Interpreting Definite Plural Subjects: a Comparison Of

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Interpreting Definite Plural Subjects: a Comparison Of Batia Snir-Ling190 1 TANJA KUPISCH AND CRISTINA PIERANTOZZI INTERPRETING DEFINITE PLURAL SUBJECTS: A COMPARISON OF GERMAN AND ITALIAN MONOLINGUAL AND BILINGUAL CHILDREN QUESTION: HOW AND WHY DOES CROSS-LINGUISTIC INFLUENCE MANIFEST IN BILINGUALS? BACKGROUND DETERMINERS IN GERMAN AND ITALIAN It has been suggested that Interface phenomena are especially vulnerable to cross-linguistic influence. Doubly so if there is overlap in the phenomena between the two languages. FIG. 1: DETERMINERS BY SUBJECT TYPE German Italian Subject DP type Specific reading (any number) DEFINITE: DEFINITE: “The cat eats mice” Die Katze frisst Mäuse. Il gatto mangia topi. “The cats eat mice” Die Katzen fressen Mäuse. I gatti mangiano topi “The wine is healthy” Der Wein ist gesund. Il vino è sano. Generic reading (singular) DEFINITE: DEFINITE: “The cat eats mice” Die Katze frisst Mäuse. Il gatto mangia topi. Generic reading (plural) BARE: DEFINITE: “Cats eat mice.” Katzen fressen Mäuse. I gatti mangiano topi. Generic reading (mass noun) BARE: DEFINITE: “Wine is healthy” Wein ist gesund. Il vino è sano. FIG. 2: INTERPRETATION BY DETERMINER TYPE German Italian Singular noun Definite Specific/Generic Specific/Generic Bare * * Plural noun Definite Specific Specific/Generic Bare Generic * Mass noun Definite Specific Specific/Generic Bare Generic * CROSS-LINGUISTIC INTERFERENCE Though the Separate System Hypothesis posits that bilinguals treat their languages discretely, there is clear evidence that the languages are in contact and influence each other accordingly. HYPOTHESES FOR CROSS-LINGUISTIC INFLUENCE Researchers have hypothesized that language influence is driven by any of a number of factors: STRUCTURAL OVERLAP: It has consistently been shown that if two languages have a great deal of structural overlap, we predict a higher degree of influence (Müller & Hulk 2000, Bernardini 2004, Nicoladis 2006) Specifically, if one language (the superset) has two options where the other (the subset) has one, the overlapping option will be overused— the subset influencing the superset. Batia Snir-Ling190 2 Here, this means we would expect Italian to influence German, with a result of definite marked DPs being acceptable with a generic reading. LANGUAGE-INTERNAL FACTORS: Influence could result from considerations of computational complexity at the syntax-semantic or syntax-pragmatic interface. (Müller & Hulk 2000, Paradis & Navarro 2003, Serratrice et al. 2009) Economy considerations might come into play if comparing bare and definite DPs, but are not relevant given the structure of the experimental task. LANGUAGE-EXTERNAL FACTORS: Alternatively, degree of influence may depend on factors such as language dominance. (Bernardini 2004, Argyri & Sorace 2007, Kupisch 2007, Yip & Matthews 2009) Here, this predicts that the German-dominant bilinguals will have problems with the Italian definite generic reading, as it is not allowed in German outside the singular. GENERICS The various studies into generics have not painted a unified picture of the phenomena. GELMAN & RAMAN 2003: 2-year-old children can discriminate between specific and generic readings PÉREZ-LEROUX, MUNN, SCHMITT & DE IRISH 2004: Monolingual Spanish and English- speaking children ages 4-7 favored generic readings more frequently than adult subjects. But experimental design is critical; there may be inadvertent pragmatic biasing going on. SERRATRICE ET AL. 2009: English-Italian bilinguals showed influence from English in their willingness to accept ungrammatical bare generics in Italian, but behaved similarly to monolinguals in English. In addition, children showed difficulty integrating semantic cues into their interpretation. Does the acceptances of syntactic violations predict an acceptance of semantic violations? What would occur in a case of semantic similarity and semantic difference? EXPERIMENT: PARTICIPANTS Child subjects fell into one of three groups: 22 German-Italian bilingual children, mean age 8;3 32 Italian monolingual children, mean age 8;8 26 German monolingual children, mean age 8;0 In addition, there were 2 groups of adult controls: 23 Italian monolingual adults, mean age 21;3 13 German monolingual adults, mean age 25;3 THE TRUTH VALUE JUDGMENT TASK The task was arranged around a series of 16 pictures, which were shown on computer screen. The pictures formed a coherent story about two children and their adventures. Each picture contained at least two anomalous objects or characters (e.g. witches flying on vacuum cleaners, cubical soccer balls), and each picture had an associated context. Subjects were shown/told the story, and instructed to answer the questions with either a “yes” or “no.” Batia Snir-Ling190 3 Task Questions The story accompanied a set of 44 questions yes/no questions, balanced to expect an equal number of each answer. Questions were both canonical and noncanonical, were one of five types: 1. Control 1(n=6): Questions with demonstrative DPs, meant to show whether subjects distinguished between definite and demonstrative DPs and check that subjects perceived the anomaly in the picture. 2. Control 2 (n=6): “free questions,” meant to control for subjects’ noticing the anomalies. 3. Control 3 (n=4): Unambiguous DPs (German only) Questions with bare nouns (Italian only): Questions with definite DPs combined with semantic cues (qui, “here”) 4. Distractors (n=16) 5. Test (n=12): Questions with subject DPs with definite articles (recall: ambiguous in Italian/specific in German) FIG. 3 TASK QUESTION SAMPLES German Italian 1 Spucken diese Drachen Feuer? Questi draghi sputano fuoco? “Do these dragons spit fire?” “Do these dragons spit fire?” 2 Kannst Du mir sagen, was hier nicht stimmt? Nell'immagine c'è un errore. Ti va di trovarlo insieme? “Can you tell me what’s strange on this picture?” “There’s a mistake on this picture. Shall we find it together?” 3 Haben Ampeln rotes, grünes und blaues Licht? Qui i semafori hanno la luce blu? “Do traffic lights have red, green and blue lights?” “Here the traffic lights have blue lights?” 4 Muss man stehen bleiben, wenn eine Ampel rot ist? Si può partire quando il semaforo è rosso? “Do you have to stop when the light is red?” “Can you move when the traffic light is red?” 5 Spucken die Drachen Wasser? I draghi sputano fuoco? “Do the dragons spit water?” “Do the dragons spit fire?” RESULTS BILINGUALS Batia Snir-Ling190 4 IN GERMAN: Children showed a strong preference (93%) for the generic reading when presented a bare noun (Control 3). Children showed a strong preference (80%) for the specific reading when presented a demonstrative (Control 1) With the definite test case, they were split between generic and specific readings, even though that case is not ambiguous in German! IN ITALIAN: Children showed a strong preference (78%) when presented with the semantic cue qui alongside a definite DP (Control 3) Children showed a strong preference (91%) for the specific reading when presented a demonstrative (Control 1) With the definite test case, children gave both generic and specific responses, but favored the former to the latter (50% vs. 28%). COMPARED TO MONOLINGUALS GERMAN Batia Snir-Ling190 5 When compared to monolingual German children, a similar pattern emerges with respect to the test questions, with the larger split falling along age lines. ITALIAN In Italian, however, there is a noticeable (and statistically significant) difference between monolingual and bilingual subjects (Remember, both readings are grammatical here) DISCUSSION Bilingual and monolingual children behaved similarly. This supports the idea of language separation. German bilingual and monolingual children gave a similar amount of generic answers for definite DPs. The superset-subset hypothesis was not supported. German-dominant children did not seem to have problems with the generic reading. Implications for language-dominance hypothesis inconclusive. .
Recommended publications
  • Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: a Case Study from Koro
    Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro By Jessica Cleary-Kemp A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair Assistant Professor Peter S. Jenks Professor William F. Hanks Summer 2015 © Copyright by Jessica Cleary-Kemp All Rights Reserved Abstract Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro by Jessica Cleary-Kemp Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair In this dissertation a methodology for identifying and analyzing serial verb constructions (SVCs) is developed, and its application is exemplified through an analysis of SVCs in Koro, an Oceanic language of Papua New Guinea. SVCs involve two main verbs that form a single predicate and share at least one of their arguments. In addition, they have shared values for tense, aspect, and mood, and they denote a single event. The unique syntactic and semantic properties of SVCs present a number of theoretical challenges, and thus they have invited great interest from syntacticians and typologists alike. But characterizing the nature of SVCs and making generalizations about the typology of serializing languages has proven difficult. There is still debate about both the surface properties of SVCs and their underlying syntactic structure. The current work addresses some of these issues by approaching serialization from two angles: the typological and the language-specific. On the typological front, it refines the definition of ‘SVC’ and develops a principled set of cross-linguistically applicable diagnostics.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Grammar Matters: Conjugating Verbs in Modern Legal Opinions Robert C
    Loyola University Chicago Law Journal Volume 40 Article 3 Issue 1 Fall 2008 2008 Why Grammar Matters: Conjugating Verbs in Modern Legal Opinions Robert C. Farrell Quinnipiac University School of Law Follow this and additional works at: http://lawecommons.luc.edu/luclj Part of the Law Commons Recommended Citation Robert C. Farrell, Why Grammar Matters: Conjugating Verbs in Modern Legal Opinions, 40 Loy. U. Chi. L. J. 1 (2008). Available at: http://lawecommons.luc.edu/luclj/vol40/iss1/3 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by LAW eCommons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Loyola University Chicago Law Journal by an authorized administrator of LAW eCommons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Why Grammar Matters: Conjugating Verbs in Modern Legal Opinions Robert C. Farrell* I. INTRODUCTION Does it matter that the editors of thirty-three law journals, including those at Yale and Michigan, think that there is a "passive tense"? l Does it matter that the United States Courts of Appeals for the Sixth2 and Eleventh3 Circuits think that there is a "passive mood"? Does it matter that the editors of fourteen law reviews think that there is a "subjunctive tense"?4 Does it matter that the United States Court of Appeals for the District of Columbia Circuit thinks that there is a "subjunctive voice'"? 5 There is, in fact, no "passive tense" or "passive mood." The passive is a voice. 6 There is no "subjunctive voice" or "subjunctive tense." The subjunctive is a mood.7 The examples in the first paragraph suggest that there is widespread unfamiliarity among lawyers and law students * B.A., Trinity College; J.D., Harvard University; Professor, Quinnipiac University School of Law.
    [Show full text]
  • Spanish Verbs and Essential Grammar Review
    Spanish Verbs and Essential Grammar Review Prepared by: Professor Carmen L. Torres-Robles Department of Foreign Languages & Literatures Purdue University Calumet Revised: 1 /2003 Layout by: Nancy J. Tilka CONTENTS Spanish Verbs Introduction 4 Indicative Mood 5 ® simple & compound tenses: present, past, future, conditional Subjunctive Mood 12 ® simple & compound tenses: present, past Ser / Estar 16 Essential Grammar Pronouns 20 Possesive Adjectives and Pronouns 23 Prepositional Pronouns 25 Por versus Para 27 Comparisons / Superlatives 31 Preterite / Imperfect 34 Subjunctive Mood 37 Commands 42 Passive Voice 46 2 Spanish Verbs 3 INTRODUCTION VERBS (VERBOS) MOODS (MODOS) There are three moods or ways to express verbs (actions) in Spanish. 1. Indicative Mood (objective) 2. Subjunctive Mood (subjective) 3. Imperative Mood (commands) INFINITIVES (INFINITIVOS) A verb in the purest form (without a noun or subject pronoun to perform the action) is called an infinitive. The infinitives in English are characterized by the prefix “to” + “verb form”, the Spanish infinitives are identified by the “r” ending. Example estudiar, comer, dormir to study, to eat, to sleep CONJUGATIONS (CONJUGACIONES) Spanish verbs are grouped in three categories or conjugations. 1. Infinitives ending in –ar belong to the first conjugation. (estudiar) 2. Infinitives ending in –er belong to the second conjugation. (comer) 3. Infinitives ending in –ir belong to the third conjugation. (dormir) VERB STRUCTURE (ESTRUCTURA VERBAL) Spanish verbs are divided into three parts. (infinitive: estudiar) 1. Stem or Root (estudi-) 2. Theme Vowel (-a-) 3. "R" Ending (-r) CONJUGATED VERBS (VERBOS CONJUGADOS) To conjugate a verb, a verb must have an explicit subject noun (ex: María), a subject pronoun (yo, tú, usted, él, ella, nosotros(as), vosotros(as), ustedes, ellos, ellas), or an implicit subject, to indicate the performer of the action.
    [Show full text]
  • 30. Tense Aspect Mood 615
    30. Tense Aspect Mood 615 Richards, Ivor Armstrong 1936 The Philosophy of Rhetoric. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Rockwell, Patricia 2007 Vocal features of conversational sarcasm: A comparison of methods. Journal of Psycho- linguistic Research 36: 361−369. Rosenblum, Doron 5. March 2004 Smart he is not. http://www.haaretz.com/print-edition/opinion/smart-he-is-not- 1.115908. Searle, John 1979 Expression and Meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Seddiq, Mirriam N. A. Why I don’t want to talk to you. http://notguiltynoway.com/2004/09/why-i-dont-want- to-talk-to-you.html. Singh, Onkar 17. December 2002 Parliament attack convicts fight in court. http://www.rediff.com/news/ 2002/dec/17parl2.htm [Accessed 24 July 2013]. Sperber, Dan and Deirdre Wilson 1986/1995 Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell. Voegele, Jason N. A. http://www.jvoegele.com/literarysf/cyberpunk.html Voyer, Daniel and Cheryl Techentin 2010 Subjective acoustic features of sarcasm: Lower, slower, and more. Metaphor and Symbol 25: 1−16. Ward, Gregory 1983 A pragmatic analysis of epitomization. Papers in Linguistics 17: 145−161. Ward, Gregory and Betty J. Birner 2006 Information structure. In: B. Aarts and A. McMahon (eds.), Handbook of English Lin- guistics, 291−317. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Rachel Giora, Tel Aviv, (Israel) 30. Tense Aspect Mood 1. Introduction 2. Metaphor: EVENTS ARE (PHYSICAL) OBJECTS 3. Polysemy, construal, profiling, and coercion 4. Interactions of tense, aspect, and mood 5. Conclusion 6. References 1. Introduction In the framework of cognitive linguistics we approach the grammatical categories of tense, aspect, and mood from the perspective of general cognitive strategies.
    [Show full text]
  • The Differences Between Spoken and Written Grammar in English, in Comparison with Vietnamese1
    GIST EDUCATION AND LEARNING RESEARCH JOURNAL. ISSN 1692-5777. NO. 11, (JULY - DECEMBER) 2015. pp. 138-153. The Differences between Spoken and Written Grammar in English, in Comparison 1 with Vietnamese Las Diferencias entre la Gramática Oral y Escrita del Idioma Inglés en Comparación con el Idioma Vietnamita Nguyen Cao Thanh2* Tan Trao University, Vietnam Abstract The fundamental point of this paper is to describe and evaluate some differences between spoken and written grammar in English, and compare some of the points with Vietnamese. This paper illustrates that spoken grammar is less rigid than written grammar. Moreover, it highlights the distinction between speaking and writing in terms of subordination and coordination. Further, the different frequency of adverbials and adjectivals between spoken and written language is also compared and analyzed. Keywords: spoken and written grammar, English, Vietnamese 138 1 Received: July 15, 2015 / Accepted: September 10, 2015 2 [email protected] No. 11 (July - December 2015) No. 11 (July - December 2015) CAO Resumen El principal objetivo de este artículo es describir y evaluar algunas diferencias entre la gramática oral y escrita del idioma inglés y comparar algunos aspectos gramaticales con el idioma vietnamita. Esta revisión muestra como la gramática oral es menos rígida que la gramática escrita. Por otra parte, se destaca la distinción entre el hablar y el escribir en términos de subordinación y coordinación. Además, la diferencia en el uso de adverbios y adjetivos entre la gramática oral y escrita también es comparada y analizada. Palabras clave: gramática oral y escrita, inglés, vietnamita Resumo O principal objetivo deste artigo é descrever e avaliar algumas diferenças entre a gramática oral e escrita do idioma inglês e comparar alguns aspectos gramaticais com o idioma vietnamita.
    [Show full text]
  • Adjective in Old English
    Adjective in Old English Adjective in Old English had five grammatical categories: three dependent grammatical categories, i.e forms of agreement of the adjective with the noun it modified – number, gender and case; definiteness – indefiniteness and degrees of comparison. Adjectives had three genders and two numbers. The category of case in adjectives differed from that of nouns: in addition to the four cases of nouns they had one more case, Instrumental. It was used when the adjective served as an attribute to a noun in the Dat. case expressing an instrumental meaning. Weak and Strong Declension Most adjectives in OE could be declined in two ways: according to the weak and to the strong declension. The formal differences between the declensions, as well as their origin, were similar to those of the noun declensions. The strong and weak declensions arose due to the use of several stem-forming suffixes in PG: vocalic a-, o-, u- and i- and consonantal n-. Accordingly, there developed sets of endings of the strong declension mainly coinciding with the endings of a-stems of nouns for adjectives in the Masc. and Neut. and of o-stems – in the Fem. Some endings in the strong declension of adjectives have no parallels in the noun paradigms; they are similar to the endings of pronouns: -um for Dat. sg, -ne for Acc. Sg Masc., [r] in some Fem. and pl endings. Therefore the strong declension of adjectives is sometimes called the ‘pronominal’ declension. As for the weak declension, it uses the same markers as n-stems of nouns except that in the Gen.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Comparing Pluralities Gregory Scontras
    Comparing Pluralities Gregory Scontras ([email protected]) Department of Linguistics, Harvard University Peter Graff ([email protected]) Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Massachusetts Institute of Technology Noah D. Goodman ([email protected]) Department of Psychology, Stanford University Abstract What does it mean to compare sets of objects along a scale, for example by saying “the men are taller than the women”? We explore comparison of pluralities in two experiments, eliciting comparison judgments while varying the properties of the members of each set. We find that a plurality is judged as “bigger” when the mean size of its members is larger than the mean size of the competing plurality. These results are incompatible with previous accounts, in which plural comparison is inferred from many instances of singular comparison between the members of the sets (Matushansky and Ruys, 2006). Our results suggest the need for a type of predication that ascribes properties to plural entities, not just individuals, based on aggregate statistics of their members. More generally, these results support the idea that sets and their properties are actively represented as single units. 1 Keywords: Comparatives; plurality; set-based properties; natural language semantics; mental representations Word count: 3801 1. Introduction When we think and talk about groups of individuals—pluralities—do we represent the collection as a single entity with its own properties? For example, when we say “the red dots are big” is there an aggregate size for the group of red dots to which we refer? In this paper we investigate this question by studying plural comparison—e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • The Grammar of Relative Adjectives and Comparison
    THE GRAMMAR OF RELATIVE ADJECTIVES AND COMPARISON Renate Bartsch FU Berlin and University of California, Los Angeles Theo Vennemann genannt Nierfeld University of California, Los Angeles 1. Earlier Approaches to the Problem of Relative Adjectives and Comparison. Speakers of English know that the following sentences have something in common. (0} John is 5 feet tall. (i) John is tall. (i,) Mary is tall. (2) John is taller than Mary. (3) John is as tall as Mary. (4) John is the tallest of Peter's sons. (5) John is short. (6) John is shorter than Mary. (7) John is as short as Mary. (8) John is the shortest of Peter's sons. Contemporary syntaeticians have tried to account for this knowledge. Most of them have assumed that sentences (i) and (I') are somehow involved in the derivation of sentences (2) - (4), viz. as part of their deep structures from which their more complex surface struc- tures are derived by means of syntactic transformations. This procedure did not, of course, arise accidentally. It is suggested by the relative complexity of the surface structures of these sentences, and is thus a direct result of the preoccupation of contemporary syntac- ticians with surface-syntactic properties of languages. The failure of most contemporary syntacticians to analyze and formulate the proper- ties of relative adjectives and comparison properly is, of course, by no means novel. On the contrary, they perpetuate (or renew) a venerable tradition which started two and a half thousand years ago with Plato's Theaetetus, and may thus consider themselves in excellent company.
    [Show full text]
  • Measuring the Similarity of Grammatical Gender Systems by Comparing Partitions
    Measuring the Similarity of Grammatical Gender Systems by Comparing Partitions Arya D. McCarthy Adina Williams Shijia Liu David Yarowsky Ryan Cotterell Johns Hopkins University, Facebook AI Research, ETH Zurich [email protected], [email protected], [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Abstract A grammatical gender system divides a lex- icon into a small number of relatively fixed grammatical categories. How similar are these gender systems across languages? To quantify the similarity, we define gender systems ex- (a) German, K = 3 (b) Spanish, K = 2 tensionally, thereby reducing the problem of comparisons between languages’ gender sys- Figure 1: Two gender systems partitioning N = 6 con- tems to cluster evaluation. We borrow a rich cepts. German (a) has three communities: Obst (fruit) inventory of statistical tools for cluster evalu- and Gras (grass) are neuter, Mond (moon) and Baum ation from the field of community detection (tree) are masculine, Blume (flower) and Sonne (sun) (Driver and Kroeber, 1932; Cattell, 1945), that are feminine. Spanish (b) has two communities: fruta enable us to craft novel information-theoretic (fruit), luna (moon), and flor are feminine, and cesped metrics for measuring similarity between gen- (grass), arbol (tree), and sol (sun) are masculine. der systems. We first validate our metrics, then use them to measure gender system similarity in 20 languages. Finally, we ask whether our exhaustively divides up the language’s nouns; that gender system similarities alone are sufficient is, the union of gender categories is the entire to reconstruct historical relationships between languages. Towards this end, we make phylo- nominal lexicon.
    [Show full text]
  • Corpus Study of Tense, Aspect, and Modality in Diglossic Speech in Cairene Arabic
    CORPUS STUDY OF TENSE, ASPECT, AND MODALITY IN DIGLOSSIC SPEECH IN CAIRENE ARABIC BY OLA AHMED MOSHREF DISSERTATION Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2012 Urbana, Illinois Doctoral Committee: Professor Elabbas Benmamoun, Chair Professor Eyamba Bokamba Professor Rakesh M. Bhatt Assistant Professor Marina Terkourafi ABSTRACT Morpho-syntactic features of Modern Standard Arabic mix intricately with those of Egyptian Colloquial Arabic in ordinary speech. I study the lexical, phonological and syntactic features of verb phrase morphemes and constituents in different tenses, aspects, moods. A corpus of over 3000 phrases was collected from religious, political/economic and sports interviews on four Egyptian satellite TV channels. The computational analysis of the data shows that systematic and content morphemes from both varieties of Arabic combine in principled ways. Syntactic considerations play a critical role with regard to the frequency and direction of code-switching between the negative marker, subject, or complement on one hand and the verb on the other. Morph-syntactic constraints regulate different types of discourse but more formal topics may exhibit more mixing between Colloquial aspect or future markers and Standard verbs. ii To the One Arab Dream that will come true inshaa’ Allah! عربية أنا.. أميت دمها خري الدماء.. كما يقول أيب الشاعر العراقي: بدر شاكر السياب Arab I am.. My nation’s blood is the finest.. As my father says Iraqi Poet: Badr Shaker Elsayyab iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I’m sincerely thankful to my advisor Prof. Elabbas Benmamoun, who during the six years of my study at UIUC was always kind, caring and supportive on the personal and academic levels.
    [Show full text]
  • Affirmative and Negated Modality*
    Quaderns de Filologia. Estudis lingüístics. Vol. XIV (2009) 169-192 AFFIRMATIVE AND NEGATED MODALITY* Günter Radden Hamburg 0. INTR O DUCTI O N 0.1. Stating the problem Descriptions of modality in English often give the impression that the behaviour of modal verbs is erratic when they occur with negation. A particularly intriguing case is the behaviour of the modal verb must under negation. In its deontic sense, must is used both in affirmative and negated sentences, expressing obligation (1a) and prohibition (1b), respectively. In its epistemic sense, however, must is only used in affirmative sentences expressing necessity (2a). The modal verb that expresses the corresponding negated epistemic modality, i.e. impossibility, is can’t (2b). (1) a. You must switch off your mobile phone. [obligation] b. You mustn’t switch off your mobile phone. [prohibition] (2) a. Your mobile phone must be switched off. [necessity] b. Your mobile phone can’t be switched off. [impossibility] Explanations that have been offered for the use of epistemic can’t are not very helpful. Palmer (1990: 61) argues that mustn’t is not used for the negation of epistemic necessity because can’t is supplied, and Coates (1983: 20) suggests that can’t is used because mustn’t is unavailable. Both “explanations” beg the question: why should can’t be used to denote negated necessity and why should mustn’t be unavailable? Such questions have apparently not been asked for two interrelated reasons: first, the study of modality and negation has been dominated by logic and, secondly, since the modal verbs apparently do * An earlier version of this paper was published in Władysław Chłopicki, Andrzej Pawelec, and Agnieszka Pokojska (eds.), 2007, Cognition in Language: Volume in Honour of Professor Elżbieta Tabakowska, 224-254.
    [Show full text]
  • Equative, Comparative & Superlative Adjectives
    Equative, Comparative & Superlative Adjectives Grammar Practice Worksheets Equative, Comparative & Superlative Adjectives Table of Contents 2 QUICK AND HANDY GRAMMAR REVIEW Descriptive, Comparative, Superlative, and Equative Forms 7 EXERCISE 1: Descriptive Adjectives Complete the sentences. 8 EXERCISE 2: Comparative Adjectives Write the appropriate adjectives. 9 EXERCISE 3: Comparative Adjectives Write the appropriate adjectives. 10 EXERCISE 4: Superlative Adjectives Unscramble the sentences. 11 EXERCISE 5: Equative and Non-Equative Adjectives Write the appropriate adjectives. 12 EXERCISE 6: Irregular Adjectives Fill in the missing words. 13 EXERCISE 7: Irregular Adjectives Fill in the missing words. 15 EXERCISE 8: Comparison Chart I Analyze the chart and write sentences. 17 EXERCISE 9: Comparison Chart II Analyze the chart and write sentences. 19 ANSWER KEY Copyright 2016, Red River Press Inc. For use by members of ESL Library in accordance with membership terms. 1 Equative, Comparative & Superlative Adjectives Grammar Practice Worksheets Quick and Handy Grammar Review DESCRIPTIVE, COMPARATIVE, SUPERLATIVE, AND EQUATIVE FORMS Adjectives have four forms. A. Descriptive Form The descriptive form is used to • The excited travellers visited a beautiful park. describe one noun or pronoun. • The suntanned lifeguard protected the young swimmers. Remember that adjectives are placed either immediately before • The Australian woman was tall and slender. the noun or after a form of the • It is dangerous to climb a mountain in the winter. verb to be. B. Comparative Form The comparative form is used to compare two nouns or pronouns. For one-syllable adjectives, • Leyla is shorter than her sister. add -er. For two-syllable adjectives • Claudia’s hair is curlier than her mother’s hair.
    [Show full text]