) was obtained, indicating that over 40% of the variance in the ICD was attributable to the number Axis I disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria. This means that an individual who met criteria for only one Axis I diagnosis engaged in less frequent cognitive distortion than individuals with a greater number of clinical syndromes. These effects were incremental, with the tendency to engage in cognitive distortion increasing as the number of Axis I clinical syndromes increased.

109

These results are consistent with the empirical findings of Najavitis et al. (2004), indicating

that individuals diagnosed with comorbid clinical syndromes exhibit greater maladaptive

cognition. This is also consistent with Beck’s (1967; Beck et al., 1979) cognitive model of

emotional disorders which predicts that dysfunctional cognition correlates with psychopatholgy -

--in this case—generally across Axis I clinical syndromes than those meeting criteria for only

one disorder.

Severity of Axis I clinical syndromes.

The severity of Axis I conditions (cumulative sum of all MCMI-III scale scores of BR 75 or greater on any of the Clinical Syndromes and/or Severe Clinical Syndrome scales) correlated significantly and positively with the frequency of cognitive distortions (r = .693, r2 = .48. p

<.001, one-tailed). This indicates that almost half of the variance in severity of Axis I syndromes

was attributable to the frequency of cognitive distortion. These results are also consistent with

Najavits et al. (2004), who found that individuals meeting criteria for two comorbid Axis I

syndromes (PTSD and substance use disorders) were more liable to engage in more frequent

cognitive distortions than those meeting criteria for only one of those disorders, operationalized

as Cognitive Distortion Scale scores.

This finding lends additional empirical suppo rt to the cognitive model of emotional

disorders for various Axis I clinical syndromes (Beck, 1967, 1976), which posits that schema-

driven “cognitive errors” powerfully influence affect and behavior (Beck et al., 1979, p. 10).

110

Perhaps at the most fundamental level, one of the principal goals of CT is to reduce these characteristic patterns of distorted thinking. Even schema-focused approaches ultimately aim to replace maladaptive core beliefs with more adaptive cognitions, which maintain, and are perpetuated by, one’s cognitive distortions (Young, 1999). Thus, as was expected, the severity and number of clinical disorders correlated with the frequency of cognitive distortion.

These findings also lend support to Millon’s assertions (Millon & Davis, 1996; Millon et al,

1997) that higher MCMI-III clinical syndrome scale scores reflect more severe Axis I severity.

As measured by the total frequency of cognitive distortion, the present study supports Millon’s contention.

Axis II Personality Disorders

Number of personality disorders.

The number of PDs for which an individual met criteria (the number of all significant

MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern scales and Severe Personality Pathology scale that scored

BR 75 or greater) correlated significantly with the frequency of cognitive distortions (r = .563, r2

= .312, p < .001, one-tailed). Consequently, it appears that 31.2% of the variance in the number

Axis II disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria was attributable to the total frequency of cognitive distortions.

A literature review revealed that studies investigating dysfunctional thinking in individuals with comorbid psychological diagnoses have focused on the influence of PDs on various Axis I conditions. No studies were found that examined the relationship between the number of multiple PD diagnoses and cognitive distortion. This is surprising because the correlation

111

between PD diagnoses have been shown to be significant. For example, Beck et al. (2001)

determined that 32% of 756 outpatients with a primary Axis II diagnosis also met criteria for a

secondary PD. These authors also posited the fact that although the Personal Belief

Questionnaire (PBQ; Beck & Beck, 1991) differentiated five separate PDs (avoidant, dependent,

obsessive–compulsive, narcissistic, and paranoid PDs), the PBQ subscales were moderately to

strongly intercorrelated. Beck et al. (2001) proposed the idea that this corroborates the cognitive

model of personality disorders and, perhaps, a general distress factor that may be a cause, a

correlate, or a product of the inner experience and interpersonal functioning of individuals with

PDs (APA, 2000; Beck et al., 2001; Millon, 1999). It is likely that cognitive distortions

contribute to this general distress factor.

. Severity of personality disorders.

There was a significant and positive relationship between the severity of PDs (cumulative sum of all MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern and Severe Personality Pathology scales scores of BR 75 or greater) and the frequency of cognitive distortions (r = .757, r2 = .573, p <.001, one-

tailed). This means that 57 percent of the variance in severity of personality disturbance was

explained by differences in total ICD scores.

These findings are consistent with a recent study conducted by O’Connor & Dyce (2001),

which demonstrated the fact that the MCMI—III was able to differentiate those individuals with

PDs from those individuals failing to meet Axis II diagnostic criteria. These investigators

determined that disordered personalities were very similar in characteristic but notably different

in the degree from which they varied from the norm. Moreover, as in the present study, this

112 difference was detected by the MCMI-III. Also similar to the present study, although the degree of variance from “normal” predicted the presence of Axis II pathology, it failed to predict any specific personality disorder.

Consequently, the ICD successfully predicted presence and severity of Axis II conditions.

Although the ICD predicted the extremity of the disorder and presence of personality disorder(s), it did not, however, predict the diagnosis.

Comorbid Axis I and Axis II Disorders

Number of comorbid clinical syndromes and personality disorders.

As was predicted, the ANOVA revealed that individuals with a greater number of comorbid

Axis I and Axis II disorders engaged in more cognitive distortion than did individuals meeting diagnostic criteria for fewer disorders (F = 74.778, df = 123, p < .001).

This finding offers support for the cognitive model of emotional disorders (Beck et al.,

1979) and the cognitive model of PDs (Beck et al., 2004), as well as the evolutionary model of psychopathology (Millon et al, 1996), which respectively assert that there may be an additive effect when comorbidity is present whether or not the disorders appear on either of the two

DSM-IV-TR diagnostic axes. Moreover, the greater the number of diagnoses, the more severe the psychopathology (Diguer, Barber, & Luborsky, 1993; Farmer & Nelson-Gray, 1990; Shea,

Glass, Pilkonis, Watkins, & Docherty, 1987), operationalized in the present case, as an increased frequency of cognitive distortions.

113

Total severity of all psychological dysfunction.

The total severity of psychological dysfunction across Axis I and Axis II (cumulative scores

of 75 or greater on all MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes, Severe Clinical Syndromes, Clinical

Personality Patterns, Severe Personality Pathology) positively correlated with cognitive

distortions (r = .748, r2 = .56, p <.001, one-tailed). This means that well over half of the variance

in severity of psychological dysfunction, across clinical axes, was attributable to differences in

total ICD scores. Moreover, the ICD was able to predict severity on both Axis I and Axis II.

Millon et al. (1997) conceptualized Axis I clinical syndromes as “extensions or distortions

of the patient’s basic personality pattern... waxing and waning over time depending on the

impact of stressful situations”(p. 22) . The multiaxial model po sits an interaction between Axis I

and Axis II symptomatology in which personality is defined as the “…overall capacity to

perceive and to cope with our psychosocial world…” (Millon et al., 1996, p. 120). Personality is

further conceptualized as analogous to an immune system, protecting the individual from the

fluctuating external stressors. However, Axis I symptoms may occur because the PD patients

dysfunctional “immune systems” fails to protect the individual from stress. When this occurs,

Axis I clinical syndromes arise in addition to the preexisting PD symptoms. This increases the total severity of the individual’s dysfunction (Millon et al., 1996) . In other words, all other things being equal, an individual meeting the diagnostic criteria for both major depressive disorder and dependent personality disorder is expected to evince more impairment and/or distress than another person meeting diagnostic criteria for a disorder on only one of these axes. This was the case, as measured by the frequency of cognitive distortions.

114

Comorbid diagnoses across axes may lead to a pattern in which cognitive distortion can

constrict behavior in a fashion that may actually exacerbate existing difficulties, leading to

vicious circles of cognitive distortion, distress, and interpersonal impairment. When this occurs,

one’s cognition may be further impaired by the resulting Axis I clinical syndromes. Prolonged

Axis I symptoms can, in turn, exacerbate personality dysfunction (Millon et al., 1996; Millon &

Davis, 1996).

Because of this reciprocal interaction between Axis I and Axis II, it was expected that

individuals with greater total pathology would experience more frequent cognitive distortions,

independent of whether or not this pathology arose from Axis I or Axis II. This explains why

Millon & Davis, (1997) posit the idea that the presence of Axis I pathology supports the presence

of personality pathology. Our findings supported this theory.

The Relationship between Cognitive Distortions and Individual Disorders

A series of Point-Biserial Correlations confirmed the fact that the hypothesized positive correlation between most of the MCMI-III individual clinical diagnostic scores and total ICD scores was significant (see Table 4). According to the cognitive model, cognitive distortions were expected to correlate positively with psychological disorders as assessed by the MCMI-III

both on Axis I or Axis II. This was the case for most Axis I and Axis II disorders. The major

findings of the present study were as follows: First: the results lend strong support for the

cognitive model. Regardless of whether or not disorders occur on Axis I or Axis II, the severity

of the disturbance was significantly attributable to the frequency of cognitive distortions. As the

frequency of cognitive distortions increased, so too did the severity of disturbance. Second, as

115

the frequency of cognitive distortions increased, so too did the number of disorders for which an

individual met criteria. In short, the more disturbed the patients were, the more frequently they

engaged in distorted thinking. This occurred regardless of the specific diagnosis.

However, a number of exceptions were discovered. First, although correlated in a positive

direction, both bipolar disorder (r = .104, p < .109, one-tailed) and drug dependence (r = .063, p

<, 228, one-tailed) failed to reach statistical significance. Lack of correlation between cognitive

distortions and these particular two Axis I syndromes may be partially explained by the

empirically demonstrated environmental, physiological, and genetic components attendant to

these syndromes. In the case of bipolar disorder, for example, this may explain why CT alone

has been shown as insufficient in ameliorating bipolar disorder and is, rather, recommended as

an adjunct to, but never a substitute for pharmacotherapy (e.g., Scott, 2001; Tsai, Chen, Kuo,

Lee, Lee, & Strakowski, 2001). If CT ameliorates cognitive distortions in bipolar disorder, but

other factors account for a greater proportion of dysfunction and distress than such dysfunctional

thinking, it would be understandable that CT would not be a sufficient treatment for this

disorder.

Similarly, the lack of significant correlations between cognitive distortions and drug

dependence may result from a significant contribution of po werful physiological and genetic

influences, as well as significant behavioral and interpersonal factors that impact substance

abusers (e.g., Higgins, Heil, Lussier, 2004; Robinson & Berridge, 2003).

Additionally, three Axis II disorders were significantly but negatively correlated with

frequency of cognitive distortion. Specifically, NPD (r = -.214, r2 = ...046, p < .05, one-tailed);

HPD (r = -.266, r2 = .071, p < .001, one-tailed); and OCPD (r =-.343. r2 = .118, p < .001).

116

However, a review of the data and the literature should help to explain the unexpected

negative correlations between reported frequency of cognitive distortions and the presence of

HPD, OCPD, and NPD. Although these results were surprising, they were not without precedent or explanation.

First, participants meeting the operational definition for the three PD diagnoses, HPD,

OCPD, and NPD reported being significantly less likely to engage cognitive distortion; these were the only participants whose MCMI Validity Scales were questionable on one very important item—disclosure. In fact, for these individuals, the MCMI-III Modifying Index X or

Disclosure Scale was as follows: HPD (r = -.252. p <. 002, two-tailed), OCPD (-.284, p < .001, two-tailed), and although not statistically significant NPD (r = -.088, p < ...277, two-tailed).

The Disclosure Scale was designed to detect the extent to which people are honest and revealing, with low scores (below 34) indicating possible withholding of information (Millon,

1987). The tendency to withhold appeared to be represented in the negative ICD correlations.

Although the Disclosure Scale was originally designed to be neutral as to simulating psychopatholgy (Millon, 1987), it has demonstrated the ability to detect both faking good and faking bad and is generally highly intercorrelated with the other two MCMI-III validity scales

(Bagby, Gillis, Toner, & Goldberg, 1991).

Significantly, in a recent study, Shoenberg, Dorr, Morgan and Burke (2004) demonstrated

remarkably similar findings, because three MMPI-2 validity scales Infrequency (F), Back-Page

Infrequency (Fb), and Dissimulation (F-K) were highly correlated with the MCMI-III Disclosure scale. Moreover, there was a significant negative correlation between these three MMPI-2 validity scales and the same three PD scales that correlated negatively with the MCMI-III

Disclosure scale: NPD, HPD, and OCPD. Thus, the present study replicated the findings that

117

individuals meeting criteria for NPD, HPD, and OCPD diagnoses disclose less information that

is likely to be interpreted as dissimulation and, in the present case, report less cognitive

distortions than individuals meeting criteria for other Axis I and Axis II conditions.

Additionally, the Disclosure Scale is a Modifying index, which allows for adjustment,

increasing the probability of still attaining an accurate diagnosis. Thus, participants may have

disclosed a sufficient amount to be properly diagnosed on the three PDs in question, yet their

tendency to withhold may have been reflected in their ICD responses (Millon et al., 1997). In

other words, with sufficient disclosure, scores of participants who met MCMI-III criteria for

NPD, HPD and OCPD may have been more positively correlated with the Total ICD.

This begs the question: Why would individuals with these three PDs be less likely to

disclose? In the case of NPD, grandiose self-importance, hypersensitivity to evaluation and need for might understandably make it difficult to disclose or even recognize certain symptoms (APA, 2000). This failure to disclose could simultaneously reduce the MCMI-III

Disclosure Scale and decrease these patients’ tendencies to divulge cognitive distortions.

Consequently, as the NPD score is compensated by the Disclosure Scale Modifying index, and the ICD score is depressed by lack of disclosure, a negative correlation may be produced between NPD scores and Total ICD scores.

It has been demonstrated in the case of HPD that individuals meeting criteria for HPD are exquisitely concerned with self-presentation, experience higher autonomic nervous system , and are easily influenced by other people and circumstances. This may make these individuals less likely to disclose their histrionic symptoms because doiong so would be incongruent with their constant —both public and private—or, alternately, they may not even be conscious of the information that they should disclose due to

118

their exteroceptive focus (APA, 2000; Beck et al., 2004; Gorenstein & Newman, 1980;

Hamburger, Lilienfeld, & Hogben, 1996).

Finally, why would individuals scoring high on the MCMI-III Compulsive Scale disclose less than other PDs? The answer is likely to be the perfectionism, indecision, and rigidity that may make them unaware of or unwilling to admit to internal thoughts and emotions (Beck et al.,

2004) that may be detected by the MCMI-III Disclosure Scale. Also, Barber and Muenz (1996) found that patients with comorbid OCPD and depression responded significantly better when treated with interpersonal therapy rather than CT; this could indicate that there may be some mechanism other than cognitive distortions, such as interpersonal or behavioral components that account for a large part of the variance in this disorder. However, Barber and Muenz hypothesized that the interpersonal therapy differentially addressed perfectionistic, internal coping strategies characteristic of OCPD, whereas CT, as operationalized in their study, was more effective in ameliorating the externalized coping strategies.

Of course there are other possible explanations for the negative correlations; one of these is that HPD, OCPD, and NPD patients actually engage in less cognitive distortions than other clinical patients. Another alternative account is that the Disclosure Scale actually invalidated the

MCMI-III profiles and that this data should be discarded. However, this would not explain why only one of three highly intercorrelated MCMI-III validity scales would correlate negatively with only these three PDs. Similar results were also obtained by Schoenberg et al. (2004), who showed that MCMI-III profiles were indeed valid on both the MCMI-III and MMPI-2 Validity

Scales. Thus, the characteristics of HPD, OCPD, and NPD likely account for the fact that these three PDs correlated negatively with Total ICD scores.

119

In support of this conclusion, T. Millon (personal communication, September 20, 2004) posited the concept that individuals scoring higher on the three PD subscales in question, NPD,

OCPD, and HPD, “tended to be closer to normal on the continuum of personality style. These individuals may or may not be pathological and this may reflect less cognitive dysfunction. They also have a tendency not to communicate as much as others regarding their problems and this is reflected in the negative correlation on the Dsclosure Scale” with the Total ICD scores. “It is not surprising that they (NPD, OCPD, and HPD patients) would engage in less cognitive dysfunction” than other PD types.

In summary, the study provided further evidence for the cognitive model; that is, that cognition, specifically cognitive distortion, significantly predicted the severity of dysfunction, including the number of disorders on both diagnostic axes. Additionally, there is evidence of an additive effect, with maladaptive thinking intensifying as severity and the number of diagnoses increases. This may explain why it is more difficult for clinicians to treat individuals with more severe and/or comorbid conditions.

Additionally, the study provided good psychometric support for the ICD in the form of concurrent validity with the MCMI-III. Conversely, the study demonstrated that the MCMI-III is sensitive to differences in cognition. As a consequence, the clinical value of the ICD and the

MCMI-III, when used as tools for assessment and treatment of psychological disorders should not be underestimated.

The ICD is an efficient, practical instrument that can provide clinicians with important data, accounting for about half of the variance in symptomatology for both clinical syndromes and

PDs. Individual ICD items can also inform the clinician about which specific maladaptive

120

thoughts and cognitive distortions to target as treatment progresses. Similarly, the MCMI-III has proven, once again, to be a valid assessment instrument for clinical research.

Limitations of the Study

The ICD appears to offer promise as an instrument, providing a cohesive and efficient means to assess cognitive distortions relevant to both Axis I and Axis II disorders. However, additional examination of the psychometric properties of this new measure is urged. Areas requiring additional inve stigation for the ICD might include additional test–retest reliability and convergent validity, comparing the ICD to other existing cognitive distortion scales; it might also involve larger sample sizes of participants with particular disorders to study the relative influence of individual cognitive distortions on those specific disorders (Schoenberg et al.,

2004).

Another potential limitation of the study is the fact that severity was operationally defined with a novel formula, the cumulative score of all MCMI-III scores of 75 or above.

However, this formula was consistent with Millon’s (et al., 1996) theory and his personal communication of October 5, 2004.

The MCMI-III is one of the most widely accepted measures of psychopathology and perhaps the most widely used measure of personality; it was, in fact, designed for diagnostic screening and assessment of clinical populations in clinical research and practice. However, others have reported its limitations. These include questions regarding reductions in construct and convergent validity from the earlier versions of the MCMI, as well as discriminant correlations that were higher than convergent validities on a number of scales. This means that

121

some scales correlated more highly with measures of other disorders than the ones they were

intended to assess (Rogers, Salekin, & Sewell, 1999). These findings and others have led Rogers

et al. to claim that the MCI, MCMI-II, and the MCMI-III are really separate measures requiring

further validation.

Finally, the study employed two self-report instruments: the ICD and the MCMI-III.

There are a number of potential limitations attendant on all such instruments, including idiosyncratic differences in the way participants interpret individual items, the effect of participants’ current affective state on responses, the reactive nature of such instruments, and the influences of social desirability and self-presentation (Kazdin, 1998). Additionally, the use of forced choices may limit and distort responses (Birelson, Hudson, Buchanan, & Wolff, 1987) .

Limitations of all ps ychometric instruments argue for a multitrait, multimethod approach to assessment (Kenny & Kashy, 1992).

Recommendations for Future Research

In future research, it would be beneficial to explore whether or not particular distortions on the ICD are more relevant to specific disorders. Longitudinal research could also investigate whether or not there is an immediate relationship between symptom remission and the abatement of cognitive distortions or whether or not there is some lag time on either factor. It would also be interesting to determine whether or not the negative correlations between HPD, OCPD, and NPD were replicable in a larger sample and with other measures of cognitive distortion.

Furthermore, Beck et al. (2001) determined that specific dysfunctional beliefs attendant on PDs (Beck & Freeman, 1990), as measured by the Personal Beliefs Questionnaire (Beck &

122

Beck, 1991), could predict specific PDs. It is recommended that the data collection begun in the current study be extended to obtain a sufficiently large sample; this would demonstrate whether or not individuals with specific disorders have a greater tendency to engage in particular cognitive distortions. Moreover, efforts should be made to replicate Yurica’s (2002) impressive test-retest findings to ensure that extraneous events, such as the terrorist attacks of September 11,

2001, did not unduly influence the ICD’s reliability results (Yurica, 2002).

Finally, while the ICD may explain up to half of the variance in psychological disorders, other factors must also contribute to psychological dysfunction. These factors may include dynamics related to an interaction of influences, be they biological, social, genetic, neurological, genetic, physiological, or aspects of cognition and behavior other than cognitive distortion.

These factors should be the subject of future research.

Conclusions

Despite the above limitations, there is proof that the ICD has once again provided a valid and reliable measure of cognitive distortion underlying a wide range of psychological disorders across both Axis I and Axis II. The current study provided further support for the psychometric properties of the ICD and validated its application beyond the realm of Axis I and into PDs in a heterogeneous, adult, outpatient sample. The ICD also appears to be valid in older populations because the present sample ranged in age from 18 to 86 years. The ICD appears to be a valuable, psychometrically sound instrument for the evaluation and treatment of clinical syndromes as well as PDs by illuminating treatable cognitive distortions that correlate so highly with dysfunction. .

123

This study has also demonstrated that reduced frequency of cognitive distortions correlates with diminution in the severity and number of diagnosable disorders. Consequently, to the extent that clinicians successfully attend to and reduce this variety of dysfunctional thinking, they should see a commensurate reduction in pathology because the present study has demonstrated the fact that the frequency of cognitive distortions accounts for up to 50% of the variance in the number and severity of many psychological disorders on both diagnostic axes. Thus, using a tool, such as the ICD, that illuminates these troubling tendencies may greatly aid in establishing crucial baseline data, advance the evaluation of treatment response, and thereby improve treatment outcome.

124

References

Abramowitz, J. S. (1997). Effectiveness of psychological and pharmacological treatments

for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a quantitative review. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology, 65, 44-52.

Agras, W. S., Rottister, E. M., Arnow, B., Schneider, J. H., Telch, C. S., Raeburn, S. D., et al.

(1992). Pharmacologic and cognitive-behavioral treatment for bulimia nervosa: A controlled

comparison. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 82-87.

Allen, J. G., Console, D. A., Brethour, J. R. (2000). Screening for trauma-related sleep

disturbance in Women Admitted for Specialized Inpatient Treatment. Journal of Trauma &

Dissociation. 1, 59-83.

Allen, J., Coyne, L. & Huntoon, J. (1998). Complex posttraumatic stress disorder in women from

a psychometric perspective. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70(2), 277-298.

Alford, B.A., & Beck, A.T. (1997). The integrative power of cognitive therapy. New York:

Guilford.

Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988). Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In

Alloy L. B. (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 223–265). New York: Guilford

Press.

Alnaes, R. & Torgensen, S. (1989). Personality and personality disorders among patients with

major depression in combination with dysthymic or cyclothymic disorders. Acta

Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 79(4), 363-369.

Alnaes, R. & Torgersen, S. (1997). Personality and personality disorders predict

development and relapses of major depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 95, 336-342.

Retrieved November 25, 2002, from OVID Database.

125

American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (Text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychological Association, (2004) Testimony of the American Psychological

Association Submitted for the record House Appropriations Subcommittee on Labor,

Health and Human Services and Education regarding funding for fiscal year 2004.

Andersson, T. 1994. Conceptual Polemics - Dialectic Studies of Concept Formation. Ph.D.

thesis, Cognitive Science Dept. Lund: Lund University.

Andrews, B. & Brewin, C.R. (1990). Attributions of blame for marital violence: A study of

antecedents and consequences. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 757-767.

Auerbach, J. (1984). Validation of two scales for narcissistic personality disorder. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 48(6), 649-653.

Bagby, R. R., Gillis, J. R., Toner, B. B., Goldberg. J. (1991). Detecting fake-good and fake-bad

responding on the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-II. Psychological Assessment

3, 496-498. Retrieved December 5, 2003, from PsycARTICLES Database.

Barber, J. P., & Muenz, L. R. (1996). The role of avoidance and obsessiveness in matching

patients to cognitive and interpersonal psychotherapy: Empirical findings from the treatment

for depression collaborative research program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 64, 951—958. Retrieved December 5, 2003, from PsycARTICLES Database.

Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R. & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic

attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and , 62, 893-912.

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. New

126

York: Cambridge University Press.

Barratt, E. S. (1983). The biological basis of impulsiveness: the significance of timing and

rhythm disorders. Personality and Individual Differences 4, 387-391.

Barriga, A. Q., Landau, J. R., Stinson, B. L., & Liau, A. K., & Gibbs, J. C. (2000). Cognitive

disortion and problem behaviors in adolescents. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27 (1), 36-

56.

Baumeister, R. F. Catanese, K, R., & Wallace, H. M. (2002). Conquest by force. A

narcissistic reactance theory of and sexual . Review of General Psychology, 6,

92-135. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsychINFO Database.

Baumeister, R. F., & Jones, E. E. (1978). When self-presentation is constrained by the target's

knowledge: Consistency and compensation. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 36, 608-618. Retrieved December 3, 2003, from PsycARTICLES Database.

Bayon, C., Hill, K., Svrakic, D., Przybeck, T. & Cloninger, C. (1996). Dimensional assessment

of personality in an out-patient sample: relations of the systems of Millon and Cloninger.

Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30(5), 341-352.

Beck, A. T. (1961). A systematic investigation of depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry,

2, 163-170.

Beck, A. T. (1963). Thinking and depression: Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions,

Psychological Bulletin, 91,3-26.

Beck, A. T. (1964). Thinking and depression: II. Theory and therapy. Archives of General

Psychiatry, 10, 561-571.

Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New

York: Harper & Row.

127

Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York:

International Universities Press.

Beck, A. T. (1991). Cognitive therapy: A 30-year retrospective. American Psychologist, 46, 368-

375. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsychINFO Database.

Beck, A. T. (1993). Cognitive therapy: Past, present, and future. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 61, 194-198. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsychINFO Databases.

Beck, A. T. (1996). Beyond belief: A theory of modes, personality, and psychopathology.

In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 1—25). New York:

Guilford Press.

Beck, A. T. (2000). Member's Corner: Cognitive approaches to schizophrenia: A

paradigm shift? Based on the 1999 Joseh Zubin award address. Psychopathology

Research, 10(2), 3-10.

Beck, A. T., & Beck, J. S. (1991). The Personality Belief Questionnaire. Unpublished

assessment instrument. The Beck Institute for Cognitive Therapy and Research, Bala

Cynwyd, Pennsylvania.

Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Berchick, R. J., Stewart, B. & Steer, R. A. (1990). Relationship

between hopelessness and ultimate suicide: A replication with psychiatric outpatients.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 190-195.

Beck Institute (2004). Meta-Analysis Shows the Effectiveness of Cognitive Therapy. Retrieved

August 35, 2004 from http://www.beckinstitute.org/beck.html

Beck, J. (1996). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: Guilford.

128

Beck, A. T., Butler, A. C., Brown, G. K., Dahlsgaard, K. K., Newman C. F., Beck, J. S. (2001).

Dysfunctional beliefs discriminate personality disorders. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 39, 1213-1225.

Beck, A. T.,Emery, G. W. & Greenberg, R. I (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias. New

York: Basic Books.

Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G. & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring

clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 56, 893—897.

Beck, A. T., Freeman, A. and Associates (1990). Cognitive Therapy of Personality

Disorders. New York: Guilford Press.

Beck, A.T., Freeman, A., Davis, D. D., & Associates (2004). Cognitive Therapy of Personality

Disorders (2nd Ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F. & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of

depression. New York: Guilford Press.

Beck, A. T. & Steer, R. A. 1990. Beck Anxiety Inventory: Manual. The Psychological

Corporation. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc, San Antonio.

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory

for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.

Beck, A. T. & Weishaar, M. E. (1995). Cognitive therapy. In R. J. Corsini & D.

Weddings (Eds.), Current psychotherapies (pp. 229—261). Itasca, IL: F. E.

Peacock.

129

Beck, A. T., Wright, F. D., Newman, C. F. & Liesse, B. S. (1993). Cognitive therapy of

. New York: Guilford Press.

Beckham, E. E., Leber, W. R., Watkins, J. T., Boyer, J. L. & Cook, J. B. (1986).

Development of an instrument to measure Beck's cognitive triad: The Cognitive

Triad Inventory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 566-567.

Belar, C.D. & Perry, N.W. (1992). The national conference on scientist-

practitioner education and training for the professional practice of

psychology. American Psychologist, 47, 71-75.

Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1997). Use of personality assessment instruments in empirically

guided treatment planning. Psychological Assessment, 9, 361-367. Retrieved January

16, 2003, from PsychINFO database.

Bender, D., Farber, B.A. & Geller, J.D. (2001). Cluster B personality traits and attachment.

Journal of the American Academy of , 29(4), 551-563.

Birelson, P., Hudson, I., Buchanan, D.G., & Wolff, S. (1987). Clinical evaluation of self-rating

scale for depressive disorder in childhood. (Depression self-rating scale). Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 43-60.

Black, D. W., Bell, S., Hulbert, J. & Nasrallah, A. (1988). The importance of Axis II in

patients with major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 14, 115-122.

Blackburn, R. (1998). Relationship of personality disorders to observer ratings of interpersonal

style in forensic psychiatric patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12(1), 77-85.

Blackburn, I. M., Eunson, K. M. & Bishop, S. (1986). A two-year naturalistic follow-up

of depressed patients treated with cognitive therapy, pharmacotherapy, and a

combination of both. Journal of Affective Disorders, 10, 67-75. January

130

10, 2003, from PsychINFO database.

Boyle, G. & Loick, D. (2000). Discriminant validity of the illness behavior questionnaire

and Millon clinical multiaxial inventory-Ill in a heterogeneous sample of psychiatric

outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 79-791 Abstract retrieved December 6,

2002, from EBSCO database.

Brewin, C. R., (1996). Theoretical foundations of cognitive-behavior therapy for anxiety and

depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 33-57.

Briere, J. (2001).The Cognitive Distortion Scale. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment

Resources, Inc.

Briere, J., Runtz, M., Giancola, P., Mezzich, A., Clark, D., & Tarter, R. (1993). Cognitive

distortions, aggressive behavior, and drug use in adolescent boys with and without a

family history of a substance use disorder. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8(3), 312–

330.

Brook, J., Whiteman, M., Finch, S. & Cohen, P. (1996). Young adult drug use and

delinquency: Childhood antecedents and adolescent mediators. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1584-1592.

Brown, G. K., Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Grisham, J. R. (2000). Risk factors for suicide in

psychiatric outpatients: A 20-year prospective study. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology. 68, 371-377. Retrieved December 28, 2002, from PsychINFO

database.

Bruns, D. (1998, October). Psychologists as primary care providers: A paradigm shift.

APA [Electronic version]. Division Newsletter. Retrieved June 8,

2002, from http://www. healthpsych.com/primarycare1.html.

131

Budman, S., & Gurman, A. (1988). Theory and practice of brief therapy. New York: The

Guilford Press.

Burns, D. D. (1980). good: The new mood therapy. New York: Signet.

Burns, D. D. (1990). The feeling good handbook. New York: William Morrow.

Burns, D. D. (1999). Feeling good: The new mood therapy. Avon Books, New York.

Butcher, J.N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., Tellegen, A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989).

Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI-2). Manual for administration

and scoring. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Butler, A. C., & Beck, J. S. (2000). Cognitive therapy outcomes: A review of meta-

analyses. Journal of the Norwegian Psychological Association, 37, 1-9.

Butler, G., Fennell, M., Robson, P. & Gelder, M. (1991). Comparison of behavior therapy and

cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 167-175.

Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.), (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York:

Guilford Press.

Chambless, D., Caputo, G., Bright, P. & Gallagher, R. (1984). Assessment of fear in

agoraphobics: The Body Sensations Questionnaire and the Agoraphobia Cognitions

Questionnaire. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 1090-1097.

Chambless, D. & Gillis, M. (1993). Cognitive Therapy of Anxiety Disorders. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 248-260.

Chambless, D. L. & Olendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically supported psychological interventions:

Controversies and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 685-716.

132

Chick, D., Sheaffer, C., Goggin, W. & Sison, G. (1993). The relationship between MCMI

personality scales and clinician- generated DSM-III-R personality disorder diagnoses.

Journal of Personality Assessment, 61(2), 264-276.

Chiu, C. Y., Krauss, R. M. & Lau, I. Y. M. (2003). Some cognitive consequences of

communication." In S.R. Fussell & R.J. Kreuz (eds.), Social and Cognitive

Psychological Approaches to Interpersonal Communication. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum

Associates.

Choca, J. P., & VanDenburg, E. (1997). Interpretive guide to the Millon Clinical Multiaxial

Inventory (2nd ed.). Washingt on, DC: American Psychological Association.

Clark, D. M. (1991, September). Cognitive therapy for panic disorder. (Paper presented

at the National Institutes of Health Consensus Development Conference on the

Treatment of Panic Disorder). Bethesda, MD.

Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M. & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of

temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-990.

Clum, G. A., Boyles, S. E., Borden, J. W. & Watkins, P. L. (1990). Validity and

reliability of the Panic Attack Symptoms and Cognitions Questionnaires. Journal

of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 12, 233-245.

Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans

reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187-276.

Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice:

The NEO personality inventory. Psychological Assessments, 4, 20—22 5-13. Retrieved

January 16, 2003, from PsychINFO databases.

Craig, R. J. (Ed.). (1993). The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory: A clinical and

133

research information synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Craig, R. J. (1997). A selected review of the MCMI empirical literature. In T. Millon (Ed.), The

Millon inventories: Clinical and personality assessment. (pp. 303—326). New York:

Guilford.

Craig, R. J. (1997). Sensitivity of MCMI-III Scales T (drugs) and B (alcohol) in detecting

substance abuse. Substance Use and Misuse, 32(10), 1385-1393.

Craig, R.J. & Olson, R. (1998). Stability of the MCMI-III in a substance-abusing inpatient

sample. Psychological Reports, 83(3), 1273-1274.

Craig, R. J. & Weinberg, D. (1993). MCMI: Review of the literature. (In R. J. Craig

(Ed.), The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory: A clinical research information synthesis

(pp. 23—70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Craighead, W. E., Craighead, L. W., & Ilardi, S. S., (1998). Psychosocial treatments for major

depressive disorder. In P. E. Nathan & J. M. Gorman (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work

(pp. 226-239). New York: Oxford University Press.

Dagan, Y., Sela, H., Omer, H., Hallis, D., & Dar, R. (1996). High prevalence of personality

disorders among circadian rhythm sleep disorders (CRSD) patients. Journal of

Psychosomatic Research, 41(4), 357-363.

Davis, S.E. & Hays, L.W. (1997). An examination of the clinical validity of the MCMI-III

Depressive Personality scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53(1), 15-23.

Deblinger, S., Steer, R.A., Lippmann, J. Two-year follow-up study of cognitive behavioral

therapy for sexually abused children suffering post-traumatic stress symptoms. Child

Abuseand , 23, 1371-1378.

134 de Groot, M.H., Franken, I.H.A., van der Meer, C.W. & Hendriks, V.M. (2003). Stability and

change in dimensional ratings of personality disorders in drug abuse patients during

treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 24(2), 115-120.

Del Rosario, P.M., McCann, J.T. & Navarra, J.W. (1994). The MCMI-II diagnosis of

schizohrenia: operating characteristics and profile analysis. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 63(3), 438-452.

DeRubeis, R. J., Crits-Christoph P. (1998) Empirically supported individual and group

psychological treatments for adult mental disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 66:37-52.

DeRubeis, R. J., Evans, M. D., Hollon, S. D., Garvey, M. J., Grove, W. M., & Tuason, V. B.

(1990). How does cognitive therapy work? Cognitive change and symptom change in

cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 58, 862-869. Retrieved December 3, 2002 from PsycINFO

database.

Devanand, DP. (2002). Comorbid psychiatric disorders in late life depression. Biological

Psychiatry, 52(3), 236-42. Retrieved January 26, 2003, from Science Direct

database.

DiGiuseppe, R., Leaf, R., Exner, T. & Robin, M. W. (1988, September). The development

of a measure of irrational/rational thinking. (Paper presented at the World Congress

on Behavior Therapy, Edinburgh, Scotland).

Diguer, L., Barber, J. P. & Luborsky, L. (1993). Three concomitants: Personality disorders,

psychiatric severity and outcome of dynamic psychotherapy of major depression. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1246-1248. Retrieved December 23, 2002 from PsycINFO

135

database.

Dobson, K.S. (1989). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of cognitive therapy for depression.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 3, 414-419.

Dobson, K. S., & Khatri, N. (2000). Cognitive therapy: Looking backward, looking forward.

Journal of Clinical Psychology. Special Issue: Advances in clinical psychology, 56, 907-

923. Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior. Child

Development, 51, 162—170.

Dodge, K. & Newman, J. (1981). Biased decision-making processes in aggressive boys.

Journal of , 90, 375-379.

Dodge, K., Price, J., Bachorowski, J., & Newman, J. (1990). Hostile attributional biases

in severely aggressive adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 385-392.

Driscoll, R. (1988). Self-condemnation: A conceptual framework for assessment and treatment.

Psychotherapy, 26, 104-111.

Duthie, B. & Vincent, K. R. (1986). Diagnostic hit rates for high point codes for the

Dianostic Inventory of Personality and Symptoms using random assignment, base

rates, and probability scales. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 612-614.

Dyce, J.A., O'Connor, B.P., Parkins, S.Y. & Janzen, H.L. (1997). Correlational structure of the

MCMI-III personality disorder scales and comparisons with other data sets. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 69(3), 568-582.

D'Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1990). Development and preliminary evaluation of the Social

Problem-solving Inventory. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 2, 156-163.

136

Eckhardt, C. I., Barbour, K. A., Davison, G. C. (1998). Articulated thoughts of maritally violent

and nonviolent men during anger arousal. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,

66, 259-269.

Elkin, I., Shea, M. T., Watkins, J. T., Imber, S. D., Sotsky, S. M., Collins, J. F., eta al. (1989).

National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research

Program:

General effectiveness of treatments. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 971-982.

Ellis, A. (1958). Rational psychotherapy. Journal of General Psychology. 59, 35-49. Reprinted:

New York: Institute for Rational-Emotive Therapy.

Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emtion in psychotherapy. New York: Lyle Stuart.

Ellis, A. (1973). Humanistic psychotherapy: The rational-emotive approach. New York:

McGraw-Hill.

Ellis, A., & Grieger, R. (1977). Handbook of rational-emotive therapy. New York:

Springer Publishers.

Epstein, S., Lipson, A., Holstein, C. & Huh, E. (1992). Irrational reactions to negative outcomes:

Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62,

328-339.

Erikson, E.H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.), New York: Norton. Espelage, D.L., Mazzeo, S.E., Sherman, R. & Thompson, R. (2002). MCMI-II profiles of women with eating disorders: A cluster analytic investigation. Journal of Personality Disorders,

16(5), 453-463.

Evans, M. D., Hollon, S. D., DeRubeis, R. J., Piasecki, J. M., Grove, W. M., Garvey, M.

J. et al. (1992). Differential relapse following cognitive therapy and

pharmacotherapy for depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 802-808.

137

Retrieved December 23, 2002 from PsychINFO database.

Eysenck, H. J. & Eysenck, S. B. J. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality

Questionnaire. Hodder & Staughton: Sevenoaks, Kent.

Fairburn, C., Jones, R., Peveler, R., Carr, S., Solomon, R., O'Connor, M. et al. (1991). Three

psychological treatments for bulimia nervosa: A comparative

trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 463-469.

Faravelli C, Albanesi G. 1987. Agoraphobia with panic attacks: 1-year prospective follow-up.

Comprehensive Psychiatry 28, 481-487.

Farmer, R. & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1990). Personality disorders and depression.

Hypothetical relations, empirical findings, and methodological considerations.

Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 453-476.

Fava, G. A., Grandi, S., Zielezny, M., Rafanelli, C. & Canestrari, R. (1996). Four-year

outcome for cognitive behavioral treatment of residua l symptoms in major

depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 945-947. Retrieved December 23,

2002 from PsychINFO database.

Fava, G. A., Rafanelli, C., Grandi, S., Conti, S. & Belluardo, P. (1998). Prevention of

recurrent depression with cognitive-behavioral therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry,

55, 816-820.

Faravelli C, Albanesi G. (1987). Agoraphobia with panic attacks: 1-year prospective followup.

Comprehensive Psychiatry;28, 481-487.

Feske, U. and Chambless, D. L. (1995). Cognitive behavioral versus exposure only treatment for

social phobia: A meta-analysis. Behavior Therapy, 26, 695-720.

138

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human relations. 7,

114-140.

Frances, A. J. (1980). The DSM—III personality disorders section: A commentary.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1050-1054.

Freeman, A. & DeWolf, R. (1990). Woulda, coulda, shoulda. New York: William Morrow.

Freeman, A. & DeWolf, R. (1992). The 10 Dumbest mistakes smart people make and

how to avoid them. New York: HarperCollins.

Freeman A., & Fusco, G. M. (2003). Borderline Personality Disorder: A Therapist's Guide to

Taking Control. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co.

Freeman, A.M., III, Kablinger, A.S., Rolland, P.D. & Brannon, G.E. (1999). Millon multiaxial

personality patterns differentiate depressed and anxious outpatients. Depression and

Anxiety, 10(2), 73-76.

Freeman, A. & Oster, C. (1999). Cognitive behavior thereapy. In M. Herson & A. S.

Bellack (eds.). Handbook of interventions for adult disorders, 2nd edition, (pp. 108-

138). New York: Wiley and sons.

Freeman, A. & Rosenfield, B. (2002). Modifying therapeutic homework for patients with

personality disorders. In Session: Psychotherapy in Practice, 58, 513-524.

Freud, S. (1899, 1962). Screen memories. In J. Strachey (Ed.) The Standard Edition of the

Complete psychological works of , Vol. 3. London: The Hogarth Press,

1962.

Giancola, P., Mezzich, A., Clark, D. & Tarter, R. (1999). Cognitive distortions,

aggressive behavior, and drug use in adolescent boys with and without a family

history of a substance use disorder. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13, 22-32.

139

Retrieved January 6, 2003, from PsychINFO database.

Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1989). Effects of upward and downward social comparison on

mood states. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 14-31.

Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1991). Downward comparison and coping with threat. In J.

Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social Comparison Processes: Contemporary theory and

research. (pp. 317 - 345). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Gilbert, P. 1997 . T he evolution o f social attractiveness a nd its r ole in shame, hu miliation, gu ilt

and therapy. British Journal of 70, 113-147.

Gilbert, P . (1998). The e volved basis a nd adaptive functions for c ognitive d istortions. British

journal of Medical Psychology,12, 447-263.

Gilson, M., & Freeman, A. (1999). A cognitive therapy approach for taming the

depression beast: Client workbook. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

Gorenstein, E. E., & Newman, J. P. (1980). Disinhibitory psychopathology: A new perspective

and a model for research. Psychological Review, 87, 301-315.

Gould, R. A., Buckminster, S., Pollack, M. H., Otto, M.W., & Yap, L. (1997). Cognitive-

behavioral and pharmacological treatment for social phobia: A meta-analysis. Clinical

Psychology – Science & Practice, 4(4), 291-306.

Gould, R. A., Otto, M. W., & Pollack, M. H. (1995). A meta-analysis of treatment outcome for

panic disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 15(8), 819-844.

Gould, R. A., Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., & Yap, L. (1997). Cognitive behavioral and

pharmacological treatment of generalized anxiety disorder: A preliminary meta-analysis.

Behavior Therapy, 28, 285-305.

140

Grossman, L.S. & Craig, R.J. (1995). Comparison of MCMI-II and 16PF validity scales. Journal

of Personality Assessment, 64(2), 384-389.

Grove, W. M. & Meehl, P. E. (1996). Comparative efficiency of informal (subjective,

impressionistic) and formal (mechanical, algorithmic) prediction procedures: The

clinical—statistical controversy. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2, 293-323.

Groth-Marnat, G. (1996). Handbook of psychological assessment. New York: Wiley & Sons.

Groth-Marnat, G. & Edkins, G. (1996). Professional psychologists in general health care

settings a review of the financial efficacy of direct treatment interventions.

Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 27, 161-174. Retrieved June 6, 2002, from

PsycARTICLES database.

Haaga, D. A. F., Dyck, M. J. & Ernst, D. (1991). Empirical status of cognitive theory of

depression. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 215-236.

Hamburger, M. E., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Hogben, M. (1996). , gender, and gender

roles: Implications for antisocial and histrionic personality disorders. Journal of Personality

Disorders, 10, 41-55.

Hardy, G. E., Barkham, M., Shapiro, D. A., Stiles, W. B., Rees, A. & Reynolds, S.

(1995). Impact of Cluster C personality disorders on outcomes of contrasting brief

psychotherapies for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63,

997-1004. Retrieved December 22, 2002, from PsychINFO Database.

Hartman, L. M. (1984). Cognitive components of anxiety. Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 40, 137-139.

Hathaway, S. R. & McKinley, J. C. A. (1943). MMPI Manual. Psychological

Corporation: New York.

141

Hawley, K. M., Weisz, J. R., (2002). Increasing the relevance of evidence-based treatment:

Review to practitioners and consumers. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 9(2),

225-230. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from Proquest database.

Head, H., Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34,

102-254.

Heath, A. C., Cloninger, C. R. & Martin, N. G. (1994). Testing a model for the genetic structure

of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66, 762-775.

Higgins, S. T., Heil, S. H., & Lussier, J. P. (2004). Clinical implications of reinforcement as a

determinant of substance use disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 431-461.

Hirschfeld, R. M. A. & Shea, M. T. (1992). Personality. In E. S. Paykel (Ed.), Handbook

of affective disorders (2nd ed., pp. 185—194). New York: Guilford Press.

Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self-statements in depression: Development of

an automatic thoughts questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 4, 383-395.

Hollon, S. D., Shelton, R. C., & Davis, D. D. (1993). Cognitive therapy for depression:

Conceptual issues and clinical efficacy. Journal of Consulting and Clininical Psychology.

61, 270-75.

Holt, S.E., Meloy, J.R. & Strack, S. (1999). Sadism and psychopathy in violent and sexually

violent offenders. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 27(1),

23-32.

Hsu, L. M. (2002). Diagnostic validity statistics and the MCMI—III. Psychological Assessment,

14, 410-422.

Hyer, L., Braswell, L., Albrecht, B., Boyd, S., Boudewyns, P. & Talbert, S. (1994). Relationship

of NEO-PI to personality styles and severity of trauma in chronic PTSD victims. Journal

142

of Clinical Psychology, 50(5), 699-707.

Ilardi, S. S. & Craighead, W. E. (1999). The Relationship Between Personality Pathology and

Dysfunctional Cognitions in Previously Depressed Adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,

108, 51-57. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from Proquest database.

Ilardi, S. S., Craighead, W., Edward, & Evans, D. D. (1997). Modeling relapse in

unipolar depression: The effects of dysfunctional cognitions and personality

disorders. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 65(3) 381-391. Retrieved January

3, 2003, from Proquest database.

Ingram, R. E., & Kendall, P. C., (1986). Cognitive clinical psychology: Implications of an

information processing perspective. in R. Ingram (Ed.), Information Processing

Approaches to Clinical Psychology, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). to depression. New

York: Guilford Press.

Joffe, R. T. & Regan, J. J. (1988). Personality and depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research

22, 279-286.

Joiner, T. E. & Rudd, M. D. (2002). The incremental validity of passive-aggressive personality

symptoms rivals or exceeds that of other personality symptoms in suicidal outpatients.

Journal of Personality Assessment, 79(1), 161-170.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of

representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430-454.

Kaplan, R. M. (2000). Two pathways to prevention. American Psychologist, 55, 382-396.

Retrieved June 12, 2002, from PsycARTICLES database.

Kay, J. H., Altshuler, L. L.,Ventura, J., & Mintz J. (2002). Impact of axis II comorbidity on the

143

course of bipolar illness in men: a retrospective chart review. Bipolar Disorders. 4(4), 237-

242.

Kazdin, A. E. (1998). Research design in clinical psychology. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Keller, A. P., Lipkus, I, M., and Rimer, B. K. (2002). Mood, framing and health-related

. Paper presented at the Association for Consumer Research Conference,

Joan Meyers-Levy and Mary Gilly (Chairpersons).

Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.

Kendall, P. C. (1985). Toward a cognitive behavioral model of shild psychotherapyand a

critique related to interventions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 357-372.

Kendall, P. C. (1992). Healthy thinking. Behavior Therapy, 23, 1-11.

Kendall, P. C., Howard, B., & Hays, R. (1989). Self-referent speech and psychopathology: The

balance of positive and negative thinking. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13, 583-598.

Kendall, P. C., Kortlander, E., Chansky, T. E. & Brady, E. U. (1992). Comorbidity of

anxiety and depression in youth: Treatment implications. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 60, 869—880. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsychINFO

database.

Kenny, D. A., & Kashy, D. A. (1992). Analysis of the multitrait-multimethod matrix by

confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 165-172.

Kessler, R. C., McGonagle K. A., Zhoa, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., & Eshleman S et

al.(1994). Lifetime, 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United

States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8-19.

Kiesler, D. J. (1986). The 1982 interpersonal circle: An analysis of DSM-III personality

disorders. In T. Millon & G. L. Klerman (Eds.), Contemporary directions in

144

psychopathology: Toward the DSM-IV (pp. 571-597). New York: Guilford.

Klerman, G. L., Weissman, M. M., Rounsaville, B. J., & Chevron, E. S. (1984). Interpersonal

psychotherapy of depression. New York: Basic Books.

Kolko, D. J., Brent. D. A., Baugher, M., Bridge, J., & Brimaher, B. (2000). Cognitive and family

therapies for adolescent depression treatment specificity, mediation, and moderation.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 603-614. Retrieved November 20, 2002,

from PsychINFO database.

Krebs, D. L. & Denton, K. (1997). Social illusions and self-: The evolution of biases in

person perception. In J. A. Simpson & D. T. Kendrick (Eds), Evolutionary Social

Psychology, pp. 21-47. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kuyken, W., Kurzer, N., DeRubeis, R. J., Beck, A.T., Brown, G. K. (2001). Response to

Cognitive therapy in depression: The role of maladaptive beliefs and personality

disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 560-566.

Langlois, J. H., & Musselman, L. (1995). The myths and mysteries of beauty. In D. R. Calhoun

(Ed.), 1996 Yearbook of Science and the Future (pp. 40-61). Chicago: Encyclopaedia

Britannica, Inc.

Layden, M. A., Newman, C. F., Freeman, A., & Morse, S. B. (1993). Cognitive Therapy of

Borderline Personality Disorder. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Lefebvre, M. F. (1981). Cognitive distortion and cognitive errors in depressed

psychiatric and low back pain patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 49, 517-525.

Leichsenring, F., & Leibing, E. (2003). The effectiveness of psychodynamic therapy and

145

cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of personality disorders: A meta-analysis.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(7), 1223-1232.

Libb, J.W., Stankovic, S., Sokol, R., Freeman, A., Houck, C. & Switzer, P. (1990). Stability of

the MCMI among depressed psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment,

55(1-2), 209-218.

Likert, R. (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Archives of Psychology, 140, 1-

55.

Livesley, W. J. (ed.) (1995). The DSM-IV Personality Disorders. Guilford Press: New York.

Lochman, J. E. & Dodge, K. A. (1994). Social-cognitive processes of severely violent,

moderately aggressive, and nonaggressive boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 62, 366-374. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsycINFO database.

Lynskey, M. & Fergusson, D. (1995). Childhood conduct problems, attention deficit

behaviors, and adolescent alcohol, tobacco, and illicit drug use. Journal of Abnormal

Child Psychology, 23, 281-302. Retrieved February 14, 2003, from PsycINFO database.

Looper, K. J. & Kirmayer, L. J. (2002). Behavioral Medicine Approaches to

Somatoform Disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 70. 810-827.

Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsycINFO database.

MacDonald A. P. (1987). The Irrational Values Scale. In K. Corcoran & J. Fischer.

Measures for clinical practice: A sourcebook. (pp. 206-208). New York: Free press.

Malouff, J. M. & Schulte, N.S. (1986) In Sourcebook of adult assessment: Applied clinical

psychology. (pp. 432-435). New York: Plenum Press.

McCrae, R.R. (1991). The five-factor model and its assessment in clinical settings. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 57(3), 399-14.

146

McDaniel, S. (1995). Collaboration between psychologist and family physicians:

Implementing the biopsychosocial model. Professional Psychology: Research and

Practice, 26, 117-122. Retrieved June 6, 2002, from PsycARTICLES database.

MacDonald, A. P. (1972). In: K. Corcoran & J. Fischer (2000). Measures for clinical practice:

A sourcebook. 3rd Ed. New York: Free Press.

McGrath, E. P., Repettie, R. L., (2002). A longitudinal study of children's depressive symptoms,

self-perceptions, and cognitive distortions about the self. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology 111, 7-87. Retrieved July 10, 2004, from PsyhcARTICLES Database.

McKee, G.R. & Klohn, L.S. (1994). MCMI profiles of pretrial defendants. Psychological

Reports, 74(3), 77-87. Retrieved August 19, 2004, from PsyhcInfo Database.

McMullin, R.E. (2000). The new handbook of cognitive behavior therapy techniques. New

York: Norton.

Massion, A. O., Dyck, I. R.. Shea, M. T., Phillips, K. A., Warshaw, M. G., Keller, M. B. (2002).

Personality disorders and time to remission in generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia,

and panic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 59(5), 434-40.

Meichenbaum, D. H. (1975). A self-instructional approach to stress management: A proposal for

stress inoculation training. In C. D. Spielberger & I. Sarason (Eds.), Stress and anxiety

(Vol. 2, pp. 237-264). New York: Wiley.

Meichenbaum, D. & Turk, D. C. (1987). Facilitating treatment adherence: A practitioner's

guidebook. New York: Plenum.

Meichenbaum, D.H. (1993). Stress inoculation training: A twenty year update. In R. L.

Woolfolk, & P. M. Lehrer (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed.)

(pp.373- 406. New York: Guilford.

147

Melartin T. K., Rytsala, H. J., Leskela, U. S., Lestela-Mielonen, P. S., Sokero, T. P., Isometsa, E.

T. (2002). Current Comorbidity of Psychiatric Disorders Among DSM-IV Major Depressive

Disorder Patients in Psychiatric Care in the Vantaa Depression Study. Journal of Clinical

Psychiatry, 63.126-134.

Meyer, J. H., Kennedy, S. H., Korman, L., Brown, G. M., DaSilva, J. N., et al. (2003).

Dysfunctional attitudes and 5-HT (2) receptors during depression and self-harm. The

American Journal of Psychiatry. 160(1), 90-100.

Millon, T. (1987). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-II: Manual for the MCMI-II.

Minneapolis, MN, National Computer Systems,Inc.

Millon, T, (1990). Current comorbidity of psychiatric disorders Among DSM-IV Major

depressive disorder patients in psychiatric care in the vantaa depression study. Toward a

new personology. New York: Wiley.

Millon, T. (1994). Manual for the MCMI-III (2nd ed.) .Minneapolis, MN: National

Comput er Systems.

Millon, T. (1999). Clinical Syndromes and Personality Disorders. Harvard Mental HealthLetter,

5, 4-7.

Millon, T. & Davis, R. D. (1996). Disorders of personality.DSM—IV and beyond (2nd

ed.). New York: Wiley.

Millon, T., Davis, R. & Millon, C. (1997). MCMI-III manual (2nd ed.). Minneapolis,

MN: National Computer Systems.

Millon, T., Davis, R. D., Millon, C. M., Wenger, A., Van Zuilen, M. H., Fuchs, M., Mulder, R.

T., & Joyce, P. R. (1994 ). The relationships of the Tridimensional Personality

148

Questionnaire to mood and personality measures in depressed patients. Psychological

Reports 75, 1315-1325.

Morgan, C., Shoenberg, M.R., Dorr, D. & Burke, M.J. (2002). Overreport on the MCMI-III:

Concurrent validation with the MMPI-2 using a psychiatric inpatient sample. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 78(2), 288-300.

Mulder, R. T.; Joyce, P. R.; Sullivan, P. F. (1999). The Relationship Among Three

Models of Personality Psychopathology: DSM-III-R Personality Disorder, TCI

Scores and DSQ Defenses. Psychological Medicine. 29, 943-951. Retrieved

December 3, 2002, from OVID Database.

Najavits, L. M. (1993). Cognitive Distortions Scale. Unpublished measure. Harvard Medical

School/McLean Hospital, Boston, MA

Najavits, L. M., Gotthardt, S., Weiss, R. D., Epstein, M. (2004). Cognitive distortions in the

dal dagnosis of PTSD and substance use disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research,

28(2), 159–172.

Narrow W. E. (1998). One-year prevalence of depressive disorders among adults 18 and over in

the U.S.: NIMH ECA prospective data. Population estimates based on U.S. Census

estimated residential population age 18 and over on July 1, 1998. Unpublished table.

Retrieved August 9, 2004, from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/publicat/numbers.cfm#5.

Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature, 1, 261-289.

Netmeyer, R. G., Williamson, D. A., Burton, S., Biswas, D. (2002). Psychometric properties of

shortened versions of the automatic thoughts questionnaire Educational and

Psychological Measuremen. 62, 111-130.

149

Newman, C. F., Leahy, R., Beck, A. T., Reilly-Harrington, N., & Gyulai, L. (2004). Bipolar

Disorder: A Cognitive Therapy Approach. Washington, D.C., American Psychiatric

Press.

Nigg, J. T. & Goldsmith, H. H. (1994). Genetics of Personality Disorders Perspectives From

Personality and Psychopathology Research. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 346-380.

Noyes, R., Reich, J., Christiansen, J., Suelzer, M., Pfohl, B., & Coryell, W.A. (1990). Outcome

of panic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 809-818.

O'Connor, B. P., & Dyce, J. A. (2001). Rigid and extreme: A geometric representation of

personality disorders in five-factor model space. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 81, 1119-1130.

Oei, T. & Sullivan, L. (1999). Cognitive changes following recovery from depression in a group

cognitive-behaviour therapy program. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,

33, 407-415.

Overholser, J. C. (1999, August). Temporal stability of the MCMI personality disorder scales.

Paper presented at the 97th annual convention of the American Psychological

Association. New Orleans, LA.

Pacini, R., Muir, F., & Epstein, S. (1998). Depressive realism from the perspective of

cognitive-experiential self-theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74( 4),

1056-1068.

Paykel, E. S., Scott, J., Teasdale, J. D., Johnson, A. L., Garland, A., Moore, R. et al. (1999).

Prevention of relapse in residual depression by cognitive therapy: A controlled trial.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 829-835. Retrieved December 23, 2002, from

PsychINFO Database.

150

Persons, J. B., Davidson, J., & Thompkins, M. A. (2001). Essential components of cognitive

behavior therapy for depression. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Peterson, C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I. & Seligman, M. E.

(1982). The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 6, 287-

300. Retrieved December 23, 2002, from PsychINFO Database.

Petrocelli, J., Glaser, B., Calhoun, G. & Campbell, L. (2001). Early maladaptive schemas of

personality disorder subtypes. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15(6), 546-559.

Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity

and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of personality and social

psychology, 46, 69-81.

Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1986). Communication and persuasion: The central and peripheral

routes to attitude change. Spring-Verlag: New York.

Phillips, K. A., Shea, T. Warshar, M., Dyck, I., (2001). The relationship between comorbid

personality disorders and treatment received in patients with anxiety disorders. Journal of

Personality Disorders, 15, 157-168.

Piaget, J. P. (1947/1950). The psychology of intelligence. New York: Hourcourt Brace.

Piaget, J. P. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. International Universities Press, New

York.

Pickstone, J. V. (2001). Ways of Knowing: A New History of Science, Technology and

Medicine. Chicago, Ill, University of Chicago Press.

Piersma, H.L. & Boes, J.L. (1997). MCMI-III as a treatment outcome measure for psychiatric

inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53(8), 825-831.

Pollack M. H., Otto M.W., Rosenbaum, J.F., Sachs G.S. (1992). Personality disorders in patients

151

with panic disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 33. 78-83. Retrieved December 4, 2002,

from OVID Database.

Pollack, M. H., Otto, M. W., Rosenbaum, J. F., Sachs, G. S., O'Neil, C., Asher, R. et al.

(1990).Longitudinal course of panic disorder: Findings from the Massachusetts

General Hospital naturalistic study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1990, 12-16.

Retrieved December 20, 2002, from OVID Database.

Pollack, M. H., Otto, M. W., Rosenbaum, J. F., & Sachs, G. S. (1992). Personality disorders in

patients with panic disorder: Association with childhood anxiety disorders, early trauma,

comorbidity and chronicity. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 33, 78-83.

Power, M. & Brewin, C. R. (1991). From Freud to cognitive science: A contemporary account

of the unconscious. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 289-310.

Rachman, S. (1999). Commentary. Rapid and not-so-rapid responses to cognitive behavioral

therapy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 293-301.

Raimy, V. C. (1950). Training in clinical psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall.

Ratneswar, S. & Chaiken, S. (1991). Comprehension’s role in persuasion: The case of its

moderating effects on source cues. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18, 52-62.

Reich, J. (2003). The effect of Axis II disorders on the outcome of treatment of anxiety and

unipolar depressive disorders: A review. Journal of Personality Disorders17, 387. Retrieved

August 26, 2004 from PschInfo Database.

Reiger, D. A., Narrow, W. E., Pae, D. S., Manderscherd, R. W., Locke, B. Z., & Goodwin, F.

K. (1993). The de facto U.S. mental and addictive service system. Epidemiologic catchment

area prospective 1–year prevalence rates of disorders and services. Archive of General

Psychiatry, 50, 84-95.

152

Retzlaff, P. D. (1995). Tactical psychotherapy of the personality disorders: An MCMI-III-based

approach. (Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Retzlaff, P.D., Ofman, P., Hyer, L. & Matheson, S. (1994). MCMI-II high-point codes: severe

personality disorder and clinical syndrome extensions. Journal of Clinical Psychology,

50(2), 228-234.

Retzlaff, P, Stoner, J, Kleinsasser, D. (2002). The use of the MCMI-III in the screening and

triage of offenders. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative

Criminology. Special Issue: Psychological Testing in Forensic Settings, 46(3), 319-332).

Ruegg, R. & Allen, F. (1995). New Research in Personality Disorders. Journal of Personality

Disorders, 9(1), 1-48.

Riggs, D . S ., & F oa, E . B . ( 1993). O bsessive compulsive d isorder. I n D . H . B arlow ( ed.).

Clinical handbook of psychological disorders (2nd ed.) (pps. 189-239). New York: The

Guilford Press.

Robins, R. W., Gosling, S. D., & Craik, K. H. (1999). An empirical analysis of trends in

psychology. American Psychologist, 54, 117-128.

Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C., (2003). Addiction. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 25-53.

Rogers, C. R. (1951). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Rogers, R. (2003). Forensic use and abuse of psychological tests: Multiscale inventories.

Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 9(4), 316-320.

Rogers, R., Salekin, R.T., & Sewell, K.W. (1999). Validation of the Million Clinical Multiaxial

Inventory for axis II disorders: Does it meet the Daubert standard? Law and Human

Behavior, 23(4), 425-443.

153

Rosen, S., Cochran, W., & Musser, L. M. (1990). Reactions to a match versus mismatch between

an applicant's self-presentational style and work reputation. Basic and Apllied Social

Psychology, 11, 117-129.

Rosenberger, P. H. & Miller, G. A. (1989). Comparing borderline definitions. DSM-III

borderline and schizotypal personality disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 161-

169.

Ross, S. M., Gottfredson, D. K., Christensen, P. & Weaver, R. (1986). Cognitive

self-statements in depression: Findings across clinical populations. Cognitive

Therapy and Research, 10, 159-166.

Rossi, G., Hauben, C., Van den Brande, I. & Sloore, H. (2003). Empirical evaluation of the

MCMI-III personality disorder scales. Psychological Reports, 92(2), 627-642.

Rossi, G., Van den Brande, I., Tobac, A., Sloore, H. & Hauben, C. (2003). Convergent validity

of the MCMI-III personality disorder scales and the MMPI-2 scales. Journal of

Personality Disorders, 17(4), 330-340.

Samuels, J. F., Nestadt, G., Romanoski, A. J., Folstein, M. F. & McHugh, P. R. (1994). DSM-III

Personality Disorders in the Community. American Journal of Psychiatry 151, 1055-1062.

Scott, J. (2001). Cognitive Therapy as an adjunct to medication for bipolar disorder. British

Journal of Psychiatry, 41, 164-168.

Shea, M. T., Elkin, I., Imber, S. D., Sotsky, F. M., Watkins, J. T., Collins, J. F.et al. (1992).

Course of depressive symptoms over follow-up: Findings from the NIMH treatment of

depression collaborative research program. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 782-787.

Retrieved December 23, 2002, from PsychINFO Database.

Shea, M. T., Glass, D. R., Pilkonis, P. A., Watkins, J. & Docherty, J. P. (1987). Frequency and

154

implications of personality disorders in a sample of depressed outpatients. Journal of

Personality Disorders, 1, 27-42.

Schoenberg, M.R., Dorr, D. & Morgan, C.D. (2003). The ability of the Millon Clinical

Multiaxial Inventory, Third Edition to detect malingering. Psychological Assessment,

15(2), 198-204.

Shorkey, C. T. & Whiteman, V. C. (1987). The Rational Behavior Inventory. In K. Corcoran &

J. Fischer (Eds.). Measures for clinical practice: A sourcebook.(pp. 270-273). New York: Free

Press.

Simons, A. D., Murphy, G. E., Levine, J. L. & Wetzel, R. D. (1986). Cognitive therapy and

pharmacotherapy for depression: Sustained improvement over one year. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 43, 43-50. Retrieved December 4, 2002, from PsychINFO Database.

Skinner, B. F. (1958). Teaching machines. Science, 128: 969-977.

Skodol, A., Gunderson, J. G., McGlashan, T. H., Dyck, I. (2002) Functional impairment in

patients with schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, or obsessive-compulsive personality

disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 2, 276-283. Retrieved January 2, 2002, from

ProQuest databases.

Sokol, L., Beck, A. T., Greenberg, R. L., Wright, F. D. & Berchick, R. J. (1989). Cognitive

therapy of panic disorder: A nonpharmacological alternative. Journal of Nervous and

Mental Disease, 177, 711-716.

Soyguet, G., Nelson, L. & Safran, J. (2001). The relationship between interpersonal schemas and

personality characteristics. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 15(2), 99-108.

Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form V). Palo Alto,

155

CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W. & Skodol, A. E. (1980). DSM—III: The major achievements

and an overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 151-164.

Steenbergh, T. A., Meyers, A.W., May, R.K., & Whelan, J.P. (2002). Development and

validation of the Gamblers' Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16,

143-149.

Sulloway, F.J. (1979). Freud: Biologist of the mind. Beyond the psychoanalytic legend. New

York: Basic Books.

Svrakic, D. M., Whitehead, C., Przybeck, T. R. & Cloninger, C. R. (1993). Differential diagnosis

of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character. Archives

of General Psychiatry 50, 991-999.

Tarafodi, R. W., Milne, A. B., Smith, A. J. (1999). The confidence of choice: Evidence for an

augmentation effect on self-perceived performance. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 25, 1405-1417.

Taylor, S. (2000). Understanding and treating panic disorder: Cognitive-behavioural

approaches. New York: Wiley.

Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., Segal, Z. V. (2002).

Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression empirical evidence.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 275-287.

Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., Ridgeway, V., Soulsby, J., & Lau, M. (2000).

Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive

therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615-623.

Toneatto, T. (1999). Cognitive psychopathology of problem gambling. Substance use & misuse,

156

34(11), 1593-1604.

Torgersen, S. & Alnaes, R. (1990). The relationship between the MCMI personality scales and

DSM-III, axis II. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(3-4), 698-707.

Tsai, S. M., Chen, C., Kuo, C., Lee, J., Lee, H., Strakowski, S. M. (2001). 15-year outcome of

treated bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 63, 215-220

Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., Borden, J. W., Stanley, M. A., & Jacob, R. G. (1991). Social

phobia: Axis I and II correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 102-106.

Turley, B., Bates, G.W., Edwards, J. & Jackson, H.J. (1992). MCMI-II personality disorders in

recent-onset bipolar disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48(3), 320-329.

Tyrer, P. (1995). Are personality disorders well classified in DSM-IV? In The DSM-IV

Personality Disorders (ed. W. J. Livesley), pp. 29-45. New York: Guilford Press.

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1999). Mental Helath: A Report of the

Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

Van Balkom, A. J. L. M., van Oppen, P., Vermeulen, A. W. A., van Dyck, R., Nauta, M. C. E.,

& Vorst, H. C. M. (1994). A meta-analysis on the treatment of obsessive-compulsive

disorder: A comparison of antidepressants, behavior, and cognitive therapy. Clinical

Psychology Review, 14, 359-381.

Vazquez, C. (1987). Judgment of contingency: Cognitive biases in depysphoric and

nondepressed students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 419-431.

Versiani, M., Amrein, R., Montgomery, R. A. (1997). Social phobia: long-term treatment

outcome and prediction of response: A moclobemide study. International Clinical

Psychopharmacology,12:239-254.

157

Versiani, M., Nardi, A. E., Mindim, F. D., Pinto, S., Saboya, E., Kovacs, R. (1996). The long-

term treatment of social phobia with moclobemide. International Clinical

Psychopharmacology, 11 83-88.

Watson, D. & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 447-448.

Wells, A. (1997). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: A practice manual and conceptual

guide. New York: Wiley

Weissman, A. N. (1979). The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale validation study. Dissertation

Abstracts (3-b), 1389-1390.

Weissman, A. N. & Beck, A. T. (1978, November). The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. (Paper

presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior

Therapy, Chicago).

Weissman, M. M., Bland, R. C., Canino, G. J., Faravelli, C., Greenwald, S., Hwu, H. G., et al.

(1997). The cross-national epidemiology of panic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry,

54:305–309.

Wetzel, R. D., Knesevich, M. A., Brown, S. L., Wolff, H. A., Horn, C. J. & Cloninger, C. R.

(1992). Correlates of Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire scales with selected

Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory scales. Psychological Reports 71, 1027-1038.

Wells, A. (1997). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders. Chichester: Wiley.

Weissman, A. (1979). The dysfunctional attitudes scale. Philadelphia: Center for Cognitive

Therapy.

Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Development and validation of the

Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the

158

Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Chicago, IL.

Weissman, A. N., Bland, R. C., Canino, G. J., Faravelli, C., Greenwald, S. Husa, H. G. et al.,

(1997). The Craoss-rational epidemiology of panic disorder, Archives of General Psychiatry,

54, 305-309.

White, M & Epston, D, 1989 Literate Means to Therapeutic Ends. DCP: Adelaide.

Widiger, T. A. (2000). Personality disorders in the 21st century. Journal of Personality

Disorders, 14, 3-16.

Widiger, T. A. & Sanderson, C. J. (1995). Assessing personality disorders.(In J. N. Butcher

(Ed.), Clinical personality assessment (pp. 380—394). New York: Oxford University Press.

Widiger, T. A. & Shea, T. (1991). Differentiation of Axis I and Axis II disorders. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 100, 399-406.

Widiger, T. A., Williams, J. B. W., Spitzer, R. L. & Frances, A. (1985). The MCMI as a measure

of DSM—III. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 366-378.

Widiger, T. A., Williams, J. B. W., Spitzer, R. L. & Frances, A. (1986). The MCMI and DSM—

III: A brief rejoinder to Millon (1985). Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 198-204.

Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and

psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24.

Wilson, G. T., Fairburn, C. G. & Agras, W. S. (1997). Cognitive—behavioral therapy for

bulimia nervosa.(In D. M. Garner & P. Garfinkel (Eds.), Handbook of treatment for eating

disorders (pp. 67—93). New York: Guilford Press.

Wise, E. (2001). The comparative validity of MCM-II and MMPI-2 personality disorder scales

with forensic examinees. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15(3), 275-279.

Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

159

Wood, J. V., Taylor, S.E., & Lichtman, R. R. (1985) Social comparison in adjustment to breast

cancer. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49(5), 1169-1183.

Woody, E. E., McClellan, A. T., Luborsky, L., & O’Brien, C. P. (1990). Psychotherapy and

counseling for methadone-maintained opiate addicts: Results of research studies. U.S.:

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. .

Wosinska, W., Dabul, A. J., Whetstone-Dion, R., & Cialdini, R. B. (1996). Self-presentational

responses to success in the organization: The costs and benefits of modesty. Basic and

Applied Social Psychology, 18(2), 229–242

Yonkers, K., Warshaw, M., Massion, A., Keller, M. (1996). Phenomenology and course of

generalised anxiety disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168(3): 308-313.

Young, J.E. (1990/1999). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: A schema-focused

approach (third edition). Professional Resource Press, Sarasota, Florida.

Young, J. E. & Brown, G. (1990). Young Schema Questionnaire-Long Form, 2nd ed. New York:

Cogntive Therapy Center of New York.

Yurica, C. & DiTomasso, R. (2002). Inventory of Cogntive Distortions. In C. Yurica, C. (2002).

Inventory of Cogntive Distortions: Validation of a Psychomatric Test for the

Masurement of Cognitive Distortions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,

Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine.

Yurica, C. (2002). Inventory of Cogntive Distortions: Validation of a Psychomatric Test for the

Masurement of Cognitive Distortions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,

Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine.

Zimmerman, M., Pfohl, B., Stangl, D., & Coryell. W. (1985), The validity of DSM-III Axis IV.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 1437-1441.