Haswelltodddissertationfinal Proquest Version

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Haswelltodddissertationfinal Proquest Version THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO THE TURN TO THE SELF: A HISTORY OF AUTISM, 1910–1944 A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE DIVISION OF THE HUMANITIES IN CANDIDACY FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF GERMANIC STUDIES BY STEPHEN D. HASWELL TODD CHICAGO, ILLINOIS DECEMBER 2015 © 2015 by Stephen Haswell Todd To my grandparents: Bunny and Jack Todd and Ruth (Haswell) and (in memoriam) Alex Sidorowicz Je n’étais pas seul, j’étais un homme quelconque. Cette formule, comment l’oublier? —Maurice Blanchot, Le Très-Haut Contents List of Tables vi Acknowledgments vii Introduction 1 Chapter One Eugen Bleuler: Autism as the Sickness of the Human 31 Chapter Two Autism between Affect and Logic: From Bleuler to Asperger 87 Chapter Three Hans Asperger: Autism as the General Possibility of Thinking 149 Chapter Four Autism Comes to America: Georg Frankl between Asperger and Kanner 199 Works Cited 263 v List of Tables 1. Fundamental distinctions between Naturwissenschaft and holism 100 2. Kretschmer’s typology of physique and character 110 3. Comparison of patients’ body types as recorded by Weiss and Asperger 220 vi Acknowledgments This text owes its existence to Christopher Wild, whose leap of faith in the beginning, in agreeing to sponsor and advise my project with no assurance of its eventual fruition, is all the more appreciated in retrospect. His presence as Doktorvater has been that of a steady hand on the wheel. David Wellbery and Mark Payne bravely committed themselves to the work of dissertation readers and displayed unaccountable patience and generosity in carrying it out. The remaining members of the Department of Germanic Studies at the University of Chicago, both faculty and graduate students, have all given something—an idea, a criticism, a kind word—to this work. Departmental coordinator Michelle Zimet has given more than most, which will surprise no one with any experience of university bureaucracy, or Michelle’s particular way of staring it down in inimitable style. The American Psychiatric Association and in particular Gary McMillan, the librarian and archivist there at the time of my research visit, have my thanks for facilitating my access to critical source material. Paul Belloni, bibliographer for psychology at the University of Chicago libraries, subsequently performed indefatigable detective work on the biographies of the elusive Anni Weiss and Georg Frankl. The staff of the University of Chicago’s interlibrary loan office went to heroic—and at times expensive—lengths to retrieve obscure sources from distant shelves, and never complained. Pat Haswell edited, proofread, and formatted the final text, making many improvements to both style and argument. Any errors that remain, however, are my own. vii A few of those who supported and contributed to this work in less specific but no less valuable ways are listed below, with gratitude (and apologies to the many I have not named): Milena Ang Collán Granillo Pat Haswell Ann Kaplan Tom Pepper Dick Todd Adam Weg viii Introduction Il y a le nom et la chose... 1 —Montaigne The chapters that follow present a history of the concept of autism and elucidate the ways in which this concept moved through psychopathological and other discourses in the early twentieth century. This is not the same as a history of the phenomenon now called autism. But the two are related. The fulcrum of this history of autism is the moment when the name “autism” and the thing now called by that name come together: a concept that already existed joins itself to a thing that, before then, had not existed in the way it does now. The thing in question, autism, is what we are all familiar with from hearing about it in the recent news: a mental abnormality or disability, enigmatic and yet recognizable, a clinical psychiatric diagnosis attached to a relatively small but ever-growing number of people. The name, on the other hand—“autism” in the full range of meanings it had before it became limited to its present object—is as strange to us as the thing we call autism is familiar. And the thing we call autism is becoming very familiar indeed. An estimate recently quoted in the press is that the label “autistic” applies to one in sixty-eight American children,2 though perhaps a more telling measure—and surely just as numerically impressive—would be a count of the newspaper and magazine articles that have been published about it in recent 1. As with so many ideas that have found their way into this work, I gratefully take this epigraph from Montaigne by way of another, my friend and teacher Thomas Pepper. 2. Sandhya Somashekhar, “How Autistic Adults Banded Together to Start a Movement,” Washington Post, July 20, 2015. https://www.washingtonpost.com/politics/the-next-civil-rights-movement-accepting-adults-with-autism/ 2015/07/20/e7c6743e-f338-11e4-bcc4-e8141e5eb0c9_story.html. Accessed October 14, 2015. 1 decades. With increasing prevalence and visibility, however, new controversy has arisen over the nature of autism. Not long ago it could be viewed simply as a disease or disability, and the only important questions were when and how it could be treated, cured, or prevented. But now, the same newspaper article that reports the latest incidence figures refers to people with autism as “a minority group” and as the subjects of “an emerging civil rights movement,”3 heirs “to the gay rights movement and to the protesters trying to improve police treatment of African Americans.”4 The ultimate goal of the “neurodiversity” movement is to remove the status of a pathology from autism and to assert an autistic identity not unlike those assumed on the bases of sexuality, race, gender, and so forth. Neurodiversity makes ambitious political claims. To many it will seem far-fetched to raise autism—this peculiar and, despite the rising numbers, still somewhat unusual phenomenon—to the same stature as categories of race and sexuality on the political stage, let alone to claim specific rights for “neurological minorities.” Whether these claims are justified is not a question this text seeks to answer. But it is a question that, even to be properly posed, calls for a far better understanding of the history of autism than has been available. In fact, it calls for a different kind of history than autism has had up to now: in addition to the “natural history” of the phenomenon of modern autism, we need a discursive history of autism in order to understand the very reasons for and implications of assigning this name to a particular category of person. How could a discursive history of autism help to clarify the stakes of the public debate over its political status? We can take the comparisons to race and sexuality made by the advocates of neurodiversity themselves as a starting point. Both race and sexuality have natural 3. John Elder Robison, quoted ibid. 4. Ibid. 2 histories, of course: in both cases, histories of biological givens and cultural norms interacting with each other in limitlessly complex ways, and ultimately receding, the further back we go in evolutionary time, into indifference from each other. But race and sexuality have also been made subjects of discursive history, and so offer us valuable precedents. Taking the history of homosexuality as a case study, we can say that natural history posits it as an unvarying factor in human biology, and goes on to study its varying manifestations in different cultural contexts (so we should perhaps more accurately term this “natural-cultural history”). There have always been people whose orientation was toward the same sex, this history assumes; the changing appearances of homosexuality through history are functions of such people’s varying ability—or lack thereof, conditioned by social stigma and oppression—to express the essence of their homosexuality in their actual behavior. But in filling out the details of this history, we run into empirical obstacles: cultures (the classic example being ancient Greece) in which not just homosexuality as a distinct “orientation,” but the very idea of a stable, innate sexual identity appears not to have made sense to the collective spirit, at least not in the way it makes self-evident sense to us now.5 And this in turn has made it possible to look even much closer to home, from the Renaissance to the European nineteenth century, for example, and to ask seriously whether homosexuality itself was not a “construct” that emerged from a shifting matrix of bodily and spiritual practices and discursive factors ranging from the poetic to the medical—rather than a practice stemming from an unchanging essence and its variable expression. Which history is “correct”? That itself is not a question to be settled by way of an empirical metahistory capable of adjudicating between the alternatives. It is a choice with its own further epistemological and political stakes. 5. See Kenneth Dover, Greek Homosexuality (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1978). 3 In any case, few scholarly programs in recent times have been as influential as Michel Foucault’s historicization of sexuality as a discourse, which opened up an entirely new register in which questions about sexuality could be asked.6 Previously, the political positions one could take regarding homosexuality were essentially limited to two: for or against. One could decry it as a pathology or an offense, or one could affirm it as an identity. Foucault’s discursive historicism unseated the claims of pathology by proposing that it was pathologization itself, as a discursive formation, that had created “the homosexual” as a new category of person—not discovered the essence of homosexuality; but Foucault mounted an equally serious challenge to the politics of identity, revealing identitarian claims to be just another set of ahistorical and essentialist propositions about homosexuality, and just as dependent on the same pathological discourses that invented the homosexual.
Recommended publications
  • Bulletin Spring 2010
    THE UNIVE RSIT Y of EDINBU RGH SPRING 2010 bTHE UNIVuERSITY OlF ElDINeBURGH StTAFFiMAn GAZINE Life through a lens Our Vet School is captured on camera – page 8 One world, Cultivating the Hard to Mapping the one vision revolution swallow universe A new health academy The collective campus Can a government policy Physicist Dr Phil Clark on the aims to make the world effort to reduce our carbon improve the treatment of University’s role in the ATLAS a better place – page 7 footprint – page 10 depression? – page14 experiment – page 17 advertisement... “I want to help future generations of researchers continue our work.” THE UNIVERSITY of EDINBURGH CAMPAIGN Kath Melia is Professor of Nursing Studies role in developing nursing at Edinburgh, at the University of Edinburgh. This year, had remembered the University in her will, she took the significant step of making a gift a gesture which is helping the important in her will to help continue the work and work we are doing today. This has inspired research of the University. me to do the same.” Kath explains, “I was touched to find that a By making a gift in your will, you, too, former colleague, who had played a pivotal can help shape the future of Edinburgh. Legacies from former members of staff at the University of If you would like more information on leaving a legacy to the Edinburgh support teaching, facilities and research across the University of Edinburgh or if you have already done so, Schools and Colleges. We are very grateful for this commitment. please contact our Legacy team in confidence: This support is vital to help this work continue and legacies, big email: [email protected] and small, can make a difference to a research project or help [email protected] support a junior researcher present their work for the first time.
    [Show full text]
  • Childhood Autism: Its Diagnosis, Nature, and Treatment
    Archives ofDisease in Childhood 1991; 66: 737-741 737 CURRENT TOPIC Arch Dis Child: first published as 10.1136/adc.66.6.737 on 1 June 1991. Downloaded from Childhood autism: its diagnosis, nature, and treatment Sula Wolff Leo Kanner,I although not the first to describe epidemiological prevalence studies have found this serious and intriguing developmental dis- two to four autistic children in every 10 000, order of early childhood,2 so precisely captured more if severely retarded children with autistic its essential features that the clinical account of features are included.2 his first 11 cases has never been bettered. The There have been several long term follow up salient characteristics he mentioned were an studies of autistic clinic attenders but only two 'extreme autistic aloneness' from the beginning of total population samples.5 6 Gillberg and of life, delay and abnormality in language Steffenburg followed up 23 autistic children and acquisition with echolalia and pronomial rever- 23 with 'other psychoses', often associated with sal, and 'an obsessional desire for the mainte- organic handicapping conditions, to the age of nance of sameness' in the presence of islets of 16-23 years.6 In 17% of both groups outcome normality, in particular an excellent rote mem- was 'good' or 'fair', and in 44% of autistic and ory. He saw the primary deficit as an inborn dis- 70% of other psychotic children it was 'poor' or turbance of affective contact and later described 'very poor'. Among this still quite young group similar, although very mild features of emotio- of subjects only one was self supporting, 23 nal coldness and obsessionality in the parents.
    [Show full text]
  • Sigmund Freud, Arthur Schnitzler, and the Birth of Psychological Man Jeffrey Erik Berry Bates College, [email protected]
    Bates College SCARAB Honors Theses Capstone Projects Spring 5-2012 Sigmund Freud, Arthur Schnitzler, and the Birth of Psychological Man Jeffrey Erik Berry Bates College, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://scarab.bates.edu/honorstheses Recommended Citation Berry, Jeffrey Erik, "Sigmund Freud, Arthur Schnitzler, and the Birth of Psychological Man" (2012). Honors Theses. 10. http://scarab.bates.edu/honorstheses/10 This Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Capstone Projects at SCARAB. It has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Theses by an authorized administrator of SCARAB. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Sigmund Freud, Arthur Schnitzler, and the Birth of Psychological Man An Honors Thesis Presented to The Faculty of the Departments of History and of German & Russian Studies Bates College In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts By Jeffrey Berry Lewiston, Maine 23 March 2012 Acknowledgements I would like to thank my thesis advisors, Professor Craig Decker from the Department of German and Russian Studies and Professor Jason Thompson of the History Department, for their patience, guidance and expertise during this extensive and rewarding process. I also would also like to extend my sincere gratitude to the people who will be participating in my defense, Professor John Cole of the Bates College History Department, Profesor Raluca Cernahoschi of the Bates College German Department, and Dr. Richard Blanke from the University of Maine at Orno History Department, for their involvement during the culminating moment of my thesis experience. Finally, I would like to thank all the other people who were indirectly involved during my research process for their support.
    [Show full text]
  • Does Autism Merit Belief? Developing an Account of Scientific Realism for Psychiatry
    Sam Fellowes, BSc, MA Does autism merit belief? Developing an account of scientific realism for psychiatry Ph.D. Philosophy March 2016 1 Abstract Sam Fellowes, BSc, MA Does autism merit belief? Developing an account of scientific realism for psychiatry Ph.D. Philosophy March 2016 The PhD outlines criteria under which a psychiatric classification merits belief and, as a case study, establishes that autism merits belief. Three chapters respond to anti- realist arguments, three chapters establish conditions under which psychiatric classifications merit belief. Chapter one addresses the pessimistic meta-induction. I historically analyse autism to show there has been sufficient historical continuity to avoid the pessimistic meta induction. Chapter two considers arguments from underdetermination. I consider the strongest candidate for an alternative to autism, classificatory changes which occurred between 1980 and 1985. I argue this does not constitute underdetermination because those changes were methodologically and evidentially flawed. Chapter three considers theory-ladenness. I consider the two strongest candidates for background theories which might have a negative epistemic effect (cognitive psychology and psychoanalysis). I show these have little influence on what symptoms are formulated or how symptoms are grouped together. Chapter four argues against psychiatric classifications as natural kinds and against notions that inductive knowledge of psychiatric classifications requires robust causes. I outline psychiatric classifications as scientific laws. They are high level idealised models which guide construction of lower level, more specific, models. This opens alternative routes to belief for psychiatric classification lacking robust causes. Chapter five shows that psychiatric classifications can set relevant populations for deriving statistically significant symptoms. The same behaviour can count as statistically significant for one psychiatric classification but not another.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurotribes 20/20
    Neurotribes 20/20, (March 13, 1992). The Street Where They Lived. Part 1, ABC News 20/20, (March 20, 1992.). The Street Where They Lived. Part 2, ABC News A Tribute to Eric Schopler (1927–2006). http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=D_THeWH0ox4 Adams, G. S. and Kanner, L. (1926). General Paralysis among the North American Indians: A Contribution to Racial Psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry Aichhorn, A. (1965). Wayward Youth. New York, NY: Penguin Books repr. ed. Aldiss, B. W. (1995). The Detached Retina: Aspects of SF and Fantasy. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press Allison, H. (1987). Perspectives on a Puzzle Piece. National AutisticSociety Aly, G. Chroust, P. and Pross, C. (1994). Cleansing the Fatherland: Nazi Medicine and Racial Hygiene, Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press American Journal of Psychiatry, (1939). News and Notes. 96(3). 736–46 American Philosophical Society. Eugenics Record Office Records, 1670–1964 American-Austrian Foundation. The Medical Club—Billrothhaus: Epoch-Making Lectures in Medical History.” http://www.aaf-online.org/ Anderson v. W. R. Grace: Background/About the Case, Seattle University School of Law. http://www.law.seattleu.edu/centers-and-institutes/films-for-justice-institute/lessons-from- woburn/about-the-case Anderson, E. L. (1988). Behavioral Treatment of Autism. Documentary by Edward L. Anderson and Robert Aller. Focus International Andreas Ströhle et al. (2008.). Karl Bonhoeffer (1868–1948). American Journal of Psychiatry Andrews, J. (1997). The History of Bethlem. (pp. 272). New, York: Psychology Press Angres, R. (Oct. 1980). Who, Really, Was Bruno Bettelheim? Commentary Anthony, E. J. (1958). An Experimental Approach to the Psychopathology of Childhood Autism.
    [Show full text]
  • Henry Walton. in Memoriam
    IN MEMORIAM Henry Walton. In Memoriam Andrzej Wojtczak El 13 de julio de 2012, la educación médica mundial On July 13th 2012 the world medical education lost Exdirector del International Institute for Medical Education perdió uno de sus más preeminentes educadores a one of it’s most prominent educators at the age of 88 (IME). la edad de 88 años, el profesor Henry Walton. En su – Professor Henry Walton. He definitely deserves the Expresidente de la Association figura debemos reconocer a uno de los padres fun- recognition, and position, of one of the founding fa- for Medical Education in Europe dadores de la educación médica contemporánea. thers of contemporary medical education. (AMEE). Nacido en Sudáfrica, se graduó en Medicina en Born in South Africa, he graduated from the E-mail: la Facultad de Medicina de Ciudad del Cabo y des- medical school in Cape Town, and after special [email protected] pués de un especial entrenamiento en Londres y training in London and New York in 1967, he be- © 2012 Educación Médica Nueva York en 1967, llegó a ser catedrático de psi- came Professor of Psychiatry at the University of Ed- quiatría en la Universidad de Edimburgo. En el año inburgh. In 1986 he took the Chair of International 1986 accedió a la Cátedra de Educación Médica In- Medical Education at the University of Edinburgh. ternacional de la referida universidad. For many of us he will be remembered as a Por muchos de nosotros será recordado como el founder and first President of the Association for fundador y primer presidente de la Association for Medical Education in Europe (AMEE) that was Medical Education in Europe established in 1972.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction Chapter 1: Asperger and I
    notes INTRODUCTION p. ix “a devastating handicap ...” Uta Frith, “Asperger and His Syndrome,” in Frith, 1991,p.5. p. ix “Paucity of empathy; naive, inappropriate, one-sided social inter- action ...” Klin, Ami, and Fred Volkmar, “Autism and Asperger’s Syndrome,” Yale University Child Study Center, July/August 8–9, 1994. p. xi “the first plausible variant to crystallize out of the autism spectrum ...” Uta Frith, “Asperger and His Syndrome,” in Frith, 1991,p.5. p. xi “More than anything, ADD represents ...” DeGrandpre, 2000,p.39. p. xiii The exchange between Diller and Greene can be read in the archives of Salon magazine (http://archive.salon.com). The fireworks began with Diller’s review of Greene’s The Explosive Child on July 18, 2001, and Greene’s response on July 19. p. xvi “The path to understanding ...” Hans Asperger as quoted in Klin et al, 2000, p. xii. CHAPTER 1: ASPERGER AND I p. 2 “I like the scenery. I like the schedules.” This and other quotations in this chapter by and about Darius McCollum are from “Irresistible Lure of Subways Keeps Landing Impostor in Jail,” by Dean E. Murphy, The New York Times, August 24, 2000,p.A1. p. 5 “Autism as a subject touches on the deepest questions of ontology ...” Sacks, 1996,p.246. p. 6 “Almost all of my social contacts ...” Sacks, 1996,p.261. p. 8 “America is the first society to be totally dominated ...” Postman, 1994,p. 145. p. 9 “What, after all, is normality?” Uta Frith, “Asperger and His Syn- drome,” in Frith, 1991,p.23.
    [Show full text]
  • Possible Aetiological Factors in Childhood Leukaemia
    Asperger's sydrome 179 acceptance of the children's difficulties as constitutional: same services as well functioning autistic children. The neither wilful nor parent engendered. In later life, many of more intelligent and less obviously handicapped Asperger/ them had good work achievements and many married, but schizoid children require above all to be identified as consti- Arch Dis Child: first published as 10.1136/adc.66.2.179 on 1 February 1991. Downloaded from their overall work adjustment, their heterosexual adjust- tutionally impaired.14 Their special make up needs to be ment, and their psychiatric state were worse than those of understood. They may also need special arrangements other referred children grown up (S Wolff, to be published). made, particularly at school, in the knowledge that in later These children resembled the schizotypal children descri- life when pressures for conformity are less than during the bed by Nagy and Szatmari,1° and their difficulties certainly school years, many will be able to find their social niche, overlapped with those of the patients of Wing and Tantam. and a few to develop their rather exceptional gifts. But whether they too should be included in an 'autistic spec- SULA WOLFF trum' will depend on the results of more definitive genetic University Department of Psychiatry, studies. The Kennedy Tower, For this reason, the arguments put forward by Professor Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Cox in this issue against a global diagnosis of autism,"1 12 Morningside Park, and in defence of a distinct diagnostic category of Asperger's Edinburgh EJO SHF syndrome, although based on Wing's rather narrow defini- 1 Asperger H.
    [Show full text]
  • Copy of WITNESS SEMINAR FINAL 2009
    WITNESS SEMINAR CENTRE FOR THE HISTORY OF MEDICINE UNIVERSITY OF GLASGOW THE DEVELOPMENT OF CHILD AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHIATRY FROM 1960 UNTIL 1990 12 May 2009 Organisers Emeritus Professor Philip Graham, Institute of Child Health, London Dr Malcolm Nicolson, Director, Centre for the History of Medicine, University of Glasgow Editors Philip Graham Helen Minnis Malcolm Nicolson Copy Editor David Sutton Contents Introduction 1 Seminar Programme 3 Transcript of Proceedings 4 Reflections 106 List of Participants 110 Appendix: Biographical Information 111 Front row: Ian Berg, Dora Black, Lionel Hersov, Bryan Lask, Philip Graham Second row: Arnon Bentovim, William Yule, Sebastian Kraemer Back row: Bob Jezzard, Michael Rutter, Malcolm Nicolson, Hugh Morton Introduction Philip Graham, Emeritus Professor of Child Psychiatry, Institute of Child Health, London The specialty of child and adolescent psychiatry began in Britain in the 1920s. 1 The period from the 1960s to the present time has been exceptionally rapid both in academic and service development. We decided that a project was needed to capture the oral history memories of some of those who participated in the earlier part of this period from 1960 to 1990. Witness seminars are features of academic contemporary history research, and have for several years been used in the exploration of medical history by the Wellcome Trust at University College London. 2 Our seminar was hosted by the Centre for the History of Medicine at the University of Glasgow in May 2009. “Witnesses” were selected on the basis of their contributions to different spheres of the specialty. Because of time constraints it was not possible to invite all of those who had played a significant part.
    [Show full text]
  • Sula (Sulammith) Wolff
    COLUMNS Obituaries Sula (Sulammith) Wolff established the first of several life-long professional Formerly Consultant in Child Psychiatry at Edinburgh affiliations, and an empirical tradition that remained undimin- Hospital ished during her subsequent career. She then left with Henry for Cape Town on his return to head the University Department Although she was 85 years old, of Psychiatry but, like many others, decided that they could not the death last year of the child remain under apartheid. They returned to the UK after a period psychiatrist Sula Wolff (on 21 in New York where Sula’s interest in the importance of September 2009) still came as a individual differences between children was fostered by shock to many because her working with Stella Chess, whose pioneering work with Alex vigour and lively presence had Thomas on children’s individual temperament inspired many seemed undiminished. Born in others too. 1924 in Berlin, treatment of a These origins are clear in the work Sula subsequently thumb injury as a 5-year-old child by a woman doctor undertook in Edinburgh - not only in how children individually determined her to become one herself. Sula’s Jewish family experience stress, but in her studies of patients whose moved to the safety of London and escaped Nazi persecution. temperamental traits seemed to isolate them from others, Once here, she quickly learned English, excelled at school, and unable to share the ‘lived experiences’ of most of their peers. went on to Oxford to study medicine. She qualified BM, BCh in Colleagues remember too her lively interest in their own 1947.
    [Show full text]
  • Copyrighted Material
    1 The Two Great Pioneers “Nothing is totally original. Everyone is influenced by what’s gone before.” (Dr. Lorna Wing in conversation with Adam Feinstein) “Whatever will they think of next?” (Reported comment by the Hollywood producer, Sam Goldwyn, on being shown an ancient sundial) The two great pioneers in the field of autism, Dr. Hans Asperger and Dr. Leo Kanner, started work in this area at roughly the same time— the 1930s. But they were very different human beings and, while their notions of the condition they first described overlapped to some extent, there are significant differences that still need exploring—and allegations of plagiarism and Nazi allegiances which also require examination. The Scottish child psychiatrist, Dr. Fred Stone, was one of the few people who met both Asperger and Kanner. Stone told me: “There couldn’t be a bigger contrast between the two men. I met Kanner in Edinburgh in the mid-1950s. He was very spruce, carefully dressed, cautious but pleasant. I liked him.”1 Stone met Hans Asperger at a conference in Vienna in the 1960s. “He was on duty ‘welcoming’ people—actually, he didn’t welcome anybody, he justCOPYRIGHTED sat there at the door of the MATERIALlecture theater. I had just heard about his syndrome from the German-speaking members of my planning committee. I could not engage him. I think that those who claim that he may have been suffering from the syndrome that would later bear his name could be right.”2 Most people who met Kanner reported on his warmth and charm.
    [Show full text]
  • The Fate of the Adopted Child SULA WOLFF from the Department of Psychological Medicine, Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Edinburgh
    Arch Dis Child: first published as 10.1136/adc.49.3.165 on 1 March 1974. Downloaded from Review article Archives of Disease in Childhood, 1974, 49, 165. The fate of the adopted child SULA WOLFF From the Department of Psychological Medicine, Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Edinburgh Problems of adoption research far from shying away from such ventures, in fact Until quite recently adoption was considered to be revealed themselves as willing and interested such a delicate social transaction that outcome research subjects. studies were attempted only on self-selected Other difficulties in the interpretation and subjects. McWhinnie (1967), for example, studied usefulness of the results of adoption research then adults whom she recruited by asking general became apparent. These are twofold. First, as practitioners to canvas patients known by them to with all long-term longitudinal studies, the factors have been adopted. Clearly, not all adults on these found to determine outcome of adoption in people doctors' lists had revealed their adoptive status and who are now adults relate only to the circumstances certainly only a proportion of these in fact agreed to which existed when these particular people were take part in McWhinnie's study. The finding that children. Public attitudes to illegitimacy, family only 21 of the 58 people studied were well or fairly size, one-parent families, and to adoption are well adjusted at the time of follow-up may reflect the changing. The choice of whether or not to place a adverse effects of growing up adopted. It is more baby for adoption may be determined by quite likely, however, to be a measure of the personality different considerations now than 30 years ago.
    [Show full text]