What Can We Learn from Twin Studies? a Comprehensive Evaluation of The

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

What Can We Learn from Twin Studies? a Comprehensive Evaluation of The The Pennsylvania State University The Graduate School College of the Liberal Arts WHAT CAN WE LEARN FROM TWIN STUDIES? A COMPREHENSIVE EVALUATION OF THE EQUAL ENVIRONMENTS ASSUMPTION A Dissertation in Sociology by Jacob Felson c 2009 Jacob Felson Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy December 2009 The dissertation of Jacob Felson was reviewed and approved∗ by the following: John D. McCarthy Professor of Sociology Head of the Department of Sociology Dissertation Advisor Alan Booth Professor of Sociology Roger Finke Professor of Sociology and Religious Studies Katerina Bodovski Assistant Professor of Education ∗Signatures are on file in the Graduate School. ii Abstract Most evidence about the effects of genes on behavior comes from twin studies. The classic twin study method compares the similarity of monozygotic (MZ) twins on some trait with the similarity of dizygotic (DZ) twins on that same trait. If the rearing environments of MZ and DZ twins are analogous, then the magnitude of genetic influence on variation in a trait can be estimated based on the extent to which the correlation for MZ twins exceeds the correlation for DZ twins. If the rearing environments of MZ and DZ twins are not analogous, then the estimates of heritability generated from the classic twin study method may be confounded with differences between MZ and DZ twins in the similarity of their rearing environment. It has long been established that MZ twins grow up in a more similar environment than do DZ twins. However, behavior geneticists have generally asserted that the greater degree of similarity in the rearing environments of MZ relative to DZ twins does not bias the results of twin studies. Behavior geneticists claim that the similarity in outcomes is largely unaffected by similarity in rearing environments for MZ and DZ twins. This claim is known as the equal environments assumption (EEA). In this paper, I argue that the empirical support for the validity of the EEA is not as strong as some behavior geneticists have claimed. I reanalyze some of the data on which ritually cited research on EEA is based, and I find that the evidence is more mixed than latter-day interpretations have suggested. I expose some of the flaws in the research that is frequently cited by researchers in support of claims about genetic influence on behavior. Finally, I conduct the most comprehensive evaluation of the equal environments assumption to date. My analysis incorporates a larger and more diverse set of out- come variables than any previous research on the EEA. In my analysis, I also use a more extensive set of controls for environmental similarity than any previous study. Finally, unlike most if not all previous research on EEA, I generate estimates of genetic influence with and without controls for environmental similarity, in order to show the extent of EEA violation in a straightforward manner. The findings are not easily categorized as supporting or undermining the EEA. Some violations of EEA were found for some outcomes, but the bias caused by these violations is likely to be modest in most cases. Researchers should not conduct twin studies without including controls for environmental similarity. However, the findings also indicate that many of the results of twin studies are robust with these controls. The flaws of twin studies are not fatal, but rather seem no worse (and maybe better) than the flaws of the typical causal study that relies on observational data. iii Contents List of Tables v List of Figures vi Acknowledgements viii 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Planforthepresentstudy . .. .. 2 1.2 Estimating the effects of genes on variation in traits with twin studies 3 1.3 Evaluating the twin study as a natural experiment . ..... 5 2 Literature Review 9 2.1 Earlieststudies(1930–1960) . 10 2.2 Earlystudies(1965–1979) . 21 2.3 Recentstudies............................... 40 2.4 Studies of adopted children and studies of twins reared apart..... 47 3 Data 48 3.1 Measures.................................. 50 3.2 Handlingmissingdata . .. .. 63 4 Methods 64 4.1 Comparisons of means and variances by zygosity . .... 64 4.2 Examining measures of environmental similarity . ...... 64 4.3 Absolutevalueregressions . 64 4.4 Defries-Fulkerregressions. .. 65 4.5 Propensityscorematching . 67 5 Results 68 5.1 Comparisons of means and variances by zygosity . .... 69 5.2 Examining measures of environmental similarity . ...... 75 5.3 Absolutevalueregressions . 80 5.4 Defries-Fulkerregressions. .. 91 5.5 Propensityscorematching . 99 6 Conclusion 114 References 116 iv List of Tables 1 Descriptive statistics for reanalysis of Wilson (1934) . ........ 13 2 Model fit statistics for reanalysis of Wilson (1934) . ..... 16 3 Mean differences between co-twins on various measures, reproduced fromScarr(1968)............................. 23 4 Parental reports of environmental similarity, by perceived and diag- nosed zygosity, NMSQT twins study . 26 5 Twin reports of environmental similarity, by perceived and diagnosed zygosity, NMSQT twins study . 28 6 Regressions of absolute twin differences in cognitive test scores on measures of perceived, physical, and genetic differences, n=226, re- produced from Scarr and Carter-Saltzman (1979) . 37 7 Average absolute differences between co-twins on Raven’s Progressive Matrices, from Scarr and Carter-Saltzman (1979) . .. 39 8 Average absolute differences between co-twins on Revised Visual Re- tention Test, from Scarr and Carter-Saltzman (1979) . ... 39 9 Recent studies testing the equal environments assumption ...... 43 10 Recent studies: measures of environmental similarity . ....... 44 11 Recent studies: outcome measures . 45 12 Recent studies: methods and findings . 46 13 Descriptive statistics for physical attributes . ........ 51 14 Descriptive statistics for measures of Health . ..... 52 15 Descriptive statistics for measures of Health-Related Behaviors . 53 16 Descriptive statistics for measures of Mental Disorders ........ 54 17 Descriptive statistics for measures of Personality . ........ 55 18 Descriptive statistics for measures of Self-Actualization ........ 55 19 Descriptive statistics for measures of Well-Being . ....... 56 20 Descriptive statistics for measures of Sociability . ........ 57 21 Descriptive statistics for measures of Marriage and Family ...... 57 22 Descriptive statistics for measures of Social Class . ....... 58 23 Descriptive statistics for measures of Socio-political Attitudes . 58 24 Descriptive statistics for measures of Social Obligations........ 59 25 Descriptive statistics for measures of religion . ....... 60 26 Components of scales measuring environmental similarity....... 61 27 Components of scales measuring environmental similarity, continued . 62 28 Standardized outcome variances by zygosity . ... 72 29 Standardized outcome variances by zygosity, continued . ....... 74 v List of Figures 1 Comparing environmental similarity of DZ twins to that of MZ twins, basedonreanalysisofWilson(1934) . 18 2 Correlations between absolute differences on traits and parents’ re- ports of environmental similarity, following Loehlin and Nichols (1976) 27 3 Correlations between absolute differences on traits and twins’ reports ofenvironmentalsimilarity. 29 4 Heritability estimates for components of National Merit Qualifying Test: unmatched and matched twin pairs, NMSQT data . 31 5 Heritability estimates for personality traits: unmatched and matched twinpairs,NMSQTdata. .. .. 32 6 Heritability estimates for personality traits: unmatched and matched twin pairs, continued, NMSQT data . 33 7 Heritability estimates for vocational interests: unmatched and matched twinpairs,NMSQTdata. .. .. 34 8 Standardized means of outcome variables by zygosity . .... 69 9 Standardized means of outcome variables by zygosity, cont....... 70 10 Standardized means of outcome variables by zygosity, cont....... 71 11 Ninety-five percent confidence intervals of environmental similarity . 75 12 Agreement between twins on measures of environmental similarity . 77 13 Twin reports about equality in relationship growing up . ...... 78 14 Perceptions of equality in relationship growing up, by zygosity . 79 15 Percent change in coefficient for zygosity when controlling childhood environment................................ 81 16 Percent change in the coefficient for zygosity when controlling for sta- tus inequality in relationship growing up . 82 17 Percent change in the coefficient for zygosity when controlling for cur- rentcontact ................................ 83 18 Percent change in the coefficient for zygosity when controlling for the proportionoflifelivedtogether . 85 19 Percent change in the coefficient for zygosity when controlling for vir- tualcontact ................................ 86 20 Percent change in the coefficient for zygosity when controlling for psy- chologicalintimacy ............................ 87 21 Percent change in the coefficient for zygosity when controlling for neg- ativityintwinrelationship . 88 vi 22 Percent change in the coefficient for zygosity when controlling for ad- vice exchanged between twins . 89 23 Percent reduction in heritability when controlling for childhood simi- larity.................................... 92 24 Percent reduction in heritability when controlling for inequality in twinrelationshipgrowingup. 93 25 Percent reduction in heritability when controlling for current contact 94 26 Percent reduction in heritability when controlling for proportion of life spenttogether............................... 95 27 Percent
Recommended publications
  • The Genetics of Tobacco Use: Methods, Findings and Policy Implications W Hall, P Madden, M Lynskey
    119 Tob Control: first published as 10.1136/tc.11.2.119 on 1 June 2002. Downloaded from REVIEW The genetics of tobacco use: methods, findings and policy implications W Hall, P Madden, M Lynskey ............................................................................................................................. Tobacco Control 2002;11:119–124 Research on the genetics of smoking has increased our Specific study designs—adoption, twin, and link- understanding of nicotine dependence, and it is likely to age designs—are needed to separate the effects of genes from those of the environment. illuminate the mechanisms by which cigarette smoking adversely effects the health of smokers. Given recent Adoption studies advances in molecular biology, including the completion In the adoption design we compare the similarity in smoking between adoptive children and their of the draft sequence of the human genome, interest has biological parents with the similarity between now turned to identifying gene markers that predict a adopted children and their adoptive parents or heightened risk of using tobacco and developing between adoptive sibling pairs and biological sib- ling pairs. If smoking is largely or wholly geneti- nicotine dependence cally determined, then there should be greater .......................................................................... similarity between children and their biological parents and siblings than between adopted hanks to RA Fisher, genetics has been linked children and their adoptive parents or siblings. to scepticism about a causal relation between One early adoption study found an association cigarette smoking and lung cancer.12 Fisher3 between the smoking of foster children and their T biological siblings but not their adoptive argued that the association between smoking and 5 6 lung cancer was explained by shared genes that siblings.
    [Show full text]
  • Nurture Might Be Nature: Cautionary Tales and Proposed Solutions Sara
    Nurture might be nature: Cautionary tales and proposed solutions Sara A. Hart* Department of Psychology and Florida Center for Reading Research Florida State University, USA Callie Little Department of Psychology University of New England, Australia Florida Center for Reading Research Florida State University, USA Elsje van Bergen Department of Biological Psychology Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, the Netherlands *Corresponding author: Sara A. Hart, 1107 W. Call Street, Tallahassee, FL, 32308. [email protected], ORCID: 0000-0001-9793-0420 FINAL IN PRESS CITATION: Hart, S.A., Little, C., & van Bergen, E. (in press). Nurture might be nature: Cautionary tales and proposed solutions. npj Science of Learning. 1 Abstract Across a wide range of studies, researchers often conclude that the home environment and children’s outcomes are causally linked. In contrast, behavioral genetic studies show that parents influence their children by providing them with both environment and genes, meaning the environment that parents provide should not be considered in the absence of genetic influences, because that can lead to erroneous conclusions on causation. This article seeks to provide behavioral scientists with a synopsis of numerous methods to estimate the direct effect of the environment, controlling for the potential of genetic confounding. Ideally, using genetically- sensitive designs can fully disentangle this genetic confound, but these require specialized samples. In the near future, researchers will likely have access to measured DNA variants (summarized in a polygenic scores), which could serve as a partial genetic control, but that is currently not an option that is ideal or widely available. We also propose a work around for when genetically sensitive data are not readily available: the Familial Control Method.
    [Show full text]
  • Nature Vs. Nurture Results in a Draw, According to Twins Meta-Study 19 May 2015
    Nature vs. nurture results in a draw, according to twins meta-study 19 May 2015 said. Although the contribution of genetic and environmental factors was balanced for most of the traits studied, the research showed there could be significant differences in individual traits. For example, risk for bipolar disorder was about 70 per cent due to genetics and 30 per cent due to environmental factors. "When visiting the nature versus nurture debate, there is overwhelming evidence that both genetic and environmental factors can influence traits and The research drew on data from almost every twin study diseases," Dr Benyamin said. across the world from the past 50 years. "What is comforting is that, on average, about 50 per cent of individual differences are genetic and 50 per cent are environmental. One of the great tussles of science – whether our health is governed by nature or nurture – has been "The findings show that we need to look at settled, and it is effectively a draw. ourselves outside of a view of nature versus nurture , and instead look at it as nature and nurture." University of Queensland researcher Dr Beben Benyamin from the Queensland Brain Institute In 69 per cent of cases, the study also revealed that collaborated with researchers at VU University of individual traits were the product of the cumulative Amsterdam to review almost every twin study effect of genetic differences. across the world from the past 50 years, involving more than 14.5 million twin pairs. "This means that there are good reasons to study the biology of human traits, and that the combined The findings, published in Nature Genetics, reveal effect of many genes on a trait is simply the sum of on average the variation for human traits and the effect of each individual gene," Dr Benyamin diseases is 49 per cent genetic, and 51 per cent said.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic and Environmental Influences on Human Behavioral Differences
    Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1998. 21:1–24 Copyright c 1998 by Annual Reviews Inc. All rights reserved GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON HUMAN BEHAVIORAL DIFFERENCES Matt McGue and Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr Department of Psychology and Institute of Human Genetics, 75 East River Road, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455; e-mail: [email protected] KEY WORDS: heritability, gene-environment interaction and correlation, nonshared environ- ment, psychiatric genetics ABSTRACT Human behavioral genetic research aimed at characterizing the existence and nature of genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in cog- nitive ability, personality and interests, and psychopathology is reviewed. Twin and adoption studies indicate that most behavioral characteristics are heritable. Nonetheless, efforts to identify the genes influencing behavior have produced a limited number of confirmed linkages or associations. Behavioral genetic re- search also documents the importance of environmental factors, but contrary to the expectations of many behavioral scientists, the relevant environmental factors appear to be those that are not shared by reared together relatives. The observation of genotype-environment correlational processes and the hypothesized existence of genotype-environment interaction effects serve to distinguish behavioral traits from the medical and physiological phenotypes studied by human geneticists. Be- havioral genetic research supports the heritability, not the genetic determination, of behavior. INTRODUCTION One of the longest, and at times most contentious, debates in Westernintellectual history concerns the relative influence of genetic and environmental factors on human behavioral differences, the so-called nature-nurture debate (Degler 1991). Remarkably, the past generation of behavioral genetic research has led 1 0147-006X/98/0301-0001$08.00 2 McGUE & BOUCHARD many to conclude that it may now be time to retire this debate in favor of a perspective that more strongly emphasizes the joint influence of genes and the environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Nitive Skills in Academic Achievement in Identical and Fraternal Twins
    Volume 9 Issue 1 (2020) AP Research Twin Study A Comparison of Nature Vs. Nurture on Cog- nitive Skills in Academic Achievement in Identical and Fraternal Twins Amina Dada1 1Lapeer High School, Lapeer, MI, USA ABSTRACT This research study is looking at the academic achievement levels in identical and fraternal twins at the high school level through a measurable cognitive perspective. The purpose is to see if the data collected agrees with past research on how much academic achievement is due to genetics or environmental influences (nature vs nurture). Twins were asked to take three online cognitive tests and score differences were computed to determine statistical significance. I then drew connections on how much the scores were due to genetics while accounting for confounding variables in the environmental influence’s aspect. Therefore, the study concludes that the majority of academic achievement is due to genetics in both identical and fraternal twin sets. This indicates my research supports past research that shows genetics plays the majority factor in academic achievement whether twins are younger or older in age. This study is purely correlational and analyzes data with the twin method which means it in no way proves anything but rather shows a connection between academic achievement and genetics. This research adds more support to the current body of knowledge, but also brings up an important point of how future research can study environmental influences in- depth to see how it influences academic achievement beyond what can be measured in the classroom. Introduction Twin studies are a very important tool that psychologists and scientists use to analyze a certain trait, and look to see how much of it is due to genetics or other environmental influences.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic Epidemiology of Epilepsy: a Twin Study
    Original Article Genetic epidemiology of epilepsy: A twin study Krishan Sharma Department of Anthropology, Panjab University, Chandigarh - 160 014, India. The study explored the genetic susceptibility and prevalence diseases/disorders having epileptic seizure as phenotypic ex- of epilepsy in twins. The data on epilepsy were retrieved pression. In fact, disorders associated with more than 10 hu- [3,4] from the health records of 199 pairs of twins. Proband con- man chromosomes are linked with epilepsy. A genetic mis- cordance rate in monozygotic (MZ) twins was four times more sense mutation has also been associated with autosomal domi- than that in dizygotic (DZ) twins (0.67 vs. 0.17). Three of 15 nant nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy.[5] Certain genetic mark- (20%) affected twin kinships had epileptic first-degree rela- ers have also been linked with epilepsy. For example, juvenile tives. These findings indicated significant underlying genetic myoclonic epilepsy has been linked to the HLA loci on Chrho- susceptibility to epilepsy with the Holzinger’s heritability es- mosome 6.[6] timate being 0.45. The prevalence of epilepsy was similar in Besides genetic factors, a large number of pre- and peri- MZ (45.45), DZ (45.11) twins, and their non-twin siblings natal factors, such as obstetric trauma, cerebral palsy, pre- (47.60). In the general population from various nationalities, maturity and neurological damage are widely believed to be the mean prevalence rate of epilepsy varied from 5 to 17 important causes of epilepsy.[7-10] Human twins have to face a per 1000. The appreciably higher prevalence rate in twin high frequency of such adverse perinatal events.
    [Show full text]
  • Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions?
    | | ⅜ Comment Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions? Jon Beckwith and Corey A. Morris Using the “classical twin method,” political scientists John Alford, Carolyn Funk, and John Hibbing conclude that political ideol- ogies are significantly influenced by genetics, an assertion that has garnered considerable media attention. Researchers have long used human twins in attempts to assess the degree of genetic influence on various behavioral traits.Today, this methodology has been largely replaced in favor of contemporary molecular genetic techniques, and thus heritability studies have seen a diminishing role in behavioral genetic research of the twenty-first century. One important reason the twin method has been superseded is that it depends upon several questionable assumptions, the most significant of which is known as the equal environments assumption. Alford, Funk, and Hibbing argue that this crucial assumption, and thus their conclusion, holds up under empirical scrutiny. They point to several studies in support of this assumption. Here, we review the evidence presented and conclude that these attempts to test the equal environments assumption are weak, suffering significant methodological and inherent design flaws. Furthermore, much of the empirical evidence provided by these studies actually argues that, contrary to the interpretation, trait-relevant equal environments assumptions have been violated. We conclude that the equal environments assumption remains untenable, and as such, twin studies are an insufficient method
    [Show full text]
  • Nature Versus Nurture Grade Level
    Assignment Discovery Online Curriculum Lesson title: Nature Versus Nurture Grade level: 9-12, with adaptation for younger students Subject area: Human Body Contemporary Studies Behavioral Science Duration: Two class periods Objectives: Students will 1. learn that environment can influence some personality traits, while others are genetic; 2. understand that the most effective way to study the concept of nature versus nurture is by conducting research with identical and fraternal twins reared separately and together; and 3. discover that the issues of nature versus nurture are still debated in the scientific community. Materials: - Computer with Internet access - Copies of Classroom Activity Sheet: Analyzing Twin Studies - Printouts of three twin studies from these Web sites: § http://www.mugu.com/cgi-bin/Upstream/Issues/psychology/IQ/bouchard-twins.html § http://www.thetimes.co.uk/article/0,,2-96428,00.html § http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/htbin- post/Entrez/query?db=m&form=6&uid=10808236&dopt=r - Copies of Take-Home Activity Sheet: What Do You Think? Procedures: 1. Begin the lesson by asking students what they think determines their likes, dislikes, and personality characteristics. Is it their genetic makeup or their environment? Discuss their ideas. 2. Explain that researchers have been debating this question for centuries. This debate is referred to as “nature versus nurture.” Researchers have studied identical and fraternal twins to try to determine whether genetics (nature) or environment (nurture) has a greater effect on personality development. Identical twins, or monozygotic twins, come from the same fertilized egg. In the earliest stages of development, the egg splits into two zygotes, creating two individuals with the same genetic code.
    [Show full text]
  • EPIB 668 Examples of Familial Aggregation and Twin Studies
    EPIB 668 Examples of familial aggregation and twin studies Aur´elieLabbe - Winter 2011. 1 / 68 OUTLINE What is familial aggregation ? Case-control and cohort design: the family history approach Example 1: Familial aggregation of lung cancer Twin study design Example 2: Familial aggregation of cancer Heritability: definition The variance component model to compute heritability Twin studies to estimate heritability Example 3: Compulsive hoarding 2 / 68 WHAT IS FAMILIAL AGGREGATION ? 3 / 68 Genetic epidemiology questions 4 / 68 What is familial aggregation ? First step in pursuing a possible genetic etiology of the disease Based on phenotypic data only (don't need DNA) Demonstrate that the disease tends to run in families more than what would expect by chance Examine how that familial tendency is modified by the degree or type of relationship, age or environmental factors Important Familial aggregation does not separate genetic from environment 5 / 68 Rational of aggregation studies Identify a group of individuals with a specific disease and determine whether relatives have an excess frequency of the same disease when compared to an appropriate reference population Often, phenotype of interest is a disease (i.e. affected vs non-affected). But it can also be a physiological trait that has a continuous distribution (e.g cholesterol levels) 6 / 68 CASE-CONTROL AND COHORT DESIGN: THE FAMILY HISTORY APPROACH 7 / 68 Issues in designing family studies Goal is to demonstrate that the disease tends to run in families more than what would expect by chance Need to sample families rather than individuals In standard epi. studies, it is hard enough to find appropriate sampling frames for individuals ..
    [Show full text]
  • Twins Reared Together and Apart: the Science Behind the Fascination1
    Twins Reared Together and Apart: The Science Behind the Fascination1 NANCY L. SEGAL Professor of Psychology and Director Twin Studies Center California State University, Fullerton he reasons behind the widespread interest in twins are fascinating to consider. There is no question that people are intrigued by T twins—wherever I go I am asked about my work by both profes- sionals and non-professionals. I often ask them, “Why do you care about this topic?” Most of them reply, “Well, it is just so interesting,” but no one can provide a more substantive answer. I have thought a great deal about this question. I believe that twins are fascinating because we expect and anticipate individual differences in behavior and appearance. There- fore, when we encounter two people who look so much alike and act so much alike, it becomes intriguing because it challenges our beliefs in the way the world works. For some people, this may be disturbing, but for many, it is quite compelling. There are also many misunderstandings surrounding the origins of twinning and twin development. I address some of them in this paper and many others in my new book Twin Mythconceptions (Segal, 2017). Two Twin Types The study of twins rests on the availability of two types of twins. Identical, or monozygotic (MZ), twins share all of their genes in common, having split from a single fertilized egg between the first and fourteenth post-conceptional day (Segal, 2000). Fraternal, or dizygotic (DZ), twins result from the fertilization of two separate eggs by two separate sperm and share 50% of their genes, on average, by descent, just like full siblings.
    [Show full text]
  • Improvements and Future Challenges for the Research Infrastructure In
    A Service of Leibniz-Informationszentrum econstor Wirtschaft Leibniz Information Centre Make Your Publications Visible. zbw for Economics Spinath, Frank M. Working Paper Improvements and Future Challenges in the Field of Genetically Sensitive Sample Designs SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research, No. 142 Provided in Cooperation with: German Institute for Economic Research (DIW Berlin) Suggested Citation: Spinath, Frank M. (2008) : Improvements and Future Challenges in the Field of Genetically Sensitive Sample Designs, SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research, No. 142, Deutsches Institut für Wirtschaftsforschung (DIW), Berlin This Version is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10419/150689 Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen: Terms of use: Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Documents in EconStor may be saved and copied for your Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden. personal and scholarly purposes. Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle You are not to copy documents for public or commercial Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich purposes, to exhibit the documents publicly, to make them machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen. publicly available on the internet, or to distribute or otherwise use the documents in public. Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, If the documents have been made available under an Open gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in der dort Content Licence (especially Creative Commons Licences), you genannten Lizenz gewährten Nutzungsrechte. may exercise further usage rights as specified in the indicated licence. www.econstor.eu Deutsches Institut für Wirtschaftsforschung www.diw.de SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research 142 Frank M.
    [Show full text]
  • The Genetic Epidemiology of Cancer: Interpreting Family and Twin Studies and Their Implications for Molecular Genetic Approaches
    Vol. 10, 733–741, July 2001 Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention 733 Review The Genetic Epidemiology of Cancer: Interpreting Family and Twin Studies and Their Implications for Molecular Genetic Approaches Neil Risch1 ingly at odds with the claims of the genomicists. The fact that Department of Genetics, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, this was by far the largest twin study in cancer yet reported California 94305-5120, and Division of Research, Kaiser Permanente, (nearly 45,000 twin pairs) also lent credence to this conclusion, Oakland, California 94611-5714 although an editorial appearing in the same issue (4) discussed some of the limitations of that study. Given the increasing emphasis on molecular genetic approaches to address familial Abstract disorders coupled with the latest evidence questioning the role The recent completion of a rough draft of the human of genetics in cancer susceptibility, a reassessment of the role genome sequence has ushered in a new era of molecular of genetic factors in cancer susceptibility generally and for genetics research into the inherited basis of a number of site-specific cancers in particular appears warranted. complex diseases such as cancer. At the same time, recent twin studies have suggested a limited role of genetic Study Designs susceptibility to many neoplasms. A reappraisal of family and twin studies for many cancer sites suggests the Familial aggregation of a trait is a necessary but not sufficient following general conclusions: (a) all cancers are familial condition to infer the importance of genetic susceptibility, be- to approximately the same degree, with only a few cause environmental and cultural influences can also aggregate exceptions (both high and low); (b) early age of diagnosis in families, leading to family clustering and excess familial risk.
    [Show full text]