Where Does One Language End and the Next Begin?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Where Does One Language End and the Next Begin? “SORRY, COULD YOU REPEAT THAT, PLEASE?!” – WHERE DOES ONE LANGUAGE END AND THE NEXT BEGIN? Angela Kluge1 2007 CONTENT 1. Introduction 2. Languages and dialects 3. Bilingualism, intelligibility, and bidialectism 3.1. Bilingualism 3.2. Intelligibility 3.3. Bidialectism 4. Intelligibility and affinity 5. Where does one language end and the next begin? 6. Summary 7. References This paper considers the question of why speakers of different dialects belonging to the same language group do not always understand each other, and explores the question of how best to determine whether different speech communities belong to the same language group. In this context three pertinent issues are considered: (1) the notions of language and dialect, (2) the notions of bilingualism, intelligibility, and bidialectism, and (3) the notion of affinity (i.e. feeling of interconnectedness) and how it impacts intelligibility. The discussion of these issues demonstrates that both linguistic and nonlinguistic factors need to be taken into account when considering the boundaries of a language group, namely structural similarity, intelligibility, and affinity, with affinity being the most decisive factor as it impacts intelligibility and may override linguistic considerations. Brown’s (1998) “ethnolinguistic” or “broad standardization” model is presented as a relevant framework for considering the question of where one language ends and the next begins.. This model identifies a language group as a group of structurally related speech varieties that recognize one common variety as their transdialectal standard, or “main dialect” based on the factors of ethnolinguistic identity, intelligibility, and relatedness. The identified main dialect in turn defines the entire language group. In the context of nonstandardized language chain situations, however, it may not be possible to identify a main dialect based on these three factors of ethnolinguistic identity, intelligibility, and relatedness. Instead, the question presents itself, whether for these situations it might be more useful to consider the language identification and standardization process in terms of relatedness, affinity, and exposure time. Given the dynamic nature of language situations and the sociolinguistic changes that the standardization process is most likely to cause, the assessment process regarding which speech varieties accept and understand the main dialect also needs to be dynamic and, furthermore, ongoing. 1 Biblographical details: Kluge, Angela. 2007. “Sorry, could you repeat that, please?!” – Where does one language end and the next begin? Paper presented at the SIL-Indonesia Eastern Indonesia Malay Seminar, Sentani, 3-6 September 2007. Kluge_A_2007_Where does one language end and the next begins.doc p. 1 / 24 1. INTRODUCTION “Sorry, could you repeat that, please?!” – Have you ever had the experience of talking to someone from an area different from your own but where people supposedly speak the same language as you and yet you did not understand what she was saying? “Sorry, could you repeat that again, please?!” A situation like that could easily happen in Germany, when someone from the Rheinland, for example from Köln, travels south and for the first time in her life meets speakers of Schwäbisch from the Schwäbische Alp south of Stuttgart or speakers of Bayrisch from the area around München. And yet, in Germany all Germans supposedly speak German. Similar situations may happen in the Anglophone world when an English speaker, for example from Cumberland in northwestern England, meets an English speaker from the Deep South (e.g. Alabama) in the USA or this English speaker from the Deep South meets an English speaker from the Australian outback (i.e. Australia’s interior region). And yet, in the Anglophone world speakers supposedly speak the same English language. Thus, do Kölsch and Schwäbisch belong to the same language or are they, in fact, two different languages, and are Cumberland English, ‘Deep South’ English, and ‘Outback’ English also, in fact, distinct languages? Most commonly these varieties are referred to as dialects of German and English, respectively, whereas English and German would be considered different languages. So why do speakers of different dialects not understand each other although they supposedly speak the same language? What about the Kölner who do not understand the Schwaben from the Schwäbische Alp, and what about the Cumberlanders who do not understand the Deep South English speakers when they first meet? When considering the question of whether different speech communities belong to the same language group, three issues need to be examined more closely: (1) the notions of language and dialect (Section 2), (2) the notions of bilingualism, intelligibility, and bidialectism (Section 3), and (3) the notion of affinity and how it impacts intelligibility (Section 4). Following these considerations, Section 5 presents Brown’s (1998) “ethnolinguistic” or “broad standardization” model, which considers the various factors that need to be taken into account with respect to the question of where one language ends and the next begins. Section 5 summarizes the most important issues that were raised in this paper, followed by a list of references (Section 6). 2. LANGUAGES AND DIALECTS There are a variety of usages of the terms language and dialect, not only in the popular but also in the formal linguistic realm. For some, language refers to the particular speech variety that has been chosen to be developed, that is, to be written and to serve as the standard for a whole group of dialects in terms of orthography, grammar and lexicon and, in many cases, also in terms of pronunciation, while dialects lack such standardization (see Brown 1998). Based on this point of view, Deutsch or Hochdeutsch is regarded a language since it has a standardized written form which is acknowledged by speakers of all German speech varieties, whereas Schwäbisch or Bayrisch are (nonstandardized) dialects of Deutsch. Other linguists employ the term dialect to refer to a set of speech varieties that are descendents of the same parent language with language in the superordinate position within a hierarchy of linguistic taxonomy, while dialects are in a subordinate position and considered subdivisions of languages. Based on this definition, all Germanic languages are regarded as dialects, that is descendants, of the Proto Germanic language. (Bussmann 2000:125) Kluge_A_2007_Where does one language end and the next begins.doc p. 2 / 24 In distinguishing language from dialect, some apply primarily linguistic criteria and focus on structural relatedness and “similarity-based” intelligibility (Simons 1979:62). That is, due to the structural similarity or genetic relationship of the speech varieties involved, speakers of different dialects of the same language can understand each other without having had to learn the other variety. Others would argue that linguistic criteria are not sufficient for making an adequate distinction between languages and dialects. Instead, the distinction needs to be based on nonlinguistic criteria which take into account socio-political, cultural, and historical factors. Based on the main criteria used to distinguish languages from dialects, different conclusions are possible in terms of language identification for a given area. Good examples for this are some of the language situations of Europe and of China. The European language situation is characterized by a number of different geographical dialect continua in which related speech varieties form dialect chains that may cover large geographical areas (see Figure 1). Figure 1: Dialect continua in Europe (Crystal 2001:9) In such language continua, or as Bloomfield designates them, dialect areas, typical communication patterns emerge (Bloomfield 2001:51): In a country like France, Italy, or Germany … every village or, at most, every group of two or three villages, has its own dialect. The differences between neighboring local dialects are usually small, but recognizable. The villagers are ready to tell in what way their neighbors’ speech differs from theirs, and often tease their neighbors about these peculiarities. The difference from place to place is small, but, as one travels in any one direction, the differences accumulate, until speakers, say from opposite ends of the country, cannot understand each other, although there is no sharp line of linguistic demarcation between the places where they live. Any such geographic area of gradual transitions is called a dialect area. The Germanic continuum, for example, covers Dutch as spoken in the Netherlands, Flemish as spoken in Belgium, and German as spoken in Germany, Austria, Switzerland, and Liechtenstein Kluge_A_2007_Where does one language end and the next begins.doc p. 3 / 24 (see Figure 2). Within this continuum neighboring groups understand each other well – even across borders, such as the Dutch-German border. Here, on either side of the border regional dialects are spoken which belong to the Rhenisch-Maas group of dialects, namely Limburgisch (Gordon 2005). Speakers of the respective Dutch and German dialects understand each other sufficiently well and – based on linguistic criteria – could therefore be considered as speakers of the same language. In fact, German speakers from near the Dutch border may understand speakers from the Dutch side of the border much better than they understand speakers of southern German dialects, e.g. Schwäbisch from the Schwäbische
Recommended publications
  • A Sociolinguistic Profile of the Kyoli (Cori) [Cry] Language of Kaduna State, Nigeria
    DigitalResources Electronic Survey Report 2020-012 A Sociolinguistic Profile of the Kyoli (Cori) [cry] Language of Kaduna State, Nigeria Ken Decker, John Muniru, Julius Dabet, Benard Abraham, Jonah Innocent A Sociolinguistic Profile of the Kyoli (Cori) [cry] Language of Kaduna State, Nigeria Ken Decker, John Muniru, Julius Dabet, Benard Abraham, Jonah Innocent SIL International® 2020 SIL Electronic Survey Report 2020-012, October 2020 © 2020 SIL International® All rights reserved Data and materials collected by researchers in an era before documentation of permission was standardized may be included in this publication. SIL makes diligent efforts to identify and acknowledge sources and to obtain appropriate permissions wherever possible, acting in good faith and on the best information available at the time of publication. Abstract This report describes a sociolinguistic survey conducted among the Kyoli-speaking communities in Jaba Local Government Area (LGA), Kaduna State, in central Nigeria. The Ethnologue (Eberhard et al. 2020a) classifies Kyoli [cry] as a Niger-Congo, Atlantic Congo, Volta-Congo, Benue-Congo, Plateau, Western, Northwestern, Hyamic language. During the survey, it was learned that the speakers of the language prefer to spell the name of their language <Kyoli>, which is pronounced as [kjoli] or [çjoli]. They refer to speakers of the language as Kwoli. We estimate that there may be about 7,000 to 8,000 speakers of Kyoli, which is most if not all the ethnic group. The goals of this research included gaining a better understanding of the role of Kyoli and other languages in the lives of the Kwoli people. Our data indicate that Kyoli is used at a sustainable level of orality, EGIDS 6a.
    [Show full text]
  • Some Principles of the Use of Macro-Areas Language Dynamics &A
    Online Appendix for Harald Hammarstr¨om& Mark Donohue (2014) Some Principles of the Use of Macro-Areas Language Dynamics & Change Harald Hammarstr¨om& Mark Donohue The following document lists the languages of the world and their as- signment to the macro-areas described in the main body of the paper as well as the WALS macro-area for languages featured in the WALS 2005 edi- tion. 7160 languages are included, which represent all languages for which we had coordinates available1. Every language is given with its ISO-639-3 code (if it has one) for proper identification. The mapping between WALS languages and ISO-codes was done by using the mapping downloadable from the 2011 online WALS edition2 (because a number of errors in the mapping were corrected for the 2011 edition). 38 WALS languages are not given an ISO-code in the 2011 mapping, 36 of these have been assigned their appropri- ate iso-code based on the sources the WALS lists for the respective language. This was not possible for Tasmanian (WALS-code: tsm) because the WALS mixes data from very different Tasmanian languages and for Kualan (WALS- code: kua) because no source is given. 17 WALS-languages were assigned ISO-codes which have subsequently been retired { these have been assigned their appropriate updated ISO-code. In many cases, a WALS-language is mapped to several ISO-codes. As this has no bearing for the assignment to macro-areas, multiple mappings have been retained. 1There are another couple of hundred languages which are attested but for which our database currently lacks coordinates.
    [Show full text]
  • Ikwerre Intergroup Relations and Its Impact on Their Culture
    83 AFRREV VOL. 11 (2), S/NO 46, APRIL, 2017 AN INTERNATIONAL MULTI-DISCIPLINARY JOURNAL, ETHIOPIA AFRREV VOL. 11 (2), SERIAL NO. 46, APRIL, 2017: 83-98 ISSN 1994-9057 (Print) ISSN 2070-0083 (Online) DOI : http://dx.doi.org/10.4314/afrrev.v11i2.7 Ikwerre Intergroup Relations and its Impact on Their Culture Chinda, C. Izeoma Department of Foundation Studies Port Harcourt Polytechnic, Rumuola Phone No: +234 703 667 4797 E-mail: [email protected] --------------------------------------------------------------------------- Abstract This paper examined the intergroup relations between the Ikwerre of the Niger Delta, South-South geopolitical zone of Nigeria and its impact on their culture. It analyzed the Ikwerre relations with her Kalabari and Okrika coastal neighbours, as well as the Etche, Eleme, Ekpeye, Ogba Abua and the Igbo of Imo state hinterland neighbours. The paper concluded that the internal developments which were stimulated by their contacts impacted significantly on their culture. Key words: Ikwerre, Intergroup Relations, Developments, Culture, Neighbour. Introduction Geographical factors aided the movement of people from one ecological zone to another in migration or interdependent relationships of trade exchange. These exchanges and contacts occurred even in pre-colonial times. The historical roots of inter-group relations of the Ikwerre with her neighbours, dates back to pre-colonial times but became prevalent from the 1850 onward when the Atlantic trade became emphatic on agrarian products as raw materials to the industrial western world. This galvanized the hitherto existing inter-group contact between the Ikwerre and her neighbouring potentates. Copyright © International Association of African Researchers and Reviewers, 2006-2017: www.afrrevjo.net.
    [Show full text]
  • Annex H. Summary of the Early Grade Reading Materials Survey in Senegal
    Annex H. Summary of the Early Grade Reading Materials Survey in Senegal Geography and Demographics 196,722 square Size: kilometers (km2) Population: 14 million (2015) Capital: Dakar Urban: 44% (2015) Administrative 14 regions Divisions: Religion: 95% Muslim 4% Christian 1% Traditional Source: Central Intelligence Agency (2015). Note: Population and percentages are rounded. Literacy Projected 2013 Primary School 2015 Age Population (aged 2.2 million Literacy a a 7–12 years): Rates: Overall Male Female Adult (aged 2013 Primary School 56% 68% 44% 84%, up from 65% in 1999 >15 years) GER:a Youth (aged 2013 Pre-primary School 70% 76% 64% 15%,up from 3% in 1999 15–24 years) GER:a Language: French Mean: 18.4 correct words per minute When: 2009 Oral Reading Fluency: Standard deviation: 20.6 Sample EGRA Where: 11 regions 18% zero scores Resultsb 11% reading with ≥60% Reading comprehension Who: 687 P3 students Comprehension: 52% zero scores Note: EGRA = Early Grade Reading Assessment; GER = Gross Enrollment Rate; P3 = Primary Grade 3. Percentages are rounded. a Source: UNESCO (2015). b Source: Pouezevara et al. (2010). Language Number of Living Languages:a 210 Major Languagesb Estimated Populationc Government Recognized Statusd 202 DERP in Africa—Reading Materials Survey Final Report 47,000 (L1) (2015) French “Official” language 3.9 million (L2) (2013) “National” language Wolof 5.2 million (L1) (2015) de facto largest LWC Pulaar 3.5 million (L1) (2015) “National” language Serer-Sine 1.4 million (L1) (2015) “National” language Maninkakan (i.e., Malinké) 1.3 million (L1) (2015) “National” language Soninke 281,000 (L1) (2015) “National” language Jola-Fonyi (i.e., Diola) 340,000 (L1) “National” language Balant, Bayot, Guñuun, Hassanya, Jalunga, Kanjaad, Laalaa, Mandinka, Manjaaku, “National” languages Mankaañ, Mënik, Ndut, Noon, __ Oniyan, Paloor, and Saafi- Saafi Note: L1 = first language; L2 = second language; LWC = language of wider communication.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Classification in the Ethnologue and Its Consequences (Paper)
    Sarah E. Cornwell The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada Language Classification in The Ethnologue and its Consequences (Paper) Abstract: The Ethnologue is a widely used classificatory standard for the world’s 7000+ natural languages. However, the motives and processes used by The Ethnologue’s governing body, SIL International, have come under criticism by linguists. This paper investigates how The Ethnologue answers the question “What is a language?” through the theoretical lens presented by Bowker and Star in Sorting Things Out (1999) and presents some consequences of those classificatory decisions. 1. Introduction Language is central to human culture and experience. It is the central core of our communicative capacity, running through all media of communication. Due to this essential role, there is a need to classify and count languages and their speakers1. Governments need to communicate with citizens, libraries need to catalogue books by language, and search engines need to return webpages in the appropriate language for each searcher. For these reasons among others, there is a strong institutional need for a standardized classification structure and labelling system for languages. The most widespread current classificatory infrastructure for languages is based on The Ethnologue. This essay will critically evaluate The Ethnologue using the Foucauldian theory developed for investigating classification structures by Bowker and Star (1999) in Sorting Things Out: Classification and its Consequences. 2. The Ethnologue The Ethnologue is a catalogue of all the world’s languages. First compiled in 1951, The Ethnologue is currently in its 20th edition and contains descriptions of 7099 living languages (2017). Since 1997, The Ethnologue has been available freely online and widely accessible (Simons & Fennig, 2017).
    [Show full text]
  • Mapping India's Language and Mother Tongue Diversity and Its
    Mapping India’s Language and Mother Tongue Diversity and its Exclusion in the Indian Census Dr. Shivakumar Jolad1 and Aayush Agarwal2 1FLAME University, Lavale, Pune, India 2Centre for Social and Behavioural Change, Ashoka University, New Delhi, India Abstract In this article, we critique the process of linguistic data enumeration and classification by the Census of India. We map out inclusion and exclusion under Scheduled and non-Scheduled languages and their mother tongues and their representation in state bureaucracies, the judiciary, and education. We highlight that Census classification leads to delegitimization of ‘mother tongues’ that deserve the status of language and official recognition by the state. We argue that the blanket exclusion of languages and mother tongues based on numerical thresholds disregards the languages of about 18.7 million speakers in India. We compute and map the Linguistic Diversity Index of India at the national and state levels and show that the exclusion of mother tongues undermines the linguistic diversity of states. We show that the Hindi belt shows the maximum divergence in Language and Mother Tongue Diversity. We stress the need for India to officially acknowledge the linguistic diversity of states and make the Census classification and enumeration to reflect the true Linguistic diversity. Introduction India and the Indian subcontinent have long been known for their rich diversity in languages and cultures which had baffled travelers, invaders, and colonizers. Amir Khusru, Sufi poet and scholar of the 13th century, wrote about the diversity of languages in Northern India from Sindhi, Punjabi, and Gujarati to Telugu and Bengali (Grierson, 1903-27, vol.
    [Show full text]
  • The Components of Sustainable Multilingual Education Programs
    A Two-Way Bridge The Components of Sustainable Multilingual Education Programs Studies demonstrate that learning is most effective when the instruction is received in the language the learner knows best. This simple truth extends from basic reading and writing skills in the first language to second language acquisition. In multilingual education programs (MLE) that start with the mother tongue, learners use their own language for learning in the early grades, while also learning the official language as a classroom subject. As learners gain competence in understanding, speaking, reading and writing the language of education, teachers begin using it for instruction. This instructional bridge between the community language and the language of wider communication enables learners—children and adults alike—to meet their broader multilingual goals while retaining their local language and culture. This booklet addresses several important aspects of MLE: ■ The voices of ethnolinguistic minority communities are often not heard. Therefore, advocacy is appropriate for these communities to meet their MLE needs. ■ Conventional instructional methods are not adequate for MLE programs. Educators at both community and national levels need to develop their capacity to design and implement MLE programs. ■ Developing a writing system for a non-dominant language is a challenging but essential early step in developing an MLE program. ■ MLE not only requires the commitment and resources of the local community, but also the resources and expertise available from government agencies, NGOs or others. Resource linking brings the partners together so that each one contributes its own particular resources. ■ MLE gives children and adults a firm foundation for continuing to learn throughout their lives.
    [Show full text]
  • English Planning for Global Cooperation in Sign Language Development Through Networks, Finland, the US, Colombia and Japan Consultants and Resources
    SIL International 2009 Partners in Language Development UPDATE YEARS 1934–2009 “No one has ever seen a book in our language before.” Until this year, the Koda community in Bangladesh did not have a written form of their language. Some Koda people thought it was impossible to write their language. A cooperative education and development program for this community was started by Food for the Hungry and SIL. In 2009, a group of eager Koda men met several times with SIL staff to make decisions about a writing system for their language. The men—some of whom had only attended school for two years—participated in an orthography workshop with SIL consultants and began to write in their own language. They worked together to identify the speech sounds that are the same as in Bangla (the national language), the several sounds that are different, and how to Each workshop represent each sound in an alphabet—the first step towards uniform spelling. participant wrote When workshop participants were asked to write stories about their daily lives and a story in Koda and culture, they responded, “We have never written a story before.” But by the end of made his own book. These men have the workshop, each participant had written a short story and made his own book to started to fulfill their take back to his village—an achievement that gave them confidence and increased community’s desire to self-esteem. protect and preserve SIL grew out of one man’s concern for people speaking languages that lacked written their language.
    [Show full text]
  • A Sketch of the Linguistic Geography of Signed Languages in the Caribbean1,2
    OCCASIONAL PAPER No. 38 A SKETCH OF THE LINGUISTIC GEOGRAPHY OF SIGNED LANGUAGES IN THE CARIBBEAN Ben Braithwaite The University of the West Indies, St. Augustine June 2017 SCL OCCASIONAL PAPERS PAPER NUMBER 38—JUNE 2017 Edited by Ronald Kephart (2014–2016) and Joseph T. Farquharson (2016–2018), SCL Publications Officers Copy editing by Sally J. Delgado and Ronald Kephart Proofreading by Paulson Skerritt and Sulare Telford EDITORIAL BOARD Joseph T. Farquharson The University of the West Indies, Mona (Chair) Janet L. Donnelly College of the Bahamas David Frank SIL International Ronald Kephart University of North Florida Salikoko S. Mufwene University of Chicago Ian E. Robertson The University of the West Indies, St. Augustine Geraldine Skeete The University of the West Indies, St. Augustine Donald C. Winford Ohio State University PUBLISHED BY THE SOCIETY FOR CARIBBEAN LINGUISTICS (SCL) c/o Department of Language, Linguistics and Philosophy, The University of the West Indies, Mona campus, Kingston 7, Jamaica. <www.scl-online.net> © 2017 Ben Braithwaite. All rights reserved. Not to be reproduced in any form without the written permission of the author. ISSN 1726–2496 THE LINGUISTIC GEOGRAPHY OF SIGNED LANGUAGES 3 A Sketch of the Linguistic Geography of Signed Languages in the Caribbean1,2 Ben Braithwaite The University of the West Indies, St. Augustine 1. Introduction HE Caribbean… is the location of almost every type of linguistic “Tphenomenon, and of every type of language situation. For example, trade and contact jargons, creole languages and dialects, ethnic vernaculars, and regional and nonstandard dialects are all spoken. There are also ancestral languages used for religious purposes…, regional standards, and international standards.
    [Show full text]
  • CALLS for PAPERS to the INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE /HUMBOLT KOLLEG on PROSODY and LANGUAGE TECHNOLOGY: CHALLENGES and PROSPECTS, ABIDJAN MAY 4Th to MAY 9Th 2014
    CALLS FOR PAPERS TO THE INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE /HUMBOLT KOLLEG ON PROSODY AND LANGUAGE TECHNOLOGY: CHALLENGES AND PROSPECTS, ABIDJAN MAY 4th to MAY 9th 2014. 1. DESCRIPTION OF CONTENT: PROSODY AS A CORE COMPONENT OF LANGUAGE AND SPEECH Prosodic systems in language range from the melodies and rhythms of intonation in sentences and dialogue on the one hand to intricate duration and tonal patterns of words. Physically, prosody is commonly defined as the variation of speech sounds and pitch patterns in time and has long been considered in traditional linguistics as peripheral to the core communicative functions of speech acts. Research into African tonal languages has played a highly significant but often unappreciated role in uncovering the significance and intricacies of prosody, revealing autosegments of tone, intonation, stress, accent, vowel harmony, nasality and other phonological features which pattern in a quasi-independent fashion in speech, and are represented as hierarchically organized parallel phonetic and phonological information streams or tiers. Prosody has been studied from the perspectives of many different disciplines: literature, (particularly poetry and drama rhetoric), musicology, cognitive and behavioral psychology, the study of language acquisition and language and speech pathologies, forensic linguistics, computational and mathematical linguistics, language typology and historical linguistic reconstruction, and engineering disciplines concerned with the speech synthesis and recognition interfaces associated with modern mobile devices. These interdisciplinary developments have also touched research on African languages, though mainly in separate universities and by individuals working without the extensive laboratory support of European, American and East Asian research groups. A strength of research on the distinctive prosodic attributes of African languages is that results have been based on extensive and detailed fieldwork on a very large variety of languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Tivoid Survey
    Tivoid Survey Alan Starr and Clark Regnier SIL International 2008 SIL Electronic Survey Report 2008-022, December 2008 Copyright © 2008 Alan Starr, Clark Regnier, and SIL International All rights reserved 2 CONTENTS Abstract 1 Background 1.1 Classification 1.2 Purposes 1.3 Selection of Sites 1.4 Description of the Area 1.5 Team and Timing 2 Procedures 2.1 Wordlists 2.2 Recorded Text Testing (RTT) 2.3 Sociolinguistic Questionnaires 2.4 Bilingual Questionnaires 2.5 Group Interviews 3 Ipulo-Eman-Caka 3.1 Lexicostatistics 3.2 Ipulo and Eman 3.3 Caka and Eman 4 Icheve 4.1 Linguistic Results and Analysis 4.2 Sociolinguistic and Multilingual Profile 4.3 Conclusions and Decisions 5 Proposed Changes to Ethnologue and ALCAM Appendix 1: Tivoid Languages and Their Neighbors in Cameroon [ALCAM p. 369] Appendix 2: Group Interview Form Appendix 3: Sociolinguistic Questionnaire Appendix 4: 1. (Omitted) 2. Multilingualism 3. Development of the language Appendix 5: Bilingual Proficiency Questionnaire References 3 Abstract This 1990 survey investigates two Tiv-related languages: Assumbo and Mesaka. The purpose is to recommend which varieties should be standardized, based on both linguistic data (shared lexical percentages and inherent intelligibility) and sociolinguistic data (language use and attitudes). In particular, since the Assumbo dialect, Ipulo, as spoken in the village of Tinta is planned for development, there was need to discover the extent to which a written form of this dialect could be used. We investigated the use of Pidgin in key domains to determine community use and proficiency. [This report has not been peer reviewed.] 1 Background 1.1 Classification The Tiv-related languages under study in this survey are identified by the Ethnologue as Assumbo (Asumbo, Badzumbo) and Mesaka (Messaga, Iyon, Banagere).
    [Show full text]
  • University of Port Harcourt Across Frontiers: Comparative Literature And
    UNIVERSITY OF PORT HARCOURT ACROSS FRONTIERS: COMPARATIVE LITERATURE AND NATIONAL INTEGRATION An Inaugural Lecture Delivered At the University of Port Harcourt on 21 April, 1994 By Chidi Tom Maduka Professor of Comparative Literature Co-ordinator, Comparative Literature Programme, University of Port Harcourt Inaugural Lecture Series No. 14 1994 DEDICATION To all the protagonist of inter-ethnic harmony pitched against a small clique of ambitious and egomaniacal damages despicably exploiting ethnic loyalties for their ignoble, selfish and avaricious end. Chidi Tom Maduka Across Fronties: Comparative Literature and National Integration Kùkùkù Kùkù KùkùKù kùkù KùkùKùkù Kùkù KùkùKùkù Kùkù (The song of a bird passing judgment on human behaviour) (Human beings are crazy Human beings are crazy Where do they see boundaries? Where do they see boundaries?) Mr. Vice-Chancellor, Sir, I would like to pay homage to the big masquerades of my clan which have performed here on the village square. We all vividly remember the first one that opened the festivity of inaugural lectures at the University of Port Harcourt. It had two intriguing faces- one in front and the other at the back. The dance steps so mesmerized the spectators that no one could precisely say which side of the masquerade was the front. That was the History-Masquerade which had Professor E. J. Alagoa as the chief attendant. Its movements, we have been told, were dictated by the nature of the discipline which probes into the past in order to illuminate the present and suggest pathways for the future. The topic was “The Python’s Eye: The Past in the Living Present”.
    [Show full text]