<<

JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN PERSONALITY Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 24–31 www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp

Neuropsychological evidence for dimensional schizotypy: Implications for and psychopathologyq

Joscelyn E. Fisher, Aprajita Mohanty, John D. Herrington, Nancy S. Koven, Gregory A. Miller, and Wendy Heller*

Department of , University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, MC 716, 603 East Daniel Street, Champaign, IL 61820, USA

Abstract

Schizotypal personality is characterized by a variety of traits, such as , un- usual perceptual experiences, and . Factor analytic studies have shown that these characteristics tend to cluster into at least two separate dimensions (positive and negative schizotypy). Schizotypy is associated with vulnerability to . However, it is also related to higher scores on measures of creativity and increased right-hemisphere brain activ- ity. In a series of recent studies investigating the behavioral, neuropsychological, and neuro- imaging correlates of positive and negative schizotypy, positive schizotypy was associated with better performance on measures of creativity, enhanced responsivity to threatening emo- tional stimuli, and more right-prefrontal cortical activity. These results support earlier psycho- logical studies suggesting that positive schizotypy is related to patterns of cognitive and emotional function (e.g., divergent thinking, heightened emotion) that are common to both creativity and psychopathology. Ó 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

q This research was supported by National Institute of Drug Abuse (R21 DA14111), National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH61358, T32 MH14257, T32 MH19554, F31 MH068123), Intercampus Research Initiative in Biotechnology of the University of Illinois, Carle Clinic, and the Beckman Institute. We thank Joseph Barkmeier, Tracey Wszalek, Holly Tracey, and Elizabeth Wenzel for their contributions to this project. * Corresponding author. Fax: 1-217-244-5876. E-mail address: [email protected] (W. Heller).

0092-6566/$ - see front matter Ó 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2003.09.014 J.E. Fisher et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 24–31 25

Creativity has been associated with mental illness for centuries. Biographical ac- counts of famous musicians, philosophers, scientists, writers, artists, and poets de- scribe psychotic episodes and suicides (Brod, 1997). In addition, psychological studies have shown that, on average, highly creative people have higher scores on measures of psychopathology than less creative people (for a review, see Andreasen, 1996). Relatives of schizophrenic or other psychotic patients have a higher incidence of creative achievement (for a review, see Brod, 1997). However, the relationship be- tween characteristics of ÔgeniusÕ and symptoms of ÔmadnessÕ is not straightforward. The majority of people who suffer from chronic mental illness experience a wide range of symptoms and deficits that interfere with the production of substantive work. Moreover, creativity is often defined as the ability to produce new ideas and adapt, improve, or create new functions for existing products (Kirton, 1989), skills that are impaired in most patients with schizophrenia. Indeed, the productive periods of most famous creative individuals occurred when their symptoms of men- tal illness were less severe (Brod, 1997). It therefore seems useful to examine whether creativity is associated with certain emotional, cognitive, or personality traits that may also predispose one to mental illness. Research focused on the traits that may be associated with both creativity and mental illness has identified schizotypal personality as a candidate for vulnerability to schizophrenia as well as a potential contributor to creativity. Much of this work emerges from the study of schizophrenia and its predispositions and hence is biased to represent the relevant personality traits as pathological. Schizotypal personality consists of traits that lead to a variety of cognitive, emo- tional, and social behaviors that are perceived by others to be unusual. For instance, individuals with schizotypal personality are often described as ‘‘odd’’ or ‘‘eccentric’’ and they may report an elevated rate of unusual perceptual experiences. Odd ideas or magical beliefs, inappropriate or constricted affect, and social and/or physical anhe- donia are also commonly observed in schizotypal individuals. Many of these charac- teristics are thought to be milder versions of symptoms observed in schizophrenia that could lead to significant impairment in emotional, social, and vocational func- tioning, even if they do not manifest as . Indeed, schizotypal personality disorder includes a constellation of characteristics whose link to schizophrenia has been theoretically and empirically supported (e.g., Chapman, Chapman, Kwapil, Eckblad, & Zinser, 1994; Fernandes & Miller, 1995). However, other researchers have argued that at least some schizotypal traits are continuously distributed throughout the population and may be associated with en- hanced adaptive functioning (McCreery & Claridge, 2002). In particular, unusual and magical beliefs may be linked to enhanced spirituality and creativity. For example, schizotypy scale items such as ‘‘It has seemed at times as if my body was melting into my surroundings’’ and ‘‘Sometimes I have had the feeling that a part of my body is larger than it usually is’’ are both common experiences of people who practice meditation (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Items such as ‘‘It is not possible to harm others merely by thinking bad thoughts about them’’ belie a growing belief among scientists, scholars, and physicians that thoughts can influence events and objects in the material world (e.g., Dossey, 1993). Jung wrote extensively about 26 J.E. Fisher et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 24–31 synchronicity, the idea that events co-occur in a meaningful way, a topic that has been taken up by more recent writers (Hopke, 1997). Individuals who believe in the concept of synchronicity might endorse items such as ‘‘I have felt that there were messages for me in the way things were arranged, like in a store window.’’ Studies have suggested that a spiritual propensity, as reflected in such beliefs, can provide an adaptive advantage to individuals when dealing with stress, such as change and loss (for a review, see Levin & Vanderpool, 1987; McFadden, 1999). Along these lines, high scorers on an Ôaberrant perceptions and beliefsÕ factor reported having out-of-body experiences but did not find them distressing. This group tended to have lower scores on anhedonia scales, suggesting more pleasurable perceptual experi- ences than the general population. Such unusual perceptual experiences were inter- preted as adaptive and consistent with out-of-body experiences leading to greater tolerance in instances of post-operative pain and reports of Ôreassuring apparitionsÕ serving to relieve people during times of stress (McCreery & Claridge, 2002). It can be noted that the importance of these empirical relationships does not depend on as- sumptions about the reality basis of such events or experiences. Unusual perceptions and magical thinking have also been associated with diver- gent thinking and creativity in students, professors, writers, and actors (e.g., Brod, 1997). Divergent thinking is considered to be an important component of the crea- tive process. Although it cannot be equated with creativity, it can predict creative po- tential (Runco, 1991). Schizotypy has also been associated with a greater number of unique responses on divergent thinking measures (Green & Williams, 1999). Other evidence illustrating ÔcreativeÕ traits in a nonclinical population comes from family histories. For instance, first-degree relatives and offspring of people with schizophre- nia tend to be more creative and enter more creative occupations than controls (e.g., Karlsson, 1984), implying that such traits may be genetically mediated. Schizotypy is thus a complex construct that seems to be related to maladaptive as well as adaptive functioning. Early on, schizotypy was often conceptualized via the positive/negative symptom dichotomy used in the schizophrenia literature. Positive traits are characteristics that the general population presumably does not experience (e.g., , , etc.), and negative traits (e.g., anhedonia, flat affect, etc.) are the absence of experiences that are normally present in the general popula- tion. More recent factor-analytic studies have yielded three to four factors to account for the dimensions of schizophrenia and schizotypy (Reynolds, Raine, Mellingenm, Venables, & Mednick, 2000). The three-factor models include dimensions related to positive schizotypy, disorganization, and negative schizotypy (e.g., Vollema & Hoijt- ink, 2000), whereas four-factor models include an additional asocial behavior/non- conformity dimension (e.g., Vollema & van den Bosch, 1995). Despite these refinements, the positive and negative dimensions continue to be fundamental to the construct. Dimensions of schizophrenia and schizotypy have also been consistently associ- ated with other personality traits. Schizophrenia has been associated with high pecu- liarity and and with low extraversion (Berenbaum & Fujita, 1994). Positive schizotypy has been associated with high neuroticism and openness to expe- rience and with low agreeableness, whereas negative schizotypy has been associated J.E. Fisher et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 24–31 27 with high neuroticism and with low extraversion, openness, and agreeableness (Ross, Lutz, & Bailley, 2002). The association of schizotypy with both adaptive and maladaptive functioning is clarified, to some degree, by examining how positive and negative schizotypy relate to cognitive and emotional variables. The majority of the literature suggests that as- pects of positive but not negative schizotypy are associated with creativity. For in- stance, in a series of studies enrolling 1108 college students, positive symptoms were positively correlated with creativity, whereas negative symptoms were nega- tively correlated (for a review, see Schuldberg, 2001). Given the links between schizotypy and creativity, it is not surprising that there has been interest in investigating the neural correlates of schizotypy, as a diagnostic category, in addition to its specific personality dimensions. Theoretical and empirical studies of creativity have consistently linked functions of the right hemisphere to greater creativity (for a review, see Heller, 1994). Neuropsychological studies indi- cate that schizotypal individuals display an asymmetry in hemispheric processing fa- voring the right hemisphere. For example, a relative deficit in verbal functions (Voglmaier et al., 2000) and a tendency to show right hemispatial inattention (Tay- lor, Zach, & Brugger, 2002) have been documented, suggesting either left-hemisphere dysfunction or a right-hemisphere bias in cognitive processing. In addition, people who had high scores on Magical Ideation (MI, Eckblad & Chapman, 1983) made more correct responses on a lexical decision task when words were presented to the right hemisphere (Leonhard & Brugger, 1998). As the right hemisphere is in- volved in holistic processing of information, this right-hemisphere bias may also ac- count for the tendency of schizotypes to make remote or ÔlooseÕ associations among concepts. For instance, high scorers on MI judged unrelated words to be more clo- sely related than lower MI subjects (Mohr, Graves, Gianotti, Pizzagalli, & Brugger, 2001). Belief in phenomena has also been associated with producing more original associations to unrelated stimuli (Gianotti, Mohr, Pizzagalli, Leh- mann, & Brugger, 2001). Linking uncommon ideas, which is facilitated by right- hemisphere activation, may be part of creative thinking, but in extreme cases it could foster delusions (Leonhard & Brugger, 1998). Along these lines, schizotypal subjects scored better on nonverbal creative tests than did controls (Poreh, Whitman, & Ross, 1994). Furthermore, within the schizotypal group, those with a left-ear (right-hemisphere) preference in a dichotic listening task had higher scores on both the schizotypy scale and on the verbal tests. In a series of studies, we investigated the behavioral, neuropsychological, and neu- roimaging correlates of positive and negative schizotypy. In the first study (Fisher, Heller, & Miller, in preparation), 20 participants with schizotypal personality (who scored at least 2 SD above the mean on either the Perceptual Aberration, (Chapman, Chapman, & Raulin, 1978) or the Magical Ideation scales or at least 1.5 SD above the mean on both scales) and 16 controls were administered a battery of questionnaires (including the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire, SPQ; Raine, 1991), neuropsychological measures (verbal and nonverbal information processing), and a semantic association task. Regression analyses across all 36 participants indi- cated that the combination of magical ideation, perceptual aberration, odd beliefs 28 J.E. Fisher et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 24–31

(SPQ), and unusual perceptions (SPQ) significantly predicted scores on figural flu- ency, a design fluency task sensitive to right-hemisphere functioning (Ruff, Light, & Evans, 1987). As fluency tests are firmly rooted in the neuropsychological tradi- tion and have been considered the best single indices of creativity (Dudek, 1993), these figural fluency data support the association of right-hemisphere functioning and creativity in positive schizotypy. The right hemisphere is also involved in emotional information processing (for a review, see Heller, 2004). An aspect of emotion processing that engages the right hemisphere involves monitoring for and responding to threatening information (for reviews, see Nitschke & Heller, 2002; Nitschke, Heller, & Miller, 2000). We em- ployed an emotional Stroop task that has been well validated by previous studies in our laboratory as a measure of emotional responsivity (e.g., Compton, Heller, Ba- nich, Palmieri, & Miller, 2000; Compton et al., 2003). The emotional Stroop task consists of blocks of positive, negative, and neutral words, and the participant is asked to respond as quickly as possible to the color of the word, ignoring its mean- ing. As this task requires the inhibition of task-irrelevant information (the emotional meaning of the word) to respond accurately to task-relevant information (the color of the word), prefrontal cortical functions of selective and the maintenance of contextual information are involved (for a review, see Banich, 2004). A behavioral study of 169 undergraduates investigated the relationship between dimensions of schizotypy, personality traits, emotional dispositions, and interference on the Emotional Stroop test (Mohanty et al., 2001). Positive but not negative schizotypy was associated with interference on the emotional Stroop task for nega- tive words. Importantly, this relationship was not mediated by anxiety or depression. These results suggest that positive schizotypy is associated with a heightened re- sponse to emotional information, particularly information that could be perceived as threatening. In a subsequent study, 16 individuals who had high scores on Perceptual Aberra- tion and/or Magical Ideation and 16 controls participated in a neuroimaging study (Mohanty et al., submitted). Consistent with findings from behavioral and neuropsy- chological studies, participants high on positive schizotypy showed greater activity in right prefrontal cortex and less activity in left prefrontal cortex than did controls (see Fig. 1). These neuropsychological, neuroimaging, and behavioral findings are consistent with studies indicating that aspects of schizotypal personality are associated with better performance on tasks related to divergent thinking and creativity. Across studies, higher scores in figural fluency and increased right prefrontal cortical activity for emotional compared to neutral words in the Stroop task were associated with po- sitive schizotypy, providing support for increased right-hemisphere activity in indi- viduals with characteristics of positive schizotypy. These findings are also consistent with a more intense emotional response to negative words in positive schizotypy. The fact that specific subscales of schizotypy rather than a global measure were as- sociated with right-hemisphere function provides support for a multidimensional per- spective regarding schizotypal personality. Without this perspective, characteristics of J.E. Fisher et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 24–31 29

Fig. 1. Compared to controls, individuals with schizotypal personality show increased right frontal brain activity for negative vs. neutral words. Highlighted voxels are those with z-scores > 2.3 (p <:01, two- tailed). schizotypy cannot be readily differentiated and understood. In addition, this ap- proach will lead to a better understanding of the multiple factors involved in the development of schizophrenia. These data and those examining the association with creativity suggest the possi- bility of a Ôhealthy schizotypyÕ (McCreery & Claridge, 2002), which may explain the persistence of genes related to schizophrenia in the population despite its many maladaptive aspects. The link between mental illness and creativity is likely due to common cognitive features involving the right hemisphere. To confirm this, it will be important in future research to examine the relationship between schizotypy, creativity, and right-hemisphere activity in the same experimental manipulations. This hypothesized process may be adaptive in some circumstances but may become problematic at extreme levels when other cognitive, social, emotional, or environ- mental resources are diminished. As such adaptive and maladaptive characteristics are investigated and better understood, the construct of schizotypy will be better defined, leading to improved methods of diagnosis and treatment, as well as a greater appreciation for what lies behind creative inspiration.

References

Andreasen, N. C. (1996). Creativity and mental illness: A conceptual and historical overview. In J. J. Schildkraut & A. Otero (Eds.), Depression and the spiritual in modern art—Homage to Miro (pp. 2–14). New York: Wiley. Banich, M. T. (2004). Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. Berenbaum, H., & Fujita, F. (1994). Schizophrenia and personality: Exploring the boundaries and connections between vulnerability and outcome. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 148–158. 30 J.E. Fisher et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 24–31

Brod, J. H. (1997). Creativity and schizotypy. In G. S. Claridge (Ed.), Schizotypy: Implications for illness and health (pp. 276–298). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., & Raulin, M. L. (1978). Body-image aberration in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 399–407. Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., Kwapil, T. R., Eckblad, M., & Zinser, M. C. (1994). Putatively psychosis-prone subjects 10 years later. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 171–183. Compton, R. J., Heller, W., Banich, M. T., Palmieri, P. A., & Miller, G. A. (2000). Responding to threat: Effects of hemispheric asymmetry and interhemispheric division of input. Neuropsychology, 14, 254–264. Compton, R. J., Banich, M. T., Mohanty, A., Milham, M. P., Herrington, J., Miller, G. A., Scalf, P. E., Webb, A., & Heller, W. (2003). Paying attention to emotion: An fMRI investigation of cognitive and emotional Stroop tasks. Cognitive, Affective, Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 81–96. Dossey, L. (1993). Healing words: The power of prayer and the practice of medicine. San Francisco: Harper. Dudek, S. Z. (1993). Creativity and : An overinclusive model. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 190–192. Eckblad, M., & Chapman, L. J. (1983). Magical ideation as an indicator of schizotypy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 215–225. Fernandes, L. O. L., & Miller, G. A. (1995). Compromised performance and abnormal associated with the Wisconsin scales of psychosis proneness. In G. A. Miller (Ed.), The behavioral high- risk paradigm in psychopathology (pp. 47–87). New York: Springer. Fisher, J., Heller, W., & Miller, G. (in preparation). Neuropsychological profiles and semantic association task performance of schizotypal individuals. Gianotti, L. R. R., Mohr, C., Pizzagalli, D., Lehmann, D., & Brugger, P. (2001). Associative processing and paranormal belief. and Clinical Neurosciences, 55, 595–603. Green, M. J., & Williams, L. M. (1999). Schizotypy and creativity as effects of reduced cognitive inhibition. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 263–276. Heller, W. (1994). Cognitive and emotional organization of the brain: Influences on the creation and of art. In D. Zaidel (Ed.), Neuropsychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Heller, W. (2004). Emotion. In M. T. Banich (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology (second ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. Hopke, R. H. (1997). There are no accidents: Synchronicity and the stories of our lives. New York: Riverhead Books. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. The program of the stress reduction clinic at the University of Massachusetts Medical Center. New York: Delta. Karlsson, J. L. (1984). Creative intelligence in relatives of mental patients. Hereditas, 100, 83–86. Kirton, M. J. (Ed.). (1989). Adaptors and innovators: Styles of creativity and problem solving. New York: Routledge. Leonhard, D., & Brugger, P. (1998). Creative, paranormal, and delusional thought: A consequence of right hemisphere semantic activation? Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 11, 177–183. Levin, J. S., & Vanderpool, H. Y. (1987). Is frequent religious attendance really conducive to better health. Toward an epidemiology of religion. Social Science and Medicine, 24, 589–600. McCreery, C., & Claridge, G. (2002). Healthy schizotypy: The case of out-of-the-body experiences. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 141–154. McFadden, S. H. (1999). Religion, personality, and aging: A life span perspective. Journal of Personality, 67, 1081–1104. Mohanty, A., Herrington, J. D., Koven, N. S., Webb, A. G., Heller, W., Banich, M. & Miller, G. (submitted). Neural mechanisms of affective interference in schizotypy. Mohanty, A., Koven, N. S., Fisher, J. E., Herrington, J. H., Stewart, J. L., Heller, W. & Miller, G. A. (2001, November). Emotion regulation and positive and negative symptoms in psychosis proneness. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Research in Psychopathology, Madison, Wisconsin. Mohr, C., Graves, R. E., Gianotti, L. R. R., Pizzagalli, D., & Brugger, P. (2001). Loose but normal: A semantic association study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 475–483. J.E. Fisher et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 24–31 31

Nitschke, J. B., & Heller, W. (2002). The neuropsychology of anxiety disorders: Affect, , and neural circuitry. In H. DÕhaenen, J. A. den Boer, & P. Willner (Eds.), Textbook of biological psychiatry (pp. 975–988). Chichester, West Sussex: Wiley. Nitschke, J. B., Heller, W., & Miller, G. A. (2000). Anxiety, stress, and cortical brain function. In J. C. Borod (Ed.), The neuropsychology of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press. Poreh, A. M., Whitman, D. R., & Ross, T. P. (1994). Creative thinking abilities and hemispheric asymmetry in schizotypal college students. Current Psychology, 12, 344–352. Raine, A. (1991). The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17, 555–564. Reynolds, C. A., Raine, A., Mellingen, K., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2000). Three-factor model of schizotypal personality: Invariance across culture, gender, religious affiliation, family adversity, and psychopathology. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 603–618. Ross, S. R., Lutz, C. J., & Bailley, S. E. (2002). Positive and negative symptoms of schizotypy and the five- factor model: A domain and facet level analysis. Journal of Personality Assessment, 79, 53–72. Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., & Evans, R. W. (1987). The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: A normative study with adults. Developmental Neuropsychology, 3, 37–51. Runco, M. (1991). Divergent thinking. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company. Schuldberg, D. (2001). Six subclinical spectrum traits in normal creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 5–16. Taylor, K. I., Zach, P., & Brugger, P. (2002). Why is magical ideation related to leftward deviation on an implicit line bisection task? Cortex, 38, 247–252. Voglmaier, M. M., Seidman, L. J., Niznikiewicz, M. A., Dickey, C. C., Shenton, M. E., & McCarley, R. W. (2000). Verbal and nonverbal neuropsychological test performance in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 787–793. Vollema, M. G., & van den Bosch, R. J. (1995). The multidimensionality of schizotypy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 21, 19–31. Vollema, M. G., & Hoijtink, H. (2000). The multidimensionality of self-report schizotypy in a psychiatric population: An analysis using multidimensional Rasch models. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 565–575.

All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.