How Did Ancient Humans Learn to Count?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

How Did Ancient Humans Learn to Count? Feature F. D’ERRICO F. Prehistoric accounting? Markings made on a hyena bone by a Neanderthal might have recorded numerical information. HOW DID ANCIENT HUMANS LEARN TO COUNT? Recent archaeological studies and other analyses have spurred researchers to construct some of the first detailed hypotheses describing the prehistoric development of number systems. By Colin Barras ome 60,000 years ago, in what is and he thinks that the hyena bone — found he was venturing into territory that few now western France, a Neanderthal in the 1970s at the site of Les Pradelles near scientists had explored: the ancient roots picked up a chunk of hyena femur Angoulême — stands out as unusual. Although of numbers. “The origin of numbers is still a and a stone tool and began to work. ancient carved artefacts are often interpreted relatively vacant niche in scientific research,” When the task was complete, the as artworks, the Les Pradelles bone seems to says Russell Gray, an evolutionary biologist bone bore nine notches that were have been more functional, says D’Errico. at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary strikingly similar and approximately He argues that it might encode numerical Anthropology in Leipzig, Germany. Research- parallel, as if they were meant to information. And if that’s correct, anatomi- ers don’t even agree, at times, on what num- Ssignify something. cally modern humans might not have been bers are, although a 2017 study2 defined them Francesco d’Errico, an archaeologist at the alone in developing a system of numerical as discrete entities with exact values that are University of Bordeaux, France, has an idea notations: Neanderthals might have begun represented by symbols in the form of words about the marks. He has examined many to do so, too1. and signs. ancient carved artefacts during his career, When D’Errico published his ideas in 2018, Now the origin of numbers is attracting 22 | Nature | Vol 594 | 3 June 2021 ©2021 Spri nger Nature Li mited. All ri ghts reserved. ©2021 Spri nger Nature Li mited. All ri ghts reserved. increasing attention as researchers from a chicks3 can instantly recognize quantities words and written signs that are used to repre- variety of fields address the problem from up to four, a skill known as subitizing. Some sent them, must be produced by cultural evo- different vantage points. animals are also capable of ‘large-quantity lution — a process in which individuals learn Cognitive scientists, anthropologists and discrimination’: they can appreciate the dif- through imitation or formal teaching to adopt psychologists are looking at contemporary ference between two large quantities if they a new skill (such as how to use a tool). cultures to understand differences among are distinct enough. Creatures with this skill Although many animals have culture, one existing number systems — defined as the could, for example, distinguish 10 objects that involves numbers is essentially unique to symbols that a society uses for counting and from 20 objects, but not 20 from 21. Six- humans. A handful of chimpanzees have been manipulating numbers. Their hope is that month-old human infants also show a similar taught in captivity to use abstract symbols to clues buried in modern systems might illu- appreciation of quantity, even before they represent quantities, but neither chimps nor minate details of their origins. Meanwhile, have had significant exposure to human cul- any other non-human species use such sym- archaeologists have begun looking for evi- ture or language. bols in the natural world. Núñez suggests that a dence of ancient numerical notations, and distinction should therefore be made between evolutionary biologists with an interest in what he has dubbed the innate ‘quantical’ cog- language are exploring the deep origins of nition seen in animals and the learnt ‘numeri- number words. These studies have spurred cal’ cognition seen in humans2. researchers to formulate some of the first “The origin of But not everyone agrees. Nieder argues that detailed hypotheses for the prehistoric devel- numbers is still a neurological studies show clear similarities opment of number systems. between the way in which quantities are pro- And an infusion of funding will stimulate relatively vacant niche cessed in the brains of non-human animals and more studies in this area. This year, an inter- in scientific research.” how the human brain processes numbers. He national research team with a €10-million says that it is misleading to draw too firm a (US$11.9-million) grant from the European line between the two behaviours4, although Research Council will start to test different What all of this suggests, says Andreas he agrees that human numerical abilities hypotheses, as part of a broader effort to Nieder, a neuroscientist at the University of are much more advanced than those of any study when, why and how number systems Tübingen, Germany, is that humans have an other animal. “No [non-human] animal is able appeared and spread around the world. The innate appreciation of numbers. That arose to truly represent number symbols,” he says. project, called the Evolution of Cognitive Tools through evolutionary processes such as nat- D’Errico’s analysis of the Les Pradelles bone for Quantification (QUANTA), might even pro- ural selection, he says, because it would have could help to provide some insights into how vide insights into whether number systems carried adaptive benefits. the earliest stages of number systems took are unique to anatomically modern humans, Others interpret the evidence differently. shape. He studied the nine notches under a or were conceivably present in nascent form Rafael Núñez, a cognitive scientist at the Uni- microscope, and says that their shapes, depths in Neanderthals. versity of California, San Diego, and one of and other details are so alike that all seem to the leaders of QUANTA, accepts that many have been made using the same stone tool, An instinct for numbers animals might have an innate appreciation of held in the same way. This suggests that all Although researchers once thought that quantity. However, he argues that the human were made by one individual in a single ses- humans were the only species with a sense of perception of numbers is typically much more sion lasting perhaps a few minutes or hours. quantity, studies since the mid-twentieth cen- sophisticated, and can’t have arisen through (At some other time, eight much shallower tury have revealed that many animals share the a process such as natural selection. Instead, marks were carved on the bone, too.) ability. For instance, fish, bees and newborn many aspects of numbers, such as the spoken However, D’Errico doesn’t think that this individual intended to produce a decora- tive pattern because the marks are uneven. For comparison, he has analysed the seven notches on a 40,000-year-old raven bone from a site of Neanderthal occupation in Crimea. Statistical analysis shows that the notches on this bone are spaced with the same sort of reg- ularity seen when modern volunteers are given a similar bone and asked to mark it with equally spaced notches5. But this type of analysis also shows that the marks on the Les Pradelles bone lack such regularity. That observation — and the fact that the notches were generated in a single session — led D’Errico to consider that they might have been merely functional, pro- viding a record of numerical information. Marks of sophistication The Les Pradelles bone is not an isolated find. For instance, during excavations at Border Cave in South Africa, archaeologists dis- covered an approximately 42,000-year-old MATTHEW HORWOOD/GETTY MATTHEW baboon fibula that was also marked with Some researchers suggest the mind extends beyond the brain to fingers and other objects. notches. D’Errico suspects that anatomically modern humans living there at the time used Nature | Vol 594 | 3 June 2021 | 23 ©2021 Spri nger Nature Li mited. All ri ghts reserved. ©2021 Spri nger Nature Li mited. All ri ghts reserved. Feature the bone to record numerical information. In QUANTA will use data from anthropology, cog- are still in use around the world. For example, the case of this bone, microscopic analysis nitive science, linguistics and archaeology to linguists Claire Bowern and Jason Zentz at of its 29 notches suggests they were carved better understand those social factors, says Yale University in New Haven, Connecticut, using four distinct tools and so represent four D’Errico, who is one of the project’s four prin- reported in a 2012 survey that 139 Aboriginal counting events, which D’Errico thinks took cipal investigators. Australian languages have an upper limit of place on four separate occasions1. Moreover, ‘three’ or ‘four’ for specific numerals. Some of he says that discoveries over the past 20 years Bones of contention those languages use natural quantifiers such show that ancient humans began producing However, QUANTA researcher Núñez, along as ‘several’ and ‘many’ to indicate higher val- abstract engravings, which hint at sophisti- with some researchers who are not involved ues9. There is even one group, the Pirahã peo- cated cognition, hundreds of thousands of in the project, cautions that ancient artefacts ple of the Brazilian Amazon, that is sometimes years earlier than was once thought. such as the Les Pradelles bone are challenging claimed not to use numbers at all10. In the light of these discoveries, D’Errico has to interpret. Karenleigh Overmann, a cognitive Overmann and other researchers stress that developed a scenario to explain how number archaeologist at the University of Colorado in there’s nothing intellectually lacking about systems might have arisen through the very Colorado Springs, highlights those difficulties societies that use relatively simple number act of producing such artefacts.
Recommended publications
  • Migracijske Teme 4/1988
    Migracijske teme 15 (1999), 1-2: 63-153 UDK: 809.45-0 Izvorni znanstveni rad Primljeno: 17. 11. 1998. Paolo Agostini University of Padova [email protected] LANGUAGE RECONSTRUCTION – APPLIED TO THE URALIC LANGUAGES* SUMMARY After pointing out the shortcomings and methodological weakness of the general theory of linguistic reconstruction, the author disputes the alleged antiquity of Uralic. Proto-Uralic as recon- structed by the scholars seems to be the sum of a set of features belonging to several distinct language families. The paper examines a number of lexical concordances with historically attested languages and comes to the conclusion that the Proto-Uralic word-stock is the result of a sum of borrowings that took place from the most disparate languages: Balto-Slavic, Old Swedish, several Turkic dialects, Mongolic, Tunguz, Aramaic, Hebrew, Arabic, late Middle Persian dialects, Byzantine Greek and Latin. Yet, other languages may also come into account: Chinese, Caucasian languages as well as lan- guages unknown in present day are possible candidates. A large number of bases of the Uralic word- stock can be easily identified by following a few phonological constraints. The linguistic features of the Uralic daughter-languages seem to show that they originated from a pidgin language spoken along the merchant routes that connected the Silk Road to North- and East-European trade. It is a well-known phenomenon that sometimes, when groups of people speaking different languages come into contact for the first time, a new restricted language system (lingua franca or pidgin) comes into being in order to cater to essential common needs.
    [Show full text]
  • CULTURAL HERITAGE in MIGRATION Published Within the Project Cultural Heritage in Migration
    CULTURAL HERITAGE IN MIGRATION Published within the project Cultural Heritage in Migration. Models of Consolidation and Institutionalization of the Bulgarian Communities Abroad funded by the Bulgarian National Science Fund © Nikolai Vukov, Lina Gergova, Tanya Matanova, Yana Gergova, editors, 2017 © Institute of Ethnology and Folklore Studies with Ethnographic Museum – BAS, 2017 © Paradigma Publishing House, 2017 ISBN 978-954-326-332-5 BULGARIAN ACADEMY OF SCIENCES INSTITUTE OF ETHNOLOGY AND FOLKLORE STUDIES WITH ETHNOGRAPHIC MUSEUM CULTURAL HERITAGE IN MIGRATION Edited by Nikolai Vukov, Lina Gergova Tanya Matanova, Yana Gergova Paradigma Sofia • 2017 CONTENTS EDITORIAL............................................................................................................................9 PART I: CULTURAL HERITAGE AS A PROCESS DISPLACEMENT – REPLACEMENT. REAL AND INTERNALIZED GEOGRAPHY IN THE PSYCHOLOGY OF MIGRATION............................................21 Slobodan Dan Paich THE RUSSIAN-LIPOVANS IN ITALY: PRESERVING CULTURAL AND RELIGIOUS HERITAGE IN MIGRATION.............................................................41 Nina Vlaskina CLASS AND RELIGION IN THE SHAPING OF TRADITION AMONG THE ISTANBUL-BASED ORTHODOX BULGARIANS...............................55 Magdalena Elchinova REPRESENTATIONS OF ‘COMPATRIOTISM’. THE SLOVAK DIASPORA POLITICS AS A TOOL FOR BUILDING AND CULTIVATING DIASPORA.............72 Natália Blahová FOLKLORE AS HERITAGE: THE EXPERIENCE OF BULGARIANS IN HUNGARY.......................................................................................................................88
    [Show full text]
  • Beginnings of Counting and Numbers Tallies and Tokens
    Beginnings of Counting and Numbers Tallies and Tokens Picture Link (http://www.flickr.com/photos/quadrofonic/834667550/) Bone Tallies • The Lebombo Bone is a portion of • The radius bone of a wolf, a baboon fibula, discovered in the discovered in Moravia, Border Cave in the Lebombo Czechoslovakia in 1937, and mountains of Swaziland. It dates dated to 30,000 years ago, has to about 35,000 years ago, and fifty‐five deep notches carved has 29 distinct notches. It is into it. Twenty‐five notches of assumed that it tallied the days of similar length, arranged in‐groups a lunar month. of five, followed by a single notch twice as long which appears to • terminate the series. Then Picture Link starting from the next notch, also (http://www.historyforkids.org/learn/africa/science/numbers.htm) twice as long, a new set of notches runs up to thirty. • Picture link (http://books.google.com/books?id=C0Wcb9c6c18C&pg=PA41&lpg=PA41 &dq=wolf+bone+moravia&source=bl&ots=1z5XhaJchP&sig=q_8WROQ1Gz l4‐6PYJ9uaaNHLhOM&hl=en&ei=J8D‐ TZSgIuPTiALxn4CEBQ&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=4&ved=0 CCsQ6AEwAw) Ishango Bone • Ishango Bone, discovered in 1961 in central Africa. About 20,000 years old. Ishango Bone Patterns • Prime 11 13 17 19 numbers? • Doubling? 11 21 19 9 • Multiplication? • Who knows? 3 6 4 8 10 5 5 7 Lartet Bone • Discovered in Dodogne, France. About 30,000 years old. It has various markings that are neither decorative nor random (different sets are made with different tools, techniques, and stroke directions).
    [Show full text]
  • Wood Identification and Chemistry' Covers the Physicalproperties and Structural Features of Hardwoods and Softwoods
    11 DOCUMENT RESUME ED 031 555 VT 007 853 Woodworking Technology. San Diego State Coll., Calif. Dept. of Industrial Arts. Spons Agency-Office of Education (DHEA Washington, D.C. Pub Date Aug 68 Note-252p.; Materials developed at NDEA Inst. for Advanced Studyin Industrial Arts (San Diego, June 24 -Au9ust 2, 1968). EDRS Price MF -$1.00 He -$13.20 Descriptors-Curriculum Development, *Industrial Arts, Instructional Materials, Learning Activities, Lesson Plans, Lumber Industry, Resource Materials, *Resource Units, Summer Institutes, Teaching Codes, *Units of Study (Sublect Fields), *Woodworking Identifiers-*National Defense Education Act TitleXIInstitute, NDEA TitleXIInstitute, Woodworking Technology SIX teaching units which were developed by the 24 institute participantsare given. "Wood Identification and Chemistry' covers the physicalproperties and structural features of hardwoods and softwoods. "Seasoning" explainsair drying, kiln drying, and seven special lumber seasoning processes. "Researchon Laminates" describes the bending of solid wood and wood laminates, beam lamination, lamination adhesives,. andplasticlaminates."Particleboard:ATeachingUnitexplains particleboard manufacturing and the several classes of particleboard and theiruses. "Lumber Merchandising" outhnes lumber grades andsome wood byproducts. "A Teaching Unitin Physical Testing of Joints, Finishes, Adhesives, and Fasterners" describes tests of four common edge pints, finishes, wood adhesives, and wood screws Each of these units includes a bibhography, glossary, and student exercises (EM) M 55, ...k.",z<ONR; z _: , , . "'zr ss\ ss s:Ts s , s' !, , , , zs "" z' s: - 55 Ts 5. , -5, 5,5 . 5, :5,5, s s``s ss ' ,,, 4 ;.< ,s ssA 11111.116; \ ss s, : , \s, s's \ , , 's's \ sz z, ;.:4 1;y: SS lza'itVs."4,z ...':',\\Z'z.,'I,,\ "t"-...,,, `,.
    [Show full text]
  • Tally Sticks, Counting Boards, and Sumerian Proto-Writing John Alan Halloran
    Early Numeration - John Alan Halloran - August 10, 2009 - Page 1 Early Numeration - Tally Sticks, Counting Boards, and Sumerian Proto-Writing John Alan Halloran http://www.sumerian.org/ Work on the published version of my Sumerian Lexicon (Logogram Publishing: 2006) has revealed that: 1) the early Sumerians used wooden tally sticks for counting; 2) tally marks led to proto-writing; 3) the Sumerian pictograms for goats and sheep probably derive from tally stick notch conventions; 4) the Uruk period civilization used counting boards; 5) the authors of the proto-cuneiform tablets drew with animal claws or bird talons; 6) the historical Sumerians used clay split tallies as credit instruments; and 7) the Sumerians may sometimes have counted by placing knots or stones on a string. 1. Counting with Wooden Tally Sticks 1.1. The following text caught my attention. It is from The Debate between Sheep and Grain, ETCSL 5.3.2, lines 130-133: "Every night your count is made and your tally-stick put into the ground, so your herdsman can tell people how many ewes there are and how many young lambs, and how many goats and how many young kids." The Sumerian reads: 130 ud šu2-uš-e niñ2-kas7-zu ba-ni-ak-e ñiš 131 ŠID-ma-zu ki i3-tag-tag-ge - the Unicode version has ñiš-šudum- ma-zu 132 na-gada-zu u8 me-a sila4 tur-tur me-a 133 ud5 me-a maš2 tur-tur me-a lu2 mu-un-na-ab-be2 š is read |sh| and ñ is read |ng|.
    [Show full text]
  • Guntram Hazod Introduction1 Hapter Two of the Old Tibetan Chronicle (PT 1287: L.63-117; Hereafter OTC.2)
    THE GRAVES OF THE CHIEF MINISTERS OF THE TIBETAN EMPIRE MAPPING CHAPTER TWO OF THE OLD TIBETAN CHRONICLE IN THE LIGHT OF THE EVIDENCE OF THE TIBETAN TUMULUS TRADITION Guntram Hazod Introduction1 hapter two of the Old Tibetan Chronicle (PT 1287: l.63-117; hereafter OTC.2)2 is well known as the short paragraph that C lists the succession of Tibet’s chief ministers (blon che, blon chen [po]) – alternatively rendered as “prime minister” or “grand chancel- lor” in the English literature. Altogether 38 such appointments among nineteen families are recorded from the time of the Yar lung king called Lde Pru bo Gnam gzhung rtsan until the end of the Tibet- an empire in the mid-ninth century. This sequence is conveyed in a continuum that does not distin- guish between the developments before and after the founding of the empire. Only indirectly is there a line that specifies the first twelve ministers as a separate group – as those who were endowed with 1 The resarch for this chapter was conducted within the framework of the two projects “The Burial Mounds of Central Tibet“, parts I and II (financed by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF); FWF P 25066, P 30393; see fn. 2) and “Materiality and Material Culture in Tibet“ (Austrian Academy of Sciences (AAS) project, IF_2015_28) – both based at the Institute for Social Anthropology at the Austrian Academy of Sciences. I wish to thank Joanna Bialek, Per K. Sørensen, and Chris- tian Jahoda for their valuable comments on the drafts of this paper, and J. Bialek especially for her assistance with lingustic issues.
    [Show full text]
  • New Revised Draft/Final Version
    The Prehistoric Origins of European Economic Integration George Grantham McGill University In recent decades the conventional dating of the origins of Western Europe’s economic ascendancy to the tenth and eleventh centuries AD has been called in question by archaeological findings and reinterpretations of the early medieval texts indicating significantly higher levels of material prosperity in Antiquity than conventional accounts consider plausible. On the basis of that evidence it appears likely that at its peak the classical economy was almost as large as that of Western Europe on the eve of the Industrial Revolution.1 Population estimates have also been revised upward. Lo Caschio has shown that the conventional estimates of the Italian population that Beloch extracted from late Republican and Augustan censuses to form the foundation of his much-cited conjectural estimate of the population of the Roman Empire significantly underreport the true population.2 That critique is supported by recent archaeological findings indicating that in the more fertile districts of southern Britain and northern Gaul rural population density in the Late Iron Age and the Roman period was as high as in the late seventeenth- century.3 As skeletal evidence shows no significant difference in body size as between 1 For a review of the evidence and its significance for modeling the pre-industrial economy, see George Grantham, ‘Contra Ricardo: On the macroeconomics of Europe’s agrarian age,’ European review of economic history 3 (1999), 199-232. 2 Elio Lo Caschio, ‘The size of the Roman population: Beloch and the meaning of the Augustan census figures,’ Journal of roman studies 84 (1994) pp.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Material Culture in Human Time Representation
    Original Paper Adaptive Behavior 19(1) 63–76 ! The Author(s) 2011 The role of material culture in human Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav time representation: Calendrical DOI: 10.1177/1059712310396382 adb.sagepub.com systems as extensions of mental time travel Johan De Smedt1 and Helen De Cruz2 Abstract Humans have cognitive mechanisms that allow them to keep track of time, represent past events, and simulate the future, but these capacities have intrinsic constraints. Here, we explore the role of material culture as an extension of internal time representations through anthropological and archeological case studies, focusing on Upper Paleolithic material culture. We argue that calendars complement and extend internal time representations, because they enable humans to project past events into the future more accurately than is possible with episodic memory alone, making them one of the factors that significantly improved foraging success during the Upper Paleolithic. We discuss the implications of the epistemic use of material culture for our understanding of the causes of shifts in human behavior during the Upper Paleolithic. Keywords Mental time travel, episodic thinking, calendars, epistemic artifacts, cognitive archeology 1 Introduction participants in studies like these are typically Ever since Darwin, comparative psychologists have Western college students, who are thoroughly encultu- considered the problem of the apparent mental discon- rated into a world filled with artifacts that serve epi- tinuity between humans and other animals. Why are stemic purposes, including banknotes, signposts, and humans, more so than other animals, capable of nutritional information on packaging. It is no exag- advanced cognition? Some authors have suggested geration to say that we inhabit a world that is primar- that a distinctive feature of human cognition is its ily made up of our own creations, and many of these interaction with the external environment (e.g., play a role in our cognitive lives.
    [Show full text]
  • (Neanderthal) Men: the Small Mammal Record of the Middle to Upper Paleolithic Transition in the Swabian Jura, Germany
    Of Mice and (Neanderthal) Men: The small mammal record of the Middle to Upper Paleolithic transition in the Swabian Jura, Germany Dissertation der Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen zur Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors der Naturwissenschaften (Dr. rer. nat.) vorgelegt von Sara E. Rhodes, M.Sc. aus Toronto (Kanada) Tübingen 2019 Gedruckt mit Genehmigung der Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen. Tag der mündlichen Qualifikation: 8. Juli 2019 Dekan: Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Rosenstiel 1. Berichterstatter: Prof. Nicholas J. Conard, PhD 2. Berichterstatter: PD Dr. Britt M. Starkovich Table of Contents I ABBREVIATIONS ................................................................................................................ III II ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .................................................................................................. IV III SUMMARY ......................................................................................................................... VI IV LIST OF PUBLICATIONS ............................................................................................... XIV V PERSONAL CONTRIBUTION .......................................................................................... XIV 1.0 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................... 1 1.1 NEANDERTHALS IN THE EUROPEAN CONTEXT ....................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Timeline for Homo Sapiens - 3Rd Edition from 2.5M B.C.E
    Timeline for Homo sapiens - 3rd Edition From 2.5M B.C.E. to 300,000 B.C.E. Hominin Species had organized the 1st Industrial Complexes making stone tools in quantities. By 1.8M B.C.E. hand axes and stone points that were flaked on two sides, hominins are demonstrating skill and technique. Undisputed evidence of a footprint that dates to 1.5M B.C.E. of a, "most Many Glacial likely", Homo erectus walking upright in Africa. Homo erectus fossils have been dated and from 1.8M B.C.E. to the 210,000 B.C.E. (12 discoveries are outside of Africa). Bone Interglacial Tools are found at the same epoch. From 1.5M B.C.E. to 790,000 B.C.E. evidence of Epochs occur hearths and cooking. The 1st living species that took control of fire. 500,000 B.C.E. over the 2.5 Hominin Species are hunting large animals with spears. Four wooden spears dated million years circa 400,000 B.C.E. have been found in Germany in 1995. Use of pigments on the body B.C.E. to and painting developed next around 400,000 B.C.E. to 300,000 B.C.E.. Descended from 300,000 B.C.E.. Sea levels rise Homo heidelbergensis, Homo neanderthalensis exists as a distinct species from around and fall 300ft 600,000 B.C.E./500,000 B.C.E. to around 26,000 B.C.E. and no fossils exist that are on average. younger than this time. 570,000/470,000 years tops Homo Sapiens by 370,000/270,000 Average years of existence as a distinct species.
    [Show full text]
  • "Seafield Tally Stick." 51 Note On
    E "SEAFIELTH NOT N O E D TALLY1 5 STICK." IV. E " SEAFIELNOTTH N EO D TALLY STICK." BY JOHN W. M. LONEY, P.S.A.ScoT. For many years there has been kept in the joiner's shop at Cullen e Housecountth n f i Banff,yo d ol ,n a whas thoughe b wa t o t t measuring rod. As a curiosity it was sent here for identification. t onca s e wa recognise t I Exchequen a s a d r Tally Stica complet n ki e state of preservation (fig. 1), and as at present there are only a few such tally e stickNationath n i s l Museum s satisfactori t i , recoro t y d Seafiele FigTh . 1 .d Tally Stick. that, for its safe-keeping, the Trustees of Caroline, Countess of Sea- field, have agreed to deposit this specimen as on loan from them. The tally sticks in the Museum are: (1) an inscribed specimen dated 177d 1793 anothe) 8an (2 , r specime f smalno l sizd apparentlean y much older, without inscription f greao t tbu , interest, inasmuc s bota h h duplicates are present, and (3) a good specimen recorded as having come from the Bombay Treasury Record Office, with a long inscrip- tion in Latin and of the time of King Charles II., and of interest as bein a g"right-hand whicn " ow stick I hshaly a m ter, lf mo explai n immediately. The name "Tally Stick" is given to the notched sticks which, till the beginning of the nineteenth century, were used in England for keeping account n Exchequeri s , answerin e doublth g e purposf o e receipt publid an s c records.1 The e describeyar s well-seasoneda d rods of hazel or willow, incised with notches on top (or bottom as required), which notches differed in breadth as standing for a penny, a shilling, 1 Chambers's Encyclopedia, 1860, vol.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction We All Know the Ten Digits of the Hindu-Arabic Counting System: 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9
    History of Numbers 1a. I am familiar with the early evolution of counting. Introduction We all know the ten digits of the Hindu-Arabic counting system: 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9. And when we read a number like 352, because of place value, we know it stands for three groups of a hundred, Iive groups of ten, and two units. Our numerals are usually arranged according to a positional base 10 (or decimal) system. PRACTICE. Are you familiar with our base 10 or decimal system and place value? In Canvas, work through these 7 practice problems: Identify value of a digit Yet while there are still Hindu-Arabic numerals involved, telling time reQuires a different system. There are 60 seconds in every minute and 60 minutes in every hour. So if your watch displays 12:59:59 right now, then you expect it to read 1:00:00 a second later: We are so used to telling time in groups of 60 that it seems natural. But why there are not 100 seconds in each minute, or 100 minutes in an hour? In the late 1700s, a French attorney suggested a system of decimal time measurement in which each day has 10 hours, each hour 100 minutes, and each minute 1000 seconds. The actual duration of these decimal hours, minutes, and seconds would be different; however, time conversions would be simpler. For example, 6 decimal hours = 600 decimal minutes = 600,000 decimal seconds. So why does our system of telling time not conform to the usual base 10 counting system that governs most other aspects of our life? BCE means "Before Common Era".
    [Show full text]