Diachronic Syntactic Change and Language Acquisition: a View from Nominative/Genitive Conversion in Japanese

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Diachronic Syntactic Change and Language Acquisition: a View from Nominative/Genitive Conversion in Japanese Interdisciplinary Information Sciences Vol. 24, No. 2 (2018) 91–179 #Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University ISSN 1340-9050 print/1347-6157 online DOI 10.4036/iis.2018.R.01 Diachronic Syntactic Change and Language Acquisition: A View from Nominative/Genitive Conversion in Japanese Yoshiki OGAWAÃ Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai 980-8579, Japan Harada (1971) argued some forty five years ago that the Japanese phenomenon called ‘‘Nominative/Genitive Conversion’’ (NGC) was undergoing a syntactic change, which was detected as idiolectal variations. Synchronically, Miyagawa (2011) argues that the NGC is not a free alternation but that more stative predicates are more likely to accept a Genitive subject. However, no one has ever proposed an argument that bridges the synchronic preference for ‘‘stativity’’ of the NGC and its diachronic syntactic change, which is characterized as ‘‘stativization.’’ In this article, we will show that the diachronic syntactic change has been in progress at least for the last 100 years. It will be shown that the semantic ‘‘stativization’’ is an epiphenomenon of the syntactic microparametric change which we refer to as ‘‘clause shrinking,’’ or a change in the syntactic size of the Genitive Subject Clause (GSC) from CP to TP to vP to VP/AP. Moreover, we will explain how such a drastic language change have actually influenced language acquisition for children who were born in different time periods, by integrating Kayne’s (2000) microparametric syntax, Snyder’s (2017) theory of competition between incompatible constructions, Lightfoot and Westergard’s (2009) micro-cue analysis of language acquisition, Manzini and Wexler’s (1987) Subset Principle, and Bosˇkovic´ (1997) Minimal Structure Principle. KEYWORDS: Nominative/Genitive Conversion, Japanese, diachronic change, frequency change, language acquisition, microparametric syntax, corpus 1. Introduction In the modern Japanese, as well as in all the other Nominative–Accusative languages in the world, the subject of a clause is usually marked for the Nominative Case morphology ga. In Japanese, however, especially in an adnominal clause, there is another option of morphological Case-marking such that the subject is marked for the Genitive Case no, as in (1): (1) a. Taro-ga sun-dei-ru mati Taro-Nom live-Prof-Nonpast town ‘the town where Taro is living’ b. Taro-no sun-dei-ru mati Taro-Gen live-Prof-Nonpast town ‘the town where Taro is living’ In generative linguistics, this phenomenon has usually been referred to as the ‘‘Nominative/Genitive Conversion’’ or ‘‘ Ga/No Conversion,’’ and a pile of synchronic analyses have been proposed to account for why the Case conversion is possible in some syntactic environments but not in others (Harada (1971, 1976); Nakai (1980); Miyagawa (1993, 2011, 2013); Watanabe (1997); Ochi (2001); Hiraiwa (2002, 2005), Maki and Uchiori (2008); among others), but almost nothing has been understood about how the phenomenon is related to the Case system of a classical Japanese (which is totally different from the current one, as shown below), whether there is an ongoing change in the applicability of the syntactic phenomenon, and if any, how such a language change can be explained under the parametric syntax, which began with the advent of the Principles and Parameters Theory (Chomsky 1981), since it is only Harada (1971, 1976) who discussed the idiolectal variation about the (im)possibility of Ga/No Conversion in certain constructions. Nambu (2007, 2014) presents a result of a corpus search which shows the existence of decline in the frequency of Genitive subject clauses in the last 100 years, though he presents no investigation of construction-by-construction diachronic change in the NGC or no syntactic explanation of any diachronic change. In this article, we will first show a result of my own large-scale corpus-based study which shows that the syntactic subconstructions in which a Genitive subject clause (henceforth, GSC) can be licensed have become more and more narrowed even in the last 100 years, and there are at least two more dialects than what Harada (1971) identified about Received September 2, 2018; Accepted November 30, 2018 ÃCorresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] 92 OGAWA the extent to which the GSC can be licensed. Since his identified dialects are called Dialects A and B, two more dialects will be referred to as Dialects C and D. We will argue that the Dialect A, which is directly linked to the grammar of the classical Japanese, has almost completely disappeared, and the remaining three dialects have been spread over three different age groups who are alive now, as of the years 2016–2018, and that the microparametric change among the four dialects has been triggered by different micro-cues available for children in different time periods. In discussing the issue of how language change is related to language acquisition, we will resort to Kayne’s (2001) microparametric syntax of language variation and Lightfoot and Westergaard’s (2007) micro-cue analysis about the relation between language change and language acquisition, as well as Manzini and Wexler’s (1987) Subset Principle and Westergaard’s (2014) economy principle of children’s production as one of the third factors (Chomsky (2005)). This article is organized as follows: in Sect. 2, we will argue how the language change in Nominative/Genitive Conversion (henceforth, NGC) can be assimilated or analogized to a well-known phenomenon of language change, and review the previous generative syntactic approaches to the language change, its acquisition, and relevant parametric change, and summarize what problems have been solved in the course. In Sect. 3, we will show from investigation of corpora how the frequency and distribution of Nominative subject adnominal clauses (NSC) and GSCs have changed from the old Japanese and to the present. In Sect. 4, we will argue that a change in the frequency and distribution of Nominative and Genitive subjects is not semantic but syntactic in nature, and make a brief review of previous analyses of the GSCs and/or the NGC in Japanese, all of which are synchronic analyses, except for Harada (1971, 1976). In Sect. 5, a new analysis of the diachronic change in the syntactic environments of the Genitive Case assignment is presented. More specifically, I propose that the GSC at the complement of the functional category D as a Genitive Case licensor has been shrinking from CP to TP to vP to VP/AP in the last 100 years or so, while the NSC has been CP throughout the history. In Sect. 6, we will argue how successfully the four different dialects can be learned by a child in different time periods and how language can change, where the Subset Principle and/or a kind of economy principle play a role. In Sect. 7, we will discuss how successfully generative syntax can explain any gradual language change, and push away critics against generative syntax from historical linguists. Section 8 is a conclusion. 2. Microparametric Syntax: How to Apply It to Diachronic Change Genitive Case is usually used for marking the ‘‘subject’’ of a NP, where the notion of ‘‘subject’’ is loosely defined, so as to cover not only the so-called ‘‘subject’’ of a derived nominal but also any element having some relation R to the head of a NP. Relatedly, the Genitive Case in Japanese is realized with the Genitive Case morphology no, whether it is a NP, a PP or an adverb (or an adverbial clause) that modifies a noun, unlike in English, where only a NP can be marked for the Genitive Case and PP and adverbial clauses have the same structural relation as they are in VP, as far as Case marking is concerned: (2) a. Taro-no/Ãga hon <NP> Taro-Gen/Nom book ‘Taro’s book’ b. Taro-kara-no tegami <PP>1 Taro-from-Gen letter ‘a letter from Taro’ c. Taro-ga kita-ato-no sawagi <adverbial clause> Taro-Nom came-after-Gen fuss ‘the fuss after Taro came’ Since the advent of generative syntax, it has tacitly been assumed that Nominative Case for the clausal subject is 1An anonymous review wonders if this instance of no is not a Genitive Case but a copula, which is sometimes spelled out as no in Japanese. Here too, one can say that tegami-wa Taro-kara da ‘The letter is from Taro.’ However, we can reject this possibility for two reasons: first, the fact that a PP in Japanese can be marked by no is part of a broader generalization in Japanese that any vategory, whether a NP or a non-NP (such as AdvP, CP), can be marked by no, as in (i): (i) a. kanari-no simpo considerably-Gen progress ‘a considerable progress’ (cf. ÃSimpo-wa kanari da.) b. Shachoo-ga kita-to-no uwasa president-Nom came-Comp-Gen rumor ‘the rumor that the president came’ (cf. ÃUwasa-wa shachoo-ga kita to da.) Second, some PPs in Japanese, including (2b), can also be marked by Nominative Case, as in (ii): (ii) Tokyo-kara-ga yasui. Tokyo-from-Nom inexpensive ‘(a ticket) from Tokyo is inexpensinve.’ For these two reasons, we suppose that they have some nominal feature compatible with Genitive Case marking. Diachronic Syntactic Change and Language Acquisition: A View from Nominative/Genitive Conversion in Japanese 93 unmarked, with the Genitive Case marked, because the so-called Nominative/Genitive Conversion (NGC) in Japanese as in (1) can only take place in an adnominal clause and elsewhere only a Nominative Case is the only Case available for a subject (except for a few stative predicates for which a dative subject is available). But a corpus study shows that it was not until the mid of the 20th century that the subject of a finite adnominal clause came to be more frequently marked for the Nominative.
Recommended publications
  • The Function of Phrasal Verbs and Their Lexical Counterparts in Technical Manuals
    Portland State University PDXScholar Dissertations and Theses Dissertations and Theses 1991 The function of phrasal verbs and their lexical counterparts in technical manuals Brock Brady Portland State University Follow this and additional works at: https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds Part of the Applied Linguistics Commons Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Brady, Brock, "The function of phrasal verbs and their lexical counterparts in technical manuals" (1991). Dissertations and Theses. Paper 4181. https://doi.org/10.15760/etd.6065 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of PDXScholar. Please contact us if we can make this document more accessible: [email protected]. AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF Brock Brady for the Master of Arts in Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages (lESOL) presented March 29th, 1991. Title: The Function of Phrasal Verbs and their Lexical Counterparts in Technical Manuals APPROVED BY THE MEMBERS OF THE THESIS COMMITTEE: { e.!I :flette S. DeCarrico, Chair Marjorie Terdal Thomas Dieterich Sister Rita Rose Vistica This study investigates the use of phrasal verbs and their lexical counterparts (i.e. nouns with a lexical structure and meaning similar to corresponding phrasal verbs) in technical manuals from three perspectives: (1) that such two-word items might be more frequent in technical writing than in general texts; (2) that these two-word items might have particular functions in technical writing; and that (3) 2 frequencies of these items might vary according to the presumed expertise of the text's audience.
    [Show full text]
  • A Minimalist Study of Complex Verb Formation: Cross-Linguistic Paerns and Variation
    A Minimalist Study of Complex Verb Formation: Cross-linguistic Paerns and Variation Chenchen Julio Song, [email protected] PhD First Year Report, June 2016 Abstract is report investigates the cross-linguistic paerns and structural variation in com- plex verbs within a Minimalist and Distributed Morphology framework. Based on data from English, German, Hungarian, Chinese, and Japanese, three general mechanisms are proposed for complex verb formation, including Akt-licensing, “two-peaked” adjunction, and trans-workspace recategorization. e interaction of these mechanisms yields three levels of complex verb formation, i.e. Root level, verbalizer level, and beyond verbalizer level. In particular, the verbalizer (together with its Akt extension) is identified as the boundary between the word-internal and word-external domains of complex verbs. With these techniques, a unified analysis for the cohesion level, separability, component cate- gory, and semantic nature of complex verbs is tentatively presented. 1 Introduction1 Complex verbs may be complex in form or meaning (or both). For example, break (an Accom- plishment verb) is simple in form but complex in meaning (with two subevents), understand (a Stative verb) is complex in form but simple in meaning, and get up is complex in both form and meaning. is report is primarily based on formal complexity2 but tries to fit meaning into the picture as well. Complex verbs are cross-linguistically common. e above-mentioned understand and get up represent just two types: prefixed verb and phrasal verb. ere are still other types of complex verb, such as compound verb (e.g. stir-fry). ese are just descriptive terms, which I use for expository convenience.
    [Show full text]
  • SYNTAX MASTER LIST Semester 2
    NOUN SYNTAX Syntax Explanation Latin I Latin II NOMINATIVE Subject The actor of an active verb, receiver of a passive verb, or “it” of an impersonal/stative verb ✓ ✓ Predicate The noun renames the subject via sum, esse ✓ ✓ GENITIVE Possession The genitive owns the noun to which it is connected (sometimes via sum, esse) ✓ ✓ Material The noun is made up of this genitive ✓ Quality The genitive gives a characteristic of the noun (requires attributive adjective) ✓ Partitive The genitive is made up of this noun ✓ Objective The genitive receives the noun as its object ✓ With Adjective The genitive is connected to an adjective ✓ With Verb The genitive is connected to a verb (verbs of reminding, accusing, condemning, feeling, etc.) ✓ Value The genitive indicates how important is an object ✓ DATIVE Indirect Object The dative receives the direct object (explicit or implicit [intransitives/compounds]) ✓ ✓ Possession The dative owns the noun to which it is connected via sum, esse ✓ Agent The dative indicates the actor of a passive verb (typically with gerundives or compoundeds) ✓ Reference The dative indicates to whom, or for whom, a statement refers or is of interest ✓ Ethical A subset of reference, typically used with person pronoun to show interested felt by a person Separation The dative indicates motion away (it has no preposition and is used with verbs of taking away) ✓ Purpose or End The dative indicates the reason for an action ✓ With Adjective The dative is connected to an adjective ✓ With Verb Another name for the dative indirect object
    [Show full text]
  • State Verb, Action Verb and Noun in the State Run Colleges in Pakistan
    International Journal of English Linguistics; Vol. 6, No. 5; 2016 ISSN 1923-869X E-ISSN 1923-8703 Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education A Study on Science Students’ Understanding of Three Lemmas: State Verb, Action Verb and Noun in the State Run Colleges in Pakistan Muhamma Imran1, Tahira Asgher2 & Mamuna Ghani3 1 Govt. Post Graduate College, Burewala, Pakistan 2 Govt. Sadiq College Women University, Bahawalpur, Pakistan 3 Department of English, The Islamia University of Bahawalpur, Pakistan Correspondence: Tahira Asgher, Govt Sadiq College Women University, Bahawalpur, Pakistan. E-mail: [email protected] Received: July 25, 2016 Accepted: August 16, 2016 Online Published: September 23, 2016 doi:10.5539/ijel.v6n5p121 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ijel.v6n5p121 Abstract English main verbs are classified as stative and dynamic. At first, this paper deals with the analysis of stative verbs, highlights their true nature and function and illustrates the concept of spontaneity within the state verbs. Secondly, it expounds how the term ‘lemma’ helps in sorting out the same words representing different parts of speech. Finally, it reports on the level of the science students’ competency in the use of state verbs, action verbs and nouns. For this purpose, 300 science students of intermediate level were selected as participants for the present study. A language proficiency test was conducted to collect data. The results revealed that majority of the students had scanty understanding of nouns and state verbs but their recognition of action verbs had been of average level. Some suggestions for improved pedagogy in teaching English grammar have been suggested on the basis of these findings.
    [Show full text]
  • Change of Location and Change of State: How Telicity Is Attained
    PACLIC 24 Proceedings 885 Change of Location and Change of State: How Telicity is Attained Chungmin Lee Department of Linguistics, Seoul National University Seoul 151-742, Korea [email protected] Abstract. This paper basically discusses parallels between change of location (or space) and change of state. Change of state is analogous to change of location, involving an abstract state Source and an abstract state Goal with abstract Path, involving telicity and showing alternations crosslinguistically. Spatial change is least abstract, whereas temporal change and state change are more abstract and psychological change is most abstract, exhibiting various phenomena of degree modification and telicity differentiations. Abstraction causes argument reduction and change in syntactic behavior. State-oriented predicates are modified by the equivalents of the degree modifier very and process-oriented ones by the quality modifier well and its equivalents. Keywords: change of location, change of state, creation/removal, telicity, degree, Generative Lexicon Theory 1. Introduction1 This paper discusses parallels between change of location and change of state, involved in locomotive (=motion), change of state, and creation/removal verbs, trying to see how telicity is attained semantically, examining cross-linguistic typological variation in lexical patterning and some syntactic behaviors. First, spatial uses of prepositions or postpositions are closely connected with temporal uses of them, although the latter are more abstract and limited because of directionality and dimensionality. Second, change of state (qualities) is structurally associated with change of location, with its Source and Goal. Change always means a shift from ¬P to P in state as well as in location through the flow of time.
    [Show full text]
  • From Translation to Adaptation: Chinese Language Texts and Early Modern Japanese Literature
    From Translation to Adaptation: Chinese Language Texts and Early Modern Japanese Literature Nan Ma Hartmann Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY 2014 © 2014 Nan Ma Hartmann All rights reserved ABSTRACT From Translation to Adaptation: Chinese Language Texts and Early Modern Japanese Literature Nan Ma Hartmann This dissertation examines the reception of Chinese language and literature during Tokugawa period Japan, highlighting the importation of vernacular Chinese, the transformation of literary styles, and the translation of narrative fiction. By analyzing the social and linguistic influences of the reception and adaptation of Chinese vernacular fiction, I hope to improve our understanding of genre development and linguistic diversification in early modern Japanese literature. This dissertation historically and linguistically contextualizes the vernacularization movements and adaptations of Chinese texts in the seventeenth to eighteenth centuries, showing how literary importation and localization were essential stimulants and also a paradigmatic shift that generated new platforms for Japanese literature. Chapter 1 places the early introduction of vernacular Chinese language in its social and cultural contexts, focusing on its route of propagation from the Nagasaki translator community to literati and scholars in Edo, and its elevation from a utilitarian language to an object of literary and political interest. Central figures include Okajima Kazan (1674-1728) and Ogyû Sorai (1666-1728). Chapter 2 continues the discussion of the popularization of vernacular Chinese among elite intellectuals, represented by the Ken’en School of scholars and their Chinese study group, “the Translation Society.” This chapter discusses the methodology of the study of Chinese by surveying a number of primers and dictionaries compiled for reading vernacular Chinese and comparing such material with methodologies for reading classical Chinese.
    [Show full text]
  • Adjectives in Qiang 307
    13 Adjectives in Qiang 307 inite markers, many can act as adverbials (taking the adverbial particle I-Jli!), and l3 many can take the postpositive adverb I-wal 'verY:' Non-stative verbs can only modify a noun in the form of a pre-head relative clause construction, while adjec­ tives can modifya noun directly in post-head position. The meaning of reduplica­ tion for most non-stative verbs is reciprocity, while the meaning of reduplication Adjectives in Qiang for adjectives is intensification or plurality.3 There is no morphology for deriving adjectives from non-adjectival verbs, although adjectives can take causative mark­ Randy f. LaPolla and Chenglong Huang ing and become transitive verbs.4 2. Semantics 1. Introduction The class of adjectives is an open class, currently with roughly 200 members, al­ though the majority of new members are loanwords from Chinese. The class in­ Qiang is a Tibeto-Burman language spoken by 70,000-80,000 people in Northern cludes items related to DIMENSION, AGE, VALUE, COLOUR, PHYSICAL PROPERTY, Sichuan Province, China, classified as being in the Qiang or Tibetan nationality by 1 HUMAN PROPENSITY, SPEED, DIFFICULTY, QUALIFICATION, and QUANTIFICATION. the Chinese government. The language is verb final, agglutinative (prefixing and Words expressing the semantic field of POSITION are (locational) nouns (Istekel suffixing), and has both head-marking and dependent-marking morphology. 'behind; Imoql 'top, above: I,,-ekul 'between, centre; I~qol/ 'below; Ipienal 'near, Nouns can be defined as underived forms which can take (in)definite marking, (be)side', and Iqo'l 'before'), NUMBERS form aseparate word class (they must ap­ numeral-cIassifier phrase"s, and/or number marking, all ofwhich follow the head.
    [Show full text]
  • An Analysis of Stative Verbs Used with the Progressive Aspect in Corpus-Informed Textbooks
    English Language Teaching; Vol. 11, No. 1; 2018 ISSN 1916-4742 E-ISSN 1916-4750 Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education An Analysis of Stative Verbs Used with the Progressive Aspect in Corpus-informed Textbooks Serap Atasever Belli1 1 Department of Foreign Language Education, English Language Teaching Program, Anadolu University, Eskişehir, Turkey Correspondence: Serap Atasever Belli, Faculty of Education, Department of Foreign Language Education, English Language Teaching Program, Anadolu University, Eskişehir, 26470, Turkey. Tel: 90-222-335-05-80-3498. E-mail: [email protected] Received: October 27, 2017 Accepted: December 13, 2017 Online Published: December 15, 2017 doi: 10.5539/elt.v11n1p120 URL: http://doi.org/10.5539/elt.v11n1p120 Abstract This study was designed to investigate whether contemporary corpus-informed grammar textbooks written for English language learners and teachers presented the progressive use of stative verbs and if yes, which stative verbs were presented to occur with the progressive aspect and for which functions they took this aspect. A corpus of six electronic copies of corpus-informed textbooks was compiled and analyzed via AntConc. 3.2.4 text analysis program to identify types and functions of stative verbs and calculate their occurrences. Overall, textbooks differed in their treatment of the progressive use of stative verbs and inclusion of the variety and numbers of types and functions. One remarkable finding was that the stative verbs taking the progressive aspect in all textbooks were found to be associated with emotions (i.e. love) whereas those not allowing progressive use were related to cognition (i.e. know). Another remarkable finding was that the textbooks which presented the highest numbers of stative verb types provided the most diverse functions whereas the textbooks which included the least numbers of stative verbs provided one or no function.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphology and Syntax of Gerunds in Truku Seediq : a Third Function of Austronesian “Voice” Morphology
    MORPHOLOGY AND SYNTAX OF GERUNDS IN TRUKU SEEDIQ : A THIRD FUNCTION OF AUSTRONESIAN “VOICE” MORPHOLOGY A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE DIVISION OF THE UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI‘I AT MĀNOA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR IN PHILOSOPHY IN LINGUISTICS SUMMER 2017 By Mayumi Oiwa-Bungard Dissertation Committee: Robert Blust, chairperson Yuko Otsuka, chairperson Lyle Campbell Shinichiro Fukuda Li Jiang Dedicated to the memory of Yudaw Pisaw, a beloved friend ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS First and foremost, I would like to express my most profound gratitude to the hospitality and generosity of the many members of the Truku community in the Bsngan and the Qowgan villages that I crossed paths with over the years. I’d like to especially acknowledge my consultants, the late 田信德 (Tian Xin-de), 朱玉茹 (Zhu Yu-ru), 戴秋貴 (Dai Qiu-gui), and 林玉 夏 (Lin Yu-xia). Their dedication and passion for the language have been an endless source of inspiration to me. Pastor Dai and Ms. Lin also provided me with what I can call home away from home, and treated me like family. I am hugely indebted to my committee members. I would like to express special thanks to my two co-chairs and mentors, Dr. Robert Blust and Dr. Yuko Otsuka. Dr. Blust encouraged me to apply for the PhD program, when I was ready to leave academia after receiving my Master’s degree. If it wasn’t for the gentle push from such a prominent figure in the field, I would never have seen the potential in myself.
    [Show full text]
  • The Possessive Dative and the Syntax of Affected Arguments*
    The possessive dative and the syntax of affected arguments* CRISTINA SÁNCHEZ LÓPEZ Abstract The goal of this paper is to provide an analysis of possessive dative constructions which explains the relationship between possessive datives and inalienable possession relation, on the one hand, and possessive datives and affectedness, on the other hand. I assume that inalienable possession is a kind of part-whole relation syntactically configured as a predication relation headed by a locative preposition. Under this assumption, I propose that the argument denoting the whole can be licensed by an extra verbal projection associated with the semantic feature of affectedness and able to give structural dative case. The analysis can explain some facts of Spanish dative constructions: the existence of diathetical alternations in transitive and unaccusative predicates (between dative/oblique adjunct and nominative/oblique adjunct, respectively), and the existence of (locative) datives within ergative predicates. 1. The problem The so-called possessive dative (cf. Bally 1926; Delbecque & Lamiroy 1996a, b among many others) denotes the possessor of the internal argument of a predicate, be it a direct object as in (1a) or a subject (as in (1b)): (1) a. El gato le arañó la cara. the cat him.DAT1 scratched the face ‘The cat scratched his/her face.’ b. A Juan le duele el brazo. to John him.DAT hurts the arm ‘His arm hurts.’ Possessive dative constructions are common to other languages, like romance languages and German. (2) are examples of transitive constructions analogous to (1a); (3) are examples of unaccusative constructions analogous to (1b): (2) a. ESP. Yo le corté los cabellos.
    [Show full text]
  • Hardening Alternation in the Mitsukaido Dialect of Japanese
    言語研究(Gengo Kenkyu)134: 85–118(2008) 85 Hardening Alternation in the Mitsukaido Dialect of Japanese Kan Sasaki Sapporo Gakuin University Abstract: Th e Mitsukaido dialect of Japanese has a phenomenon in which a stem-initial fricative turns into a stop (or an aff ricate) when the stem is the second member of a compound. Th is hardening alternation results from opaque interactions among four phonological processes: sequential voicing, debuccaliza- tion (p→h), continuancy neutralization and consonant devoicing. Consonant devoicing counterfeeds sequential voicing and p→h, and counterbleeds continu- ancy neutralization. Classic Optimality Th eory cannot deal with the phonologi- cal opacity behind the hardening alternation. Stratal OT, a weak parallelist OT extension which incorporates level ordering, provides a solution for this phe- nomenon. Strict parallelist extensions such as Sympathy theory and Candidate Chain theory make the wrong prediction for the hardening alternation.* Key words: devoicing, hardening, Optimality Th eory, phonological opacity, Stratal OT 1. Introduction Th e Mitsukaido dialect of Japanese, spoken in the southwestern part of Ibaraki Prefecture, has a mixed phonological system displaying both Tohoku dialect prop- erties, such as intervocalic voicing and consonant devoicing, and southern Kanto dialect properties, such as lack of prenasal consonants. Its mixed nature is also found in morphosyntactic properties like the presence of the dative/allative case particle -sa peculiar to Tohoku dialects and the absence of productive anticausative voice, as in southern Kanto dialects. Due to this mixed character, the Mitsukaido dialect is often referred to as Kanto no Tohoku-hogen ‘Tohoku dialect spoken in Kanto’. Th e phenomenon I analyze in this paper is the hardening alternation exemplifi ed in (1), a phonological alternation derived from the mixed nature of this dialect.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Expressing Generalities in Tlingit: Imperfective Mode Vs. Habitual
    Expressing Generalities in Tlingit: Imperfective Mode vs. Habitual Mode (With a Few Notes on the Ergative Postposition) Seth Cable University of Massachusetts Amherst June 22, 2018 1. Introduction and Overview This report summarizes the main empirical findings of my most recent field research on the Tlingit language, which was supported by project grant from the National Science Foundation (#BCS-1322770). As the grant ended in February of this year, the research reported here represents the last of the work supported under that grant. This work was carried out in Juneau, AK from 7/15/2017 – 7/22/2017. During my time in Juneau, I had the great fortune to work with five exceptional elders, all native speakers of the Tlingit language: Yax̱duláḵt (Lillian Austin), Shakʼsháani (Margaret Dutson), Ḵaséix̱ (Selena Everson), Ḵ’altseen (Carolyn Martin), and Keihéenák’w (John Martin). I am extremely grateful for their generosity and their patience, and for all that they’ve taught me; aatlein gunalchéesh! This year’s study was largely devoted to questions related to how the Tlingit language expresses so-called ‘generalities’. I use the label ‘generalities’ to cover both so-called ‘generic statements’ and so-called ‘habitual statements’. Generic statements are ones like the English sentences under (1); they express the general properties of a kind or type. For example, (1a) states that things falling under the kind ‘boy’ generally have the property of loving dogs. Sentence (1c) states that things falling under the kind ‘good person’ generally have the property of helping others, etc. (1) Generic Statements in English a. Boys love dogs.
    [Show full text]