French Grammar

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

French Grammar FRENCH GRAMMAR. A FRENCH GRAMMAR, BASED ON BY HERMANN BREYMANN, Ph.D., Lecturer on French Language and Literature at the Owens College, Manchester. THIRD EDITION. MACMILLAN AND CO, 1880, [The Right of Translation and Reproduction is reserved.] 445 B75f 1880 LONDON : R. CLAY, SONS, AND TAYLOR, BREAD STREET HILL, E. C. TO PROFESSOR DIEZ, THE FOUNDER OF ROMANCE PHILOLOGY, THIS BOOK IS IN TESTIMONY OF THE AUTHOR'S SINCERE RESPECT AND ADMIRATION. PREFACE, WHOEVER ventures on offering a new French Grammar to a public already provided with a considerable number of "Aids," "Grammars," and "Manuals" for learning French, is doubtless under the obligation of stating the reasons why he has undertaken what may at first sight seem so superfluous a task. Before doing so, I may be allowed to premise a few general remarks on the object at which, in my judgment, all instruction ought to aim. Every kind of instruction must tend to bestow on the student—and here I will make use of terms easily intelligible by a consideration of their primary significance— i. " Formal Culture ; " i.e. it must awaken and in­ tensify his powers of reasoning ; it must tend to impart to his mind promptitude, precision, and clearness of judgment. 2. "Real Culture;" i.e. it must assure to him the acquisition of positive knowledge admitting of being used in practical life, whether this knowledge be of a general or special kind, as designed for a particular profession or calling. Any teaching which fails to form or educate the mind must remain barren ; for of infinitely higher importance than mere knowledge without the Dower of applying it vin PREFACE. is that power itself. A well-trained and clear-sighted intelligence is at all times capable not only of acquiring new stores of positive knowledge, but also of applying it easily and immediately to the various demands of life. On the other hand, he who has acquired positive know­ ledge only, without that critical faculty which combines with vigour of decision keenness of judgment, has gained the letter only, but not the spirit. Experience teaches that it is the man whose intellectual powers have been formed by theoretical instruction—who easily applies what he has laboriously learned. For to him and to no other are known those general laws which determine and cover every special case, whether foreseen or unforeseen. What, then, has been, up to the present time, the method generally followed in teaching French ? It has been, and still is, as far as I know, that system of in­ struction of which the Ollendorff Grammars are popularly regarded as the type. Now, it is quite true, as the dis­ tinguished Professor of Sanskrit in Yale College, Mr. William Whitney, remarks, that " such a system has its unquestionable advantages where learning to speak is the main object directly aimed at, and where the small- ness of the classes and the time spent with the instructor render it possible for the latter to give each pupil that amount of personal attention and drilling which is needed in order to make the system yield its best results." Yet, there are two serious objections to it. " Firstly, it is"— to quote Mr. Whitney once more—" for the most part impracticable in schools and colleges. Their circum­ stances and methods of instruction render translation and construction the means by which the most useful knowledge and the best discipline can be gained. To PREFACE. IX the very great majority of those who learn German"— and I must be allowed to add French—" ability to speak is an object inferior in importance to ability to under­ stand those languages accurately and readily: and the attainment of the former is properly to be made posterior to that of the latter. One who has mastered the prin­ ciples of grammar, and acquired by reading a fair vocabulary and a feeling for the light use of it, will learn to speak and write rapidly and well when circum­ stances require of him that ability." The second objection I have to urge against this system is, that it fails fully to satisfy the first of the objects at which every kind of instruction ought to aim ; it addresses itself rather to the memory of the learner than to his faculty of judgment ; it gives rule after rule, with as many exceptions for each, without making the least attempt to explain them or to give a reason for them. Yet, sharing the view of a recent writer,1 that the " study of modern languages must be placed on a higher and more scientific foundation7' than is at present the case, and that these languages must be taught " according to the more logical method2 which is applied to the teaching of the dead languages," I hold, firstly, that such a system is not the most prac­ ticable, especially for schools and colleges, and, secondly, that instruction in the French language ought to be at once historical and comparative, " The study of the history of the changes which have taken place in a language ought not to be stigmatized as unpractical erudition, for erudition is in this instance 1 See the Educational Review of French Language and Literature\ p. 21 2 The Annual Report of the Delegacy uncbr Statute : " De Examinatione qui non sunt de corpore Universitatis." See the Educational Review, p. 9. X PREFACE. the means and not the end ; " * it awakens, as has been said, and intensifies the powers of reasoning and the faculty of judgment. If we follow the historical and comparative method, most of the grammatical forms which used to be considered irregular appear in quite a different light; as apparent exceptions to the rule, they only serve to confirm it And;: in a book recently published by one who speaks with authority on these matters,2 I read : " There is no longer an excuse why, even in the most elementary lessons—nay, I should say why more particularly in these elementary lessons—the dark and dreary passages of Greek and Latin, of French and German grammar, should not be brightened up by the electric light of Comparative Philology." This method, far from making the work more difficult for the student, on the contrary, will render it easier and, let us hope, more interesting ; because, being based on historical foundations, it is more true than any other, and because it leads the student to see that the different forms of this language have obeyed a substantially uni­ form action of certain definite laws. If it be true, on the one hand, that * the better we understand a thing, the better we are able to learn and to retain it, and, on the other hand, that nothing renders both teaching and learning more cheerful than a clear insight into the living organism of a language, it is certainly not un­ reasonable to hope that these studies may become more and more domesticated among us. I am the more hopeful that, sooner or later, such a result will be brought about, because it is in accordance with the 1 Educational Review, Sec, p. 19. 2 Max Miiller, " Lectures on the Science of Religion," p 3. PREFACE. xi labours and wishes of men whom all teachers of French have to acknowledge as their masters, viz. Diez, Littré, Mâtzner, Paris, and Brachet. Whatever there may be good and useful in the present book, it is to them that I owe it.T Yet, it will be admitted, I hope, that I have not carried the "jurare in verba magistriv too far. Refusing to look upon modern languages from a merely practical point of view, or to consider them only as so-called social accomplishments which, in many cir­ cumstances of life, may be very useful and agreeable, but which are incompetent as means of education, I firmly believe that, taught in the right way, they are entitled to share with the ancient languages the task and mission of promoting a true and sound studlum humanitatis. As it seems to be a quality inherent in man to oppose at first whatever is new to him—be it in the world of ideas or in that of facts—I am prepared to meet with strong opposition from many quarters. , Yet, as I shall never be brought to believe that the training of the mind to think is valueless, I am not only deter­ mined to learn myself more and more the right way of teaching in the spirit of the scholars mentioned above, but I have also set before myself, as the object of my life, to try whether or not young minds will derive from this new system the benefit I confidently expect from it. It will be seen that the present Grammar is intended to occupy an intermediate position between the elaborate works ofMatzner, Stadler, the Grammaire des Grammaires, &c, and the more or less elementary treatises which still 1 It seems almost unnecessary to say that, besides having studied the works of these scholars, I have, to a certain extent, used other valuable grammars, as those of Boniface, Meissner, Schmitz, Noël et Chapsal, Poitevin, Ploetz, Kôrting, Eugène, Havet, Tarver, Delille, &c. I am happy to say that, as to matters of detail, I have learnt at least something from each of them. XII PREFACE. continue to be used even in the higher forms of schools, or in colleges. It is not meant for beginners,1 unless they are of a certain age—say sixteen or seventeen—but for those who, having mastered the very elements of the language, wish for a more systematic and scientific study.
Recommended publications
  • Copyright © 2014 Richard Charles Mcdonald All Rights Reserved. The
    Copyright © 2014 Richard Charles McDonald All rights reserved. The Southern Baptist Theological Seminary has permission to reproduce and disseminate this document in any form by any means for purposes chosen by the Seminary, including, without, limitation, preservation or instruction. GRAMMATICAL ANALYSIS OF VARIOUS BIBLICAL HEBREW TEXTS ACCORDING TO A TRADITIONAL SEMITIC GRAMMAR __________________ A Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of The Southern Baptist Theological Seminary __________________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy __________________ by Richard Charles McDonald December 2014 APPROVAL SHEET GRAMMATICAL ANALYSIS OF VARIOUS BIBLICAL HEBREW TEXTS ACCORDING TO A TRADITIONAL SEMITIC GRAMMAR Richard Charles McDonald Read and Approved by: __________________________________________ Russell T. Fuller (Chair) __________________________________________ Terry J. Betts __________________________________________ John B. Polhill Date______________________________ I dedicate this dissertation to my wife, Nancy. Without her support, encouragement, and love I could not have completed this arduous task. I also dedicate this dissertation to my parents, Charles and Shelly McDonald, who instilled in me the love of the Lord and the love of His Word. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS.............................................................................................vi LIST OF TABLES.............................................................................................................vii
    [Show full text]
  • French Grammar Basics and Beyond
    French Grammar Basics and Beyond Easy explanations in English of French Grammar with more than 200 exercises, and solutions! This grammar book is for students of the A1 levels (total beginners) to B1 level (intermediate level). Click here to order this e-book at Special Price Only $24.50/ 21.90 € / £18.99! Paperback and Kindle version also available – Details here French Grammar Basics and Beyond ȱ ȱ ȱ ȱ ȱ ȱ ©ȱ2020ȱbyȱLearn French at Home.ȱAllȱrightsȱreserved.ȱ NoȱpartȱofȱthisȱeȬbookȱmayȱbeȱreproducedȱinȱanyȱwriĴen,ȱelectronic,ȱrecording,ȱorȱphotocopyingȱwithȬ outȱwriĴenȱpermissionȱofȱtheȱpublisherȱorȱauthors.ȱȱ ȱ Publishedȱby:ȱLearn French at Home www.learnfrenchathome.comȱ Authors:ȱCélineȱVanȱLoanȱandȱAnnickȱStevensonȱ 3rdȱedition.ȱ1stȱeditionȱpublishedȱinȱ2011.ȱ ȱ DateȱofȱPublication:ȱ2020.ȱ ISBN:ȱ9798664830743ȱȱ ȱ ȱ ȱ ȱ ȱ ȱ ȱ Coverȱphoto:ȱȱ Paris,ȱtheȱRodinȱMuseumȱ©ȱCélineȱVanȱLoanȱ Backȱcoverȱphoto:ȱ CélineȱVanȱLoanȱ©ȱMartyȱVanȱLoanȱ 2 French Grammar Basics and Beyond AboutLearn French at Home Learn French at Home,ȱ createdȱ inȱ 2004ȱ byȱ Célineȱ andȱ Vincentȱ Anthonioz, hasȱ helpedȱ thousandsȱ ofȱ Frenchȱlearners,ȱeachȱwithȱveryȱdiěerentȱlearningȱgoals.ȱTheȱmainȱingredientȱofȱourȱsuccessȱliesȱinȱourȱ teamȱofȱprofessionalȱandȱfriendlyȱnativeȱFrenchȱteachersȱwhoȱtakeȱtheȱtimeȱtoȱpersonalizeȱeveryȱsingle lessonȱaccordingȱtoȱtheȱstudentȇsȱpersonalȱandȱprofessionalȱgoal.ȱOurȱmainȱpurposeȱisȱtoȱdeliverȱtrueȱ qualityȱserviceȱtoȱeachȱstudent.ȱ ȱ Sinceȱtheȱlessonsȱtakeȱplaceȱinȱtheȱstudentȇsȱhomeȱorȱworkplace,ȱitȱdoesnȇtȱmaĴerȱwhereȱyouȱlive.ȱ
    [Show full text]
  • French Present to Passe Compose Converter
    French Present To Passe Compose Converter Dominated Ingemar always print his mid-Victorian if Bancroft is easeful or implant unswervingly. Lascivious Vinod pre-empts or hunkers some caffeine voicelessly, however Presbyterian Thebault roll-ons overpoweringly or reests. Ambrosi chronicles dully. No products in ongoing cart. Learn about French negation with Lingolia, then practise in human free exercises. Je vois tout le monde. The nice part free to heel the stem. The second discount is placed directly after the auxiliary of human challenge event or inspect, there. When slave start learning this tense and silk make sentences with it, stop often suffer the auxiliary. Google Translator is focusing on the way to began the language. How thin I translate that using the passe simple. You original set up consent preferences and given how much want your data yet be used based on the purposes below. Language is dynamic and most dismiss the verse not static. Are soon going to retaliate a lot? Le cadre réponse based on anything at what verb avoir, then would knowledge! Convertir in context, with a shelter on sentence structure and tenses did shine its. Reading audience will also trim any bookmarked pages associated with a title. Simultaneously negate nouns then use si instead of oui sentences of tube type are changed to type! This survey an online conjugator for Persian verbs. Join our popular discussion forum. In blow, the French regularly make mistakes when they amplify the passé composé. Learn table to make simple sentence negative coffee black an Assertive or declarative sentence construction a negative question makes.
    [Show full text]
  • Annotating Tense, Mood and Voice for English, French and German
    Annotating tense, mood and voice for English, French and German Anita Ramm1;4 Sharid Loaiciga´ 2;3 Annemarie Friedrich4 Alexander Fraser4 1Institut fur¨ Maschinelle Sprachverarbeitung, Universitat¨ Stuttgart 2Departement´ de Linguistique, Universite´ de Geneve` 3Department of Linguistics and Philology, Uppsala University 4Centrum fur¨ Informations- und Sprachverarbeitung, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitat¨ Munchen¨ [email protected] [email protected] fanne,[email protected] Abstract features. They may, for instance, be used to clas- sify texts with respect to the epoch or region in We present the first open-source tool for which they have been produced, or for assigning annotating morphosyntactic tense, mood texts to a specific author. Moreover, in cross- and voice for English, French and Ger- lingual research, tense, mood, and voice have been man verbal complexes. The annotation is used to model the translation of tense between based on a set of language-specific rules, different language pairs (Santos, 2004; Loaiciga´ which are applied on dependency trees et al., 2014; Ramm and Fraser, 2016)). Identi- and leverage information about lemmas, fying the morphosyntactic tense is also a neces- morphological properties and POS-tags of sary prerequisite for identifying the semantic tense the verbs. Our tool has an average accu- in synthetic languages such as English, French racy of about 76%. The tense, mood and or German (Reichart and Rappoport, 2010). The voice features are useful both as features extracted tense-mood-voice (TMV) features may in computational modeling and for corpus- also be useful for training models in computational linguistic research. linguistics, e.g., for modeling of temporal relations (Costa and Branco, 2012; UzZaman et al., 2013).
    [Show full text]
  • Formal Grammar, Usage Probabilities, and English Tensed Auxiliary Contraction (Revised June 15, 2020)
    Formal grammar, usage probabilities, and English tensed auxiliary contraction (revised June 15, 2020) Joan Bresnan 1 At first sight, formal theories of grammar and usage-based linguistics ap- pear completely opposed in their fundamental assumptions. As Diessel (2007) describes it, formal theories adhere to a rigid division between grammar and language use: grammatical structures are independent of their use, grammar is a closed and stable system, and is not affected by pragmatic and psycholinguis- tic principles involved in language use. Usage-based theories, in contrast, view grammatical structures as emerging from language use and constantly changing through psychological processing. Yet there are hybrid models of the mental lexicon that combine formal representational and usage-based features, thereby accounting for properties unexplained by either component of the model alone (e.g. Pierrehumbert 2001, 2002, 2006) at the level of word phonetics. Such hybrid models may associate a set of ‘labels’ (for example levels of representation from formal grammar) with memory traces of language use, providing detailed probability distributions learned from experience and constantly updated through life. The present study presents evidence for a hybrid model at the syntactic level for English tensed auxiliary contractions, using lfg with lexical sharing (Wescoat 2002, 2005) as the representational basis for the syntax and a dynamic exemplar model of the mental lexicon similar to the hybrid model proposals at the phonetic word level. However, the aim of this study is not to present a formalization of a particular hybrid model or to argue for a specific formal grammar. The aim is to show the empirical and theoretical value of combining formal and usage-based data and methods into a shared framework—a theory of lexical syntax and a dynamic usage-based lexicon that includes multi-word sequences.
    [Show full text]
  • The Shared Lexicon of Baltic, Slavic and Germanic
    THE SHARED LEXICON OF BALTIC, SLAVIC AND GERMANIC VINCENT F. VAN DER HEIJDEN ******** Thesis for the Master Comparative Indo-European Linguistics under supervision of prof.dr. A.M. Lubotsky Universiteit Leiden, 2018 Table of contents 1. Introduction 2 2. Background topics 3 2.1. Non-lexical similarities between Baltic, Slavic and Germanic 3 2.2. The Prehistory of Balto-Slavic and Germanic 3 2.2.1. Northwestern Indo-European 3 2.2.2. The Origins of Baltic, Slavic and Germanic 4 2.3. Possible substrates in Balto-Slavic and Germanic 6 2.3.1. Hunter-gatherer languages 6 2.3.2. Neolithic languages 7 2.3.3. The Corded Ware culture 7 2.3.4. Temematic 7 2.3.5. Uralic 9 2.4. Recapitulation 9 3. The shared lexicon of Baltic, Slavic and Germanic 11 3.1. Forms that belong to the shared lexicon 11 3.1.1. Baltic-Slavic-Germanic forms 11 3.1.2. Baltic-Germanic forms 19 3.1.3. Slavic-Germanic forms 24 3.2. Forms that do not belong to the shared lexicon 27 3.2.1. Indo-European forms 27 3.2.2. Forms restricted to Europe 32 3.2.3. Possible Germanic borrowings into Baltic and Slavic 40 3.2.4. Uncertain forms and invalid comparisons 42 4. Analysis 48 4.1. Morphology of the forms 49 4.2. Semantics of the forms 49 4.2.1. Natural terms 49 4.2.2. Cultural terms 50 4.3. Origin of the forms 52 5. Conclusion 54 Abbreviations 56 Bibliography 57 1 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Core French Grammar
    Understanding Core French Grammar Andrew Betts Lancing College, England Vernon Series in Language and Linguistics Copyright © 2016 Vernon Press, an imprint of Vernon Art and Science Inc, on behalf of the author. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Vernon Art and Ascience Inc. www.vernonpress.com In the Americas: In the rest of the world: Vernon Press Vernon Press 1000 N West Street, C/Sancti Espiritu 17, Suite 1200, Wilmington, Malaga, 29006 Delaware 19801 Spain United States Vernon Series in Language and Linguistics Library of Congress Control Number: 2016947126 ISBN: 978-1-62273-068-1 Product and company names mentioned in this work are the trademarks of their respec- tive owners. While every care has been taken in preparing this work, neither the authors nor Vernon Art and Science Inc. may be held responsible for any loss or damage caused or alleged to be caused directly or indirectly by the information contained in it. Table of Contents Acknowledgements xi Introduction xiii Chapter 1 Tense Formation 15 1.0 Tenses – Summary 15 1.1 Simple (One-Word) Tenses: 15 1.2 Compound (Two-word) Tenses: 17 2.0 Present Tense 18 2.1 Regular Verbs 18 2.2 Irregular verbs 19 2.3 Difficulties with the Present Tense 19 3.0 Imperfect Tense 20 4.0 Future Tense and Conditional Tense 21 5.0 Perfect Tense 24 6.0 Compound Tense Past Participle Agreement 28 6.1
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: ORTHOGRAPHIC DEPTH and ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING 1
    Running head: ORTHOGRAPHIC DEPTH AND ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING 1 This is a post-peer-review, pre-copyedit version of an article accepted in "Reading and Writing". The final authenticated version will be available at link.springer.com. The Effect of Orthographic Depth on Letter String Processing: The Case of Visual Attention Span and Rapid Automatized Naming Alexia Antzaka (ORCID iD: 0000-0002-3975-1122) Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009, San Sebastián, Spain Departamento de Lengua Vasca y Comunicación, UPV/EHU, 48940, Leioa, Spain Clara Martin (ORCID iD: 0000-0003-2701-5045) Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009, San Sebastián, Spain Ikerbasque, Basque Foundation for Science, 48013, Bilbao, Spain Sendy Caffarra (ORCID iD: 0000-0003-3667-5061) Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009, San Sebastián, Spain Sophie Schlöffel Running head: ORTHOGRAPHIC DEPTH AND ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING 1 Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009, San Sebastián, Spain Departamento de Lengua Vasca y Comunicación, UPV/EHU, 48940, Leioa, Spain Manuel Carreiras (ORCID iD: 0000-0001-6726-7613) Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009, San Sebastián, Spain Departamento de Lengua Vasca y Comunicación, UPV/EHU, 48940, Leioa, Spain Ikerbasque, Basque Foundation for Science, 48013, Bilbao, Spain Marie Lallier (ORCID iD: 0000-0003-4340-1296) Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009, San Sebastián, Spain Author note The authors acknowledge financial support from the Basque Government (PRE_2015_2_0049 to A.A, PI_2015_1_25 to C.M, PRE_2015_2_0247 to S.S), the European Research Council (ERC-2011-ADG-295362 to M.C.), the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (PSI20153653383P to M.L., PSI20153673533R to M.
    [Show full text]
  • First Year Latin Texts and Methods in America
    PL •? * H * " * • > * life,-. ' " * 4? W>' V » * 111 A1&- ii^-iv # «T <k. - . **J) ' ^* I* 'Mfc #^ • 1 I G IS. «J* | *'• * • iir V .T*: ' :4^sS Is T.JNTV. <JV ILLINOIS LIBRARY * Jf ^ w ^ v'^K * * * it lK ^ UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS LIBRARY Class ' Book Volumt M 1 20M 4^ *4 ^ 4 is ^ t * -4- 4, 4* liiiiiiil FIRST YEAR LATIN TEXTS AND METHODS IN AMERICA. THEIR HISTORY AND STATUS BY FRANK WATERS THOMASi A. B. Indiana University, 1905 THESIS Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF ARTS IN LATIN IN THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS 1910 ^10 UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS THE GRADUATE SCHOOL HEREBY RECOMMEND THAT THE THESIS PREPARED UNDER MY SUPERVISION BY ENTITLED -FuxrtJr^ of BE ACCEPTED AS FULFILLING THIS PART OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF ^2Ji^^_JZx^-- qJLv~£c> n Charge of Major Work Head of Department Recommendation concurred in: Committee on Final Examination 168020 Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2013 http://archive.org/details/firstyearlatinteOOthom First Year Latin Texts and Methods in America: Their History and Status. The type of first year Latin book with which every high t school boy is familiar is a comparatively modem product. Men are yet living who learned their elementary Latin before any one had dared publish a beginner's book that dispensed with the use of a grammar in the first year. But in spite of this fact the number of such texts is legion. What educational problems have furnished the cause or at least the excuse for this multiplicity of texts, and what theories underlie the numerous attempts to solv<* these problems, I have tried in this investigation to determine.
    [Show full text]
  • INTERSECTING CONSTRAINT FAMILIES: an ARGUMENT for HARMONIC GRAMMAR KIE Zuraw BRUCE HAYES University of California, Los Angeles U
    INTERSECTING CONSTRAINT FAMILIES: AN ARGUMENT FOR HARMONIC GRAMMAR Kie Zuraw Bruce Hayes University of California, Los Angeles University of California, Los Angeles In the analysis of free variation in phonology, we often encounter the effects of intersecting constraint families: there are two independent families of constraints, each of which has a quantifiable effect on the outcome. A challenge for theories is to account for the patterns that emerge from such intersection. We address three cases: Tagalog nasal substitution, French liai- son/elision, and Hungarian vowel harmony, using corpus data. We characterize the patterns we find as across-the-board effects in both dimensions, restrained by floor and ceiling limits. We ana- lyze these patterns using several formal frameworks, and find that an accurate account is best based on harmonic grammar (in one of its two primary quantitative implementations). Our work also suggests that certain lexical distinctions treated as discrete by classical phonological theory (e.g. ‘h-aspiré’ vs. ordinary vowel-initial words of French) are in fact gradient and require quanti- tative treatment.* Keywords: harmonic grammar, noisy harmonic grammar, maximum entropy grammars, Tagalog, French, Hungarian, intersecting constraint families 1. Introduction. A key debate in constraint-based linguistic frameworks concerns ranking versus weighting. Optimality theory (OT; Prince & Smolensky 2004 [1993]) uses strict ranking: candidate A is preferred to candidate B if the highest-ranked constraint that distinguishes between them prefers A. In harmonic grammar (Le- gendre et al. 1990, Legendre et al. 2006, Potts et al. 2010, and others), the constraints bear numeric weights, and the winner is the candidate with the best harmony value (a weighted sum of constraint violations).
    [Show full text]
  • Academic Plan - Flamingo Spanish School 4 Weeks Program A1 to B2
    Academic Plan - Flamingo Spanish School 4 weeks program A1 to B2 Basic Spanish Level A1: Week 1 Objective Grammar Vocabulary Orthography Culture Introduce yourself ● ● Greetings Greetings ● ● Subjects Pronouns ● Farewells ● The alphabet Unit 1 Spelling names ● Countries in South America ● ● Verb “llamarse” ● Useful expressions ● The vowels The Class Ask and give personal ● Numbers information ● ● Subject Pronouns ● Introduce someone ● Verb “ser” simple present Unit 2 (identification, origen and ● Countries ● Ask and say the nationality nationality) Identify people Demonyms The accent Cognates ● Decir que idioma(s) habla ● Demonyms ● ● ● Languages ● Say what language you ● Verb “estar” (Place) ● speak ● Verb “hablar” ● Verb “ser” (occupation or profession) ● Ask and say the occupation or profession ● The questions ● Places where people work ● Ask and answer where a ● Indefinite article Unit 3 Parts of the day Intonation in questions The occupations better person work Number and gender of the ● ● ● ● paid in Latinoamerica Occupations ● Ask and answer where a noun ● Numbers person lives ● Regular verbs * occupation or profession ● Ask and answer the time ● Present of indicative ● Interrogative pronouns ● Identify kinship ● Article determined: gender Unit 4 relationships and number Identify the gender of Verb ser. Present of ● ● Relationships Sound /r/ Do not be confused The family objects indicative (possession, ● ● ● destination, purpose) ● Express destiny, purpose and possession ● Prepositions (de, para) Week 1 Objective Grammar Vocabulary Orthography Culture ● Adjectives: gender and number Unit 5 ● Make physical and ● Adverbs of quantity (muy, personality descriptions bastante, un poco, nothing) Description of ● Adjectives ● The letter “hache” ● The Latin American family ● Express possession ● Verb “tener”. Present of people indicative ● Possessive adjectives ● Determined article ● The impersonal form “hay” ● Hay / está ● Express existence ● Interrogative pronouns (cuánto / s, cuánta / s) ● Foods Unit 6 Interact in a restaurant ● Intonation ● ● Verb “estar”.
    [Show full text]
  • Decomposability and Mental Representation of French Verbs Gustavo Estivalet, Fanny Meunier
    Decomposability and mental representation of French verbs Gustavo Estivalet, Fanny Meunier To cite this version: Gustavo Estivalet, Fanny Meunier. Decomposability and mental representation of French verbs. Fron- tiers in Human Neuroscience, Frontiers, 2015, 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00004. hal-01247734 HAL Id: hal-01247734 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01247734 Submitted on 22 Dec 2015 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 20 January 2015 HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00004 Decomposability and mental representation of French verbs Gustavo L. Estivalet 1,2* and Fanny E. Meunier 1,2 1 Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique UMR5304, Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition, Lyon, France 2 Université de Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Lyon, France Edited by: In French, regardless of stem regularity, inflectional verbal suffixes are extremely regular Mirjana Bozic, University of and paradigmatic. Considering the complexity of the French verbal system, we argue that Cambridge, UK all French verbs are polymorphemic forms that are decomposed during visual recognition Reviewed by: independently of their stem regularity. We conducted a behavioral experiment in which we Marcus Taft, University of New South Wales, Australia manipulated the surface and cumulative frequencies of verbal inflected forms and asked João Veríssimo, University of participants to perform a visual lexical decision task.
    [Show full text]