Redalyc.AN ACCOUNT on the HISTORY of ETHOLOGY

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Redalyc.AN ACCOUNT on the HISTORY of ETHOLOGY Suma Psicológica ISSN: 0121-4381 [email protected] Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz Colombia Moreno, Carlos B.; Muñoz-Delgado, Jairo AN ACCOUNT ON THE HISTORY OF ETHOLOGY Suma Psicológica, vol. 14, núm. 2, septiembre, 2007, pp. 213-224 Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz Bogotá, Colombia Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=134216871001 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Anccount on the history of ethology 213 Suma Psicológica, Vol. 14 N° 2 Septiembre de 2007, 213-224, ISSN 0121-4381 AN ACCOUNT ON THE HISTORY OF ETHOLOGY Carlos B. Moreno* Universidad del Rosario, Colombia Jairo Muñoz-Delgado** UNAM, México ABSTRACT Ethology has been traditionally defined as the biological study of behavior. Although its initial studies go back to the second half of the XIX century, its acknowledgement as a scientific discipline dates only from 1973, when the Nobel Prize for Medicine and Physiology is awarded to three of its most outstanding representatives: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen and Karl von Frisch. In this text a journey that includes the main historical landmarks of ethology will be undertaken, making special emphasis on conceptual and theoretical aspects. A revision of the background that allows the establishment of ethology as a discipline will be made, as well as a view of its development * Professor of Neuroscience, Neuroscience Group (NEUROS), Faculty of Medicine, Universidad del Rosario, Bogotá, Colombia. ISHE (International Society for Human Ethology) member. Correspondence: Carlos B. Moreno and/or Address: Grupo de Neurociencias, Instituto de Ciencias Básicas, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad del Rosario, Carrera 24, calle 63D, Quinta de Mutis, Bogotá, DC. Colombia. E-mail: [email protected] ** Chronoecology and Human Ethology, Departament of Ethology, Division of Neurosciences, Instituto Nacional de Psiquiatría Ramón de la Fuente Muñíz, México, D.F. Psychology Faculty, UNAM.Correspondence:Jairo Muñoz-Delgado Address: Instituto Nacional de Psiquiatría Ramón de la Fuente. Dirección de Neurociencias. Avenida México-Xochimilco 101, San Lorenzo-Huipulco, CP. 14370, delegación Tlalpan, México, D.F. e-mail: [email protected] Suma Psicológica, Vol. 14 N° 2: 213-224, septiembre 2007, Bogotá (Col.) 214 Carlos B. Moreno, Jairo Muñoz-Delgado and maturity. Finally, the birth of the subdivision known as Human Ethology together with its contemporary tendencies will be addressed. Key words: Classic ethology, human ethology, urban ethology, animal behaviour RESUMEN La Etología se ha definido tradicionalmente como «el estudio biológi- co del comportamiento». Aunque sus estudios iniciales surgen des- de la segunda mitad del siglo XIX, su reconocimiento como disciplina científica se fecha solamente a partir de 1973, cuando el premio Nobel para la Medicina y la Fisiología se concede a tres de sus representantes más excepcionales: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen y Karl von Frisch. En este texto, se emprende un viaje que incluye las señales históricas principales de la Etología, con un énfasis especial en los aspectos conceptuales y teóricos. Esta revisión ilus- tra el establecimiento de la Etología como disciplina, así como una visión de su desarrollo y madurez. Finalmente, se analiza el naci- miento de la subdivisión conocida como etología humana junto con sus tendencias contemporáneas. Palabras Clave: Etología clásica, Etología humana, Etología urba- na, Comportamiento animal he term ethology takes various science. The English philosopher and Tacceptations before it is economist John Stuart Mill, in the employed in its current form. second volume of his work Logic in The word originates from the Greek 1893, considers ethology to be the word ethos having various meanings, science corresponding to the act of but it can be adequately translated into educating, in its wider sense, including character, indicating some distinction the formation of national and or peculiarity of a person or animal or, collective characters, as well as the in the sense of moral character, as a individual one. result of a habit (Jaynes, 1969). While this is taking place in In the XVII century an actor or England, Isidore Geoffroy-Saint-Hilaire, mimic is known as an ethologist, since in France in 1859, uses the word he portrays human characters in the ethology in quite an informal manner theater. But in the XVIII century, to refer to an extensive classification ethology is the science of ethics and of biology associated with the study of ethologist the person studying this animals not as inert beings kept in Suma Psicológica, Vol. 14 N° 2: 213-224, septiembre 2007, Bogotá (Col.) Anccount on the history of ethology 215 formaldehyde, but as living organisms animals, The walking of animals and On in their natural environment. Around the generation of animals. this time, in 1857, Ernst Haeckel intro- The first document is an duces the term ecology and defines it as enumeration of observations on the relation of a living organism with comparative anatomy, reproductive its environment (Haeckel, 1898). This functions, locomotion and sensory term gains wide acceptance toward the aspects of several animal species; all the texts contain descriptions and end of the XIX century, whereas that classification elements. Aristotle of ethology comes to the brink of utilizes the comparative approach, disappearance. Alfred Giard, an provides reasoning by analogy, verifies important French biologist teaching at conclusions and extends his studies to the Sorbonne, starts using with all the circumstances of animal life: increasing frequency the word ethology in the 1870s in reference to ways of living, habits, diseases, those studies relating a living influence of climate, etc. In his work organism to its immediate habitat. On the generation of animals, he gives During the first years of the XX an account of the genders, copulation, century, the use of the term is restricted fecundation, embryology, birth, to the study of animal behavior in its heredity, and care given to young natural environment, thus marking a individuals. Aristotle must be admired difference with comparative psychology for the accurate description of and especially with classic behavioral observed facts, the vastness of his studies emphasizing learning under knowledge and his theoretical controlled laboratory conditions. constructions. For instance, in his treatises he mentions 495 different FORERUNNERS species that he observed himself; he always avoided the reference of A rapid retrospective look into the history animals he did not know directly. of biology allows us to identify various During the Hellenic and Roman scholars who render descriptions of periods, the study of animals suffers various aspects of animal behavior, in a real decadence. The interest of the what it may be considered a pre-history majority of studies is centered in man, of ethology. astronomy, mathematics, and Possibly, the first one is Aristotle physics; an exception is the work of (384-322/21 B.C). This natural Claudius Eliano (circa 170-249 A.D.): philosopher, who bequeaths biology History of Animals. In this treatise, with several fundamental concepts, the author describes the behavior of writes various treatises dealing with animals in the three elements (water, natural history during his last 12 earth and air), with the intention of years of existence; we can mention showing that animals fulfill among them: History of animals, On the completely their own aims, unlike parts of the animals, The movement of Suma Psicológica, Vol. 14 N° 2: 213-224, septiembre 2007, Bogotá (Col.) 216 Carlos B. Moreno, Jairo Muñoz-Delgado what occurs very often with humans. when compared to the ideas postulated On the other hand, for the Arabs, na- by current comparative psychologists. tural sciences, in the proper sense of In this manner, for example, he writes the word, are only auxiliaries to in relation to instinctive behavior: agriculture and medicine, that is, they birds of the same class make their are considered practical arts. This is the nests of the same material, build them reason of the lack of documented in the same order and give them the evidence on observations of animal same shape, for on seeing the nest behavior for several centuries. one can know with certainty to which In the XIII century, possibly type of bird it belongs; birds are able to coinciding with the teachings of Saint do it even though they have never Francis of Assisi (1181-1226), a seen a nest before or made one, that significant change in attitude toward is, even though they had been nature takes place, particularly in snatched from it and brought up reference to animals. Albertus outside of it (Thorpe, 1982). Magnus (1200?-1280), known as the In the XVIII, the seedling of ethology Universal Doctor, writes an extensive starts germinating in France. Georges work called De animalibus, which Leroy (1723-1789) publishes, in 1764, consists of 26 books gathering origi- The intelligence and the affectability of nal observations on, for example, the animals from a philosophical point of description on how a spider weaves its view, with a few words on Man. The book net, or how an ant, having one of its is written as epistles for her friend antennae severed, is incapable of Madame dAngiviller; here he states returning to its nest without the help that in the study of animals isolated of its companions (Taton, 1957). facts must be excluded. What In the XVI century, naturalists, constitutes the true field of observation such as Aldrovani, Gesner and lies in their daily behavior, the set of Turner, make important descriptions all their acts with underlying of various animal species; in their modifications in accordance with the work they add fantastic details as well circumstances, all collaborating toward as the description of some beasts the objectives that must necessarily entirely imaginary. keep present, each according to its own In the XVII century, the English nature.
Recommended publications
  • Animal Welfare and the Paradox of Animal Consciousness
    ARTICLE IN PRESS Animal Welfare and the Paradox of Animal Consciousness Marian Dawkins1 Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK 1Corresponding author: e-mail address: [email protected] Contents 1. Introduction 1 2. Animal Consciousness: The Heart of the Paradox 2 2.1 Behaviorism Applies to Other People Too 5 3. Human Emotions and Animals Emotions 7 3.1 Physiological Indicators of Emotion 7 3.2 Behavioral Components of Emotion 8 3.2.1 Vacuum Behavior 10 3.2.2 Rebound 10 3.2.3 “Abnormal” Behavior 10 3.2.4 The Animal’s Point of View 11 3.2.5 Cognitive Bias 15 3.2.6 Expressions of the Emotions 15 3.3 The Third Component of Emotion: Consciousness 16 4. Definitions of Animal Welfare 24 5. Conclusions 26 References 27 1. INTRODUCTION Consciousness has always been both central to and a stumbling block for animal welfare. On the one hand, the belief that nonhuman animals suffer and feel pain is what draws many people to want to study animal welfare in the first place. Animal welfare is seen as fundamentally different from plant “welfare” or the welfare of works of art precisely because of the widely held belief that animals have feelings and experience emotions in ways that plants or inanimate objectsdhowever valuableddo not (Midgley, 1983; Regan, 1984; Rollin, 1989; Singer, 1975). On the other hand, consciousness is also the most elusive and difficult to study of any biological phenomenon (Blackmore, 2012; Koch, 2004). Even with our own human consciousness, we are still baffled as to how Advances in the Study of Behavior, Volume 47 ISSN 0065-3454 © 2014 Elsevier Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Natural History Matters
    Why Practice Natural History? Why Natural History Matters Thomas L. Fleischner Thomas L. Fleischner ([email protected]) is a professor in the Environmental Studies Program at Prescott College, 220 Grove Avenue, Prescott, Arizona 86301 U.S.A., and founding President of the Natural History Network. The world needs natural history now more children: we turn over stones, we crouch to than ever. Because natural history – which look at insects crawling past, we turn our I have defined as “a practice of intentional heads to listen to new sounds. Indeed, as focused attentiveness and receptivity to the we grow older we have to learn to not pay more-than-human world, guided by attention to our world. The advertising honesty and accuracy” (Fleischner 2001, industry and mass consumer culture 2005) – makes us better, more complete collude to encourage this shrinking of the human beings. This process of “careful, scope of our attention. But natural history patient … sympathetic observation” attentiveness is inherent in us, and it can be (Norment 2008) – paying attention to the reawakened readily. larger than human world – allows us to build better human societies, ones that are It is easy to forget what an anomalous time less destructive and dysfunctional. Natural we now live in. Natural history is the history helps us see the world, and thus oldest continuous human tradition. ourselves, more accurately. Moreover, it Throughout human history and encourages and inspires better stewardship “prehistory,” attentiveness to nature was so of the Earth. completely entwined with daily life and survival that it was never considered as a Natural history encourages our conscious, practice separate from life itself.
    [Show full text]
  • The Voc and Swedish Natural History. the Transmission of Scientific Knowledge in the Eighteenth Century
    THE VOC AND SWEDISH NATURAL HISTORY. THE TRANSMISSION OF SCIENTIFIC KNOWLEDGE IN THE EIGHTEENTH CENTURY Christina Skott In the later part of the eighteenth century Sweden held a place as one of the foremost nations in the European world of science. This was mainly due to the fame of Carl Linnaeus (1707–78, in 1762 enno- bled von Linné), whose ground breaking new system for classifying the natural world created a uniform system of scientific nomenclature that would be adopted by scientists all over Europe by the end of the century. Linnaeus had first proposed his new method of classifying plants in the slim volume Systema Naturae, published in 1735, while he was working and studying in Holland. There, he could for the first time himself examine the flora of the Indies: living plants brought in and cultivated in Dutch gardens and greenhouses as well as exotic her- baria collected by employees of the VOC. After returning to his native Sweden in 1737 Linnaeus would not leave his native country again. But, throughout his lifetime, Systema Naturae would appear in numerous augmented editions, each one describing new East Indian plants and animals. The Linnean project of mapping the natural world was driven by a strong patriotic ethos, and Linneaus would rely heavily on Swed- ish scientists and amateur collectors employed by the Swedish East India Company; but the links to the Dutch were never severed, and he maintained extensive contacts with leading Dutch scientists through- out his life. Linnaeus’ Dutch connections meant that his own students would become associated with the VOC.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 SUSAN TUFTS FISKE November 2017
    SUSAN TUFTS FISKE November 2017 Eugene Higgins Professor phone: 609-258-0655 Psychology and Public Affairs fax: 609-258-1113 Department of Psychology and e-mail: [email protected] Woodrow Wilson School of web: http://www.fiskelab.org Public and International Affairs, office: Peretsman-Scully Hall 331 Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Degrees Docteur Honoris Causa: 2017, Universidad de Granada, Spain; 2013, Universität Basel, Switzerland; 2009, Universiteit Leiden, Netherlands; 1995, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium Ph.D. 1978, Social Psychology, Department of Psychology and Social Relations, Harvard University A.B. 1973, magna cum laude, Social Relations, Radcliffe College, Harvard University Academic Honors 2017 Wundt-James Award, European Federation of Psychologists’ Associations 2017 Association for Psychological Science James McKeen Cattell Award 2016 Fellow, Society of Experimental Psychology 2016 Association for Psychological Science Mentor Award for Lifetime Achievement 2016 American Psychological Association Award for Distinguished Service to Psychological Science 2016 Honoree, Federation of Associations in Behavior and Brain Sciences 2016 Teachers’ College, Columbia University, Medal for Distinguished Service 2014 McGovern Award in the Behavioral Sciences, American Association for the Advancement of Science 2014 Distinguished Scientist Award, Society of Experimental Social Psychology 2014 Elected Member, American Philosophical Society 2014 Codol Award for the Advancement of Social Psychology in Europe, European Association of Social Psychology 2014 Kurt Lewin Award, Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues 2013 Elected Member, National Academy of Sciences 2012-2017 President-Elect, President, and Past-President, Federation of Associations in Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2012 Leadership in Diversity Science Award, University of California at Los Angeles 2011 Corresponding Fellow, British Academy 2011 Gordon W.
    [Show full text]
  • How to Behave in a Beehive Eusociality, What Is It? the Colony
    1/6-2017 How to behave in a beehive Krisztina Csiki Popular scientific review of Independent Project in Biology 2017 Biology Education Centre, Uppsala University Did you know that the European honeybee has other interesting properties besides producing honey? It is a widely studied insect today, but the fascination goes back to the19th century and Sir Charles Darwin. So what is this fascination about? They have a strange genetic makeup which is very different from ours. How can a female be more related to her sister than to her offspring, and why are some individuals sterile? Moreover, some bees seem to work every waking hour while others spend their time doing practically nothing! Eusociality, what is it? Honeybees are social animals. However, their social system is organized in a different fashion than ours. Eusociality is in fact the most advanced social system known in the animal kingdom! What makes it special is that it is made up of large colonies where the tasks of guarding, nursing, foraging and reproducing are divided amongst its members. Individuals are usually born into performing particular tasks and are unable to change this. Moreover, at least some members in these systems are sterile, and others help the reproducers to nurse offspring. Many theories have tried to explaining this behaviour. They all hope to answer the question why sterile individuals have evolved. The most famous theory is called kin selection, which is a modification of Darwin’s theory. It suggests that the gene - rather than the individual itself - is the fundamental unit of selection. It is thus the gene that “wants” to survive.
    [Show full text]
  • The Strange Survival and Apparent Resurgence of Sociobiology
    This is a repository copy of The strange survival and apparent resurgence of sociobiology. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/118157/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Dennis, A. orcid.org/0000-0003-4625-1123 (2018) The strange survival and apparent resurgence of sociobiology. History of the Human Sciences, 31 (1). pp. 19-35. ISSN 0952- 6951 https://doi.org/10.1177/0952695117735966 Dennis A. The strange survival and apparent resurgence of sociobiology. History of the Human Sciences. 2018;31(1):19-35. Copyright © 2017 The Author(s). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0952695117735966. Article available under the terms of the CC- BY-NC-ND licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). Reuse This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs (CC BY-NC-ND) licence. This licence only allows you to download this work and share it with others as long as you credit the authors, but you can’t change the article in any way or use it commercially. More information and the full terms of the licence here: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/ Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ The strange survival and apparent resurgence of sociobiology Abstract A recent dispute between Richard Dawkins and Edward O. Wilson concerning fundamental concepts in sociobiology is examined.
    [Show full text]
  • Mental States in Animals: Cognitive Ethology Jacques Vauclair
    Mental states in animals: cognitive ethology Jacques Vauclair This artHe addresses the quegtion of mentaJ states in animak as viewed in ‘cognitive ethology”. In effect, thk field of research aims at studying naturally occurring behaviours such as food caching, individual recognition, imitation, tool use and communication in wild animals, in order to seek for evidence of mental experiences, self-aw&&reness and intentional@. Cognitive ethologists use some philosophical cencepts (e.g., the ‘intentional stance’) to carry out their programme of the investigation of natural behaviours. A comparison between cognitive ethology and other approaches to the investigation of cognitive processes in animals (e.g., experimental animal psychology) helps to point out the strengths and weaknesses of cognitive ethology. Moreover, laboratory attempts to analyse experimentally Mentional behaviours such as deception, the relationship between seeing and knowing, as well as the ability of animals to monitor their own states of knowing, suggest that cognitive ethology could benefit significantly from the conceptual frameworks and methods of animal cognitive psychology. Both disciplines could, in fact, co&ribute to the understanding of which cognitive abilities are evolutionary adaptations. T he term ‘cognirive ethology’ (CE) was comed by that it advances a purposive or Intentional interpretation Griffin in The Question ofAnimd Au~aarmess’ and later de- for activities which are a mixture of some fixed genetically veloped in other publications’ ‘_ Although Griffin‘s IS’6 transmitred elements with more hexible behaviour?. book was first a strong (and certainly salutary) reacrion (Cognitive ethologists USC conceptual frameworks against the inhibitions imposed by strict behaviourism in provided by philosophers (such as rhe ‘intentional stance’)“.
    [Show full text]
  • 200 Central Park West - American Museum of Natural History, Borough of Manhattan
    June 15, 2021 Name of Landmark Building Type of Presentation Month xx, year Public Hearing The current proposal is: Preservation Department – Item 4, LPC-21-08864 200 Central Park West - American Museum of Natural History, Borough of Manhattan How to Testify Via Zoom: https://us02web.zoom.us/j/84946202332?pwd=MjdJUjY2a0d3TWEwUDVlVEorM2lCQT09 Note: If you want to testify on an item, join the Webinar ID: 849 4620 2332 Zoom webinar at the agenda’s “Be Here by” Passcode: 951064 time (about an hour in advance). When the By Phone: Chair indicates it’s time to testify, “raise your 1 646-558-8656 US (New York) hand” via the Zoom app if you want to speak (*9 on the phone). Those who signed up in 877-853-5257 US (Toll free) advance will be called first. 888-475-4499 US (Toll free) Equestrian Statue of Theodore Roosevelt Proposed Relocation Theodore Roosevelt Park, MN NYC Parks American Museum of Natural History Site Location Manhattan American Museum of Natural History Frederick Douglass Circle Upper West Side 2 Site Location View of the American Museum of Natural History, 2020, NYC Parks 3 Site Location Equestrian Statue of Theodore Roosevelt, 2020, NYC Parks 4 Existing Site Views Equestrian Statue of Theodore Roosevelt looking northwest (left) and southwest (right) 2015, NYC Parks 5 History Roosevelt Memorial model, Date unknown, AMNH 6 History October 28, 1940, New York Times October 1940 view of the equestrian statue, from the south. Photo by Thane L. Bierwert, AMNH Research Library / Digital Special Collections 7 History Landmarks Preservation Commission designation photo of the Roosevelt Memorial, 1967, LPC 8 Composition and Context 9 Photo by Denis Finnin / AMNH Sustained Public Controversy Vandalism of Equestrian Statue of Vandalism of Equestrian Statue of Theodore Roosevelt, Theodore Roosevelt, 1971, New 2017, NYC Parks 10 York Times Sustained Public Controversy Addressing the Statue exhibition at AMNH, C.
    [Show full text]
  • Biography (Modified, After Festetics 1983)
    Konrad Lorenz’s Biography (modified, after Festetics 1983) 1903: Konrad Zacharias Lorenz (KL) was born in Altenberg /Austria on Nov. 7 as the last of three children of Emma Lorenz and Dr. Adolf Lorenz, professor for orthopedics at the Medical branch of the University of Vienna. In the same year the representative and spacious Altenberg family home was finished. 1907: KL starts keeping animals, such as spotted newts in aquaria, raises some ducklings and is not pleased by his first experiences with a dachshound. Niko Tinbergen, his lifelong colleague and friend, is born on April 15 in Den Haag, The Netherlands. 1909: KL enters elementary school and engages in systematic studies in crustaceans. 1910: Oskar Heinroth, biologist and founder of "Vergleichende Verhaltensforschung" (comparative ethology) from Berlin and fatherlike scientific mentor of the young KL publishes his classical paper on the ethology of ducks. 1915: KL enters highschool (Schottengymnasium Wien), keeps and breeds songbirds. 1918: Wallace Craig publishes the comparative ethology of Columbidae (pigeons), a classics of late US biologist Charles O. Whitman, who was like O. Heinroth, a founding father of comparative ethology. 1921: KL excels in his final exams. Together with friend Bernhard Hellmann, he observes and experiments with aggression in a cichlid (Herichthys cyanoguttatum). This was the base for KL's psychohydraulic model of motivation. 1922: Father Adolf sends KL to New York to take 2 semesters of medicine courses at the ColumbiaUniversity, but mainly to interrupt the relationship of KL with longterm girlfriend Gretl Gebhart, his later wife. This paternal attempt to influence the mate choice of KL failed.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Origins
    HUMAN ORIGINS Methodology and History in Anthropology Series Editors: David Parkin, Fellow of All Souls College, University of Oxford David Gellner, Fellow of All Souls College, University of Oxford Volume 1 Volume 17 Marcel Mauss: A Centenary Tribute Learning Religion: Anthropological Approaches Edited by Wendy James and N.J. Allen Edited by David Berliner and Ramon Sarró Volume 2 Volume 18 Franz Baerman Steiner: Selected Writings Ways of Knowing: New Approaches in the Anthropology of Volume I: Taboo, Truth and Religion. Knowledge and Learning Franz B. Steiner Edited by Mark Harris Edited by Jeremy Adler and Richard Fardon Volume 19 Volume 3 Difficult Folk? A Political History of Social Anthropology Franz Baerman Steiner. Selected Writings By David Mills Volume II: Orientpolitik, Value, and Civilisation. Volume 20 Franz B. Steiner Human Nature as Capacity: Transcending Discourse and Edited by Jeremy Adler and Richard Fardon Classification Volume 4 Edited by Nigel Rapport The Problem of Context Volume 21 Edited by Roy Dilley The Life of Property: House, Family and Inheritance in Volume 5 Béarn, South-West France Religion in English Everyday Life By Timothy Jenkins By Timothy Jenkins Volume 22 Volume 6 Out of the Study and Into the Field: Ethnographic Theory Hunting the Gatherers: Ethnographic Collectors, Agents and Practice in French Anthropology and Agency in Melanesia, 1870s–1930s Edited by Robert Parkin and Anna de Sales Edited by Michael O’Hanlon and Robert L. Welsh Volume 23 Volume 7 The Scope of Anthropology: Maurice Godelier’s Work in Anthropologists in a Wider World: Essays on Field Context Research Edited by Laurent Dousset and Serge Tcherkézoff Edited by Paul Dresch, Wendy James, and David Parkin Volume 24 Volume 8 Anyone: The Cosmopolitan Subject of Anthropology Categories and Classifications: Maussian Reflections on By Nigel Rapport the Social Volume 25 By N.J.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Social Behavioral Systems:Ethological
    Human Ethology Bulletin 29 (2014)1: 39-65 Teoretical Reviews HUMAN SOCIAL BEHAVIORL SYSTEMS: ETHOLOGICAL FRMEWORK FOR A UNIFIED THEORY Liane J. Leedom University of Bridgeport, Psychology Department, CT, US [email protected] ABSTRCT Drive theories of motivation proposed by Lorenz and Tinbergen did not survive experimental scrutiny; however these were replaced by the behavioral systems famework. Unfortunately, political forces within science including the rise of sociobiology and comparative psychology, caused neglect of this important famework. Tis review revives the concept of behavioral systems and demonstrates its utility in the development of a unifed theory of human social behavior and social bonding. Although the term “atachment” has been used to indicate social bonds which motivate afliation, four diferentiable social reward systems mediate social proximity and bond formation: the afliation (atachment), caregiving, dominance and sexual behavioral systems. Ethology is dedicated to integrating inborn capacities with experiential learning as well as the proximal and ultimate causes of behavior. Hence, the behavioral systems famework developed by ethologists nearly 50 years ago, enables discussion of a unifed theory of human social behavior. Key words: atachment, caregiving, dominance, sex, behavioral systems _________________________________________________________ INTRODUCTION Te last 50 years has seen the waning of ethology in the United States, accompanied by the growth of sociobiology, comparative psychology and evolutionary psychology (Barlow, 1989; Burkhardt, 2005; Greenberg, 2010). Sociobiologists, in particular, proclaimed the death of ethology and promised their nascent discipline and genetic mechanisms would provide an understanding of human social behavior (Barlow, 1989; E. O. Wilson, 2000). It has been 13 years since the sequencing of the human genome (Venter et al., 2001), sociobiology has fallen into “disarray” (D.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Ethology: Theory Or Poetry?
    Cognitive ethology: Theory or poetry? Jonathan Bennett from Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1983), pp. 356–358. Comments on Daniel Dennett, ‘Intentional Systems in Cognitive Ethology: the “Panglossian Paradigm” defended’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1983), pp. 343–345. Dennett is perhaps the most interesting, fertile, and chal- I take it as uncontroversial that the intentional stance— lenging philosopher of mind on the contemporary scene, and considered as a program for theorizing about behavior—must I count myself among his grateful admirers. But this present be centered on the idea that beliefs are functions from desires paper of his, enjoyable as it is to read, and acceptable as to behavior, and that desires are functions from beliefs to its conclusions are, is likely to do more harm than good. behavior. Down in the foundations, then, we need some Some will object that the intentional stance is a dead end; theory about what behavior must be like to be reasonably but I think, as Dennett does, that it is premature to turn interpreted as manifesting beliefs and desires; and these our backs on explanations of animal behavior in terms of concepts must presumably tail off somehow, being strongly desires and beliefs, and I am in favor of continuing with applicable to men and apes, less strongly to monkeys, and this endeavor; but only if it gets some structure, only if it so on down to animals that do not have beliefs and desires is guided by some firm underlying theory. That is what but can be described in terms of weaker analogues of those the ethologists might get from philosophy, but Dennett has notions.
    [Show full text]