Growing up and Growing Apart: a Developmental Meta-Analysis of Twin Studies

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Growing up and Growing Apart: a Developmental Meta-Analysis of Twin Studies Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1990. Vol. 107, No. 2, 226-237 0033-2909/90/S00.75 Growing Up and Growing Apart: A Developmental Meta-Analysis of Twin Studies Kathleen McCartney Monica J. Harris University of New Hampshire University of Kentucky Frank Bernieri Oregon State University Developmental change in twin similarity was examined with age contrasts in a meta-analysis of twin studies from 1967 through 1985. Intraclass re were coded from 103 papers that included data for monozygotic or dizygotic twins, or for both, on personality or intelligence variables. Analyses indi- cated that there was a general tendency for some intraclass rs to decrease with age. In other words, as twins grow up, they grow apart. There were also developmental differences associated with compo- nents of variance for heritability, the shared environment, and the nonshared environment. Mecha- nisms through which the nonshared environment may operate are discussed. The purpose of this article is twofold. First, we offer meta- meta-analysis include (a) obtaining a combined estimate from analysis as a tool for developmental hypothesis testing. The field all available evidence, (b) identification of robust findings, and of developmental psychology is often criticized for being not a (c) the ability to determine whether age interacts with other study of development but rather a study of the child at a given variables of interest (e.g., methodological moderators). How- age (McCall, 1977). Meta-analysis can be used to combine stud- ever, meta-analysis does not permit the same degree of causal ies of subjects at different ages, as in a cross-sectional study. inference found in longitudinal studies (Cronbach. 1984). Spe- Second, we examined whether there are age-related differences cifically, differences among age groups can result from either age in twin similarity. On the basis of recent behavior genetics the- effects or cohort effects, the latter of which is only interesting ory and data (McCall, 1983; Plomin & Daniels, 1987; Rowe & from a historical perspective. Thus, like most methodologies, Plomin, 1981), we predict that as twins grow up, they grow developmental meta-analysis has both strengths and weak- apart; that is, twin similarity should decline with age. The im- nesses. portance of the nonshared environment is presumed to in- Twin studies are based on a philosophy and methodology that crease over time, as children's experiences move outside the make them perfect candidates for meta-analysis. Researchers family. have traditionally studied twins to compute heritability coeffi- Researchers have begun to realize that meta-analysis can be cients. The basic procedure is the same in all twin studies: First, used to summarize a body of research through the computation a sample of twins is obtained. Second, the zygosity of the twin of effect-size estimates. However, most researchers may be un- pairs is determined (i.e., monozygotic, or MZ, twins share aware that specific hypotheses can be tested by performing con- 100% of their genes and dizygotic, or DZ, twins share, on aver- trast analyses on the effect sizes of independent studies (Rosen- age, 50% of their genes). Third, individual difference measures thai, 1984). A developmental meta-analysis is thus denned as a are administered to all twins. Fourth, an intraclass correlation meta-analysis with age contrasts. between twins is computed, once for the MZ twins and once for The advantages of being able to perform age contrasts within the DZ twins. The expectation is that if a given trait or ability has a genetic component, then the intraclass correlation will be higher for the MZ twins than for the DZ twins. This research was supported by grants from the Harvard Graduate Consequently, every twin study contains the data for, and ide- Society and by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH41807 awarded to Kathleen McCartney, who also acknowledges the support ally should report, the MZ and DZ intraclass correlations. of the Whitcomb family. These correlations can easily be used in a number of meta-ana- We would like to thank Robert Rosenthal for frequent statistical con- lytic procedures, including age contrasts; however, this has not sultations and Maura Kerrigan for her help in coding articles. We would been done. Typically, the twin method has been used to estimate also like to thank the reviewers, David Buss, Elizabeth Jordan, Carolyn components of variance for genetic and environmental influ- Mebert, Edward J. O'Brien, Sandra Scarr, Deborah Vandell, members ence. The assumptions of the method continue to be questioned of Murray Straus's research seminar, and especially Victor Benassi for (Goldsmith, 1983; Hoffman, 1985). The most critical assump- their comments on an earlier draft of this article. This article was pre- tion is that the degree of environmental similarity is the same sented by Kathleen McCartney at the International Conference on Psy- chology in Sydney, Australia, August 1988. for MZ and DZ twins. If MZ twins, because they look alike, Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kath- experience more similar environments than DZ twins, then leen McCartney, Department of Psychology, Conant Hall, University of heritability computations would also reflect this environmental New Hampshire, Durham, New Hampshire 03824. similarity (Plornin, DeFries, & McClearn, 1980). 226 GROWING UP AND GROWING APART 227 However, twin studies are useful for purposes other than the Table 1 estimation of heritability. Twin studies are actually within-fam- Categorization of Identified Studies From Literature ily studies, in that more than one member of a family is studied. Search (Based on Abstracts) Longitudinal, within-family studies are needed to assess the rel- Category n ative importance of shared and nonshared experiences of sib- lings. Codable 130' 17 Several longitudinal, within-family studies do, in fact, suggest Uncodable that the older siblings become, the less like one another they Anecdotal or case studies 120 16 11 appear on various indexes of intelligence and personality. For Theoretical 81 No relevant data 114 15 example, three adoption studies have shown that adopted sib- Foreign language 83 11 lings, who are genetically unrelated, look moderately similar on Biological 141 18 intelligence tests and personality inventories in early childhood Animal 9 1 but do not resemble one another at all by later adolescence, Predated 9 1 Dissertations 52 7 presumably because the effect of the shared environment wanes Miscellaneous 25 3 (Kent, 1985; Loehlin, Willerman, & Horn, 1987;Scarr&Wein- berg, 1978). The Louisville twin study (Wilson, 1983) showed Total 764 100 that DZ twins became less similar over time on IQ. However, • Of these 130 papers, 103 were actually used in the meta-analysis. The MZ twins became more similar over time, perhaps because they remaining 27 papers did not contain data that contained intraclass rs seek and are exposed to highly similar environments. or that could be converted to intraclass rs. These 27 papers were receded In addition, a preliminary report of the Minnesota study of to the no-relevant-data category after the papers were read. twins reared apart and together showed that the common family environment accounted for little variance on 12 of 14 personal- were excluded because intraclass rs were not included and could not be ity scales (Tellegen, Lykken, Bouchard, Wilcox, Segal, & Rich, calculated; (d) foreign language papers; (e) studies on psychopathology 1988). Because the "environment," broadly calculated, did ac- or biology, which were excluded because the dependent variables were count for substantial variance, one possibility is that nonshared not in the targeted areas of personality and intelligence; (f) animal stud- experiences play a prominent role in development. ies; (g) predated studies, which were excluded because an updated study These findings were at first surprising to developmentalists, with more data was available; (h) dissertations, which were excluded who have traditionally focused on the role of the shared family because many were duplicated in later journal articles; and (i) miscella- environment in psychological development, as reflected in tra- neous studies that included articles we could not locate through interli- ditional socialization theory (see Maccoby & Martin, 1983). brary loan, papers from various conferences and proceedings, and pa- This perspective assumes that influential environmental experi- pere by Sir Cyril Burt, which have been shown to contain fraudulent data (Heamshaw, 1979). It should also be noted that unrefereed books ences are shared by siblings, because they take place within the and book chapters were excluded by the computer literature search pro- family (e.g., parental discipline, attitudes, and values). The pre- cedure. Thus, this search was limited to journal articles on the similar- diction that follows from this perspective is that the longer sib- ity of twins' personality and intelligence. Table 1 displays the number lings live together, the more like one another they should be- and percentage of studies per category. Note that 130 twin studies were come. read for this meta-analysis. Of these 130 papers, 103 actually contained A developmental meta-analysis of twin studies allows the intraclass re or data that could be converted to intraclass re; these 103 comparison of predictions concerning the importance of the papers were used in the
Recommended publications
  • The Genetics of Tobacco Use: Methods, Findings and Policy Implications W Hall, P Madden, M Lynskey
    119 Tob Control: first published as 10.1136/tc.11.2.119 on 1 June 2002. Downloaded from REVIEW The genetics of tobacco use: methods, findings and policy implications W Hall, P Madden, M Lynskey ............................................................................................................................. Tobacco Control 2002;11:119–124 Research on the genetics of smoking has increased our Specific study designs—adoption, twin, and link- understanding of nicotine dependence, and it is likely to age designs—are needed to separate the effects of genes from those of the environment. illuminate the mechanisms by which cigarette smoking adversely effects the health of smokers. Given recent Adoption studies advances in molecular biology, including the completion In the adoption design we compare the similarity in smoking between adoptive children and their of the draft sequence of the human genome, interest has biological parents with the similarity between now turned to identifying gene markers that predict a adopted children and their adoptive parents or heightened risk of using tobacco and developing between adoptive sibling pairs and biological sib- ling pairs. If smoking is largely or wholly geneti- nicotine dependence cally determined, then there should be greater .......................................................................... similarity between children and their biological parents and siblings than between adopted hanks to RA Fisher, genetics has been linked children and their adoptive parents or siblings. to scepticism about a causal relation between One early adoption study found an association cigarette smoking and lung cancer.12 Fisher3 between the smoking of foster children and their T biological siblings but not their adoptive argued that the association between smoking and 5 6 lung cancer was explained by shared genes that siblings.
    [Show full text]
  • Nurture Might Be Nature: Cautionary Tales and Proposed Solutions Sara
    Nurture might be nature: Cautionary tales and proposed solutions Sara A. Hart* Department of Psychology and Florida Center for Reading Research Florida State University, USA Callie Little Department of Psychology University of New England, Australia Florida Center for Reading Research Florida State University, USA Elsje van Bergen Department of Biological Psychology Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, the Netherlands *Corresponding author: Sara A. Hart, 1107 W. Call Street, Tallahassee, FL, 32308. [email protected], ORCID: 0000-0001-9793-0420 FINAL IN PRESS CITATION: Hart, S.A., Little, C., & van Bergen, E. (in press). Nurture might be nature: Cautionary tales and proposed solutions. npj Science of Learning. 1 Abstract Across a wide range of studies, researchers often conclude that the home environment and children’s outcomes are causally linked. In contrast, behavioral genetic studies show that parents influence their children by providing them with both environment and genes, meaning the environment that parents provide should not be considered in the absence of genetic influences, because that can lead to erroneous conclusions on causation. This article seeks to provide behavioral scientists with a synopsis of numerous methods to estimate the direct effect of the environment, controlling for the potential of genetic confounding. Ideally, using genetically- sensitive designs can fully disentangle this genetic confound, but these require specialized samples. In the near future, researchers will likely have access to measured DNA variants (summarized in a polygenic scores), which could serve as a partial genetic control, but that is currently not an option that is ideal or widely available. We also propose a work around for when genetically sensitive data are not readily available: the Familial Control Method.
    [Show full text]
  • Nature Vs. Nurture Results in a Draw, According to Twins Meta-Study 19 May 2015
    Nature vs. nurture results in a draw, according to twins meta-study 19 May 2015 said. Although the contribution of genetic and environmental factors was balanced for most of the traits studied, the research showed there could be significant differences in individual traits. For example, risk for bipolar disorder was about 70 per cent due to genetics and 30 per cent due to environmental factors. "When visiting the nature versus nurture debate, there is overwhelming evidence that both genetic and environmental factors can influence traits and The research drew on data from almost every twin study diseases," Dr Benyamin said. across the world from the past 50 years. "What is comforting is that, on average, about 50 per cent of individual differences are genetic and 50 per cent are environmental. One of the great tussles of science – whether our health is governed by nature or nurture – has been "The findings show that we need to look at settled, and it is effectively a draw. ourselves outside of a view of nature versus nurture , and instead look at it as nature and nurture." University of Queensland researcher Dr Beben Benyamin from the Queensland Brain Institute In 69 per cent of cases, the study also revealed that collaborated with researchers at VU University of individual traits were the product of the cumulative Amsterdam to review almost every twin study effect of genetic differences. across the world from the past 50 years, involving more than 14.5 million twin pairs. "This means that there are good reasons to study the biology of human traits, and that the combined The findings, published in Nature Genetics, reveal effect of many genes on a trait is simply the sum of on average the variation for human traits and the effect of each individual gene," Dr Benyamin diseases is 49 per cent genetic, and 51 per cent said.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic and Environmental Influences on Human Behavioral Differences
    Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1998. 21:1–24 Copyright c 1998 by Annual Reviews Inc. All rights reserved GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON HUMAN BEHAVIORAL DIFFERENCES Matt McGue and Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr Department of Psychology and Institute of Human Genetics, 75 East River Road, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455; e-mail: [email protected] KEY WORDS: heritability, gene-environment interaction and correlation, nonshared environ- ment, psychiatric genetics ABSTRACT Human behavioral genetic research aimed at characterizing the existence and nature of genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in cog- nitive ability, personality and interests, and psychopathology is reviewed. Twin and adoption studies indicate that most behavioral characteristics are heritable. Nonetheless, efforts to identify the genes influencing behavior have produced a limited number of confirmed linkages or associations. Behavioral genetic re- search also documents the importance of environmental factors, but contrary to the expectations of many behavioral scientists, the relevant environmental factors appear to be those that are not shared by reared together relatives. The observation of genotype-environment correlational processes and the hypothesized existence of genotype-environment interaction effects serve to distinguish behavioral traits from the medical and physiological phenotypes studied by human geneticists. Be- havioral genetic research supports the heritability, not the genetic determination, of behavior. INTRODUCTION One of the longest, and at times most contentious, debates in Westernintellectual history concerns the relative influence of genetic and environmental factors on human behavioral differences, the so-called nature-nurture debate (Degler 1991). Remarkably, the past generation of behavioral genetic research has led 1 0147-006X/98/0301-0001$08.00 2 McGUE & BOUCHARD many to conclude that it may now be time to retire this debate in favor of a perspective that more strongly emphasizes the joint influence of genes and the environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Nitive Skills in Academic Achievement in Identical and Fraternal Twins
    Volume 9 Issue 1 (2020) AP Research Twin Study A Comparison of Nature Vs. Nurture on Cog- nitive Skills in Academic Achievement in Identical and Fraternal Twins Amina Dada1 1Lapeer High School, Lapeer, MI, USA ABSTRACT This research study is looking at the academic achievement levels in identical and fraternal twins at the high school level through a measurable cognitive perspective. The purpose is to see if the data collected agrees with past research on how much academic achievement is due to genetics or environmental influences (nature vs nurture). Twins were asked to take three online cognitive tests and score differences were computed to determine statistical significance. I then drew connections on how much the scores were due to genetics while accounting for confounding variables in the environmental influence’s aspect. Therefore, the study concludes that the majority of academic achievement is due to genetics in both identical and fraternal twin sets. This indicates my research supports past research that shows genetics plays the majority factor in academic achievement whether twins are younger or older in age. This study is purely correlational and analyzes data with the twin method which means it in no way proves anything but rather shows a connection between academic achievement and genetics. This research adds more support to the current body of knowledge, but also brings up an important point of how future research can study environmental influences in- depth to see how it influences academic achievement beyond what can be measured in the classroom. Introduction Twin studies are a very important tool that psychologists and scientists use to analyze a certain trait, and look to see how much of it is due to genetics or other environmental influences.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic Epidemiology of Epilepsy: a Twin Study
    Original Article Genetic epidemiology of epilepsy: A twin study Krishan Sharma Department of Anthropology, Panjab University, Chandigarh - 160 014, India. The study explored the genetic susceptibility and prevalence diseases/disorders having epileptic seizure as phenotypic ex- of epilepsy in twins. The data on epilepsy were retrieved pression. In fact, disorders associated with more than 10 hu- [3,4] from the health records of 199 pairs of twins. Proband con- man chromosomes are linked with epilepsy. A genetic mis- cordance rate in monozygotic (MZ) twins was four times more sense mutation has also been associated with autosomal domi- than that in dizygotic (DZ) twins (0.67 vs. 0.17). Three of 15 nant nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy.[5] Certain genetic mark- (20%) affected twin kinships had epileptic first-degree rela- ers have also been linked with epilepsy. For example, juvenile tives. These findings indicated significant underlying genetic myoclonic epilepsy has been linked to the HLA loci on Chrho- susceptibility to epilepsy with the Holzinger’s heritability es- mosome 6.[6] timate being 0.45. The prevalence of epilepsy was similar in Besides genetic factors, a large number of pre- and peri- MZ (45.45), DZ (45.11) twins, and their non-twin siblings natal factors, such as obstetric trauma, cerebral palsy, pre- (47.60). In the general population from various nationalities, maturity and neurological damage are widely believed to be the mean prevalence rate of epilepsy varied from 5 to 17 important causes of epilepsy.[7-10] Human twins have to face a per 1000. The appreciably higher prevalence rate in twin high frequency of such adverse perinatal events.
    [Show full text]
  • Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions?
    | | ⅜ Comment Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions? Jon Beckwith and Corey A. Morris Using the “classical twin method,” political scientists John Alford, Carolyn Funk, and John Hibbing conclude that political ideol- ogies are significantly influenced by genetics, an assertion that has garnered considerable media attention. Researchers have long used human twins in attempts to assess the degree of genetic influence on various behavioral traits.Today, this methodology has been largely replaced in favor of contemporary molecular genetic techniques, and thus heritability studies have seen a diminishing role in behavioral genetic research of the twenty-first century. One important reason the twin method has been superseded is that it depends upon several questionable assumptions, the most significant of which is known as the equal environments assumption. Alford, Funk, and Hibbing argue that this crucial assumption, and thus their conclusion, holds up under empirical scrutiny. They point to several studies in support of this assumption. Here, we review the evidence presented and conclude that these attempts to test the equal environments assumption are weak, suffering significant methodological and inherent design flaws. Furthermore, much of the empirical evidence provided by these studies actually argues that, contrary to the interpretation, trait-relevant equal environments assumptions have been violated. We conclude that the equal environments assumption remains untenable, and as such, twin studies are an insufficient method
    [Show full text]
  • Nature Versus Nurture Grade Level
    Assignment Discovery Online Curriculum Lesson title: Nature Versus Nurture Grade level: 9-12, with adaptation for younger students Subject area: Human Body Contemporary Studies Behavioral Science Duration: Two class periods Objectives: Students will 1. learn that environment can influence some personality traits, while others are genetic; 2. understand that the most effective way to study the concept of nature versus nurture is by conducting research with identical and fraternal twins reared separately and together; and 3. discover that the issues of nature versus nurture are still debated in the scientific community. Materials: - Computer with Internet access - Copies of Classroom Activity Sheet: Analyzing Twin Studies - Printouts of three twin studies from these Web sites: § http://www.mugu.com/cgi-bin/Upstream/Issues/psychology/IQ/bouchard-twins.html § http://www.thetimes.co.uk/article/0,,2-96428,00.html § http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin- post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&uid=10808236&dopt=r - Copies of Take-Home Activity Sheet: What Do You Think? Procedures: 1. Begin the lesson by asking students what they think determines their likes, dislikes, and personality characteristics. Is it their genetic makeup or their environment? Discuss their ideas. 2. Explain that researchers have been debating this question for centuries. This debate is referred to as “nature versus nurture.” Researchers have studied identical and fraternal twins to try to determine whether genetics (nature) or environment (nurture) has a greater effect on personality development. Identical twins, or monozygotic twins, come from the same fertilized egg. In the earliest stages of development, the egg splits into two zygotes, creating two individuals with the same genetic code.
    [Show full text]
  • EPIB 668 Examples of Familial Aggregation and Twin Studies
    EPIB 668 Examples of familial aggregation and twin studies Aur´elieLabbe - Winter 2011. 1 / 68 OUTLINE What is familial aggregation ? Case-control and cohort design: the family history approach Example 1: Familial aggregation of lung cancer Twin study design Example 2: Familial aggregation of cancer Heritability: definition The variance component model to compute heritability Twin studies to estimate heritability Example 3: Compulsive hoarding 2 / 68 WHAT IS FAMILIAL AGGREGATION ? 3 / 68 Genetic epidemiology questions 4 / 68 What is familial aggregation ? First step in pursuing a possible genetic etiology of the disease Based on phenotypic data only (don't need DNA) Demonstrate that the disease tends to run in families more than what would expect by chance Examine how that familial tendency is modified by the degree or type of relationship, age or environmental factors Important Familial aggregation does not separate genetic from environment 5 / 68 Rational of aggregation studies Identify a group of individuals with a specific disease and determine whether relatives have an excess frequency of the same disease when compared to an appropriate reference population Often, phenotype of interest is a disease (i.e. affected vs non-affected). But it can also be a physiological trait that has a continuous distribution (e.g cholesterol levels) 6 / 68 CASE-CONTROL AND COHORT DESIGN: THE FAMILY HISTORY APPROACH 7 / 68 Issues in designing family studies Goal is to demonstrate that the disease tends to run in families more than what would expect by chance Need to sample families rather than individuals In standard epi. studies, it is hard enough to find appropriate sampling frames for individuals ..
    [Show full text]
  • Twins Reared Together and Apart: the Science Behind the Fascination1
    Twins Reared Together and Apart: The Science Behind the Fascination1 NANCY L. SEGAL Professor of Psychology and Director Twin Studies Center California State University, Fullerton he reasons behind the widespread interest in twins are fascinating to consider. There is no question that people are intrigued by T twins—wherever I go I am asked about my work by both profes- sionals and non-professionals. I often ask them, “Why do you care about this topic?” Most of them reply, “Well, it is just so interesting,” but no one can provide a more substantive answer. I have thought a great deal about this question. I believe that twins are fascinating because we expect and anticipate individual differences in behavior and appearance. There- fore, when we encounter two people who look so much alike and act so much alike, it becomes intriguing because it challenges our beliefs in the way the world works. For some people, this may be disturbing, but for many, it is quite compelling. There are also many misunderstandings surrounding the origins of twinning and twin development. I address some of them in this paper and many others in my new book Twin Mythconceptions (Segal, 2017). Two Twin Types The study of twins rests on the availability of two types of twins. Identical, or monozygotic (MZ), twins share all of their genes in common, having split from a single fertilized egg between the first and fourteenth post-conceptional day (Segal, 2000). Fraternal, or dizygotic (DZ), twins result from the fertilization of two separate eggs by two separate sperm and share 50% of their genes, on average, by descent, just like full siblings.
    [Show full text]
  • Improvements and Future Challenges for the Research Infrastructure In
    A Service of Leibniz-Informationszentrum econstor Wirtschaft Leibniz Information Centre Make Your Publications Visible. zbw for Economics Spinath, Frank M. Working Paper Improvements and Future Challenges in the Field of Genetically Sensitive Sample Designs SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research, No. 142 Provided in Cooperation with: German Institute for Economic Research (DIW Berlin) Suggested Citation: Spinath, Frank M. (2008) : Improvements and Future Challenges in the Field of Genetically Sensitive Sample Designs, SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research, No. 142, Deutsches Institut für Wirtschaftsforschung (DIW), Berlin This Version is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10419/150689 Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen: Terms of use: Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Documents in EconStor may be saved and copied for your Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden. personal and scholarly purposes. Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle You are not to copy documents for public or commercial Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich purposes, to exhibit the documents publicly, to make them machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen. publicly available on the internet, or to distribute or otherwise use the documents in public. Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, If the documents have been made available under an Open gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in der dort Content Licence (especially Creative Commons Licences), you genannten Lizenz gewährten Nutzungsrechte. may exercise further usage rights as specified in the indicated licence. www.econstor.eu Deutsches Institut für Wirtschaftsforschung www.diw.de SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research 142 Frank M.
    [Show full text]
  • The Genetic Epidemiology of Cancer: Interpreting Family and Twin Studies and Their Implications for Molecular Genetic Approaches
    Vol. 10, 733–741, July 2001 Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention 733 Review The Genetic Epidemiology of Cancer: Interpreting Family and Twin Studies and Their Implications for Molecular Genetic Approaches Neil Risch1 ingly at odds with the claims of the genomicists. The fact that Department of Genetics, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, this was by far the largest twin study in cancer yet reported California 94305-5120, and Division of Research, Kaiser Permanente, (nearly 45,000 twin pairs) also lent credence to this conclusion, Oakland, California 94611-5714 although an editorial appearing in the same issue (4) discussed some of the limitations of that study. Given the increasing emphasis on molecular genetic approaches to address familial Abstract disorders coupled with the latest evidence questioning the role The recent completion of a rough draft of the human of genetics in cancer susceptibility, a reassessment of the role genome sequence has ushered in a new era of molecular of genetic factors in cancer susceptibility generally and for genetics research into the inherited basis of a number of site-specific cancers in particular appears warranted. complex diseases such as cancer. At the same time, recent twin studies have suggested a limited role of genetic Study Designs susceptibility to many neoplasms. A reappraisal of family and twin studies for many cancer sites suggests the Familial aggregation of a trait is a necessary but not sufficient following general conclusions: (a) all cancers are familial condition to infer the importance of genetic susceptibility, be- to approximately the same degree, with only a few cause environmental and cultural influences can also aggregate exceptions (both high and low); (b) early age of diagnosis in families, leading to family clustering and excess familial risk.
    [Show full text]