Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: a 48-Nation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: a 48-Nation BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2005) 28,247-311 Printed in the United States of America Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: A 48-nation ' study of sex, culture, and strategies of human mating David P. Schmitt Depanment of Psychology, Bradley University. Peoria, IL 62625 dpsC?bradley.edu http://www.bradley.edu/academics/las/psy/schmitt.html http://schmitt.sociaIpsychology.org/index.htm Abstract: The Sociosexual Orientation Inventorv (SOI: Sim~son& Gan~estad1991) is a self-renort measure of individual differences , ' I 0 in human mating strateges. Low SO1 scores signify that a person is sociosexudly restricted, or follows a more monogarnous mating strat- egy. High SO1 scores indicate that an individual is unrestricted, or has a more promiscuous mating strategy. As part of the International Sexuality Description Project (ISDP),the SO1 was translated from English into 25 additional languages and administered to a total sam- ple of 14,059 people across 48 nations. Responses to the SO1 were used to address four main issues. First, the psychometric properties of the SO1 were examined in cross-cultural I~ers~ective. I The SO1 ~ossessedadeauate reliabilitv and validitv both within and across a di- verse range of modem cultures. Second, theories concerning the systematic distribution of sociosexuality across cultures were evaluated. Both operational sex ratios and reproductively demanding environments related in evolutionary-predicted ways to national levels of so- ciosexuality. Third, sex differences in sociosexuality were generally large and demonstrated cross-cultural universality across the 48 na- tions of the ISDP, confirming several evolutionary theories of human mating. Fourth, sex differences in sociosexuality were significantly larger when reproductive environments were demanding but were reduced to more moderate levels in cultures with more political and economic gender equality. Ilnplications for evolutionary and social role theories of human sexuality are discussed. Keywords: culture; gender; mating; reproduction; sex differences; sex roles; sexual strateges; sociosexuality Over a decade ago, Simpson and Gangestad (1991) intro- it appears to have become the measure of choice when at- duced a self-report measure of human sexuality called the tempting to relate human mating strategies to other sex-re- Sociosexual Orientation Inventory (SOI; see also Ganges- lated phenomena (Allen 2000; Bleske-Rechek & Buss 2001; tad & Simpson 1990; Simpson 1998).Originally designed to Clark 2004; Gangestad & Thornhill 1997; Isaacson 2001; capture variability in the willingness to have sex outside of Reise & Wright 1996; Schmitt 2005; Seal & Agostinelli a committed air-bond. the SO1 contains numerous aues- 1994; Simon 1997, Simpson et al. 1999; Townsend & tions about human mating behavior, romantic fantasies, re- Wasserman 1988). Despite its widespread use, very little is lational emotions, and attitudes toward casual sex. Re- known about the cross-cultural utility of the SOI, with only sponses to the SO1 are typically used to differentiate people a handful of studies directly measuring sociosexuality out- along a single strategic dimension of human mating called side the United States (e.g., Bailey et al. 2000). This is es- sociosexuality (Simpson & Gangestad 1991). Those who score relatively low on this dimension are said to possess - a restricted sociosexual orientation thevJ tend toward monogamy, prolonged courtship, and heavy emotional DAVIDP. SCHMITTreceived his Ph.D. from the Uni- investment in long-term relationships. Those residing at versity of Michigan in 1995 and is currently an Associ- the high end of sociosexuality are considered more unre- ate Professor of Psychology at Bradley University. He is Founding Director of the International Sexuality stricted in mating orientation, they tend toward promiscu- Description Project (ISDP), a collaborative research ity, are quick to have sex, and experience lower levels of group of over 100 scientists from around the world. One romantic relationship closeness1 (Simpson & Gangestad of the aims of the ISDP is to uncover the ways in which 1991). gender, personality, and culture influence basic human Since its introduction. the SO1 has become an increas- mating strategies. The second wave of the ISDP is cur- ingly popular tool for measuring individual dfferences in rently underway and will investigate the personality and basic human mating strategies (Hebl & Kashy 1995; Jones cultural predictors of sexual aggression, domestic vio- 1998; Seal et al. 1994; Simpson 1998; Simpson et al. 2004; lence, and risky behavior related to HIV/AIDS. Stephan & Bachman 1999; Wright & Reise 1997). Indeed, O 2005 Cambridge University Press 0140-525x105 $1250 247 Schmitt: Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe pecially unfortunate because a cross-culturally validated Although the majority of SO1 validation research has measure of human mating strategies would help to address proven highly supportive in American samples, it remains many recent developments in evolutionary psychology and unclear whether the psychometric soundness of the SO1 social role theory (e.g., Gangestad & Simpson 2000; Wood generalizes to other languages and cultures. Do the seven & Eagly 2002). items of the SO1 form one coherent dimension within all In the current study, the SO1 was translated from English cultures, or do sociosexual attitudes and behaviors become into 25 addtional languages and administered to samples dsconnected in certain regions of the world? Can socio- from 48 nations as part of the International Sexuality De- sexuality be accurately gauged using self-report methods scription Project (ISDP; Schmitt et al. 2003a). The result- across all cultures, or does the validty of the SO1 fluctuate ing ISDP database on sociosexuality was used to address across language, geography, ethnicity, history, politics, eco- four main issues. First, the psychometric properties of the nomics, or religion? Do responses to the SOI correlate with SOI were examined in cross-cultural perspective. Second, similar psychological and physical attributes across differ- theories concerning the dstribution of sociosexualityacross ent cultures, or does sociosexuality take different anthro- cultures were evaluated (Belsky et al. 1991; Gangestad & pometric forms around the world? If the SO1 were proven Simpson 2000; Pedersen 1991). Third, the cultural univer- reliable and valid across cultures. this could have im~or-1 sality of sex differences in sociosexuality was investigated tant implications for advancing our understanding of the (Buss & Schmitt 1993; Symons 1979; Trivers 1972). Fourth, links between culture and human mating strategies. How- theories concerning the degree of sexual dfferentiation in ever, no study has evaluated the psychometrics of the SO1 sociosexuality across cultures were examined (Buss & in a language other than English, nor has the SO1 been ad- Barnes 1986; Eagly & Wood 1999; Kasser & Sharma 1999; ministered to samples from non-Western cultures. In the Wood & Eagly 2002). Because national profiles of men's current study, the reliability and validity of the SO1 were and women's sociosexual mating strategies would have lit- evaluated across 26 languages and 48 nations, including tle value if the SO1 were not reliable and valid across cul- multiple cultures from North America, South America, tures, a review of the psychometrics of the SO1 will be ad- Western Europe, Eastern Europe, Southern Europe, the dressed first. Middle East, Africa, Oceania, South/Southeast Asia, and East Asia. 1. Psychometrics of the SO1 2. Sex differences in sociosexuality Simpson and Gangestad (1991) conducted several studies to evaluate the psychometric qualities of the SOI. In their On average, men tend to possess more positive attitudes to- original study, 204 women and 202 men from Texas A&M ward casual, low-investment sex than women do (Carrol et University completed an initial pool of 11 items related to al. 1985; Fisher et al. 1988; Hendrick et al. 1985; Oliver & a conceptual definition of sociosexuality. Using principal Hyde 1993; Townsend 1995;Wilson 1987). Men also report axis factor analysis, Simpson and Gangestad documented that they fantasize about having sex with multiple partners that seven of these items - the seven items eventually in- more than women do (Ellis & Svrnons 1990: Malamuth cluded in the SO1 - formed a coherent unitary factor 1996), and men behaiorally seLk short-term mateships structure. They also found that the resulting seven-item more than women do (Blumstein & Schwartz 1994; SO1 scale possessed adequate levels of internal reliability Eysenck 1976; Laumman et al. 1994; Wiederman 1997). (a = 0.73). In an unpublished study, Simpson and Gan- EmenmentalL tests have further confirmed that men are gestad (1989) documented the high temporal reliability of more likely than women to consent to sex with a stranger sociosexuality (r = +0.94) over a two-month test-retest when approached in a community setting (Clark & Hatfield period. 1989), even when the stranger is "vouched for" by a partic- Simpson and Gangestad (1991) evaluated the valihty of i~ant'ssame-sex friend (Clark 1990). the SO1 using standard construct validation techniques This pervasive of sexual differences - across atti- (Cronbach & Meehl 1955). In a validation study involving tudes, fantasy, and behavior - implies that men should be 144 romantic couples, participants were asked to complete higher or more unrestricted on sociosexuality than women. the SO1
Recommended publications
  • MIAMI UNIVERSITY the Graduate School
    MIAMI UNIVERSITY The Graduate School Certificate for Approving the Dissertation We hereby approve the Dissertation of Steven Almaraz Candidate for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy ______________________________________ Director Kurt Hugenberg ______________________________________ Reader Allen McConnell ______________________________________ Reader Jonathan Kunstman ______________________________________ Graduate School Representative Monica Schneider ABSTRACT APPARENT SOCIOSEXUAL ORIENTATION: FACIAL CORRELATES AND CONSEQUENCES OF WOMEN’S UNRESTRICTED APPEARANCE by Steven M. Almaraz People make quick work of forming a variety of impressions of one another based on minimal information. Recent work has shown that people are able to make judgments of others’ Apparent Sociosexual Orientation (ASO) – an estimation of how interested another person is in uncommitted sexual activity – based on facial information alone. In the present work, I used three studies to expand the understanding of this poorly understood facial judgment by investigating the dimensionality of ASO (Study 1), the facial predictors of ASO (Study 2), and the consequences of these ASO judgments on men’s hostility and benevolence towards women (Study 3). In Study 1, I showed that men’s judgments of women’s Apparent Sociosexual Orientation were organized into judgments of women’s appearance of unrestricted attitudes and desires (Intrapersonal ASO) and their appearance of unrestricted behaviors (Behavioral ASO). Study 2 revealed that more attractive and more dominant appearing women were perceived as more sexually unrestricted. In Study 3, I found that women who appeared to engage in more unrestricted behavior were subjected to increased benevolent sexism, though this effect was primarily driven by unrestricted appearing women’s attractiveness. However, women who appeared to have sexually unrestricted attitudes and desires were subjected to increased hostility, even when controlling for the effects of the facial correlates found in Study 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Theory Throughout the Text)
    Author Bios: Jon A. Sefcek, MA, Barbara H. Brumbach, MA, Geneva Vasquez, MA, are PhD students in the Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ; and Geoffrey F. Miller, PhD, is Assistant Professor of Psychology at the University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM. Address correspondence to Jon A. Sefcek at [email protected] The Evolutionary Psychology of Human Mate Choice: How Ecology, Genes, Fertility, and Fashion Influence Mating Behavior Jon A. Sefcek, MA, Barbara H. Brumbach, MA, Geneva Vasquez, MA, and Geoffrey F. Miller, PhD The recent incorporation of sexual selection theories into the rubric of Evolutionary Psychology has produced an important framework from which to examine human mating behavior. Here we review the extant empirical and theoretical work regarding heterosexual human mating preferences and reproductive strategies. Initially, we review contemporary Evolutionary Psychology’s adaptationism, including the incorporation of modern theories of sexual selection, adaptive genetic variation, and mate choice. Next, we examine women’s and men’s mating preferences focusing on the adaptive significance of material, genetic and fertility benefits, and their relationship to environmental characteristics. Following this, we consider human mate choice in relation to non-adaptive preferences. This discussion ends with a look at context effects for individual differences in mate-preferences and reproductive strategies. KEY WORDS: Sexual selection, fitness indicators, mate choice, mating preferences, mating strategies, parental investment From colorful birds and dancing bees to Sinatra’s crooning, Betty Davis’s eyes, and Monty Python’s satires, various phenotypic traits serve as signals to others. Aposematism (warning coloration) in butterflies, for example, cautions predators that the butterfly is poisonous (Joron, 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • Revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory 1
    Revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory 1 RUNNING HEAD: Revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory The revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory Lars Penke Centre for Cognitive Aging and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh, EH8 9JZ, UK Tel.: +44 131 6511304; fax: +44 131 6503461; email address: [email protected] Word count: 1,300 8 references, 1 table To appear as: Penke, L. (in press). The revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory. In T. D. Fisher, C. M. Davis, W. L. Yarber & S. L. Davis (Eds.): Handbook of sexuality- related measures (3 rd Ed.). Taylor & Francis. Revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory 2 Purpose : The construct of sociosexuality or sociosexual orientation captures individual differences in the tendency to have casual, uncommitted sexual relationships. The term was introduced by Alfred Kinsey, who used it to describe the individual differences in sexual permissiveness and promiscuity that he found in his ground- breaking survey studies on sexual behavior (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948; Kinsey, Pomeroy, Martin, & Gebhard, 1953). The amount of scientific research on sociosexuality increased markedly when Simpson and Gangestad (1991) published the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory (SOI), a 7-item self-report questionnaire that assesses sociosexual orientations along a single dimension from “restricted” (indicating a tendency to have sex exclusively in emotionally close and committed relationships) to “unrestricted” (indicating a tendency for sexual relationships with low commitment and investment, often after short periods of acquaintance and with changing partners). On average, men tend to be more unrestricted than women in their sociosexual orientations, though there are also large individual differences within both sexes (Schmitt, 2005).
    [Show full text]
  • A Different Kind of Animal: How Culture
    © Copyright, Princeton University Press. No part of this book may be distributed, posted, or reproduced in any form by digital or mechanical means without prior written permission of the publisher. INTRODUCTION Stephen Macedo What makes humans special? Is it, as many have argued, our superior intelligence that sets us apart from other species? In the lectures and discussions that follow, Robert Boyd, a distinguished professor of human evolution and social change, refines the question and rejects the common answer. Putting aside the more familiar question of human unique- ness, Boyd asks why humans so exceed other species when it comes to broad indices of ecological success such as our ability to adapt to and thrive in such a wide variety of hab- itats across the globe. Ten thousand years ago, humans al- ready occupied the entire globe except Antarctica and a few remote islands. No other species comes close. What explains our outlier status if not our “big brains”? Humans adapt to a vast variety of changing environments not mainly by applying individual intelligence to solve prob- lems, but rather via “cumulative cultural adaptation” and, over the longer term, Darwinian selection among cultures with different social norms and moral values. Not only are humans part of the natural world, argues Boyd, but human culture is part of the natural world. Culture makes us “a different kind of animal,” and “culture is as much a part of human biology as our peculiar pelvis or the thick enamel that covers our molars.” With his many coauthors, especially Peter Richerson, Robert Boyd has for three decades pioneered an important approach to the study of human evolution that focuses on the population dynamics of culturally transmitted informa- tion.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Mating Strategies Human Mating Strategies
    Human Mating Strategies Human Mating Strategies As descendants of a long line of successful maters, modern humans have inherited the mating strategies that led to their forebear’s success. These include long-term mating, short-term mating, and mixed mating strategies. This article presents empirical evi- dence supporting evolution-based hypotheses about the complexities of these mating strategies, which differ substantially for men and women. array of adaptations specifically dedicated to the David M. Buss, Professor, task of mating. Department of Psychology, Nowhere do people have an equal desire to mate University of Texas, Austin with all people. Everywhere, some people are pre- ferred as mates, others shunned. Desires are central to all facets of mating. They determine who we are attracted to, and who is attracted to us. They influ- ence which attraction tactics will be successful (those that fulfill desires) and which attraction tac- tics will fail (those that violate desires). Successful mate retention tactics involve continuing to provide resources that fulfill the desires of a mate. Failure to Perhaps no adaptive domain is more central to re- fulfill these desires causes breakup and divorce. At production than mating. Those in our evolutionary every step of the mating process, from mate selec- past who failed to mate failed to become ancestors. tion to mate expulsion, desires determine the Modern humans are all descendants of a long and ground rules. unbroken line of ancestors who succeeded in the complex and sometimes circuitous tasks involved in Sexual Selection and Parental Investment mating. As their descendants, modern humans have Although Charles Darwin (1859) recognized that inherited the adaptations that led to the success of survival was central to the evolutionary process, their ancestors.
    [Show full text]
  • Sociosexuality, Testosterone, and Life History Status: Prospective Associations and Longitudinal Changes Among Men in Cebu, Philippines Lee T
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by UNL | Libraries University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Child, Youth, Child, Youth, and Family Studies, Department of and Family Studies 2019 Sociosexuality, testosterone, and life history status: Prospective associations and longitudinal changes among men in Cebu, Philippines Lee T. Gettler University of Notre Dame, [email protected] Patty X. Kuo University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Stacy Rosenbaum Northwestern University, [email protected] Josephine L. Avila University of San Carlos, [email protected] Thomas W. McDade Northwestern University, [email protected] See next page for additional authors Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/famconfacpub Part of the Developmental Psychology Commons, Family, Life Course, and Society Commons, Other Psychology Commons, and the Other Sociology Commons Gettler, Lee T.; Kuo, Patty X.; Rosenbaum, Stacy; Avila, Josephine L.; McDade, Thomas W.; and Kuzawa, Christopher W., "Sociosexuality, testosterone, and life history status: Prospective associations and longitudinal changes among men in Cebu, Philippines" (2019). Faculty Publications, Department of Child, Youth, and Family Studies. 267. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/famconfacpub/267 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Child, Youth, and Family Studies, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications, Department of Child, Youth, and Family Studies by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Authors Lee T. Gettler, Patty X. Kuo, Stacy Rosenbaum, Josephine L.
    [Show full text]
  • A Polyandrous Society in Transition: a CASE STUDY of JAUNSAR-BAWAR
    10/31/2014 A Polyandrous Society in transition: A CASE STUDY OF JAUNSAR-BAWAR SUBMITTED BY: Nargis Jahan IndraniTalukdar ShrutiChoudhary (Department of Sociology) (Delhi School of Economics) Abstract Man being a social animal cannot survive alone and has therefore been livingin groups or communities called families for ages. How these ‘families’ come about through the institution of marriage or any other way is rather an elaborate and an arduous notion. India along with its diverse people and societies offers innumerable ways by which people unite to come together as a family. Polyandry is one such way that has been prevalent in various regions of the sub-continent evidently among the Paharis of Himachal Pradesh, the Todas of Nilgiris, Nairs of Travancore and the Ezhavas of Malabar. While polyandrous unions have disappeared from the traditions of many of the groups and tribes, it is still practiced by some Jaunsaris—an ethnic group living in the lower Himalayan range—especially in the JaunsarBawar region of Uttarakhand.The concept of polyandry is so vast and mystifying that people who have just heard of the practice or the people who even did an in- depth study of it are confused in certain matters regarding it. This thesis aims at providing answers to many questions arising in the minds of people who have little or no knowledge of this subject. In this paper we have tried to find out why people follow this tradition and whether or not it has undergone transition. Also its various characteristics along with its socio-economic issues like the state and position of women in such a society and how the economic balance in a polyandrous family is maintained has been looked into.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Group Selection Plays an Essential Role in Explaining Human Cooperation: a Sketch of the Evidence
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2016), Page 1 of 68 doi:10.1017/S0140525X1400106X, e30 Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence Peter Richerson Emily K. Newton Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California– Department of Psychology, Dominican University of California, San Rafael, CA Davis, Davis, CA 95616 94901 [email protected] [email protected] http://emilyknewton.weebly.com/ www.des.ucdavis.edu/faculty/richerson/richerson.htm Nicole Naar Ryan Baldini Department of Anthropology, University of California–Davis, Graduate Group in Ecology, University of California–Davis, Davis, CA 95616 Davis, CA 95616 [email protected] https://sites.google.com/site/ryanbaldini/ [email protected] Adrian V. Bell Lesley Newson Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112 Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California– [email protected] http://adrianbell.wordpress.com/ Davis, Davis, CA 95616 [email protected] [email protected] Kathryn Demps https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Lesley_Newson/ Department of Anthropology, Boise State University, Boise, ID 83725 [email protected] Cody Ross http://sspa.boisestate.edu/anthropology/faculty-and-staff/kathryn- Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501 demps/ [email protected] http://scholar.google.com/citations?user=xSugEskAAAAJ Karl Frost Graduate Group in Ecology, University of California–Davis, Davis, CA 95616 Paul E. Smaldino [email protected] https://sites.google.com/site/karljosephfrost/ Department of Anthropology, University of California–Davis, Davis, CA 95616 [email protected] http://www.smaldino.com/ Vicken Hillis Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California– Timothy M.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Human Mating: Trade-Offs and Strategic Pluralism
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2000) 23, 573–644 Printed in the United States of America The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism Steven W. Gangestad Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131 [email protected] Jeffry A. Simpson Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843 [email protected]. Abstract: During human evolutionary history, there were “trade-offs” between expending time and energy on child-rearing and mating, so both men and women evolved conditional mating strategies guided by cues signaling the circumstances. Many short-term matings might be successful for some men; others might try to find and keep a single mate, investing their effort in rearing her offspring. Recent evidence suggests that men with features signaling genetic benefits to offspring should be preferred by women as short-term mates, but there are trade-offs between a mate’s genetic fitness and his willingness to help in child-rearing. It is these circumstances and the cues that signal them that underlie the variation in short- and long-term mating strategies between and within the sexes. Keywords: conditional strategies; evolutionary psychology; fluctuating asymmetry; mating; reproductive strategies; sexual selection Research on interpersonal relationships, especially roman- attributes (e.g., physical attractiveness) tend to assume tic ones, has increased markedly in the last three decades greater importance in mating relationships than in other (see Berscheid & Reis 1998) across a variety of fields, in- types of relationships (Buss 1989; Gangestad & Buss 1993 cluding social psychology, anthropology, ethology, sociol- [see also Kenrick & Keefe: “Age Preferences in Mates Re- ogy, developmental psychology, and personology (Ber- flect Sex Differences in Human Reproductive Strategies” scheid 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • Arousing Imaginations
    Evolutionary Psychology www.epjournal.net – 2007. 5(4): 778-785 ¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯ Book Review Arousing Imaginations A review of Michael R. Kauth, (Ed.), Handbook of the Evolution of Human Sexuality, Haworth Press, Binghamton, NY, 2006, 395 pp., $50.00 (softbound), $130.00 (hardbound) Reviewed by David Puts, Ph.D. Department of Anthropology, 424 Carpenter Building, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802. Email: [email protected] In the quarter century since Donald Symons (1979) wrote The Evolution of Human Sexuality, research in this area has flourished. Michael Kauth’s edited volume Handbook of the Evolution of Human Sexuality (co-published as Volume 18, Numbers 2/3 and 4 2006 of the Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality) is therefore a welcome addition. Kauth assembles researchers in diverse areas of human sexuality, including heterosexual mate choice, sexual arousal and olfactory preferences, but the focus is heavily upon the evolution and development of homosexuality, with five of the book’s ten chapters centering on this topic. Given that only 2-3% of men and 1-2% of women report predominantly homosexual preferences, this focus may seem disproportionate. However, the volume is intended not to cover all aspects of human sexuality but to serve as a springboard for future research, a mission that it will undoubtedly accomplish. The scholarship in the Handbook is variable but generally good. The content is often heavy on speculation and light on evidence, which would be less concerning if authors acknowledged this more openly. Instead, the chapters of the Handbook often reify “facts” that are still quite theoretical—for example, Kauth’s statement that “physical symmetry and extravagant sexual ornamentation are ‘honest’ signals of health and genetic quality” (p.
    [Show full text]
  • Clearing the Highest Hurdle: Human-Based Case Studies Broaden Students’ Knowledge of Core Evolutionary Concepts
    The Journal of Effective Teaching an online journal devoted to teaching excellence Clearing the highest hurdle: Human-based case studies broaden students’ knowledge of core evolutionary concepts Alexander J. Werth1 Hampden-Sydney College, Hampden-Sydney, VA 23943 Abstract An anonymous survey instrument was used for a ten year study to gauge college student attitudes toward evolution. Results indicate that students are most likely to accept evolu- tion as a historical process for change in physical features of non-human organisms. They are less likely to accept evolution as an ongoing process that shapes all traits (including biochemical, physiological, and behavioral) in humans. Students who fail to accept the factual nature of human evolution do not gain an accurate view of evolution, let alone modern biology. Fortunately, because of students’ natural curiosity about their bodies and related topics (e.g., medicine, vestigial features, human prehistory), a pedagogical focus on human evolution provides a fun and effective way to teach core evolutionary concepts, as quantified by the survey. Results of the study are presented along with useful case studies involving human evolution. Keywords: Survey, pedagogy, biology, evolution, Darwin. All science educators face pedagogical difficulties, but when considering the social rami- fications of scientific ideas, few face as great a challenge as biology teachers. Studies consistently show more than half of Americans reject any concept of biological evolution (Harris Poll, 2005, Miller et al., 2006). Students embody just such a cross-section of soci- ety. Much ink has been spilled explaining how best to teach evolution, particularly to unwilling students (Cavallo and McCall, 2008, Nelson, 2008).
    [Show full text]
  • Polygynandry, Extra-Group Paternity and Multiple-Paternity Litters In
    Molecular Ecology (2007) 16, 5294–5306 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2007.03571.x Polygynandry,Blackwell Publishing Ltd extra-group paternity and multiple-paternity litters in European badger (Meles meles) social groups HANNAH L. DUGDALE,*† DAVID W. MACDONALD,* LISA C. POPE† and TERRY BURKE† *Wildlife Conservation Research Unit, Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Tubney House, Abingdon Road, Tubney OX13 5QL, UK, †Department of Animal and Plant Sciences, University of Sheffield, Western Bank, Sheffield S10 2TN, UK Abstract The costs and benefits of natal philopatry are central to the formation and maintenance of social groups. Badger groups, thought to form passively according to the resource dispersion hypothesis (RDH), are maintained through natal philopatry and delayed dispersal; however, there is minimal evidence for the functional benefits of such grouping. We assigned parentage to 630 badger cubs from a high-density population in Wytham Woods, Oxford, born between 1988 and 2005. Our methodological approach was different to previous studies; we used 22 microsatellite loci to assign parent pairs, which in combination with sibship inference provided a high parentage assignment rate. We assigned both parents to 331 cubs at ≥ 95% confidence, revealing a polygynandrous mating system with up to five mothers and five fathers within a social group. We estimated that only 27% of adult males and 31% of adult females bred each year, suggesting a cost to group living for both sexes. Any strong motivation or selection to disperse, however, may be reduced because just under half of the paternities were gained by extra-group males, mainly from neighbouring groups, with males displaying a mixture of paternity strategies.
    [Show full text]