<<

Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons

LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School

June 2019 The Application of Nonverbal in Scratches, An Original Composition For Trumpet and Chamber Ensemble Shane Courville Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_dissertations Part of the Music Performance Commons

Recommended Citation Courville, Shane, "The Application of Nonverbal Prosody in Scratches, An Original Composition For Trumpet and Chamber Ensemble" (2019). LSU Doctoral Dissertations. 4977. https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_dissertations/4977

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized graduate school editor of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please [email protected]. THE APPLICATION OF NONVERBAL PROSODY IN SCRATCHES, AN ORIGINALY COMPOSITION FOR TRUMPET AND CHAMBER ENSEMBLE

A Dissertation

Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Musical Arts

in

The School of Music

by Shane Courville B.Mus., Loyola University New Orleans, 2001 M.A. Philosophy and Theology, Loyola University Chicago 2005 M.Mus., University of Louisiana Lafayette, 2014 August 2019

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to express so much gratitude for all the people in my life who have supported me through this long and crazy journey. From my first teachers, Ms. Robin Daigle and Dr. Delon Lyren who saw music come alive in me and fostered that, through my high school teachers who pushed me become a great musician, to all my professors in college who put up with my disorganization and cloud-headedness in order to help me mature into a professional musician and teacher, I’m forever grateful.

Dr. Brian Shaw, thank you for your trust and guidance through my time at LSU. I don’t think I could have done this with anyone else. You understand me as a musician and person and pushed me in ways that worked. Not only have you helped me become a better trumpet player

(at 40!), but you have been a great example of how to use your personal strengths and weaknesses to teach as a human being rather than just as an academic.

I would also like to thank Dr. Mara Gibson for taking me on as a new composition student. For so long I was afraid to let people hear what I composed. You have given me so much knowledge and confidence and continue to be a great inspiration to me.

Dr. Brett Dietz, thank you so much for your time and work on my committee. Your work as an artist and teacher has shown me how to be true to my own work.

Finally, I’m forever grateful to my partner Layne and my family. My mom and dad have spent their entire life together supporting me. Their sacrifices paved the way for me to become the artist and teacher I am today. No matter what crazy project or direction in life I decided to take, they have always been there to support me. I’m indebted for life. Thank you.

ii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Acknowledgements …………………………………………………………………………..ii

Abstract………………………………………………………….………...... iv

Introduction…………………………………………………………………………………...1

Part I: Prosody…………………………………………………………...... 5

Chapter 1. Understanding Prosody……………………………………………………………6

Chapter 2. Studies in Prosody …………………………………………………………….....10

Chapter 3. Compositional Process and Prosody……………………………………………..15

Part II. Scratches……………………………………………………………………..19

Chapter 4. Composer’s Statement…………………………………………………………...20

Scratches, an Original Composition for Trumpet and Chamber Ensemble by Shane Courville…………………………………………………………………………………...... 27

Bibliography…………………………………………………………………………………75

Vita…………………………………………………………………………………………..79

iii

ABSTRACT

This project uses research in prosody (the patterns of stress and intonation in language) to inform the compositional process of an original piece of music for trumpet, piano, string quartet, and sign language entitled Scratches. In this this paper, I propose that both verbal and nonverbal prosody affects music across all cultures at both the subtle and overt levels consciousness. It shows up in chanting and rhythmic structures of folk music as well as composers who use the influence of language explicitly. In conjunction with this document, I will discuss and perform the piece I compose through the inspiration of my research in a lecture recital format. The culmination of this research is especially relevant and personal to me in that it combines both my major in trumpet performance and my minor in composition. The end result for this project is especially relevant to me in that it combines both my major in trumpet performance and my minor in composition.

iv

INTRODUCTION

The first chapter of this dissertation defines prosody by going through each of its four types: syllabic, accentual, accentual-syllabic, and quantitative. I then provide a brief account of the history of prosodic research with examples found in ’s , e.e.cumming’s what if a much of a which of a wind, and Dylan Thomas’s In my Craft or Sullen Art. Chapter 1 concludes by showing that contemporary study (such as the evolution of prosodic study and prosody without words) has a particular influence on my own composition.

Chapter 2 concerns various linguistic connections to music expressed through culture, including nonverbal language. Some musicologists and linguists suggest that the prosody of a given culture’s speech patterns can affect the entire structure of that particular region’s music and, as a result, have developed a way to measure this. One study, conducted by Aniruddh Patel, finds that rhythmic patterns in English and French have directly affected the musical themes in each of those regions and so there now is an empirical basis in which to make these claims.1

There is also a study authored by Lauren Applebaum, Marie Coppola, and Susan Goldin-

Meadow where prosody in a communication system without a spoken language model exists

(such as sign language) which suggests that rhythm and intonation go much deeper than spoken language and actually appear in body movement.2

Chapter 3 will explore how selected composers throughout history have used prosody in their compositional processes. First, Gregorian chant and folk music have been influenced

1 Patel, Aniruddh D. “An empircal comparison of rhythm in language and music”. Cognition. Vol. 87, Issue 1. Feb. 2003, pp. B35-B45. 2 Applebaum Lauren, Marie Coppola, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. “Prosody in a Communication System Developed without a Language Model.” Sign language and linguistics 17.2 (2014): pp. 181–212.

1

directly by language, albeit unconsciously.3 Second, there are some composers who use prosody very specifically: for example, Karlheinz Stockhausen in Michaels

Reise. Other composers acknowledge the influence of prosody but use it less explicitly, such as

Gjorgy Ligeti in his Glissandi and Steve Reich in The Cave.

Chapter 4 explains my own compositional process for Scratches. The above research guided the creation of Scratches and will also use speech and body movement prosody I have encountered from three distinct times in my life. The first movement is based on my parent’s

Cajun speech patterns, the second movement is derived from my time learning Spanish on the streets of Tijuana, and the third movement is inspired by the time I was in the crowd for Barack

Obama’s 2008 presidential victory speech in Chicago. Each of these three experiences has greatly influenced my music and my use of the trumpet in my composing and playing in particular.

Accompanying the trumpet in the first movement will be a string quartet – two violins, one viola, and one cello. These instruments provide the chordal and timbral accompaniment for the trumpet solo. They also form the “landscape” for the trumpet and have short conversations with the trumpet by responding to and repeating what the trumpet plays. The second movement is accompanied by piano only, which supports the trumpet’s Spanish mimicry. I listened to

Coldplay during my time in Tijuana and have based the piano parts on Coldplay’s style. The instrumentation for the third movement consists of piccolo trumpet, string quartet, piano, and a person signing in American Sign Language (ASL). The piccolo trumpet expresses those languages that tend to be higher pitched with rapid prosodic structure. The two violins and viola play a direct rhythmic transcription of Obama’s 2008 speech mentioned above while the cello

3 Mocquerau, Dom Andre. The Art of Gregorian Music. Musica Sacra: Church Music Association of America. Catholic Education Press: 1896.

2

expresses the prosodic structure of languages with lower pitched and slower prosody. The ASL signer will be signing the parts of Obama’s speech that I use in the strings, which shows how prosody also manifests itself in body movement.

When I sit down to compose, the first sounds to spring up in my mind are the vocal sounds I encounter day to day. Particular words do not surface, but rather their general sound and rhythm. I often try to imitate these sounds when improvising on the trumpet – so my compositional process stems from this very basic mimicry. I grew up with a Cajun accent which has a very distinct tone and rhythm, examples of which I will show in this document. In the first movement of Scratches, I applied this rhythm to melodic passages passed around the string quartet and in the solo trumpet.

In 2005 I was sent to Tijuana by the Jesuits to live on the streets for six months without any knowledge of Spanish. In order to learn the language, I listened to the rhythm and tonal inflections used by the locals and would try to mimic them until I started to understand particular words and phrases. The second movement of Scratches is a guided improvisation imitating this process. The trumpet tells a musical story using tonal inflection, timbral change, and rhythm particularly through employing half-valve technique which is a method of playing which requires partially pressing the valves of the trumpet down. This technique can create a very human- sounding timbre and is effective in expressing this process.

I was in Chicago in November 2008 when Barack Obama won the U.S. presidential election. While standing in the Grant Park crowd for his victory speech, I heard many different languages being spoken simultaneously. I have a distinct memory of hearing the rhythmic and tonal play of the combined languages very distinctly. In the final movement of Scratches, the soloist and each instrument of the group (including someone using ASL) take on a particular

3

language through timbre and rhythm and combine to form something akin to what I experienced in that crowd on that historic night.

4

PART I. PROSODY

5

CHAPTER 1. UNDERSTANDING PROSODY

Contemporary prosody is the study of pitch and rhythm of speech and how these elements contribute to the meaning of language. It is typically applied to those aspects of speech that apply to a level above that of the individual phoneme (any distinct unit of sound) and often to sequences of words. Those who study prosody call the sequences of words above the level of a phoneme suprasegmentals, thus prosody can be said to be concerned with the phonetic study of the suprasegmental aspects of speech. At the phonetic level, prosody is concerned with vocal pitch, loudness, and rhythm.4 Most scholars studying these elements concentrate on measuring these characteristics.

Prosody has been studied from very different perspectives across the spectrum of linguistic science and because of that there has been a diversity in approaches. Distinctions occur from looking at prosody through the perspective of discourse, grammar, pragmatics, and of phonetics. Through the lens of discourse, prosody is seen as solely as communicative – giving meaning through conversation. Grammatically, prosody adds another dimension to language by giving it grammatical rules that preserve a language. Pragmatics examines the distinction between the literal meaning of a sentence and the meaning intended by the speaker, such as attitude (sarcasm, irony, humor, etc).5 And finally, phonetically speaking, it concerns itself with the basic fundamentals of the structure itself.

Prosody tends to overlap with emotion in speech. The same acoustic features that are used to express prosody (intensity, pitch, rhythm, etc.) are also affected by emotion in the voice.

Speech has different levels of information. Its linguistic aspect is the direct expression of

4 Palmer, Caroline and Michael H. Kelly. “Linguistic Prosody and Musical Meter in Song.” Journal of Memory and Language, New York Vol. 31, Iss. 4, August 1, 1992: 525-542. 5 5Gross, Harvey. “The Aesthetic Function of Prosody.” The Centennial Review of Arts and Sciences, vol. 7, no. 2, 1963, pp. 204–218.

6

meaning. Its paralinguistic aspect indicates attitude or membership of a speech community. Its nonlinguistic aspect indicates something about the speaker’s vocal physiology or emotional state.6

The paralinguistic aspect, interestingly, features an indication of a speaker’s membership within a particular speech community. Effectively they are markers of socioeconomic and sociolinguistic status. For instance, Cajun French speech patterns are very distinguishable from

Parisian French patterns. It goes even deeper within these communities as well. A farmer in the country may have a very different pronunciation and pattern for a given phrase than, for example, a business owner in the city.

Gender is another differentiator, which has both paralinguistic and non-linguistic aspects.

Some features may be regarded as more masculine or feminine by a speech community (degree of the use of nasal sounds for instance like in the Arabic community),7 but, feaures that are purely a consequence of physiological differences are non-linguistic aspects of speech. A speaker’s emotional state is often evident in the speaker’s voice. These features are linguistic to the extent that they are relevant to the immediate meaning of something. Even the state of health of someone can be evident in speech.8

For this paper, I focus on linguistic and para-linguistic aspects of prosody using four distinct types of prosody are used. First, syllabic prosody counts a fixed number of in each line, while accent, tone, and quantity play a secondary role. Second, accentual prosody measures only the accents or stresses in a line of verse, while the overall number of syllables

6 Gross 7 Pynte, Joel. “The Role of Prosody in Semantic Interpretation.” Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, vol. 16, no. 1, 1998, pp. 79–97. 8 Mannell, Rober, Felicity Cox and Jonathan Harrington. An introduction to phonetics and phonology. Department of Linguistics, Macaquarie University, 2000.

7

may vary in a line. It is very common in Germanic, old English, and modern English verses.

Third, accentual-syllabic prosody counts both the number of syllables and accents in each line.

We commonly find it in English poetry. And finally, quantitative prosody does not measure the number of syllables, but depends on the duration of syllables. This can be determined by the amount of time used on pronunciation, such as a free-verse poem that consists of unmeasured lines. We find this type of prosody in Roman and classical Greek poetry.

Syllabic prosody is the most straightforward. Each line consists of a particular number of syllables. Some poems will keep the exact number the same and others will alternate to create a pattern. A good example of the former is Dylan Thomas’s “In My Craft or Sullen Art.”

In my craft or sullen art Exercised in the still night When only the moon rages And the lovers lie abed With all their griefs in their arms, I labour by singing light … But for the common wages of their most secret heart.

Each line consists of seven syllables, except the last line. The natural accents of English are not followed here, just the number of syllables.

A good example of accentual prosody can be found in e.e. cummings’s “what if a much of a which of a wind.”

what if a much of a which of a wind gives the truth to summer’s lie; bloodies with dizzying leaves the sun and yanks immortal stars awry? Blow king to beggar and queen to seem (blow friend to fiend: blow space and time)…

Each underlined segment represents the stress of a phrase and each line consists of four of these.

Just looking at the accents, it comes down to long, short, short, long, short, short, long, short,

8

short, long – almost like a waltz in music. Each line, though, does not contain the same number of syllables which distinguishes this type of prosody from syllabic prosody.

Virgil’s Aeneid is a good example of quantitative prosody. The opening line “Arma virumque cano, Troiae qui primus ab oris..” elucidates a stress pattern that is irregular.

Quantitative prosody is unconcerned with number of syllables but with the duration of syllables.

ARma viRUmque caNO, TROIAE qui PRImus ab orIS. Each capitalized segment is stressed based on the time it takes to naturally pronounce the line.

9

CHAPTER 2. STUDIES IN PROSODY

As the study of prosody began to evolve, the ways in which it could be discussed increased. Some musicologists and linguists suggest that the prosody of a culture’s speech patterns can affect the entire structure of the music of a particular region. As a result, these scholars have developed a way to measure this change. Aniruddh Patel conducted a study which found that rhythmic patterns in English and French have directly affected the musical themes in each of those regions and so there is an empirical basis upon which to make these claims.9

Another study authored by Applebaum, Coppola, and Goldin-Meadow showed that prosody in a communication system without a spoken language model exists (such as sign language) and suggests that rhythm and intonation go much deeper than spoken language and actually show up in body movement.10

Patel’s study measures the ways in which humans produce organized rhythmic and melodic patterns in two forms: prosody and music. While these patterns are typically studied by different research communities, their relationship has long interested scholars in both linguistics and musicology. For example, linguists such as Nick Nicholas have borrowed musicological concepts in building prosodic theories and musicologists including Gerald Abraham have used tools from linguistic theory to describe musical structure.11 Patel claims that a composer’s music reflects prosodic patterns in their native language, and scholars over the past half century have voiced this idea repeatedly.12 For example, the English musicologist Gerald Abraham explored this topic at length, citing as one example Ralph Kirkpatrick’s comment on French keyboard

9 Patel, Aniruddh D. “An empirical comparison of rhythm in language and music”. Cognition. Vol. 87, Issue 1. Feb. 2003, B35-B45. 10 Applebaum Lauren, Marie Coppola, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. “Prosody in a Communication System Developed without a Language Model.” Sign language and linguistics 17.2 (2014): 181–212. 11 Patel 12 Ibid.

10

music: “Both Couperin and Rameau, like Fauré and Debussy, are thoroughly conditioned by the nuances and inflections of spoken French. On no Western music has the influence of language been stronger.”13 In essence, Kirkpatrick, a harpsichordist and musical scholar, was claiming that

French keyboard music sounded like the French language. Similar claims have been made about the instrumental music of other cultures. For instance, the linguist David Hall, for instance, suggested a resemblance between Elgar’s music and the intonation of British speech.14

What makes these claims interesting to me is that they concern instrumental music. It might not be surprising if vocal music reflected speech prosody because that sort of music is directly involved with the rhythmic and melodic properties of a given text in a specific language.

But the idea that speech patterns are mirrored in instrumental music is much more controversial.

But in his study, Patel found a way to quantify this concept in his study. Without getting into detail beyond the scope of this paper, Patel uses an nPVI or “normalized Pairwise Variability

Index” to measure the degree of durational contrast between successive elements in a sequence and was developed to explore rhythmic differences between “stress-timed” and “-timed” languages. Evidence gathered using the nPVI supports Patel’s theory.15

Empirical work in phonetics has revealed that the nPVI of vowel durations in sentences is significantly higher in stress-timed languages such as British English than in syllable-timed languages such as French.16 Patel applied the nPVI to the durations of notes in instrumental classical themes from England and France and found that English music had a significantly higher nPVI than French music.17

13 Patel 14 Ibid. 15 Ibid. 16 Ibid. 17 Ibid.

11

Going even further, Patel asks whether speech melody is reflected in music. The original idea of a connection between prosody and instrumental music was not just about rhythm but also about melody. Patel’s study addresses this issue via a quantitative comparison of intonation and musical melody. Earlier work on rhythm had the benefit of an empirical measure which could readily be applied to music. In the case of intonation, no such measure was available. To overcome this problem, this study employs a recent computational model of speech intonation perception known as the “prosogram” The prosogram converts a sentence’s fundamental frequency contour into a sequence of discrete tonal segments, producing a representation which is meant to capture the perceived pitch pattern of a speech melody.18 This representation allows a quantitative comparison of pitch variability in speech and music.19

The rhythms and melodies of speech and instrumental music can be quantitatively compared using tools from modern phonetics. Using these tools, an investigation of language and music from England and France confirms the intuition that music reflects the prosody of a composer’s native language. The approaches developed here can be applied to the study of language-music relations in other cultures and may prove useful in quantifying non-native prosody. These findings can also give creative power to composers who want to use prosody as a conscious basis for their music.

I am also interested in how prosodic structures show up not just in vocal language but also in physical language. According to a study conducted by Lauren Applebaum, prosody is considered to be the “music” of language and is an important aspect of all natural languages, spoken and signed. She asks the question: if a child were not exposed to a conventional language

18 Patel 19 Ibid.

12

and had to construct his own communication system, would that system contain prosodic structure? She addresses this question by observing a deaf child who had received no sign language input and whose hearing loss prevented him from acquiring spoken language. Despite his lack of a conventional language model, this child developed his own gestural system.20 In this system, features known to mark phrase and utterance boundaries in established sign languages were used to consistently mark the ends of utterances, but not to mark phrase or utterance internal boundaries. A single child can thus develop the seeds of a prosodic system.21 I intuit that composers are affected this way as well.

Prosodic structures, like all other language properties, are produced not only by the vocal cords of spoken language users, but also by the hands, faces, heads, and bodies of sign language users.22 Although the physical realization of prosodic features differs in signed and spoken languages, their function is the same; in both types, prosody is used to mark structure at syntactic, semantic, and discourse levels of analysis.23

Even though prosody shows up differently in spoken and signed languages, the features in each type serve similar functions. For example, the breath, which can mark intonational phrases in spoken languages, is comparable to the blink in sign languages: both are necessary biological events; both can be only temporarily postponed; and both play similar roles by marking intonational phrases.24 Even beyond natural biological constraints, features in spoken language have their equivalent in sign language.25 For example, facial expressions are considered the melodies of sign languages; pitch accents and boundary tones are the melodies of spoken

20 Applebaum 21 Ibid. 22 Ibid. 23 Ibid. 24 Ibid. 25 Ibid.

13

languages.26 Research has also shown that prosodic markers used in sign languages, such as eye gaze, body leans, and eyebrow movements, are used and coordinated with speech in hearing individuals, a pattern that begins to develop as early as the second year of life.27

The studies by both Patel and Applebaum draw the conclusion that the formation of prosodic structure in any type of communication is innate for humans. Whether through vocal word formation or body movement patterns of communication, there are stress and rhythmic structures that develop and aid in understanding and communicating. This shows up in a culture’s music as well. For centuries this was done unconsciously, but with the onset of greater experimentation in music from the mid-20th century and onward, prosody has actually been explicitly used to compose music.

26 Ibid. 27 Applebaum

14

CHAPTER 3. COMPOSITIONAL PROCESS AND PROSODY

Composers have often used prosody in their compositional processes. First, Gregorian chant is directly affected language. Second, there are some composers who use prosody very specifically: for example, Karlheinz Stockhausen in Michaels Reise. He took essential elements of their compositional styles from both Greek prosody and intuitive moments of guttural vocal expression. There are also some composers who acknowledge the influence of prosody but use it less explicitly, such as Gjorgy Ligeti in his Glissandi and Artikulations and Steve Reich in The

Cave and Tehillim.

Gregorian chant and folk music are obviously affected by prosody. Language is directly used by both and its musical structure is based on words and phrasing. Chants/cantillations in different languages affect the rhythmic motion and intonation of the “music”. Gregorian intonation, based in , tends to remain around the ordinary male pitch structure. The same goes for the same cantillations in Hebrew. Rhythm and pitch movement is directly connected the individual language of each region.28

Michaels Reise is the 2nd act of Stockhausen's opera, Licht with Donnerstag Aus Licht

(Thursday from Light), being the section it is part of. Licht is a work for acoustic and electronic operatic instruments divided into the seven days with one opera for each day. This opera cycle revolves around three archetypical characters, Michael, Eve and Lucifer, and over the work’s 29 hours, each of these characters are introduced, come into conflict, face temptation and finally come into union. The music is almost entirely based on a “super-formula”, which is a 3-layered melodic-thematic representation of the 3 characters. These formula-themes are threaded throughout the opera's vocal and instrumental fabric. Story-wise, actors and narrative can (and

28 Dresher, B. Elan. Between music and speech: The relationship between Gregorian and Hebrew chant. All the Things You Are: A Festschrift in Honour of Jack Chambers. Vol 27. 2008.

15

often do) change from act to act and the libretto text is sometimes made up of non-traditional grammar, or even purely phonetic sounds.

Donnerstag (Thursday) is about Michael’s experience as a human being on Earth, and the celebration of his return to "Heaven". Act II, Michaels Reise um die Erde (Michael’s Journey

Round the Earth) depicts Michael solely by a trumpet theme, and this theme "visits" various cities on Earth and interacts with their individual characters/textures. While traveling to the seventh Station (Africa), a basset horn (representing Eve) is heard, at which point the trumpet pursues the basset horn in a seductive dialogue. Eventually the pair fly off into the distance in harmony.

Michael’s Reise is not a typical composition with melodic phrasing supported by harmonic formulae. Instead, it consists of guttural starts and stops, similar to a child first learning language through making noise. The entrance begins with aggressive brass stabs followed by the trumpet playing Michael’s formula: high D-A-B-flat followed by a dotted-eighth note A-flat and sixteenth note D-flat, quarter note D-flat.

Figure 1. Trumpet Theme from Michaels Reise um die Erde by Karlheinz Stockhausen. This motif appears throughout the piece. After this happens, Michael departs and soft, hesitant figures sound, mimicking the sound of crying and longing. In each station that follows, the trumpet comes into conversation with instruments associated with each part of the world.

Stockhausen does not use the prosodic structure of each region, but uses basic intuitive guttural expressions found in all human language, in the sort of way filmmakers portray “cavemen”.29

29 Stockhausen, Karlheinz. Michales Reise um die Erde Score.

16

The Cave is a multimedia opera in three acts by Steve Reich, set to an English libretto by his wife, Beryl Korot. It was first performed in 1993 in Vienna by the Steve Reich Ensemble conducted by Paul Hillier. The title of the opera refers to the Cave of the Patriarchs, located in

Hebron, where Abraham and Sarah (and several other major religious figures) are buried.

Brief spoken extracts from interviews with individuals from three major religious contexts were used both as they were recorded during the interviews, but also as repeated musical phrases. The melodic phrases used in the opera are all taken directly from the intonation, tone, stress, and rhythm of the natural spoken phrases and sentences used by the individuals interviewed. In other words, the musical phrases are based on the prosody which can be heard in the phrases and sentences spoken by the individuals. Images of the interviewees are also shown on an array of video screens shown during the performance.

Glissandi (1957) is the first electronic composition that Ligeti wrote. It precedes his better known Artikulation, which was written a year later. In Glissandi, the sole effect of music is the glissando, or a continuous slide upward or downward between musical notes. Why does he choose the glissando as a sole element for this piece? Ligeti was interested in speech patterns, especially its articulation. In the study of prosody, smooth pitch movements are thought to be important for their “cumulative function”. 30In a sentence, the pitch slides up and down, much like a glissando. Ligeti’s focus on glissandi in this piece takes apart the consonant articulation of speech and only uses the pitch changes in language. From there he constructs different types of glissandi: upward and downward motion in the mid-high frequency range, downward glissandi in the mid frequency range with harmonic spectra which sound “nasal”, colored noise glissandi, glissandi controlled by controlling the speed of a recorded tape and sinusoidal glissandi in

30 Trubezkoy N.S., Principles of Phonology. University of California Press. 1939.

17

counterpoint within the high range. Unlike Reich, Ligeti took a more abstract approach to prosody and used his interest and intuition to guide his composition.

18

PART II. SCRATCHES

19

CHAPTER 4. COMPOSER’S STATEMENT

Scratches I

In the first movement of Scratches, I use my parents’ Cajun speech patterns as the basis for the rhythmic and accentual structure of the melody. Cajun is technically considered a dialect of the French language, although Cajun and standard French are not always mutually intelligible.

The French language is classified as a Romance language belonging to the Italic subfamily of the

Indo-European family of languages.31 A wide variety of French dialects can be found around the world, from Africa to Haiti, Canada, Indochina and more; so, the Cajun French spoken in central and southern Louisiana is just one of many types of French, a truly global language.32

Generally speaking, Cajun French is considered to be a flat, monotonous language with many repeated tones in one sentence. Its accents tend to be at the beginning of a sentence and the speaker’s tongue sits low and towards the back of the mouth. For example: The famous phrase,

“Laissez les bon temps rouler” is accented at the “Lai” syllable only. This phrase can be written as accented 16th note-8th note-16th note followed by 4 eighth notes.

Figure 2. “Laissez les bon temp rouler”

The opening statement of the trumpet in the first movement of Scratches is based off of this. In measure 32 the trumpet comes in with its opening statement almost identical to “Laissez…” The same thing occurs again at measure 51 with some variation in pitch movement (though it’s only a

31 Whatley, Randall P. and Harry Jannise. Conversational Cajun French I. Pelican Publishing, Baton Rouge, LA. (1982). 32 Whatley.

20

step away from the D).

Figure 3. Scratches I meausre 51 Cajuns have the reputation of being boisterous conversationalists. Oftentimes I remember my family speaking over top of each other in very quick, short snippets of phrase that almost sounded like they were making fun of each other – almost mockingly. I try to imitate this in measure 36 with the conversation that happens between the trumpet and the second violin.

Figure 4. Scratches 1 measure 36 The violin starts the conversation with a quick two-note utterance followed the by the same in the trumpet immediately. The violin tries to say it three times with the trumpet interrupting each time but finally gives in by the second measure. The trumpet continues the conversation and finishes it by laughing (the triplets in measure 40).

Figure 5. Scratches I measure 40

21

The first and second violins continue the same conversation almost like the second violin telling the first violin what just happened.

Finally, I close the first movement with a phrase my dad used often: “It’s easy to be a good person rather than a bad one.” He would never say “easier.” I have the trumpet move pitches here while the viola and cello keep the monophony going. Each end note of the phrase is brought up a fifth, then a fourth in the trumpet ending on a high C. The movement closes with the long tones from the very beginning of the movement.

Figure 6. Scratches I measure 55

Scratches II

In the second movement of Scratches, I use my experience of learning Spanish in Tijuana as a starting point. The trumpet part is accompanied by a fairly minimalist piano part. During my time in Tijuana, I listened to the band Coldplay often and I associate a lot of memories from this time with them. Accordingly, the piano part mimics Coldplay’s use of the Zen principle in music

(minimalism) used by bands like U2 and others. Chords are arpeggiated or repeated rapidly for extended lengths of time. On top of this minimalist piano part is a muted trumpet using half- valve technique throughout in order to represent the accentual prosody of the Spanish I heard in

Tijuana.

The Spanish language is distinct in its sound – whether in Spain or Latin America, people are able to mimic its prosodic structure fairly easily. This is how I learned Spanish – through

22

mimicry. The trumpet technique being used is an attempt to sound human. By holding the valves halfway down and using hand stops and hand motion at the bell of the trumpet a “humanlike” sound is made. King Oliver, jazz trumpet player in the early 20th century, was adept at this.

King Oliver was an American jazz cornet player and bandleader. He was particularly recognized for his playing style and his pioneering use of mutes in jazz.33 Also a notable composer, he wrote many tunes still played today, including Dippermouth Blues, Sweet Like

This, Canal Street Blues, and Doctor Jazz. He was the mentor and teacher of Louis Armstrong.

His mute work was well known, and he used a variety of different types in order to get the human-like sound he was known for. Besides mutes, he used derby hats, bottles and cups to alter his sound. 34

The beginning of the second movement starts out with the piano only playing very rapid sixteenth note hand trills in Cm7. It then moves through Cm7 adding a D, Bbsus, Ab2, Bb7, Eb,

Bbm7, Db, Ab2, and so on for a 32 measure sequence. At measure 33, the piano jumps up an octave and the trumpet enters using half-valve-muted technique. The trumpet line is only one line with the central line indicating the most comfortable register of the trumpet’s range – around a G above middle C. Each measure following contains a line indicating the direction of the trumpet should take in its improvisation. This movement is a very loose memory of hearing

Spanish spoken through the walls of the chapel I slept in at the convent in Tijuana. I remember phrases starting out high and almost glissing low unless there was a question which then it would gliss upwards.

33 Williams, Martin. King Oliver Kings of Jazz. Nabu Press: Charleston, SC, 2011. 34 Martin.

23

Scratches III

The third movement of Scratches is based off of the first two paragraphs of Barack

Obama’s acceptance speech in Chicago in 2008 and the myriad languages I heard around me while standing in Grant Park. The piano is used as a base and is played freely while the rest of the instruments play their lines separately and not in tempo with each other. They come together at the end of the piece following the piano’s ostinato figure and slowly morphing into playing the ostinato with the piano.

The two violins and viola are used as Barack Obama’s voice. Obama has a very distinct prosody. The strings mimic the rhythm and accent of his use of language:

Hello, Chicago! If there is anyone out there who still doubts that America is a place where all things are possible; who still wonders if the dream of our founders is alive in our time; who still questions the power of our democracy, tonight is your answer.

It's the answer told by lines that stretched around schools and churches in numbers this nation has never seen; by people who waited three hours and four hours, many for the very first time in their lives, because they believed that this time must be different; that their voices could be that difference.35

He starts and stops in ways that pull the listener in, demanding their attention, so I try to copy this exactly.

The piccolo trumpet is representative of the higher pitched, sing-song languages like

Hindi and other Asian languages. They rise and fall very quickly. The opening is an example of this:

35 Speights, Regina. November 4, 2008: The Mood of a Nation. E-BookTime LLC. 2009.

24

Figure 7. Scratches III measure 1 The trumpet bounces on the same note until near the end of the phrase when it jumps up and falls very quickly.

The cello is representative of the lower pitched, slower languages like Russian and

Polish. Eastern European and Russian languages tend to be slower and have downward glisses.

The cello mimics this by glissing downward at the beginning of each phrase:

Figure 8. Scratches III measure 1

These languages also tend to have large skips in pitch between words and the cello follows this as well:

Figure 9. Scratches III measure 61

These three sections and the piano begin the piece in different tempi. As soon as the piano starts its arpeggios, the other instruments lock into its tempo. This is to represent how I felt the people in the crowd reacted to Obama’s speech: slowly, they began to merge together.

Everyone, all different races, nationalities, and age were united by Obama’s message that night

25

and so the instruments rhythmically come together. Eventually, the piano starts an ostinato on C and the other instruments begin the same until they are all playing the C ostinato.

Figure 10. Scratches III measure 116 Conclusion

The overall harmonic structure of Scratches is simple. The first movement is centered in

C, the second movement is centered in A-flat, and the third movement moves back to C. The first movement uses the ostinato in the strings as basis for its harmonic structure which stays in C for the entire piece except for a measure of E-flat major in measures 38, 59, and the final repetition measures 64 to the end.

Throughout history, composers like me have been inspired by language in both direct and indirect ways. Having been inspired by this phenomenon, especially as manifest by Stockhausen,

Ligeti, and Reich, I composed Scratches, an original composition for trumpet and chamber ensemble using various prosodic situations from my past. The study of prosody has recently shed light on how it affects musical composition and there are now ways of measuring this – in particular how different cultures languages affects the rhythm and intonation of its music. I hope this dive into my own musical inspiration will enliven further studies and compositional practice.

26

SCRATCHES: AN ORIGINAL COMPOSITION FOR TRUMPET AND CHAMBER ENSEMBLE BY SHANE COURVILLE

Instrumentation

C trumpet A piccolo trumpet Piano Violin I Violin II Viola Cello Signer in American Sign Language (ASL)

Program Notes

Research in prosody (the study of accents and intonation in language) guided the creation of Scratches. It uses speech and body movement prosody I have encountered from three distinct times in my life. The first movement is based on my parent’s Cajun speech patterns, the second movement is derived from my time learning Spanish on the streets of Tijuana, and the third movement is inspired by the time I was in the crowd for Barack Obama’s 2008 presidential victory speech in Chicago.

Accompanying the trumpet in the first movement will be a string quartet – two violins, one viola, and one cello. These instruments provide the chordal and timbral accompaniment for the trumpet solo. They also form the “landscape” for the trumpet and have short conversations with the trumpet by responding to and repeating what the trumpet plays. The second movement is accompanied by piano only, which supports the trumpet’s Spanish mimicry. I listened to

Coldplay during my time in Tijuana and have based the piano parts on Coldplay’s style.

The instrumentation for the third movement consists of piccolo trumpet, string quartet, piano, and a person signing in American Sign Language (ASL). The piccolo trumpet expresses those languages that tend to be higher pitched with rapid prosodic structure. The two violins and

27

viola play a direct rhythmic transcription of Obama’s 2008 speech mentioned above while the cello expresses the prosodic structure of languages with lower pitched and slower prosody. The

ASL signer will be signing the parts of Obama’s speech that I use in the strings, which shows how prosody also manifests itself in body movement.

Performance Notes

Scratches I 1. This movement is straightforward. The first violin should direct the various entrances for the quartet. 2. The violins at the beginning and end should bleed seamlessly into one another as if they are one instrument.

Scratches II

1. The trumpet should compress their valves half-way throughout this entire movement.

2. The direction of the line indicates the direction of the pitch. 3. The lines are suggestions/guidelines for register and contour with pitches indicated at arrival points.

28 8

42

C Tpt. & 42 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

44 C Tpt. bœ & 44 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

4. Some lines are curved more than others. The performer should follow the lines 46 reasonably close. If a line has a higher curve, make the pitch change more. If the line C Tpt. has less of a curve, move in a more direct motion to the next pitch.

& 46 œ œ œ œ œ5. Theœ performerœ œ shouldœ feel freeœ to œimproviseœ inœ orderœ to createœ aœ vocal-like sound; ie using bœ œ œ œ œ theœ handœ overœ the mutebœ to makeœ theœ “waœ-wa” œsound.œ A plungerœ œis recommended. œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈Scratches III≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ 1.œ Theœ trumpet,œ piano,bœ andœ cello œare eachœ a separateœ œ line œwhileœ the two violins and viola ? work as a single voice. 2. Each instrument or group may start at a different time and go at a different tempo until the piano starts the arpeggiated ostinato. 3. The first violin should aid in the starts of each phrase for the trio. 4. When the instruments hear the piano ostinato, they are to move into that tempo to finish out the piece. 5. The American Sign Language Signer should feel free to sign throughout the piece. They do not have to follow the rhythm of the strings but find their own rhythm. It should look as close to a dance as possible. 6. The piece finishes after all the instruments have played the final C ostinato for 30 seconds or more. 7. The trumpet should drop out first, followed by the violins, viola, cello, and finally the piano.

29

Score Scratches

I Shane Courville

q = 60 Trumpet in C & 4 ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ w w w w w Violin I & 4 ∑ ∑ p bleed seamlessly Violin II w w w w w & 4 ∑ ∑ p Viola 4 B 4 ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Cello ? 4 4 ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

8

C Tpt. & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

8 w w w w w w w w Vln. I & w w w w Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vla. B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

©

30

2 16

C Tpt. & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ 16 w w w w w w w w Vln. I & w w w w Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vla. B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

24

C Tpt. & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ 24 w w w w Vln. I & ∑ f w w w w Vln. II & ∑ f œ Vla. B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ J F Vc. œ œ œ œ œ ? œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó F

31

3 29

C Tpt. & ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ 29 F Vln. I & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ J œ œ Œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Œ œ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ J3 3 3 J

Vc. œ œ œ œ ? œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó

33 C Tpt. ˙ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ ˙ ∑ 33 ˙ w Vln. I & ∑ Ó f ˙ œ œ Vln. II ‰ œ œ œ & ∑ Ó ‰ œ ‰ ‰ ‰ f œ œ œ œ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ J 3 3 J

Vc. œ. œ. œ. œ ? œ. ‰ Ó œ. ‰ Ó œ. œ. œ. œ.

32

4 36 C Tpt. œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œœœœ & ‰ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ ˙ œœœœœ 36

Vln. I & ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ. œ œ ˙. Vln. II & ‰ ≈ ≈ ‰ ‰ Œ F œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ J J

Vc. œ. œ. ? œ. ‰ Ó œ. ‰ Ó

38 œ 3 C Tpt. ˙ œ œ œ œ œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈ 3 œ œ 3 œ œ œ 38 f Vln. I bœ œ œ œ œ œ w & 3 3 ∑

Vln. II bœ œ œ œ œ œ w & 3 3 ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ J J 3 3 œ œ œ œ Vc. œ. œ œ œ œ œ. ? œ. ‰ Ó œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ. ‰ Ó

33

5 41

C Tpt. & ˙ ˙ w 41 œ œ œ œ œ œ. Vln. I & ∑ ‰ ≈ ≈ ‰ ‰ Œ

Vln. II œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ & ∑ ‰ Œ œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Vla. B Œ œ œ œ œ œ Œ œ œ ‰ œ œ œ 3 3

Vc. œ œ ? œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó

43 3 C Tpt. ∑ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ & 3 œ œ 3 œ œ 43 œ œ ˙ Vln. I & ‰ ∑

Vln. II œ œ œ œ œ ˙ Ó & ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ œ œ ‰ œ œ œ J 3

Vc. œ œ ? œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó

34

6 45 ˙ w C Tpt. œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ & ˙ œ œ œ œ 45 F Vln. I œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ∑ ∑

Vln. II & ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ∑ œ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ J œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ J F Vc. ? œ. ‰ Ó œ. ‰ Ó œ. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ. 48 F C Tpt. œ œ & ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œœœ œ œ œœ

48

Vln. I & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ J J J J

Vc. œ œ œ œ ? œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó

35

7 52 ˙ C Tpt. ˙ œœœœ j œ œ & œ œ œ J ˙ ˙ 52

Vln. I & ∑ ∑ ∑

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ J J J

Vc. œ œ œ ? œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó

55 œ œ œ œ œ C Tpt. œ œ œ œ œ & ‰ œœ≈ œœœ≈ œ œ œ œ ≈ œœœœœ≈ œœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ 55

Vln. I & ∑ ∑

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ J J

Vc. œ œ ? œ. . ‰ Ó œ. . ‰ Ó

36

8 57 w C Tpt. & ∑

57 œ œ œ œ œ œ. œ œ ˙ Vln. I & ‰ ≈ ≈ ‰ ‰ Œ ‰

Vln. II œ œ ‰ œ œ. Œ œ œ œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œœœœ & œ œ œ ˙ œœœœ œ œ Vla. B Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ J J

Vc. œ. œ. ? œ. ‰ Ó œ. ‰ Ó

59

C Tpt. & ∑ ∑ ∑

59 œ œ Vln. I bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ Œ ‰ œ œ & 3 3 J J

Vln. II w & œ. ‰ Ó œ. ‰ Ó œ. œ.

Vla. œ. œ. . œ. œ B œ. œ ∑ ∑

Vc. bœ . œ. w ? bœ œ œ. œ œ. ∑ 3

37

9 62 C Tpt. œ œ œ œ w & ≈ œ œ ≈ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® 62 œ œ Vln. I œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & Œ ‰ J Œ ‰ J

Vln. II . ‰ Ó . ‰ Ó & œ. œ œ. œ

Vla. B ≈ bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® w

Vc. ? ≈ bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® w

64 C Tpt. œ œ œ w & ≈ œ œ œ ≈ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® 64 œ œ Vln. I œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & Œ ‰ J Œ ‰ J

Vln. II . ‰ Ó . ‰ Ó & œ. œ œ. œ

Vla. B ≈ bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® w

Vc. ? ≈ bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® w

38

10 66 C Tpt. œ œ œ œ w & ≈ œ œ ≈ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® 66 œ œ Vln. I œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & Œ ‰ J Œ ‰ J

Vln. II . ‰ Ó . ‰ Ó & œ. œ œ. œ

Vla. B ≈ bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® w

Vc. ? ≈ bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® w

68 w C Tpt. œ œ œ & ≈ œ œ œ ≈ ≈ œ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® ∑ ∑ ∑ 68 œ w w w Vln. I œ œ œ œ & Œ ‰ J ∑ p Vln. II w w w . ‰ Ó ∑ & œ. œ p Vla. B ≈bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? ≈bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ® ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

39

11 73

C Tpt. & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

73 w w w w Vln. I &

Vln. II w w & w w

Vla. B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

40

Score Scratches II Shane Courville

q =plunger 144 Trumpet in C 4 & 4

4 ≈bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ & 4 bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Piano ? 4 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ 4 œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ Ped. f 3

C Tpt. &

3 bœœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ bœœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ & ≈bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈

5

C Tpt. &

5 ≈bbœœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈bbœœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ ≈ œœ & bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈

©

41

2

7

C Tpt. &

7 bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈

9

C Tpt. &

9 ≈bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ & bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈

11

C Tpt. &

11 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno.

? bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈

42

3

13

C Tpt. &

13 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno. ? bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

15

C Tpt. &

15 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno. ? bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

17

C Tpt. &

17 bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno. ? bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

43

4

19

C Tpt. &

19 bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno.

? bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

21

C Tpt. &

21 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno.

? ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ 23

C Tpt. &

23 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno.

? ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

44

5

25

C Tpt. &

25 bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno. r ? ‰ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

27

C Tpt. &

27 bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno. ? bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bbœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

29

C Tpt. &

29 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno.

? bœ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ b œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ b œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

45

6

31

C Tpt. &

31 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ Pno. ? bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ ≈ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ 33 begin half-valve improvisation C Tpt. & œ

33 bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ p 35

C Tpt. &

35 bbœœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ bbœœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ b œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ b œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ?

46

7

37

C Tpt. & œ

37 bbœœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ b œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

38

C Tpt. &

38 bbœœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ œœ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ b œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

40 C Tpt. œ &

40 bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

47

8

42

C Tpt. &

42 bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

44 C Tpt. bœ & 44 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

46

C Tpt. & 46 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ?

48

9

48

C Tpt. & 48 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ?

50

C Tpt. & 50 bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ?

52

C Tpt. & bœ 52 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

49

10

54

C Tpt. & 54 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

56

C Tpt. & bœ 56 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ‰ ≈ R ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

58

C Tpt. & 58 bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ ≈ bœ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ?

50

11

60

C Tpt. & bœ 60 bbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ cresc.

62

C Tpt. & 62 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

64

C Tpt. & 64 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ & ≈ ≈ ≈ ≈ bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. f ? bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bw bw

51

12

66

C Tpt. &

66

& bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bw bw bw bw

68 C Tpt. b˙ b˙ &

68

& bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœbœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ Pno. ? bw bœ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ bw 3

70 p

C Tpt. &

70 bœ œ œ bœ œ œ & bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœbœbœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bw bw bw bw f

52

13

72

C Tpt. &

72

& bœbœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœbœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bw bw bw bw

74 C Tpt. b˙ b˙ &

74 bœ œ & bœbœ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ ≈ œ œ œ œ ≈ œ œ œ ≈ œ Pno. ? bœ œ œ œ œ bœ. œ. œ bœ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ 3 . .

76 p

C Tpt. &

76 bœbœ œ œ bœbœ œ œ œ œ & bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bw bw bw bw f

53

14

78

C Tpt. &

78 bœ œ œ bœ œ & bœbœbœ bœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœbœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bw bw bw bw

80 b˙ C Tpt. b˙ b˙ & b˙

80 bœ bœ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ & bœ bœ ≈ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ bœ bœ œ ≈ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ Pno. ? bœ. œ. œ bœ. œ. œ bœ. œ. œ bœ. œ. œ

82 p

C Tpt. b˙ & b˙

82 bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ bœbœ œ œ œ œ & ≈ bœ bœ ≈ œ ≈ œ œ œ bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bœ. œ. œ bw bœ. œ. œ bw f

54

15

84

C Tpt. &

84 bœ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ & bœbœbœ bœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. ? bw bw bw bw

86 C Tpt. b˙ & b˙

86 bœ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ œ œ bœbœ œ œ œ œ ≈ ≈ & bœbœbœbœ œ œ œ œ œ bœ ≈ œ œ ≈ Pno. ? bw bw bw p 88

C Tpt. b˙ b˙ b˙ & b˙

88 bœ œ œ œ bœ œ œ ≈ œ ≈ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ & ≈ ≈ bœ œ ≈ bœ bœ ≈ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ Pno. ? bw bw

55

16

90 b˙ C Tpt. b˙ b˙ b˙ &

90 bœ bœ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ & bœ bœ œ ≈ ≈ œ œ ≈ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno.

? ∑ ∑

92 b˙ C Tpt. b˙ b˙ & b˙

92 bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ & bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. w ? ∑

94

C Tpt. b˙ b˙ b˙ b˙ & 94 bœ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ & bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. bw ? ∑

56

17

96

C Tpt. b˙ b˙ & 96 bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ & Pno. w ? ∑

98

C Tpt. & 98 bœ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ bœ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & Pno. ? bw bw

100

C Tpt. &

100 bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ & bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno.

? ∑ ∑

57

18

102

C Tpt. &

102 bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ & bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ Pno. w ? bw

104

C Tpt. & 104 bœ œ œ œ bœ œ œ œ bœ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & Pno.

? ∑ ∑

106

C Tpt. & 106 bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ bœ bœ œ œ œ œ œ œ & Pno. w ? bw

58

19

108

C Tpt. &

108

& ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ Pno. ? bw bw bw bw

59

Score Scratches III Shane Courville

q = 144 Piccolo Trumpet in A bb 5 œ. œœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œœœ œ œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ. œ. œ. ≈ œ. œ. œ. ≈ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & b 8 œ J J J p

j j j j j j 5 ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ j ˙ œ ˙ œ & 8 ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ Piano j ? 5 ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ 8 ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ J J J J ˙ œ J J p

Violin I & 85 ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ j ≈Œ ‰ ∑ œ. œ. œ œ. œ . F

Violin II & 85 ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ j ≈Œ ‰ ∑ œ. œ. œ œ. œ . F

Viola j B 85 ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ. œ. œ œ. œ . ≈Œ ‰ ∑ F improvised glisses Cello j j j j j j ? 85 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ p

ASL & 85 Hello Chicago!

©

60

2

8 Picc.Tpt. bb œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ ‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ & b J

8 j j j ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ & ˙ œ ˙ œ œ ˙ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ J J J J J Pno.

œ ˙ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ? ˙ œ ˙ œ œ ˙ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ J J J J J J J J

8

Vln. I & Œ Œ j j Œ ∑ j œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ. œ œ œ

Vln. II & Œ Œ j j Œ ∑ j œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ. œ œ œ

Vla. j j j B œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ œ Œ Œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ. Œ ∑ œ œ œ

Vc. ? j œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ∑ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ∑ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰

8

& if there is anyone out there who still doubts that America is a place where all things

61

3

16 Picc.Tpt. b ˙ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ. œ. œ œ œ œ œ œ & b b ‰ ∑ J œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

16 œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ b˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ bœ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ J J J 3 J J J J J J Pno.

œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ? œ œ œ œ œ œ b˙˙ œœ ˙˙ œœ ˙˙ œœ œœ bœœ œœ ˙ œ ˙ œ J J J 3 J J J J J J

16

Vln. I ∑ j j ∑ & œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ.

Vln. II & ∑ j j ∑ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ.

Vla. j j B œ œ ˙ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ. ∑

Vc. r r ? ∑ ˙ ‰ ∑ ‰ ˙ œ ˙ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ J

16

& possible who still wonders if the dream of our founders is alive in our time

62

4

23 Picc.Tpt. bb j j ‰ j j œ œ. œ. œ. œ œ œ œ & b ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ J

23 ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ‰ œ œ œ & J J J J J J J J J J J J J J Pno.

˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ J J J J J J J J J J J J J ‰ J

23 Vln. I j & ∑ ∑ ∑ ˙ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ. ∑ ∑

Vln. II ∑ ∑ ∑ ‰ j ∑ ∑ & ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ.

Vla. B ∑ ∑ ∑ ˙ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ. ∑ ∑ J

Vc. ? ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ‰ J J J J ˙ ˙

23

& who still questions the power of our democracy.

63

5

32 Picc.Tpt. œœ œ œœœ œ bb œ œ Œ ∑ ∑ ∑ J Œ ‰ J Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ. & b J J J

32 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙˙ œœ ˙˙ œœ & J J J ‰ J J J J ∑ J J Pno.

œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ J J J ‰ J J J J ∑ J J

32

Vln. I ∑ ∑ œœ œ œ œ œ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ j j & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ œœ œ œ œ œ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ j j œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Vla. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ B ∑ ∑ œœ œ œ œ œ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ J J

Vc. ? ‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ j j ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

32

& tonight is your answer. It's the answer told by lines

64

6

41 Picc.Tpt. b œ. œ. œ. œ. œ. œ. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & b b ∑ J ≈ J ≈ œ œ œ œ œ œ ≈ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ∑

41 j j ˙˙ œœ ˙˙ œœ œœ œ œ ˙˙ œœ œ œ œ ˙˙ œœ & ∑ J J œ œ J œ œ œ J Pno. ˙ œ ˙ œ œ ˙ œ ˙ œ ? ˙ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ ∑ J J J J J J

41

Vln. I j‰ Œ ‰ ∑ j j j ∑ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Vln. II & j‰ Œ ‰ ∑ j j j ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Vla. œ j j j B J ‰ Œ ‰ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ∑

Vc. ? j œ œ œ ∑ œ œ ‰ œ œ ‰ œ œ ‰ œ Œ ‰ œ. œ

41

& that stretched around schools and churches in numbers this nation has never seen

65

7

48 Picc.Tpt. r bb ∑ ∑ œ ˙ j ˙ œ ‰ ≈ j ‰ jŒ ‰ & b ˙ J œ J ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œœœ

48 ˙˙ œœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & J J J J J J Pno. ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? J

48

Vln. I ∑ j r r ≈ ‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ & œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vln. II & ∑ j r r ≈ ‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vla. j r r B ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ ≈ ‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? j j j j j j j œ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ‰

48

& that their voice could be that difference.

66

8

56 Picc.Tpt. r bb ≈ Œ ‰ œœ œœ ∑ œœœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œœ œœ œ œ œ ‰ & b œ œ œ œ œ 3 œ œ ˙

56 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ J œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

56

Vln. I ∑ ∑ ∑ r ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ & œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ r ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vla. r B ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? œ œ œ ‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

56

&

67

9

64 Picc.Tpt. b œœ œœœœ & b b ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ∑ ‰ Œ ‰ Œ ‰

64 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

64

Vln. I ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ r ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ & œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ r ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vla. r B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

64

&

68

10

73 Picc.Tpt. œ bb œœ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ‰ j ‰ ∑ ∑ & b J J J œ œ œ œ ˙ ˙ œ ˙

73 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

73

Vln. I ∑ r ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ & œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vln. II & ∑ r ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vla. r B ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

73

&

69

11

82 Picc.Tpt. b œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ j j œœ & b b ∑ ∑ ‰ Œ ‰ ‰ Œ ‰ œ œ œ œ œ ˙ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

82 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

82

Vln. I & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vla. B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

œ œ œ œ Vc. œ œ œ. œ. œ œ œ œ œ ? ∑ ∑ Œ Œ Œ Œ ∑ œ

82

&

70

12

90 Picc.Tpt. b œœ œœ œœ œ œ ˙ œœ ˙ & b b ∑ J ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

90 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

90

Vln. I ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ r ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ & œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ r ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ

Vla. r B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ. œ œ œ œ œ ≈‰ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰ ˙ ‰

90

&

71

13

99 Picc.Tpt. ˙ ˙ œ ˙ bb ∑ œœ œ œ ‰ œœœœ œ œ œœœœœ œœœœœ ‰ ∑ ‰ J ∑ & b J J

99 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ J

Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

99

Vln. I & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vla. B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

Vc. ? œ œ œ œ œ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

99

&

72

14

108 10 seconds Picc.Tpt. b œ œ œ œ œ & b b ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ π

108 œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Pno. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ? & œ. œ π 15 seconds 108 œ œ œ œ œ Vln. I & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ π

15 seconds Vln. II & ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ π 15 seconds Vla. B ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ π 20 seconds Vc. ? ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ œ œ œ œ œ π

108

&

73

15

117 etc. Picc.Tpt. b œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & b b ‘ ‘ ‘

117 play until all instruments have dropped out

& œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ‘

Pno.

& œ. œ œ. œ œ. œ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑ ∑

117 etc. œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ Vln. I & ‘ ‘

etc. Vln. II & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ‘ ‘

etc. Vla. B œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ‘ ‘

etc. Vc. ? œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ ‘

117

&

74

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Adler, Samuel. The Study of Orchestration, 4th edition. WW Norton & Company Incorporated: New York, 2002.

Adorno, Theodor W. Philosophy of Modern Music: A Continuum Book. Seabury Press: New York, 1973.

Applebaum, Lauren, Marie Coppola, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. “Prosody in a Communication System Developed without a Language Model.” Sign language and linguistics 17.2 (2014): 181– 212. PMC. Web. 1 Oct. 2018.

Berthelot, Michael Joseph. “A Symphonic Poem on Dante's Inferno and a Study on Karlheinz Stockhausen and his effect on the trumpet”, PhD Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, 2008. 3187.

Bradshaw, Robert J. “Composing an Opera for Trumpet.” ITG Journal 39 (4): 66. 2015.

Blatter, Alfred. Instrumentation and Orchestration. Cengage Learning: Boston, MA. 1997.

Brant, Henry. Textures and Timbres: An Orchestrator's Handbook. Carl Fischer Music: New York, 2009.

Cassone, Gabriele. The Trumpet Book. Zecchini: Italy, 2009.

Cook, Nicholas and Pople, Anthony. The Cambridge History of twentieth- Century Music. Cambridge University Press: United Kingdom, 2004.

Cowell, Henry, and David Nicholls. New Musical Resources. Cambridge University Press: United Kingdom, 1996.

Cross, Jonathan. The Stravinsky Legacy. Cambridge University Press: United Kingdom, 1998.

Dresher, B. Elan. Between music and speech: The relationship between Gregorian and Hebrew chant. All the Things You Are: A Festschrift in Honour of Jack Chambers. Vol 27. 2008.

Drew, Joseph. Michael from Licht: A character study of Karlheinz Stockhausen's hero. PhD Dissertation. New York University, 2014.

Duchesneau, Louise, and Wolfgang Marx. 2011. György Ligeti: Of Foreign Lands and Strange Sounds. Boydell Press: Woodbridge, Suffolk; Rochester, NY, 2011.

Eve, Robin. “Similarities between Prosodic Structures in Language and and Rhythmic and Melodic Structures in Music”. Jaila Journal, issue 3, 2017.

75

Ewazen, Eric. “A Lifetime of Composing for Trumpet.” ITG Journal 39 (1): 81–83. 2014.

Gerou, Tom, David Black. Essential Dictionary of Orchestration. Alfred Music: Los Angeles, 2009.

Gioia, Ted. The History of Jazz. Oxford University Press, 2nd edition: United Kingdom 2011.

Gould, Elaine. Behind Bars: The Definitive Guide to Music Notation. Alfred Music: Los Angeles, 2011.

Griffiths, Paul. Modern Music and After Directions since 1945. Oxford University Press: United Kingdom, 2002.

Gross, Harvey. “The Aesthetic Function of Prosody.” The Centennial Review of Arts and Sciences, vol. 7, no. 2, 1963, pp. 204–218.

Harvey, Jonathan. 1975. The Music of Stockhausen : An Introduction. University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, 1975.

Herbert, Trevor and Wallace, John. The Cambridge Companion to Brass Instruments. Cambridge University Press: United Kingdom, 1997.

Ilie, Gabriella, and William Forde Thompson. “A Comparison of Acoustic Cues in Music and Speech for Three Dimensions of Affect.” Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, vol. 23, no. 4, 2006, pp. 319–330.

Kello, Christopher T., Simone Dalla Bella, Butovens Mede, and Ramesh Balasubramaniam. “Hierarchical temporal structure in music, speech, and animal vocalizations: jazz is like conversation, humpbacks sing like hermit thrushes.” Journal of the Royal Society. October 11, 2017. vol 14 issue 135.

Kennan, Kent. Counterpoint. Pearson: London. 1998.

Kennan, Kent. Technique of Orchestration. Prentice-Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2002.

Kramer, Richard. Unfinished Music. Oxford University Press. 2008.

Levy, Benjamin R. 2017. Metamorphosis in Music : The Compositions of György Ligeti in the 1950s and 1960s. Oxford University Press: New York, 2017

Livingstone, Steven Robert, and William Forde Thompson. “Multimodal Affective Interaction: A Comment on Musical Origins.” Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, vol. 24, no. 1, 2006, pp. 89–94.

76

Maconie, Robin. 2005. Other Planets : The Music of Karlheinz Stockhausen. Scarecrow Press: Lanham, MD, 2005.

McKay, George Frederick. Creative Orchestration. CreateSpace: self published. 2016.

Monroe, Harriet. “Prosody.” Poetry, vol. 20, no. 3, 1922, pp. 148–152.

Muthukumar, V.N. “Beyond Tālaprastāra in Indian Music: Prosody as a Generating Function of Rhythmic Complexity in Aruṇakirinātar's Tiruppukaḻ.” Asian Music, vol. 41, no. 1, 2010, pp. 60–88.

Nestico, Sammy. The Complete Arranger. Fenwood: Carlsbad, CA, 2015.

Palmer, Caroline and Michael H. Kelly. “Linguistic Prosody and Musical Meter in Song.” Journal of Memory and Language, New York Vol. 31, Iss. 4, August 1, 1992: 525-542.

Palmer, Caroline and Sean Hutchins. “What is Musical Prosody?” Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Volume 46, 2006, pages 245-278.

Patel, Aniruddh D. “An empircal comparison of rhythm in language and music.” Cognition. Vol. 87, Issue 1. Feb. 2003, pgs B35-B45.

Persichetti, Vincent. Twentieth-Century Harmony. W.W. Norton: New York, 1961.

Piston, Walter. Orchestration. W.W. Norton: New York, 1955.

Pynte, Joel. “The Role of Prosody in Semantic Interpretation.” Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, vol. 16, no. 1, 1998, pp. 79–97.

Rimsky-Korsakov, Nicolai. Principles of Orchestration. Dover: New York, 1964.

Russo, William. Jazz Composition and Orchestration. University of Chicago Press: Chicago, 1997.

Sabina, Leslie. Jazz Arranging and Orchestration. Wadsworth: Belmont, CA, 2002.

Schoenberger, Casey. “Rhythm and Prosody in a Late Imperial Musical Adaptation of Tang Poetry: Expectations Created, Fulfilled, and Denied.” Chinese Literature: Essays, Articles, Reviews (CLEAR), vol. 35, 2013, pp. 155–175.

Speights, Regina. November 4, 2008: The Mood of a Nation. E-BookTime LLC. 2009.

Stockhausen, Karlheinz, and Robin Maconie. 1989. Stockhausen on Music : Lectures and Interviews. London ; New York : M. Boyars ; New York : Distributed in the U.S. by Kampmann, 1989.

77

Stockhausen, Karlheinz, and Tim Nevill. 1989. Towards a Cosmic Music. Element: Shaftesbury, Dorset, 1989.

Thompson, William Forde, et al. “Reduced Sensitivity to Emotional Prosody in Congenital Amusia Rekindles the Musical Protolanguage Hypothesis.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 109, no. 46, 2012, pp. 19027–19032.

Trubezkoy N.S., Principles of Phonology. University of California Press. 1939.

Tymoczko, Dmitri. A Geometry of Music: Harmony and Counterpoint in the Extended Common Practice. Oxford University Press: Oxford, 2011.

Williams, Martin. King Oliver Kings of Jazz. Nabu Press: Charleston, SC, 2011.

Wright, Laurie. “King” Oliver. Storyville Publications: London. 1987.

78

VITA

Shane Courville was born in Lafayette, LA and started playing the trumpet in the 6thgrade. After finishing his BM in trumpet performance at Loyola University New Orleans he entered the

Jesuits and taught high school band in Tampa, FL while composing and performing for different groups including Rome Festival Orchestra. He finished his MA in Philosophy at Loyola

University Chicago focusing on social justice advocacy, gender and music and has lived in

Bolivia, Mexico, and Italy which have influenced his music and art. After leaving the Jesuits in

2009 he moved to New Orleans where he taught and collaborated with different groups including

Chard Gonzalez Dance Theatre and Rivertown Repertory Theatre. After finishing his MM at UL

Lafayette in trumpet performance and founding their New Music Ensemble he began working on his DMA in trumpet performance and composition at LSU in Baton Rouge. He currently is a member of Versipel, a New Music Ensemble based in New Orleans and is collaborating with multiple composers throughout the South. His opera, The Return, was premiered during his residency with the Acadiana Center for the Arts in 2016 and has two more currently being finished.

79